You are on page 1of 7

Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Patients with bipolar disorder show impaired performance on complex tests


of social cognition
Andrée M. Cusi a,b, Glenda M. MacQueen c, Margaret C. McKinnon a,b,d,n
a
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
b
Mood Disorders Program, St. Joseph’s Healthcare Hamilton, Ontario, Canada
c
Department of Psychiatry, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
d
Kunin-Lunenfeld Applied Research Unit, Baycrest, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The literature concerning social cognitive performance in people with bipolar disorder (BD) reveals a
Received 23 September 2011 mixed pattern of findings. We compared performance between patients with BD and matched controls
Received in revised form on two social cognitive tasks that involved: (i) the decoding of mental states from pictures of eyes
14 June 2012
(Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test), and (ii) a video-based test that requires participants to
Accepted 15 June 2012
discriminate social cues to make interpersonal judgments (Interpersonal Perception Task-15; IPT-15).
We also sought to evaluate the association between symptom severity, social functioning, and social
Keywords: cognitive ability in patients with BD. Relative to controls, patients with BD were impaired at
Cognition discriminating mental states from pictures of eyes and in making complex social judgments. Impaired
Theory of mind
responding on the IPT-15 was also associated with reduced psychosocial functioning. These results
Mood disorders
provide evidence of impaired performance on complex tests of social cognition in patients with BD.
Social perception
Social adjustment Impairments in social cognition may be associated with well-documented declines in the frequency of
social interactions and development of interpersonal relationships found in this patient population.
& 2012 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.

1. Introduction 2006). Given that patients with BD demonstrate well-documented


deficits on cognitive and affective tasks that tap these same central
Bipolar disorder (BD) is characterized by impairment in multi- processing resources (Kurtz and Gerraty, 2009; Kohler et al., 2011),
ple domains, including interpersonal and social functioning (Elgie it appears likely that patients with BD will show performance
and Morselli, 2007; Depp et al., 2010). Recently, there has been deficits on measures of social cognition.
increased interest in utilizing a social cognitive framework to Here, we examined the performance of patients with BD on tests
understand the mechanisms underlying social impairment among of theory of mind (ToM) and social perception. ToM is defined as the
patients with neuropsychiatric disorders (Bora et al., 2005; Olley ability to infer the mental states of others, including their beliefs,
et al., 2005; Lahera et al., 2008). Social cognition refers to the emotions, and intentions in order to explain or predict their
ability to perceive, understand, and respond to the intentions, behavior (Premack and Woodruff, 1978). A significant component
behaviors, and dispositions of others (Brothers, 1990; Adolphs, of ToM involves accurately decoding mental states from available
2001; Green et al., 2008). Social cognition encompasses a broad perceptual social information such as a person’s facial expressions,
range of domains, including social perception, emotion recogni- tone of voice, or gestures (Stone et al., 1998; Sabbagh, 2004). Social
tion, theory of mind, and empathy. Recent theoretical models perception is an aspect of social cognition that involves identifying
propose that this construct draws on both cognitive and affective ‘‘social roles, societal rules, and social context’’ (Green et al., 2008, p.
processing resources (Leslie et al., 2004; McKinnon and Moscovitch, 1212). During social perception tasks, individuals must process and
2007). Indeed, emerging evidence suggests that cognitive processes decode social cues (e.g., facial expressions, tone of voice, body
such as executive functioning and affective components such as language) to make inferences about complex social situations such
emotion recognition contribute to social cognitive performance (e.g., as kinship and status (Green et al., 2008; Vaskinn et al., 2009). Taken
Bora et al., 2005; Brüne, 2005; Henry et al., 2006; Sabbagh et al., together, these theoretical definitions reveal significant overlap, as
well as distinct processes, involved in these key components of
n
social cognition.
Corresponding author at: Mood Disorders Program, St. Joseph’s Healthcare,
100 West 5th Street, Box 585, Hamilton, ON, Canada L8N 3K7.
The majority of social cognition studies in BD to date have
Tel.: þ1 905 522 1155x35438; fax: þ 1 905 381 5610. focused on facial emotion recognition tasks, which evaluate the
E-mail address: mmckinno@stjosham.on.ca (M.C. McKinnon). accuracy of emotion perception conveyed in human faces (see

0165-1781/$ - see front matter & 2012 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.021
A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264 259

Kohler et al. (2011) for a recent review). The results of these central processing resources because participants are not asked to
studies are not entirely consistent; with some studies reporting retain information as it is a self-paced task, it is considered an
performance deficits (e.g., Yurgelun-Todd et al., 2000; Summers advanced measure of ToM ability for several reasons: first, the RMET
et al., 2006; Schenkel et al., 2007), and others demonstrating only includes complex mental states such as ‘contemplative’ and
equivalent (Addington and Addington, 1998; Edwards et al., 2001; ‘caution’ to increase variability in performance. Second, participants
Lembke and Ketter, 2002; Venn et al., 2004; Malhi et al., 2007b; must choose the correct mental state from one of four response
Vaskinn et al., 2007), and even enhanced facial emotion recogni- options, increasing the ability to detect individual differences in
tion performance in patients with BD relative to controls (Harmer performance. Third, the mental state distractor terms (response
et al., 2002). For example, a mood congruent bias in the appraisal options) are matched closely in emotional valence to the target
of emotional facial expressions has been identified among word, making it possible to detect subtle differences in performance
patients with active depressive and manic symptoms (Gur et al., (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). In contrast to Bora et al.’s (2005) study,
1992; Lembke and Ketter, 2002; Lennox et al., 2004; Almeida however, Shamay-Tsoory et al. (2009) reported that euthymic
et al., 2010; Douglas and Porter, 2010; Versace et al., 2010). patients showed intact recognition of both basic and complex
Euthymic patients with BD also show generalized facial emotion emotions depicted in sets of eyes. Importantly, Shamay-Tsoory
recognition deficits (McClure et al., 2005), and emotion-specific et al.’s (2009) task may not have been as cognitively challenging
impairments in recognizing fearful (Yurgelun-Todd et al., 2000) for the patient sample, as participants were required to choose the
and surprised faces (Summers et al., 2006). Several studies report, most accurate mental state from one of two mental state descrip-
however, that BD patients are equally as accurate as controls in tors, rather than four mental state words that are found in the
identifying facial emotion (Addington and Addington, 1998; original RMET task (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). The performance of
Edwards et al., 2001; Lembke and Ketter, 2002; Venn et al., patients with BD in mood states other than euthymia on the RMET
2004; Malhi et al., 2007b; Vaskinn et al., 2007). Affective prosody task is unknown.
recognition among patients with BD has been also been shown to Research examining the performance of BD patients on para-
be impaired (Bozikas et al., 2007) and intact (Vaskinn et al., 2007). digms that are ecologically valid and that approximate real-world
Preliminary evidence of altered empathic responding, defined as social cognition are widely lacking. One study by Montag et al.
the ability to understand and respond to another’s mental state (2010) found that relative to controls, euthymic BD patients
(Decety and Jackson, 2004), has also been reported in patients demonstrated impaired performance on the Movie for Assess-
with BD (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Cusi et al., 2010). ment of Social Cognition, a test that requires participants to
Research examining theory of mind (ToM) reveals similarly ascribe mental states to actors in everyday social situations. In a
mixed findings. One study reported deficits in a combined sample recent study, manic but not depressed patients showed signifi-
of remitted patients with unipolar and bipolar depression on cant deficits on another naturalistic video-based task involving
cognitively challenging ToM tasks that involve integrating and the attribution of intentions to movie actors (Bazin et al., 2009).
understanding the perspective of two characters simultaneously The Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15) is an example of a
(i.e., second-order false-belief questions) but not on less challen- measure that allows investigators to examine social perception in
ging ToM tests that involve inferring the perspective of a single an ecologically valid and complex manner (Costanzo and Archer,
character (i.e., first-order false-belief question; Inoue et al., 2004). 1993; Vaskinn et al., 2009). The IPT-15 assesses the participant’s
In patients with subsyndromal illness, however, deficits emerge ability to accurately perceive diverse verbal and nonverbal cues
on both first-order and second-order ToM questions; notably the (e.g., voice tone, gestures, haptics, posture, and facial expressions)
magnitude of deficit observed here is greater for the more in order to judge varied aspects of social situations, including
cognitively challenging second-order ToM tasks (McKinnon kinship, intimacy, deception, competition, and social status
et al., 2010). Kerr et al. (2003) also reported deficits on both first- (Costanzo and Archer, 1993; Vaskinn et al., 2009). The Kinship
and second-order ToM tests in BD patients who were actively subscale assesses the participant’s ability to determine the nature
depressed or manic; remitted patients, however, were unim- of the relationship between characters depicted on-screen (e.g.,
paired. Recent reports also point towards performance deficits What is the relationship between the man and woman?). The
on cognitively demanding tests of mental state attribution (e.g., Intimacy subscale involves assessing the level of intimacy
interpreting double bluffs, deception, and persuasions, and faux between characters depicted on-screen (e.g., How long have they
pas) among euthymic patients with BD (Olley et al., 2005; Lahera been dating, 2 weeks or 2 years?). The Status subscale involves
et al., 2008; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009). These particular tasks determining the social status of the portrayed characters (e.g.,
are considered cognitively challenging because they involve Which person is the other person’s boss?). The Deception subscale
integrating and holding in mind the mental states of two or more involves determining the veracity of the statements made by a
characters. For example, detecting a faux pas (when someone says single character (e.g., Which is the lie and which is the truth?).
something they should not have said, not realizing their mistake This subscale appears to overlap with the construct of ToM, given
is) requires the representation of both the person who committed that accurate performance on this measure relies heavily on
the faux pas and the listener’s mental state, as well as an identification of the feeling state and thought processes of the
understanding of the listener’s feelings or emotional response to depicted characters in order to detect deception. Moreover, the
the faux pas (Brüne and Brüne-Cohrs, 2006). Taken together, detection of deception is widely considered to be one aspect of
these results suggest that ToM performance among patients with ToM ability (e.g., Happe, 1994). The Competition subscale
BD is likely moderated by a number of key variables, with more assesses the ability to determine who won a sporting match
severe deficits emerging in patients with heightened illness (e.g., Who won the racquetball game?). The IPT-15 Competition
severity and for ToM tasks of greater cognitive complexity. subscale also appears to tap constructs similar to ToM, since one
In line with these findings, Bora et al. (2005) reported that, must decode non-verbal and verbal cues to interpret that one
relative to matched controls, euthymic patients with BD showed individual feels ‘‘positive’’ towards winning and the other ‘‘nega-
impaired mental state discrimination on the Reading the Mind in tive’’ towards the loss.
the Eyes Task (RMET; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001), an advanced ToM Taken together, the literature concerning social cognitive
task that involves inferring the mental state of a person from their performance in BD reveals a conflicting pattern of findings, high-
eye gaze, and thus decoding the mental state depicted by the eyes’ lighting the need for further investigation. The primary goal of the
expression. Although the RMET limits the demands it places on present study was to examine aspects of social cognition in
260 A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264

patients with BD and matched controls using two standardized Table 1


tests: (1) the RMET; and (2) the IPT-15. Consistent with an Demographic and clinical characteristics of study sample.
emerging body of literature demonstrating ToM impairment
Characteristic Controls BD group
among this patient population, we predicted that, relative to (n¼ 25) Total sample
control participants, patients with BD would demonstrate deficits (n ¼25)
on the RMET. Given recent evidence that individuals with BD are
impaired on ToM tasks assessing the detection of deception n n
Sex Male 7 7
(Lahera et al., 2008; Wolf et al., 2010) and feeling states of others Female 18 18
(e.g., McKinnon et al., 2010), we predicted that patients in our Mean Mean
sample would also show deficits on the IPT-15 Deception and Age 44.2(11.8) 45.2(10.8)
Competition subscales, respectively. Recent evidence also points Education 16.7(3.0) 15.3(2.6)
Number of affective episodes 18.2(10.9)
towards deficits on tests of social inference (e.g., Montag et al.,
Onset of illness (in years) 22.8(11.0)
2010) and in the perception and recognition of social stimuli such Duration of illness (in years) 23.1(11.2)
as emotional facial expressions (e.g., Kohler et al., 2011) and HAM-D score 1.9(2.7) 8.1(6.2)*
affective prosody (Bozikas et al., 2007), skills that are likely YMRS 0.2(0.7) 2.0(2.0)*
needed when inferring the social status of individual, level of GAF score 79.6(4.1) 67.2(9.6)*

intimacy between individuals, and nature of a relation between Values are n or mean (standard deviation).
individuals. Hence, we expected that individuals with BD would Abbreviations: BD, bipolar disorder group; HAM-D, 17-item Hamilton Depression
show impairments on the Status, Intimacy, Kinship, and global Rating Scale; YMRS, Young Mania Rating Scale; GAF, Global Assessment of
scores on the IPT-15. Functioning Scale.
Our secondary goal was to evaluate the relation of sympto-
n
Significant results (P o0.05).
matology and illness burden (i.e., number of affective episodes,
symptoms was assessed using the Young Mania Rating Scale (YMRS; Young et al.,
duration of illness, age at onset of illness) to social cognitive
1978). Depressive symptoms were examined using the Hamilton Rating Scale for
performance. Given that impaired cognitive and social cognitive Depression (HAM-D; Hamilton, 1960). Overall functioning was evaluated using
performance among patients with BD has been shown to be the Global Assessment of Functioning Scale (American Psychiatric Association,
associated with increased symptom severity and a prolonged 1994). Control participants also received these measures to rule out the presence
of subthreshold psychiatric illness. Participants were tested in varying states of
course of illness (Summers et al., 2006; Malhi et al., 2007a;
illness, where 10 patients were euthymic (HAM-D less than 7), 12 patients
Martinez-Aran et al., 2007; Schenkel et al., 2008; Kurtz and were experiencing subsyndromal depression (HAM-D between 7 and 14) and 3
Gerraty, 2009; Cusi et al., 2010; Wolf et al., 2010), we hypothe- were moderately depressed (HAM-D between 15 and 30). None of the patients
sized that ToM responding would be negatively associated with were experiencing manic or hypomanic symptoms (YMRS less than 8). Patients
these illness variables. Finally, it remains an open question were on a variety of psychotropic medications including lithium (N¼ 11), antic-
onvulsants (N¼ 22), selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (N ¼5), tricyclic anti-
whether impairments in social cognition are related to deficits
depressants (N ¼3), typical antipsychotics (N ¼ 3), atypical antipsychotics (N ¼18),
in everyday social functioning; previous studies that have exam- monoamine oxidase inhibitors (N ¼1), benzodiazepines (N ¼ 22), sedatives/hypno-
ined this relation report conflicting findings. One study showed tics (N ¼ 5), bupropion (N ¼2), serotonin–norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors
that alterations in affective empathy were related to reductions in (N ¼ 2), trazadone (N ¼3), selective norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors (N ¼1),
and no medication (N ¼ 1).
psychosocial adjustment in a sample of patients with BD (Cusi
Exclusion criteria for patients and comparison subjects were: (i) inability to
et al., 2010), while another study failed to find a significant provide informed consent, (ii) history of electroconvulsive therapy or transcranial
relation between ToM performance and social functioning (Olley magnetic stimulation therapy, within one year, (iii) substance abuse based on
et al., 2005). Examining this particular relation is important given DSM-IV criteria in the last six months, (iv) current or lifetime history of substance
that deficits on tests of social cognition are associated with higher dependence based on DSM-IV criteria, (v) current or prior history of untreated
significant medical illness (e.g., cancer) or of neurological illness (e.g., Parkinson’s
rates of relapse among patients with mood disorders (Inoue et al.,
disease, epilepsy), (vi) history of traumatic brain injury and/or loss of conscious-
2006), and that social cognitive impairment appears to be a key ness (lasting more than 60 s), (vii) YMRS score4 10, (viii) use of benzodiazepines
determinant of intact daily functioning (Baron-Cohen and within 12 h prior to testing.
Wheelwright, 2004; Green et al., 2008; Spreng et al., 2009). The study received ethical approval from the St. Joseph’s Healthcare Hamilton
Hence, we also investigated relations between psychosocial Ethics Board and all participants provided written informed consent.

functioning and social cognitive performance by administering a


standardized measure of psychosocial functioning, the Social 2.2. Social functioning
Adjustment Scale Self-Report (SAS-SR; Weissman and Bothwell,
1976). We expected that poor performance on the social cognition 2.2.1. Social Adjustment Scale Self-Report
The Social Adjustment Scale Self-Report (SAS-SR; Weissman and Bothwell,
measures in patients with BD would be associated with lower
1976) assesses a broad range of social domains including work/school role, social/
levels of social functioning in a real-world context. leisure activities, relationship with extended family, marital role, parental role and
membership within a family unit. Individual scores are obtained in each of these
areas, and an overall score for psychosocial adjustment is generated. In this self-
2. Methods rated questionnaire, scores range from 1 (optimal functioning) to 5 (extremely
poor functioning). The SAS-SR has been found to have adequate reliability and
validity (Weissman and Bothwell, 1976; Weissman and MHS Staff, 1999; Zweig
2.1. Participants and Turkel, 2007).

Twenty-five patients (7 males, 18 females; 17 bipolar I, 7 bipolar II, 1 bipolar


not otherwise specified) who met DSM-IV criteria for BD were recruited from the 2.3. Social cognition tasks
Mood Disorders Program at St. Joseph’s Healthcare Hamilton. Both inpatients
(N ¼3) and outpatients (N ¼22) were included in the BD group. One patient had a 2.3.1. Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test-revised
history of psychotic symptoms. We also recruited 25 healthy comparison subjects In this task, participants were presented with 36 sets of eyes depicting
who were matched for age, gender, and education. Controls were excluded if they complex mental states (e.g., jealousy, desire; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001). Four
reported a personal history of a psychiatric disorder or if they had a family adjectives corresponding to mental state descriptors (e.g., ‘‘hateful,’’ ‘‘panicked’’)
member with BD or schizophrenia. The demographic and clinical characteristics of were displayed on each slide. Three of the adjectives were distractors, while one of
the sample are shown in Table 1. the adjectives correctly described the mental state of the person in the photo-
In order to assess the relation between level of symptom severity and ToM graph. Participants were required to decide which of 4 mental state descriptors
ability, patients in varying mood states were recruited. The severity of manic best described what the individual in the photograph was thinking or feeling.
A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264 261

Stimuli were presented on a computer screen and both reaction time and error about everyday social situations. Moreover, the BD group scored
rates recorded. There was no time limit for responses. Participants were instructed
lower than controls on the IPT-15 Kinship scale (U¼216.50,
to consult a glossary of all mental state words found in the task when they were
unsure of the meaning of a word, following the same procedures used by Baron- Po0.05), and the Competition subscale (U¼211.50, P¼0.03). No
Cohen et al. (2001). Scores were calculated as the total number of correct significant group differences were found for the IPT-15 Intimacy,
responses for all 36 items. Status, and Deception subscales (P’s 40.05).

2.3.2. Interpersonal Perception Task-15


The Interpersonal Perception Task-15 (IPT-15; Costanzo and Archer, 1993) is a
3.2. Relation between social cognitive performance and
measure of social perception consisting of 15 videotaped scenes, depicting one to demographic and clinical variables
four individuals interacting or speaking. Each vignette is followed by a multiple
choice question. Each question has an objectively correct answer. For example, A significant positive association emerged between RMET
one scene depicts a short interaction between two people and then the viewer is
performance and duration of illness in the BD group, (rho ¼0.48,
asked to judge which of the two people is the boss. The scenes are edited so that
there are no obvious verbal clues present when answering each question. The task P¼0.02). The IPT-15 Intimacy scale was negatively associated
includes five different types of social judgements, including kinship, intimacy, with the YMRS [rho¼ 0.34, P¼0.02]. No significant correlations
deception, competition, and status, with three scenes in each area. The maximum were found between the social cognition measures (RMET, IPT-
score for each subscale is 3 and the maximum total score for the test is 15. The 15) and depressive symptom severity as assessed by the HAM-D.
IPT-15 differs from other measures of social perception by depicting unscripted
and spontaneous behavior by nonactors in everyday social situations, and includes
individuals that are diverse in terms of gender, age, and ethnic background 3.3. Social functioning
(Costanzo and Archer, 1993). The IPT-15 has been shown to demonstrate good
convergent validity (r¼ 0.64), test–retest reliability (r¼ 0.73), and inter-rater
reliability (r¼ 0.81) (Costanzo and Archer, 1993). On the SAS-SR, individuals with BD were impaired in overall
functioning (U¼99.50, P¼0.001) as well as Work/School Role
2.4. Statistical methods (U ¼133.00, P¼0.03), Social/Leisure Activities (U¼ 120.00, P¼
0.004), and Membership within Family Unit (U¼156.50, P¼
Data were explored for assumptions of normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. 0.04) domains. No other significant group differences emerged
Independent sample t-tests and univariate analysis of variance were applied to on the SAS-SR (P’s 40.14).
dependent variables that were normally distributed. Group differences on data
that was not normally distributed were analyzed using Mann–Whitney U-tests.
Estimated effect sizes for parametric and nonparametric data were analyzed by 3.4. Relation between social cognition and social functioning
partial eta square and Cohen’s r values, respectively. Associations between the
scores on social cognition measures (RMET, IPT-15), the clinical (HAM-D, YMRS,
The IPT-15 Kinship scale was negatively associated with the
GAF, illness duration, age at onset of illness, number of affective episodes) and
social functioning data were analyzed using Spearman rank correlation coeffi- Marital Role domain of the SAS-SR (rho¼  0.41, P¼0.03).
cients. Level of significance was set at 0.05, and all reported results were two- A significant negative association was also found between the
tailed. IPT-15 Intimacy scale and the SAS-SR Work/School Role domain
(rho ¼ 0.32, P¼0.04). RMET performance was not associated
with any domain of the SAS-SR for all analyses (P’s 40.05).
3. Results

3.1. Performance on social cognition measures


4. Discussion

Table 2 summarizes the descriptive statistics for the two


The primary goal of the present study was to investigate social
groups along with the effect size of each measure.
cognitive ability in patients with BD using both complex and
ecologically valid measures. We predicted that individuals with
3.1.1. RMET performance BD would show impaired social cognitive performance on the
Relative to controls, patients with BD showed diminished tasks assessed, and that these deficits would be associated with a
accuracy on the RMET [F(1, 48) ¼5.74; P¼0.02], suggesting higher level of symptom severity and increased burden of illness.
impairments in discriminating complex mental states among this Patients were less accurate than control participants in attribut-
sample. ing mental states from pictures of eyes and showed impaired
performance on a complex task that required social inferencing.
3.1.2. IPT-15 performance These deficits appear to have implications for interpersonal
On the IPT-15 total score, the BD group performed significantly relationships held by patients with BD; we found preliminary
worse than the HC group [F(1, 48)¼ 4.89, P¼0.03], suggesting that evidence that impaired responding on the IPT-15 was associated
individuals with BD show impairments in making judgments with reduced self-reported levels of psychosocial functioning.

Table 2
Group differences on social cognition tests.

Theory of mind measures Controls BD group Results Effect size (partial eta squared; Cohen’s r)

RMET Total score 26.2(3.1) 23.9(3.6) F(1, 48) ¼5.74, P¼ 0.02n


Z2p ¼ 0.17
IPT-15 Total score 10.3(1.5) 9.4(1.6) F(1, 48) ¼4.89, P¼ 0.03n Z2p ¼ 0.09
IPT-15 Kinship score 2.3(0.8) 1.9(0.8) U ¼216.50, P o0.05n r¼ 0.28
IPT-15 Intimacy score 1.8(0.7) 1.6(0.9) U ¼277.00, P¼ 0.46 r¼ 0.11
IPT-15 Competition score 2.6(0.6) 2.2(0.7) U ¼211.50, P ¼0.03n r¼ 0.31
IPT-15 Status score 2.4(0.7) 2.3(0.6) U ¼286.00, P¼ 0.57 r¼ 0.08
IPT-15 Deception score 1.3(0.8) 1.5(0.9) U ¼283.50, P ¼0.54 r¼ 0.09

Values are n or mean (standard deviation).


Abbreviations: BD, bipolar disorder group; IPT-15, Interpersonal Perception Task 15; RMET, Reading the Mind’s Eye Test.
n
Significant results (Po 0.05).
262 A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264

Our finding of impaired performance by patients with BD on 2005; Olley et al., 2005; Lahera et al., 2008; Wolf et al., 2010).
the RMET is in line with a prior study reporting deficits in mental Future work that includes measures of neuropsychological func-
state recognition from eye expressions in euthymic individuals tioning (e.g., IQ, executive function) and emotional processing
with BD (Bora et al., 2005) and extends these findings to include (e.g., emotion recognition) will more adequately assess the con-
patients in a subsyndromal illness state. The present finding of a tribution of cognitive and affective processing resources to social
deficit on the RMET among patients with BD is also in line with cognitive ability among patients with BD.
numerous studies reporting a general impairment in facial emo- The results of the present study also do not adequately address
tion recognition among this population (see Kohler et al., 2011 for whether the impairments observed are a specific deficit in social
a recent review; but see Harmer et al., 2002; Venn et al., 2004; cognition or that they may be secondary to clinical factors such as
Vaskinn et al., 2007; Schaefer et al., 2010 for contradictory symptom severity. No significant associations were found
findings). between depressive symptom severity and social cognitive per-
Consistent with our hypotheses, patients also showed overall formance. The inclusion of primarily euthymic patients and
reduced performance on the IPT-15, which relies heavily on social patients with subsyndromal depression in this study likely
inferencing. This deficit was particularly apparent on the IPT-15 limited our ability to detect a relation between depressive
Kinship scale, indicating that patients experienced difficulties symptom severity and social cognition. We found limited evi-
interpreting the nature of the relationship between individuals dence that YMRS scores were negatively associated with IPT-15
depicted in each kinship scene. Individuals with BD also scored Intimacy subscale scores. This finding, however, should be treated
lower than controls on the IPT-15 Competition subscale, where with caution in light of the overall low scores and small variance
one must decode social cues to infer the ‘‘feeling’’ state of the found in the YMRS scores of the sample. Prior work examining the
characters and then apply this knowledge to determine who won relation between symptom severity and ToM ability in patients
a sporting match. The finding of impaired performance on the with BD reports a similar pattern of conflicting findings. For
Competition scale is in line with research demonstrating ToM example, McKinnon et al. (2010) found a negative relation
deficits in patients with BD (e.g., Bora et al., 2005; Lahera et al., between severity of depression and both first- and second-order
2008). This result is also broadly consistent with recent studies ToM scores in a sample of patients with subsyndromal BD. Other
demonstrating that individuals with BD report reduced levels of studies, however, report no such associations between ToM
cognitive empathy (Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2009; Cusi et al., 2010), ability and symptom severity in BD (Bora et al., 2005; Wolf
involving the cognitive understanding and comprehension of et al., 2010). Further studies including larger patient samples in
another person’s mental state, a psychological construct thought different mood states, such as acute depression and mania, will be
to be closely related to ToM. Taken together, impairments on the required to address adequately the association between sympto-
Kinship and Competition subscales of the IPT-15 indicate that matology and social cognitive response.
patients with BD experience difficulties interpreting social cues to An extended duration of illness was associated with enhanced
make interpersonal judgments. Similar to the RMET results, these performance on the RMET. These findings stand in contrast to
findings are also consistent with prior studies that show impair- preliminary evidence that an earlier illness onset (Schenkel et al.,
ment in social perception in this patient population, including the 2008; Wolf et al., 2010) and greater illness duration (McKinnon
recognition of emotion in facial stimuli (Kohler et al., 2011) and et al., 2010) are associated with ToM deficits in patients with BD.
on naturalistic tests of social cognition (Bazin et al., 2009; Montag One possible explanation for this unexpected finding is that a more
et al., 2010). chronic course of illness might be related to less satisfactory social
Contradictory to our expectations, patients with BD performed interactions and thus these patients may be highly attuned to social
comparably to controls in detecting lies (Deception subscale). This cues. We failed to find any additional significant associations
finding conflicts with prior reports of ToM deficits on measures between social cognitive performance and other burden of illness
examining the detection of deception (e.g., Lahera et al., 2008; variables (number of affective episodes, age at onset of illness). This
Wolf et al., 2010). This discrepant finding seems to support the absence of an association is in line with recent studies (Inoue et al.,
idea that although the Deception subscale may, in part, measure 2004; Bora et al., 2005) that did not find any relation between
aspects similar to ToM, this measure may also assess different course of illness variables and social cognitive response in BD. Given
facets of social cognition that are not affected in individuals with the small size of our sample, however, further research is needed to
BD. Importantly, analyses of the other IPT-15 subscales showed clarify the relation between burden of illness and social cognition in
that patients with BD demonstrated intact performance in inter- patients with BD.
preting levels of intimacy between individuals (Intimacy sub- Finally, we found early evidence that impaired performance on
scale), and the status of individuals relative to one another (Status the IPT-15 was associated with poor functioning in the Work/
subscale). Intact performance on these IPT-15 subscales suggest School Role and Marital Role domains of the SAS-SR. These
that social perceptual ability is not entirely impacted in this findings support the notion that disruptions in social cognitive
patient sample, and is consistent with the idea that this social processing may be associated with declines in patients’ social
cognitive domain is a multi-faceted psychological construct (e.g., functioning (Olley et al., 2005; Cusi et al., 2010). These prelimin-
Green et al., 2008). ary results are also in accord with findings suggesting that poor
Taken together, we speculate that the deficits observed across performance on tests of ToM is associated with a poorer prognosis
the RMET and IPT-15 are not strictly related to a social cognitive in patients with major depressive disorder and BD (Inoue et al.,
deficit per se, but may be partially mediated by well-known 2006). Poor interpersonal functioning is also associated with
deficits in cognitive (e.g., working memory, attention) and affec- impairments in ToM abilities in other psychiatric disorders such
tive (e.g., emotion recognition and affect regulation) processing as schizophrenia and autistic spectrum disorders (Baron-Cohen
resources thought requisite to social cognitive function (Decety et al., 2005; Schenkel et al., 2005; Shamay-Tsoory et al., 2007).
and Lamm, 2006; McKinnon et al., 2007), and which have been However, in light of evidence suggesting that scores on self-rated
shown to be affected in patients with BD (Phillips et al., 2008; measures are highly sensitive to fluctuations in affective symp-
Kurtz and Gerraty, 2009). Indeed, early evidence indicates that toms in patients with mood disorders (e.g., McKendree-Smith and
measures of intelligence, attention, executive functioning and Scogin, 2000; Fu et al., 2005), these findings should be interpreted
emotion recognition may moderate certain aspects of social with caution. Due to our relatively small sample size these
cognition, including ToM ability in patients with BD (Bora et al., findings are preliminary in nature and further studies are needed
A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264 263

to determine the association between altered social cognitive Costanzo, M., Archer, B., 1993. The Interpersonal Perception Task 15 (IPT-15):
performance and everyday functioning in BD. A Guide for Researchers and Teachers. University of California Center for
Media and Independent Learning, Berkeley, California.
Our study has several limitations. The relatively modest Cusi, A.M., MacQueen, G.M., McKinnon, M.C., 2010. Altered empathic responding
sample size of the study likely limited the statistical power of in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Research 178 (2), 354–358.
our findings. Caution is warranted when interpreting the results Decety, J., Jackson, P.L., 2004. The functional architecture of human empathy.
Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews 3 (2), 71–100.
of this study as these findings need to be replicated in future Decety, J., Lamm, C., 2006. Human empathy through the lens of social neu-
studies that employ larger sample sizes. Another limitation is that roscience. ScientificWorldJournal 6, 1146–1163.
the cross-sectional nature of the study precludes the ability to Depp, C.A., Mausbach, B.T., Harvey, P.D., Bowie, C.R., Wolyniec, P.S., Thornquist,
M.H., Luke, J.R., McGrath, J.A., Pulver, A.E., Patterson, T.L., 2010. Social
make any firm conclusions about the directionality of the associa-
competence and observer-rated social functioning in bipolar disorder. Bipolar
tions between course of illness variables, social functioning, and Disorders 12 (8), 843–850.
social cognition. Future investigations should explore these issues Douglas, K.M., Porter, R.J., 2010. Recognition of disgusted facial expressions in
further by using a longitudinal design. Moreover, given our small severe depression. British Journal of Psychiatry 197, 156–157.
Edwards, J., Pattison, P.E., Jackson, H.J., Wales, R.J., 2001. Facial affect and affective
sample and the heterogeneous medication status of our partici- prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research
pants, we did not have the opportunity to examine possible 48, 235–253.
differences in social cognitive performance between patients on Elgie, R., Morselli, P.L., 2007. Social functioning in bipolar patients: the perception
and perspective of patients, relatives and advocacy organizations—a review.
certain types of medication. Additional work is required to Bipolar Disorders 9 (1–2), 144–157.
examine the influence of specific medication classes on social Fu, T., Koutstaal, W., Fu, C.H.Y., Poon, L., Cleare, A.J., 2005. Depression, confidence,
cognitive ability in patients with BD. Finally, longitudinal studies and decision: evidence against depressive realism. Journal of Psychopathology
and Behavioral Assessment 27 (4), 243–252.
that follow individuals who are at risk for BD and patients Green, M.F., Penn, D.L., Bentall, R., Carpenter, W.T., Gaebel, W., Gur, R.C., Kring,
through acute and remitted phases of illness will determine A.M., Park, S., Silverstein, S.M., Heinssen, R., 2008. Social cognition in schizo-
whether impairments in social cognition are a state-like phenom- phrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research oppor-
tunities. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34 (6), 1211–1220.
enon or a stable trait in individuals with BD.
Gur, R.C., Erwin, R.J., Gur, R.E., Zwil, A.S., Heimberg, C., Kraemer, H.C., 1992. Facial
In summary, the present study provides additional empirical emotion discrimination: II. Behavioral findings in depression. Psychiatry
evidence for impaired social cognition in patients with BD. The Research 42, 241–251.
ability to make accurate inferences about the mental states of Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neuro-
surgery, and Psychiatry 23, 56–62.
others and correctly perceive social cues are essential for day-to- Happe, F.G., 1994. An advanced test of theory of mind: understanding of story
day social interactions. Preliminary findings from this study characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and
indicate that deficits in interpersonal functioning may stem from normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 24 (2),
129–154.
impairments on social cognitive tasks in BD. The ability to Harmer, C.J., Grayson, L., Goodwin, G.M., 2002. Enhanced recognition of disgust in
understand and appreciate mental states and to perceive social bipolar illness. Biological Psychiatry 51, 298–304.
information should be taken into consideration in treatment Henry, J.D., Phillips, L.H., Crawford, J.R., Ietswaart, M., Summers, F., 2006. Theory of
mind following traumatic brain injury: the role of emotion recognition and
interventions for BD. Treatment approaches that incorporate executive dysfunction. Neuropsychologia 44 (10), 1623–1628.
social skills training may help improve the social dysfunction Inoue, Y., Tonooka, Y., Yamada, K., Kanba, S., 2004. Deficiency of theory of mind in
that accompanies this disorder. patients with remitted mood disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 82, 403–409.
Inoue, Y., Yamada, K., Kanba, S., 2006. Deficit in theory of mind is a risk for relapse
of major depression. Journal of Affective Disorders 95, 125–127.
Kerr, N., Dunbar, R.I.M., Bentall, R.P., 2003. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar
affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders 73, 253–259.
References
Kohler, C.G., Hoffman, L.J., Eastman, L.B., Healey, K., Moberg, P.J., 2011. Facial
emotion perception in depression and bipolar disorder: a quantitative review.
Addington, J., Addington, D., 1998. Facial affect recognition and information Psychiatry Research 188 (3), 303–309.
processing in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Research 32 Kurtz, M.M., Gerraty, R.T., 2009. A meta-analytic investigation of neurocognitive
(3), 171–181. deficits in bipolar illness: profile and effects of clinical state. Neuropsychology
Adolphs, R., 2001. The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in 23 (5), 551–562.
Neurobiology 11 (2), 231–239. Lahera, G., Montes, J.M., Benito, A., Valdivia, M., Medina, E., Mirapeix, I., Saiz-Ruiz,
Almeida, J.R., Versace, A., Hassel, S., Kupfer, D.J., Phillips, M.L., 2010. Elevated J., 2008. Theory of mind deficit in bipolar disorder: is it related to a previous
amygdala activity to sad facial expressions: a state marker of bipolar but not history of psychotic symptoms? Psychiatry Research 161 (3), 309–317.
unipolar depression. Biological Psychiatry 67 (5), 414–421. Lembke, A., Ketter, T.A., 2002. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania.
American Psychiatric Association, 1994. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 302–304.
Mental Disorders, 4th ed. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC. Lennox, B.R., Jacob, R., Calder, A.J., Lupson, V., Bullmore, E.T., 2004. Behavioural and
Baron-Cohen, S., Knickmeyer, R.C., Belmonte, M.K., 2005. Sex differences in the neurocognitive responses to sad facial affect are attenuated in patients with
brain: implications for explaining autism. Science 310 (5749), 819–823. mania. Psychological Medicine 34, 795–802.
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., 2004. The empathy quotient: an investigation of Leslie, A.M., Friedman, O., German, T.P., 2004. Core mechanisms in ‘theory of
adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex mind’. Trends in Cognitive Science 8, 528–533.
differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 163–175. Malhi, G.S., Ivanovski, B., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Mitchell, P.B., Vieta, E., Sachdev, P.,
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, S.K., Raste, Y., Plumb, I., 2001. The ‘‘Reading 2007a. Neuropsychological deficits and functional impairment in bipolar
the Mind in the Eyes’’ Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and depression, hypomania and euthymia. Bipolar Disorders 9, 114–125.
adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Malhi, G.S., Lagopoulos, J., Sachdev, P.S., Ivanovski, B., Shnier, R., Ketter, T., 2007b.
Psychology and Psychiatry 42, 241–251. Is a lack of disgust something to fear? A functional magnetic resonance
Bazin, N., Brunet-Gouet, E., Bourdet, C., Kayser, N., Falissard, B., Hardy-Bayle, M.C., imaging facial emotion recognition study in euthymic bipolar disorder
Passerieux, C., 2009. Quantitative assessment of attribution of intentions to patients. Bipolar Disorders 9 (4), 345–357.
others in schizophrenia using an ecological video-based task: a comparison Martinez-Aran, A., Vieta, E., Torrent, C., Sanchez-Moreno, J., Goikolea, J.M.,
with manic and depressed patients. Psychiatry Research 167 (1–2), 28–35. Salamero, M., Malhi, G.S., Gonzalez-Pinto, A., Daban, C., Alvarez-Grandi, S.,
Bora, E., Vahip, S., Gonul, A.S., Akdeniz, F., Alkan, M., Ogut, M., Eryavuz, A., 2005. Fountoulakis, K., Kaprinis, G., Tabares-Seisdedos, R., Ayuso-Mateos, J.L., 2007.
Evidence for theory of mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar Functional outcome in bipolar disorder: the role of clinical and cognitive
disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 112, 110–116. factors. Bipolar Disorders 9 (1–2), 103–113.
Bozikas, V.P., Kosmidis, M.H., Tonia, T., Andreou, C., Focas, K., Karavatos, A., 2007. McClure, E.B., Treland, J.E., Snow, J., Schmajuk, M., Dickstein, D.P., Towbin, K.E.,
Impaired perception of affective prosody in remitted patients with bipolar Charney, D.S., Pine, D.S., Leibenluft, E., 2005. Deficits in social cognition and
disorder. Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 19, 436–440. response flexibility in pediatric bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychia-
Brothers, L., 1990. The social brain: a project for integrating primate behavior and try 162, 1644–1651.
neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience 1, 27–51. McKendree-Smith, N., Scogin, F., 2000. Depressive realism: effects of depression
Brüne, M., 2005. Emotion recognition, ‘theory of mind,’ and social behavior in severity and interpretation time. Journal of Clinical Psychology 56 (12),
schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research 133 (2–3), 135–147. 1601–1608.
Brüne, M., Brüne-Cohrs, U., 2006. Theory of mind-evolution, ontogeny, brain McKinnon, M.C., Cusi, A.M., MacQueen, G.M., 2010. Impaired theory of mind
mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews performance in patients with recurrent bipolar disorder: moderating effect of
30, 437–455. cognitive load. Psychiatry Research 177, 261–262.
264 A.M. Cusi et al. / Psychiatry Research 200 (2012) 258–264

McKinnon, M.C., Levine, B., Moscovitch, M., 2007. Domain-general contributions to Shamay-Tsoory, S.G., Shur, S., Harari, H., Levkovitz, Y., 2007. Neurocognitive basis
social reasoning: the perspective from cognitive neuroscience. In: Roberts, M.J. of impaired empathy in schizophrenia. Neuropsychology 21 (4), 431–438.
(Ed.), Integrating the Mind. Psychology Press, Hove, United Kingdom, Spreng, R.N., McKinnon, M.C., Mar, R.A., Levine, B., 2009. The Toronto Empathy
pp. 153–177. Questionnaire: scale development and initial validation of a factor-analytic
McKinnon, M.C., Moscovitch, M., 2007. Domain-general contributions to social solution to multiple empathy measures. Journal of Personality Assessment 91
reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. Cognition 102, (1), 62–71.
179–218. Stone, V.E., Baron-Cohen, S., Knight, R.T., 1998. Frontal lobe contributions to theory
Montag, C., Ehrlich, A., Neuhaus, K., Dziobek, I., Heekeren, H.R., Heinz, A., Gallinat, of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10, 640–656.
J., 2010. Theory of mind impairments in euthymic bipolar patients. Journal of Summers, M., Papadopoulou, K., Bruno, S., Cipolotti, L., Ron, M.A., 2006. Bipolar I
Affective Disorders 123 (1–3), 264–269. and bipolar II disorder: cognition and emotion processing. Psychological
Olley, A.L., Malhi, G.S., Bachelor, J., Cahill, C.M., Mitchell, P.B., Berk, M., 2005. Medicine 36, 1799–1809.
Executive functioning and theory of mind in euthymic bipolar disorder. Vaskinn, A., Sergi, M.J., Green, M.F., 2009. The challenges of ecological validity in
Bipolar Disorders 7 (Suppl. 5), 43–52. the measurement of social perception in schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous
Phillips, M.L., Ladouceur, C.D., Drevets, W.C., 2008. A neural model of voluntary and Mental Disease 197 (9), 700–702.
and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the patho- Vaskinn, A., Sundet, K., Friis, S., Simonsen, C., Birkenaes, A.B., Engh, J.A., Jónsdóttir,
physiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Molecular Psychiatry H., Ringen, P.A., Opjordsmoen, S., Andreassen, O.A., 2007. The effect of gender
13 (9), 829–857. on emotion perception in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Acta Psychia-
Premack, D., Woodruff, G., 1978. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? trica Scandinavica 116, 263–270.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences 4, 515–526. Venn, H.R., Gray, J.M., Montagne, B., Murray, L.K., Michael Burt, D., Frigerio, E.,
Sabbagh, M.A., 2004. Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind Perrett, D.I., Young, A.H., 2004. Perception of facial expressions of emotion in
reasoning: implications for autism. Brain and Cognition 55 (1), 209–219. bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders 6 (4), 286–293.
Sabbagh, M.A., Xu, F., Carlson, S.M., Moses, L.J., Lee, K., 2006. The development of Versace, A., Thompson, W.K., Zhou, D., Almeida, J.R., Hassel, S., Klein, C.R., Kupfer,
executive functioning and theory of mind. A comparison of Chinese and U.S. D.J., Phillips, M.L., 2010. Abnormal left and right amygdala–orbitofrontal
preschoolers. Psychological Science 17 (1), 74–81. cortical functional connectivity to emotional faces: state versus trait vulner-
Schaefer, K.L., Baumann, J., Rich, B.A., Luckenbaugh, D.A., Zarate Jr., C.A., 2010. ability markers of depression in bipolar disorder. Biological Psychiatry 67 (5),
Perception of facial emotion in adults with bipolar or unipolar depression and 422–431.
controls. Journal of Psychiatric Research 44 (16), 1229–1235. Weissman, M.M., Bothwell, S., 1976. Assessment of social adjustment by patient
Schenkel, L.S., Marlow-O’Connor, M., Moss, J.A., Sweeney, A., Pavuluri, M.N., 2008. self-report. Archives of General Psychiatry 32, 1111–1115.
Theory of mind and social inference in children and adolescents with bipolar Weissman, M., M.H.S. Staff, 1999. Social Adjustment Scale–Self-Report (SAS).
disorder. Psychological Medicine 38, 791–800. Multi-Health Systems Inc., Tonawanda, New York.
Schenkel, L.S., Pavuluri, M.N., Herbener, E.S., Harral, E.M., Sweeney, J.A., 2007. Wolf, F., Brune, M., Assion, H.J., 2010. Theory of mind and neurocognitive
Facial emotion processing in acutely ill and euthymic patients with pediatric functioning in patients with bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders 12 (6),
bipolar disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent 657–666.
Psychiatry 46, 1070–1079. Young, R.C., Biggs, J.T., Ziegler, V.E., Meyer, D.A., 1978. A rating scale for mania:
Schenkel, L.S., Spaulding, W.D., Silverstein, S.M., 2005. Poor premorbid social reliability, validity and sensitivity. British Journal of Psychiatry 133, 429–435.
functioning and theory of mind deficit in schizophrenia: evidence of reduced Yurgelun-Todd, D.A., Gruber, S.A., Kanayama, G., Killgore, W.D., Baird, A.A., Young,
context processing? Journal of Psychiatric Research 39 (5), 499–508. A.D., 2000. fMRI during affect discrimination in bipolar affective disorder.
Shamay-Tsoory, S., Harari, H., Szepsenwol, O., Levkovitz, Y., 2009. Neuropsycho- Bipolar Disorders 2 (3 Pt. 2), 237–248.
logical evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder. Zweig, R.A., Turkel, E., 2007. The social adjustment scale-self-report: psychometric
Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 21 (1), 59–67. properties for older adults. Psychological Report 101 (3 Pt. 1), 920–926.

You might also like