You are on page 1of 11

Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Review article

Social cognition and empathy in adults with obsessive compulsive disorder:


A meta-analysis
Emre Bora a, b, c, *
a
Department of Psychiatry, Dokuz Eylul University Medical School, Izmir 35340, Turkey
b
Department of Neurosciences, Institute of Health Sciences, Dokuz Eylul University, Izmir, Turkey
c
Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne and, Melbourne Health, Carlton South, Victoria 3053, Australia

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is characterized by intrusive thoughts (obsessions) and compulsions and
Obsessive-compulsive disorder has been associated with psychosocial impairment. Indeed, a number of studies have highlighted impairments in
Social cognition both social cognitive functions and empathic skills in OCD, despite several inconsistencies. This study aimed to
Theory of mind
investigate social cognitive dysfunction and empathy deficits in patients with OCD using a meta-analytic
Empathy
approach. A literature search was conducted using the databases Pubmed, PsycINFO, ProQuest and Scopus to
Emotion recognition
identify the relevant studies (January 1980 to March 2020). Following the systematic review of relevant OCD
studies, a random-effects meta-analysis was conducted. The current meta-analysis included 25 studies consisting
of 1161 patients with OCD and 1329 healthy controls. OCD was associated with decreased performance in theory
of mind (ToM). In the facial emotion recognition domain, patients with OCD significantly underperformed
healthy controls only in their recognition of disgust. OCD was significantly related to reduced cognitive empathy.
OCD was associated with medium-sized impairments in ToM and cognitive empathy, which can likely contribute
to psychosocial impairment in this disorder. Further studies are needed to investigate state and trait-related
factors using experimental measures of empathy.

1. Introduction but also for social cognition and empathy (Abu-Akel and Sha­
may-Tsoory, 2011; Cox et al., 2012). Recent studies have showed that
Obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) is a chronic neuropsychiatric patients with diseases affecting the basal ganglia and its connections
disorder characterized by recurrent obsessions and compulsions. OCD is within fronto-striato-thalamic circuits, including Parkinson’s disease
commonly associated with impairments in social functioning and dete­ and Huntington’s disease, have significant deficits in social cognition
rioration in the quality of life (Bobes et al., 2001; Bystritsky et al., 2001; and empathy (Bora et al., 2015; Bora et al., 2016; Maurage et al., 2016).
Huppert et al., 2009; Ruscio et al., 2010). In OCD, multiple factors, Therefore, it is important to investigate empathy and social cognitive
including symptoms, co-morbidities and neurocognitive functioning impairments in OCD.
contribute to social dysfunction (Markarian et al., 2010). Potential ab­ Traditional neuropsychological domains, such as executive functions
normalities in social cognition and empathy in OCD might be clinically and memory, have been extensively studied in OCD. However, social
relevant as such deficits contribute to functional impairment and poor cognition is relatively an understudied subject in OCD. The recognition
insight in other psychiatric disorders including schizophrenia, autism of emotions from facial perceptual cues, and theory of mind (ToM), the
spectrum disorder, and major depressive disorder (Baron-Cohen et al., ability to attribute mental states (feelings, beliefs, intentions, and de­
2001; Bora, 2017; Bora et al., 2006; Lee et al., 2005). sires) to others and understand and predict others’ behavior based on
The current dominant neuroanatomical model of OCD, which fo­ their mental states, are the most commonly studied domains of social
cuses on abnormalities in the frontal lobes, basal ganglia, and cognition in OCD. An earlier study reported impaired recognition of
orbitofronto-striato-thalamic circuits is compatible with potential diffi­ disgust from facial stimuli in OCD (Sprengelmeyer et al., 1997). How­
culties in social cognition and empathy in this disorder (Menzies et al., ever, the subsequent studies have not supported a specific impairment in
2008). These brain networks are not only critical for executive functions recognition of disgust in OCD (Aigner et al., 2007; Corcoran et al., 2008;

* Corresponding author at: Dokuz Eylul Universitesi Tip Fakultesi, Psikiyatri Anabilim Dali, Mithatpaşa Cad. No:1606 Inciraltı Yerleskesi 35340 Balcova Izmir.
E-mail addresses: emre.bora@deu.edu.tr, ibora@unimelb.edu.au.

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114752
Received 8 April 2020; Received in revised form 18 July 2022; Accepted 27 July 2022
Available online 28 July 2022
0165-1781/© 2022 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Daros et al., 2014; Parker et al., 2004). Relatively few studies have due to an insufficient number of studies investigating these domains in
examined ToM skills in OCD (Jansen et al., 2020). Several studies have OCD.
found no evidence of ToM impairment in OCD (Buhlmann et al., 2015;
Mavrogiorgou et al., 2016). On the other hand, other studies have 2.2. Social cognition and empathy measures
suggested that patients with OCD might have difficulties in ToM tasks
(Pino et al., 2016; Sayin et al., 2010). 2.2.1. Social cognition measures
Empathy is a construct that is partly related to social cognition. Studies have investigated facial emotion recognition with a variety
Abnormalities in empathic skills can contribute to problems in social of methods (Ekman and Friesen, 1976). Different ToM tasks have been
functioning in OCD. In studies concerning OCD, empathy is most utilized across studies. The most commonly used ToM tasks included the
commonly measured with self-report measures such as the interpersonal Reading the Mind in the Eyes Task (RMET) and the Hinting tasks. Other
reactivity index (IRI) and the Empathy Quotient (EQ) (Baron-Cohen and studies have included the faux pas recognition (the task involves
Wheelright, 2004; Davis, 1980). Empathy has several dimensions recognizing faux pas in series of short stories), false belief tasks, and the
including cognitive (i.e., the perspective-taking subscale in IRI) and af­ Happe stories (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Corcoran et al., 1995; Stone
fective components (i.e. the empathic concern and personal distress et al., 1998). In the RMET, individuals are instructed to look at a series of
subscales in IRI). Cognitive empathy measures the ability to understand photographs of the eye region of the face and select the word that best
the psychological viewpoints of others and is conceptually similar to describes what the person in the photo is thinking or feeling. In the
ToM and emotion recognition. On the other hand, affective components Hinting task, participants read a number of short passages of dialogues
of empathy are constructs that are relatively independent of traditional between 2 characters. In each conversation, one of the characters tries to
social cognitive abilities (ToM and emotion recognition). Empathic indirectly convey a certain request or intention to the other. Participants
concern measures the individual’s tendency to experience "other-or­ are required to correctly infer this request or intention.
iented" feelings of sympathy and concern for others. Unlike empathic There are two separable aspects of TOM: (1) decoding mental states
concern, “personal distress” is a self-focused emotional reaction and is from perceivable social information such as facial expression, tone of
often related to emotional contagion. It reflects one’s anxiety and voice, body posture (i.e., decoding irony from voice tone and facial
emotional reactivity in response to the comprehension of another’s expression), and (2) reasoning about mental states by integrating
emotional state or condition. Developmentally, personal distress and contextual and historical information about a person (e.g., knowledge,
emotional contagion emerge earlier than empathic concern. Investi­ attitudes, long-term beliefs) (Sabbagh, 2004). The RMET was the only
gating multiple aspects of empathy can help us to understand the source measure for ToM-decoding (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Sabbagh et al.,
of functional impairment in OCD. Relatively few studies have examined 2004). All other ToM tasks were measuring ToM-reasoning (Bora and
empathy skills in OCD. These studies reported increased personal Köse, 2016; Sabbagh, 2004).
distress but found inconsistent findings regarding the other aspects of
empathy (Cain et al., 2015; Fontenelle et al., 2009; Pino et al., 2016). 2.2.2. Empathy measures
Some of the inconsistent findings of studies investigating social The interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) was the main measure used
cognition and empathy in OCD might be related to the low statistical to measure empathy in OCD (Davis, 1980). The IRI has 28-items
power of individual studies, as many of the available studies have small answered on a 5-point Likert scale. IRI consists of 4 subscales
sample sizes. It is also important to note that factors including clinical including perspective taking, fantasy, personal distress and empathic
and demographic characteristics, executive functions, and co-morbid concern. The Empathy Quotient (EQ), and the Basic Empathy Scale
depressive symptoms can influence the estimated effect sizes for social (BES) were the other measures that were used to assess empathy (Bar­
cognition and empathy abnormalities in OCD. The current meta-analysis on-Cohen and Wheelright, 2004; Jolliffe and Farrington, 2006).
was conducted to provide a reliable estimate of abnormalities in social
cognition and empathy in OCD. An additional aim was to investigate the 2.3. Data extraction and computation of effect sizes
effect of confounding variables on group differences in social cognition
between OCD and healthy controls. Data extraction was performed separately by E.B and B.Y (See
acknowledgement section). The quality of the studies included was
2. Methods assessed using the selection and quality of outcome ascertainment sec­
tions (maximum score = 8) of a modified (for cross-sectional studies)
2.1. Study selection version of the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (Wells et al., 2000). The main
outcome measure was the standardized mean effect size (Cohen’s d) for
In this meta-analysis, PRISMA guidelines were followed (Moher the studies comparing ToM, facial emotion recognition, or empathy
et al., 2009). A literature search was conducted using the databases performances between individuals with OCD and healthy controls. De­
Pubmed, PsycINFO, ProQuest, and Scopus to identify the relevant mographic variables (age, gender, duration of education), clinical var­
studies (January 1980 to March 2020) using the combination of key­ iables (duration of illness, age of onset of illness), the effect size for the
words as follows: (“Theory of mind” OR “emotion recognition” OR between-group differences for the measures of executive functions,
“social cognition” OR empathy OR mentali*) AND “obsessive-­ mean total scores of the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive Scale (Y-
compulsive disorder”. Reference lists of published reports were also BOCS) (Goodman et al., 1989) and ratings for depression symptoms
reviewed for additional studies. Inclusion criteria were studies that: (1) were also coded.
Compared ToM, empathy, facial emotion recognition performances of
adult patients with OCD and healthy controls. OCD diagnosis was 2.4. Statistical analyses
required to be based on DSM or ICD criteria using a clinical interview;
(2) reported sufficient data to calculate the effect size and standard error Effect sizes (Hedges’ g) were calculated for the comparison of OCD vs
of the social cognition or empathy measures; (3) had been published in a healthy controls on facial emotion recognition, ToM, and empathy
peer-reviewed journal indexed in Science Citation Index. Studies scores reported within each study.
investigating emotion recognition abilities with methods other than ToM: Effect sizes for ToM were calculated. For studies that reported
facial recognition were excluded as only a few studies investigated OCD more than one ToM task, pooled effect sizes (Cohen’s d) were calculated.
using such methods (i.e. vocal). Also, behavioral results of fMRI tasks Separate effect sizes for ToM-decoding (RMET) and ToM-reasoning were
were not included. Other aspects of social cognition including emotion also calculated.
regulation and social perception were not included in this meta-analysis Facial emotion recognition: Effect sizes for facial emotion recognition

2
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

total score were calculated. In addition to the total emotion labeling estimate) was used. Homogeneity of the distribution of weighted effect
score, separate effect sizes for six basic emotions (anger, fear, disgust, sizes was tested with the Q and I2 tests (I2 values <50% indicate low
sadness, happiness, surprise) were also calculated. heterogeneity, I2 >50% indicate moderate heterogeneity, and I2 >75%
Empathy: First, effect sizes for total empathy scores were calculated. indicate large heterogeneity). Publication bias was assessed by inspec­
In addition, effect sizes for cognitive and affective empathy scores were tion of funnel plots, Egger’s test, and trim and fill method. Egger’s test
calculated. Separate meta-analyses were also conducted for the total and relies on the theory that studies with significant rather than negative
the 4 subscale scores of the IRI. findings in studies with small sample sizes would be more likely to be
Meta-analyses were performed using the “metaphor” package in R reported while large-scale studies would be more likely to be published
environment (Viechtbauer, 2010). Effect sizes (Hedges’ g) were regardless of the significance of the findings. In Egger’s test, the p-value
weighted using the inverse variance method. 95% confidence intervals less than 0.05 implicates publication bias.
(CI) were also reported. A random-effects model (DerSimonian–Laird Meta-regression analyses were conducted for age, gender (ratio of

Table 1
Characteristics of the studies included.
Study Sample Sex Age Mean (SD) Social cognition and Clinical characteristics Outcome
(male empathy measures
%)

Aigner et al., 2007 40 OCD40 60% 34.8 (10.4) Emotion recognition OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I co- No difference
HC 60% 34.7 (8.7) morbiditiesexcluded.
Altinöz et al. 2019 48 OCD39 44% 33.0 (median) RMET, Hinting OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not RMET impaired in OCD
HC 41% 35.0 (median) excluded
Bozikas et al., 2009 25 OCD25 40% 32.7 (8.9)33.4 Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
HC 56% (7.3) excluded
Buhlmann et al., 20 OCD20 40% 31.0 (10.5) Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
2004 HC 35% 32.9 (11.7) excluded
Buhlmann et al. 35 OCD35 51% 34.0 (9.1)32.7 MASC OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not Not impaired
2015 HC 40% (11.0) excluded
Cain et al., 2015 25 OCD25 52% 33.7 (9.1)33.7 IRI OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not OCD is associated with-decreased cognitive
HC 40% (10.8) excluded empathy-increased personal distress
Corcoran et al. 2008 40 OCD36 Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not OCD is associated with impaireddisgust
HC excluded recognition
Fontenelle et al. 53 OCD53 45% 39.3 (13.8) IRI OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not Increased empathic concernand personal
2009 HC 34% 35.5 (13.0) excluded distress in OCD
Jhung et al., 2010 41 OCD37 78% 24.9 (5.3)25.9 Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference in non-ambiguousstimuli
HC 76% (6.0) excluded
Kang et al., 2012 107 67% 27.5 (9.2)26 IRI OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not Decreased perspectivetaking and
OCD130 63% (4.8) excluded increasedpersonal distress in OCD in IRI
HC
Kim et al., 2018 277 29.9 (10.7) IRI OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not Decreased perspectivetaking and
OCD395 22.2 (2.7) excluded increasedpersonal distress in OCD in IRI
HC
Kornreich et al., 22 OCD22 37.3 (8.0)37.2 Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
2001 HC (9.0) excluded
Liu et al., 2017 40 OCD38 45% 24.6 (4.1)23.3 Yoni OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I excluded OCD is associated with
HC 42% (2.7) impairedperformance in second-
orderaffective ToM task
Lochner et al., 2012 20 OCD20 55% 34.1 (11.0) Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I excluded No difference
HC 45% 34.8 (10.8)
López-Del-Hoyo 31 OCD30 65% 40.2 (11.9) Hinting, RMET, IRI OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not ToM is impaired in OCD. Increasedpersonal
et al., 2019 HC 23% 46.4 (11.2) excluded distress in OCD
Mavrioglu et al. 20 OCD20 60% 38.1 (10.6) Hinting, faux pas, OCD criteria: DSM-IV andICD 10Axis No difference
2016 HC 60% 38.2 (13.0) facial emotion I not excluded
Mısır et al., 2018 34 OCD30 38% 32.4 (10.0) RMET, ToM stories OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not ToM is impaired in OCD
HC 57% 34.4 (9.7) excluded
Montagne et al., 21 OCD47 43% 36.0 (10.3) Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not A subgroup of OCD patients withchecking
2008 HC 51% 40.6 (12.3) excluded behavior are more sensitiveto fear and
happiness
Parker et al. 2004 15 OCD15 47% 37.7 (10.7) Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
HC 31.3 (12.2) excluded
Pertusa et al. 2012 31 OCD55 39% 39.0 (13.4) RMET OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
HC 51% 37.4 (15.4) excluded
Pino et al., 2016 23 OCD24 52% 39.1 (12.9) EQ, BES, RMET OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I excluded Cognitive empathy is impairedin OCD
HC 54% 38.7 (11.9)
Salazar-Kampf et al. 64 OCD62 41% 34.2 (9.9)35.8 IRI, EmpaToM OCD criteria: DSM-IVSubstance Increasedpersonal distress in OCD in IRI
2022 HC 39% (10.2) dependenceexcluded
Sayın et al. 2010 30 OCD30 33% 34.3 (11.5) ToM stories, false OCD criteria: DSM-IVMDD is Advanced ToM is impairedin OCD
HC 33% 33.0 (10.6) belief excluded
Toh et al., 2015 19 OCD21 26% 37.0 (10.4) Facial emotion OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not No difference
HC 38% 35.7 (10.6) excluded
Tulaci et al. 2018 80 OCD80 35% 29.5 (9.7) Hinting, RMET, OCD criteria: DSM-IVAxis I not ToM impaired in OCD
HC Faux Pas, excluded (otherthan severe
FalseBelief disorders)

BES=Basic empathy scale, EQ=Empathy Quotient, IRI=Interpersonal reactivity index, RMET=Reading the mind in the eyes test, EQ=Empathy Quotient,.
ToM=Theory of Mind, MASC=Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition, OCD=Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, HC– – Healthy controls, MDD= Major Depressive
Disorder.

3
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

males in OCD group) with a random-effects model were using the DSM-IV criteria. There was no significant between-group difference for
restricted-information maximum likelihood method with a significance age (g = 0.01, CI=− 0.09, 0.11, p = 0.87). The duration of education
level set at p<0.05. Several ToM studies also reported correlations of tended to be shorter in patients with OCD compared to healthy controls
ToM with Y-BOCS, depression ratings, and executive functions (using but the between-group difference was not statistically significant (g =
the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and verbal fluency tests). Meta-analyses 0.14, CI=− 0.01, 0.29, p = 0.06). The mean Y-BOCS score in the included
of correlations (r) for these variables were also conducted for exploring studies was 24.2. The range of Y-BOCS scores (20.6 to 33.3, reported in
the influence of these variables on ToM performance in OCD. 18 studies) across studies suggested that patients included in the current
meta-analysis had predominantly moderate or severe current symptom
3. Results severity. The quality scores of the included studies are reported in
Table 1s in the supplement.
The current meta-analysis included 25 studies involving 1161
(51.6% males) patients with OCD and 1329 (49.4% males) healthy
controls (Table 1) (see Fig. 1 for the flowchart of the study selection
process). In all of the studies included, OCD was diagnosed based on

Fig. 1. Flow diagram of the meta-analysis.

4
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

3.1. Social cognition of effect sizes for the IRI Perspective-taking subscale (I2=81%). As ex­
pected, there was no between-group difference for the Fantasy subscale
3.1.1. ToM of the IRI (g=− 0.05, CI=− 0.24,0.10).
The distribution of effect sizes for ToM-reasoning was homogeneous There was no evidence of publication bias for any of the cognitive
(I2= 0%). The distribution of effect sizes for the ToM-decoding (RMET) empathy measures.
task was extremely heterogeneous (I2=97%) but this heterogeneity was
related to the effect of a single study (López-Del-Hoyo et al., 2019) 3.2.2. Affective empathy
reporting an extreme outlier value for ToM-decoding (g = 3.73) (eFig­ Contrary to the findings for cognitive empathy, OCD was associated
ure1). The distribution of effect sizes for ToM-decoding was homoge­ with a higher total affective empathy score (g=− 0.44, CI=− 0.16, − 0.73,
neous after removing this study (I2= 0%). The ToM-decoding k = 7; Fig. 6) which was more robust when the analysis was restricted to
meta-analyses were conducted after removing this outlier study. the IRI total affective empathy score (g=− 0.54, CI=− 0.37, − 0.72).
The performance of patients with OCD was significantly poorer than There was a significant heterogeneity in the distribution of effect sizes
healthy controls for ToM (g = 0.43, CI=0.24, 0.62, k = 11; Fig. 2) (Table- for affective empathy (I2=79%). However, heterogeneity in this analysis
2). Among ToM domains, both ToM-reasoning (g = 0.54, CI=0.32, 0.77, was related to the effect of the psychometric differences between the
k = 8) and ToM-decoding (g = 0.46, CI=0.28, 0.66, k = 5) were affective empathy measures used. The distribution of affective empathy
significantly impaired in OCD. There was no evidence of publication was less heterogenous when this analysis was conducted only with the
bias. IRI affective empathy score (I2=40%).
Further analyses of the IRI subscales suggested that affective
3.1.2. Emotion recognition empathy enhancement in OCD was related to increased personal distress
There was no significant difference between OCD and healthy con­ subscale (g=− 0.93, CI=− 0.74, − 1.13; Fig. 7). There was no significant
trol groups in total emotion recognition score (g = 0.13, CI=− 0.04, 0.29, difference in the IRI empathic concern between patients with OCD and
k = 11; Fig. 3) (Table-2). In the analysis of individual emotions, patients healthy controls (g=− 0.16, CI=− 0.42, 0.10) (Table-2). The distribu­
with OCD significantly underperformed controls in recognition of tions of effect sizes for affective empathy subscales of the IRI were
disgust (g = 0.26, CI=0.04, 0.48, k = 7; Fig. 4), but not in other emotions homogenous.
(Table 2). The distributions of effect sizes for emotion recognition var­ There was no evidence of publication bias for any of the affective
iables, except recognition of happiness, were homogenous (Table-2). empathy measures.
There was no evidence of publication bias (Table-2).
3.3. Meta-regression and correlational meta-analyses
3.2. Empathy
In study-level meta-regression analyses, age and gender had no sig­
3.2.1. Cognitive empathy nificant effect on between-group differences in social cognition. The
In meta-analyses of empathy, OCD was associated with reduced between-group differences in duration of education were not signifi­
cognitive empathy (g = 0.44, CI=0.10, 0.78, k = 7) (Fig. 5). There was cantly related to effect sizes for ToM (Z = 0.07, p = 0.95), empathy
significant heterogeneity in the distribution of effect sizes for cognitive (Z=− 1.34, p = 0.18), and emotion recognition (Z = 1.24, p = 0.22)
empathy (I2=85%). When a meta-analysis of cognitive empathy was differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls. The quality
conducted using only the IRI Perspective taking score, cognitive score of studies included had no significant effect on group differences in
empathy impairment in OCD remained significant (g = 0.33, CI=0.03, ToM (Z = 0.47, p = 0.54), empathy (Z=− 0.68, p = 0.49), and emotion
0.63). However, there was significant heterogeneity in the distribution recognition (Z = 0.53, p = 0.60) between patients with OCD and healthy

Fig. 2. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for ToM diffences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random effects model.

5
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Fig. 3. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for emotion recognition differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random
effects model.

Fig. 4. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for disgust recognition differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random
effects model.

controls. was possible to conduct correlational meta-analyses for these variables.


Meta-regression analyses have limitations as they are based on study- In correlation meta-analyses of studies investigating ToM (k = 4–7), Y-
level rather than individual-level data. Unfortunately, most studies have BOCS (r=− 0.05, CI=0.08, − 0.17, Z = 0.70, p = 0.48, 259 patients with
not reported correlations between social cognition/empathy and most of OCD) and depression ratings (r = 0.04, CI= − 0.14, 0.21, Z = 0.40, p =
the other demographic and clinical variables. However, as an exception, 0.69, 131 patients with OCD) were not significantly associated with ToM
some studies reported results of correlation between ToM and the Y- scores in OCD. However, executive function performance was signifi­
BOCS scores, depression ratings, and executive functions. Therefore, it cantly associated with ToM scores in OCD (r = 0.20, CI=0.03, 0.36, Z =

6
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Table 2
. mean weighted effect sizes for social cognitive and empathy differences between OCD and healthy controls.
Test k OCD HC g 95% CI Z P Q test Q-test p I2 Bias

Emotion recognition 11 283 303 0.13 − 0.04 0.29 1.54 0.12 7.5 0.68 0 0.97
Happy 5 115 143 − 0.15 − 0.58 0.28 0.68 0.50 10.8 0.03 64 0.65
Sad 7 196 216 0.09 − 0.11 0.29 0.91 0.36 8.3 0.22 0 0.58
Anger 6 156 176 0.19 − 0.10 0.47 1.28 0.20 8.2 0.15 38 0.74
Fear 6 156 176 0.07 − 0.18 0.32 0.51 0.61 6.2 0.29 23 0.60
Disgust 7 176 196 0.26 0.04 0.48 2.32 0.02 6.2 0.40 10 0.61
Surprise 4 75 103 − 0.06 − 0.51 0.39 0.26 0.80 6.2 0.10 52 0.88
ToM 11 436 443 0.43 0.24 0.62 4.38 <0.001 19.4 0.04 48 0.93
-ToM-reasoning 8 318 302 0.54 0.32 0.77 4.78 <0.001 12.6 0.08 45 0.45
-ToM-decoding (RMET) 5 216 228 0.46 0.28 0.66 4.83 <0.001 4.0 0.40 0 0.82
Empathy
-Cognitive empathy 7 580 719 0.44 0.10 0.78 2.55 0.01 26.7 <0.01 85 0.08
-IRI PT 6 557 695 0.33 0.03 0.64 2.16 0.03 18.3 0.003 81 0.21
-Affective empathy 7 580 719 − 0.44 − 0.16 − 0.73 3.06 0.002 18.9 0.004 79 0.77
-Affective empathy (IRI) 6 557 695 − 0.54 − 0.37 − 0.72 6.22 <0.001 8.1 0.15 40 0.48
-IRI EC 6 557 695 − 0.16 − 0.42 0.10 1.21 0.23 15.3 <0.01 74 0.91
-IRI PD 6 557 695 − 0.93 − 1.13 − 0.74 9.3 <0.001 10.1 0.07 50 0.13
-IRI F 6 557 695 − 0.05 − 0.24 0.15 0.45 0.65 10.9 0.06 56 0.25

OCD=Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder, ToM=Theory of mind, HC– –Healthy controls, IRI=Interpersonal reactivity index, PT=Perspective taking, EC=Empathic
Concern, PD=Personal Distress, F=Fantasy, Bias= p value for the Egger’s test, g=Hedges g.

Fig. 5. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for cognitive empathy differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random
effects model.

2.32, p = 0.03, 138 patients with OCD). well-known enhanced reactivity to disgust which is evident in many
patients with OCD (Bhikram et al., 2017; Knowles et al., 2018; Olatunji
4. Discussion et al., 2019). However, recognition and appraisal of disgust are separate
processes. Current findings suggest that the exacerbated disgust
The current meta-analysis investigated social cognition and empathy response in OCD is not related to the enhancement of perceptual/emo­
in patients with OCD compared to healthy controls. The findings of this tional recognition of aversive stimuli. Disgust proneness in OCD is more
meta-analysis showed that patients with OCD performed significantly likely to be the result of abnormalities in the appraisal of disgust and
poorer than healthy controls in ToM and they also had mild impairment other aversive stimuli which might be related to functional abnormal­
in recognition of disgust. OCD was significantly associated with reduced ities in the orbitofrontal cortex (Bhikram et al., 2017; Evans et al. 2004).
cognitive empathy but affective empathy was significantly enhanced in Impairment in recognition of disgust, which was mild but relatively
OCD compared to healthy controls. Moreover, enhanced affective more pronounced than other emotions, is compatible with the findings
empathy in OCD was related to the increased the IRI personal distress in major depression (Dalili et al., 2015).
rather than the IRI empathic concern. In the current meta-analysis, OCD was associated with medium-sized
In OCD, the recognition of basic emotions was mostly intact deficits in ToM (g = 0.43). Both decoding and reasoning aspects of ToM
compared to healthy controls. Mild impairment in recognition of disgust were impaired (g = 0.46, 0.54). The severity of impairment in ToM was
was the only exception (g = 0.26). This finding seemingly contradicts the comparable in magnitude to other neurocognitive deficits as reported in

7
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Fig. 6. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for affective empathy differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random
effects model.

Fig. 7. Forest plot of effect sizes and 95% confidence intervals for IRI Personal Distress differences between patients with OCD and healthy controls using random
effects model.

a previous meta-analysis in OCD (Shin et al., 2014). OCD is associated are necessary to investigate the relationship between
with abnormalities in prefrontal brain regions that have a role in ToM obsessive-compulsive symptoms and social cognition/empathy. The ef­
and social cognition (Del Casale et al. 2017; Rasgon et al., 2017; fects of cognitive deficits on ToM impairment in OCD is another
Thorsen et al., 2018). ToM impairment in OCD might be likely related to important consideration. A preliminary meta-analysis of correlational
abnormalities in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and its subcortical data provided evidence for a significant but modest association between
connections. However, there is a paucity of studies directly investigating executive functions and ToM suggesting that cognitive impairment
the neural correlates of ToM deficits in OCD. Another consideration is might influence the effect size of ToM impairment in OCD.
the relationship between state factors and ToM in OCD. ToM impairment OCD was associated with a reduction in the self-reported cognitive
in OCD was not significantly related to the severity of empathy score (g = 0.44). This finding was in accordance with ToM
obsessive-compulsive symptoms. These findings might suggest that ToM impairment in this meta-analysis. Contrary to the findings of the meta-
impairment is potentially a trait feature of the illness but further studies analysis of cognitive empathy, affective empathy was not reduced in

8
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

OCD, and actually, patients with OCD had a robust enhancement in the and the level of functional impairment in OCD. Previous studies have
IRI Personal Distress (g = 0.93) domain of affective empathy. This was shown positive effects of social cognitive training on ToM and emotion
the most robust finding of the current meta-analysis. Increasing evi­ recognition in other psychiatric conditions including major depression
dence suggests that elevated personal distress, the tendency to feel self- and schizophrenia (Prikken et al., 2019; Zhu et al., 2018). However,
oriented anxiety and worry (as an emotional contagion) when exposed social cognitive training has not been systematically explored in OCD.
to the misfortune or suffering of others, is a transdiagnostic feature of Social cognitive training, in the subgroup of patients presenting with
many psychopathological conditions including anxiety disorders, major difficulties in social cognition and cognitive empathy, might be a po­
depression, anorexia nervosa and borderline personality disorder tential approach in the management of OCD.
(Dziobek et al., 2011; Flashback et al. 2017; Kerr-Gaffney et al., 2019; There were several limitations of the current meta-analysis. The
Schreiter et al., 2013). The IRI Personal Distress might be a develop­ number of available studies was relatively small. Another limitation was
mental precursor of affective empathic concern and might be an indi­ the lack of neurocognitive assessment in most studies included, so it was
cator of the level of affective empathy when co-expressed with empathic not possible to explore the contribution of cognitive impairment on
concern. However, it is important to note that elevated the IRI personal observed between-group differences except for ToM. Also, the self-
distress might reflect state-related factors (i.e. current depressive report nature of empathy scales used in the available studies is one of
symptoms or nonspecific anxiety) rather than genuine trait-related the important considerations, as there might be a discrepancy between
enhanced personal distress and emotional contagion. Elevated PD performance-based empathy tasks and how empathetic individuals
might potentially simply reflect general sensitivity to negative stimuli in perceive themselves to be. However, similar levels of impairment in self-
a broader context (not only in response to the comprehension of others’ report empathy and performance-based ToM measures were found in
mental states). In the current meta-analysis, only the IRI Personal this meta-analysis, which increases the confidence in the current
Distress but not the IRI Empathic Concern was enhanced in OCD sug­ findings.
gesting that there might not be an actual change in affective empathy As a conclusion, the findings of this meta-analysis provided sub­
levels in OCD. Therefore, further studies investigating the personal stantial evidence for deficits in social cognition and cognitive empathy
distress aspect of empathy in OCD using experimental procedures, rather in OCD. Future research should examine the influence of comorbid
than a psychometric measure, are necessary. psychopathological traits and neurocognitive difficulties on social
Interestingly, the pattern and magnitude of social cognitive deficits cognition in OCD.
in OCD in this meta-analysis were very similar to what was found for
major depressive disorder (MDD) in earlier meta-analyses (Bora and Financial support
Berk, 2016; Dalili et al., 2015; Screiter et al. 2013). Both OCD and MDD
seem to be associated with enhanced IRI Personal Distress score, ab­ This research did not receive any specific grant from funding
normality in recognition of disgust, and deficits in ToM and cognitive agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.
empathy. MDD is a very common comorbidity in OCD and most of the
patients in this meta-analysis had mild depression. Therefore, it is
Conflict of interest
important to investigate the effect of depressive symptoms on social
cognitive impairment, and empathy abnormalities in OCD. Unfortu­
None
nately, only a few of the studies included in the current meta-analysis
explored individual level direct correlations between depression rat­
Acknowledgements
ings and emotion recognition or empathy. Therefore, a meta-analysis of
such correlations was not possible. However, it was possible to conduct a
I would like to thank to Berna Yalınçetin for her support in coding the
preliminary meta-analysis of correlational data for the relationship be­
data and literature search.
tween ToM and depression ratings. In this analysis, no significant rela­
tionship between ToM and depression scores was evident. Differences in
the levels of insight in OCD samples across studies might also be an Supplementary materials
important moderator (Liu et al., 2017; Tulaci et al. 2018; Jansen et al.,
2020). However, it was not possible to investigate the relationship be­ Supplementary material associated with this article can be found, in
tween insight and social cognition as the vast majority of the included the online version, at doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2022.114752.
studies have not reported this variable. It is also important to investigate
the moderating effects of other psychopathological variables on the ef­ References
fect sizes for the relationship between OCD and social cognition/em­
Abu-Akel, Aş, Shamay-Tsoory, S., 2011. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical bases of
pathy. One important candidate is autism spectrum disorder, which is theory of mind. Neuropsychologia 49 (11), 2971–2984.
robustly associated with deficits in social cognition and empathy (Bora Aigner, M., Sachs, G., Bruckmüller, E., Winklbaur, B., Zitterl, W., Kryspin-Exner, I.,
and Pantelis, 2016; Harmsen et al. 2019). Autistic traits are significantly Gur, R., Katschnig, H., 2007. Cognitive and emotion recognition deficits in obsessive-
compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 149 (1–3), 121–128.
prevalent in patients with OCD compared to controls (Bejerot et al., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., Plumb, I., 2001. The “Reading the
2001; Griffiths et al., 2017; Ivarsson et al. 2008; Wikramanayake et al., mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adultswith
2018). It is likely that deficits in social cognition and empathy might be Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child. Psychol. Psychiatry 42,
241–251.
evident mostly in OCD patients with autistic features. Unfortunately,
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., 2004. The empathy quotient: an investigation of
previous studies have not investigated these potential associations. adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex
Therefore, it was not possible to include autistic traits and other differences. J Autism Dev. Disord. 34 (2), 163–175.
Bejerot, S., Nylander, L., Lindström, E., 2001. Autistic traits in obsessive-compulsive
co-morbidities (i.e., personality disorders) as moderators in the current
disorder. Nord. J. Psychiatry 55 (3), 169–176.
quantitative systematic review. In the future studies, it would be Bhikram, T., Abi-Jaoude, E., Sandor, P., 2017. OCD: obsessive-compulsive … disgust?
important to ascertain whether or not deficits in social cognition and The role of disgust in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 42 (5),
empathy in OCD are characteristics of particular clinical subgroups of 300–306.
Bobes, J., González, M.P., Bascarán, M.T., Arango, C., Sáiz, P.A., Bousoño, M., 2001.
this disorder. Quality of life and disability in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Eur.
The current findings might have clinical implications. Abnormalities Psychiatry 16 (4), 239–245.
in social cognition and empathy are likely candidates for playing a sig­ Bora, E., 2017. Relationship between insight and theory of mind in schizophrenia: a
meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 190, 11–17.
nificant role in interpersonal dysfunction in OCD. Further studies are Bora, E., Berk, M., 2016. Theory of mind in major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis.
needed to directly investigate the relationship between social cognition J. Affect Disord. 191, 49–55.

9
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Bora, E., Köse, S., 2016. Meta-analysis of theory of mind in anorexia nervosa and bulimia compared with obsessive-compulsive disorder and normal controls. Psychiatry Res.
nervosa: a specific impairment of cognitive perspective taking in anorexia nervosa? 102 (3), 235–248.
Int. J. Eat. Disord. 49 (8), 739–740. Lee, L., Harkness, K.L., Sabbagh, M.A., Jacobson, J.A., 2005. Mental state decoding
Bora, E., Pantelis, C., 2016. Meta-analysis of social cognition in attention deficit/ abilities in clinical depression. J. Affect. Disord. 86, 247–258.
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): comparison with healthy controls and autistic Liu, W., Fan, J., Gan, J., Lei, H., Niu, C., Chan, R.C.K., Zhu, X., 2017. Disassociation of
spectrum disorder. Psychol. Med. 46 (4), 699–716. cognitive and affective aspects of theory of mind in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Bora, E., Eryavuz, A., Kayahan, B., 2006. Social functioning, theory of mind and Psychiatry Res. 255, 367–372.
neurocognition in outpatients with schizophrenia; mental state decoding may be a Lochner, C., Simmons, C., Kidd, M., Chamberlain, S.R., Fineberg, N.A., van Honk, J.,
better predictor of social functioning than mental state reasoning. Psychiatry Res. Ipser, J., Stein, D.J., 2012. Differential effects of escitalopram challenge on disgust
145, 95–103. processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 226 (1), 274–280.
Bora, E., Walterfang, M., Velakoulis, D., 2015. Theory of mind in Parkinson’s disease: a López-Del-Hoyo, Y., Panzano, M.G., Lahera, G., Herrera-Mercadal, P.,., Navarro-Gil, M.,
meta-analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 292, 515–520. Campos, D., Borao, L., Morillo, H., García-Campayo, J., 2019. Differences between
Bora, E., Velakoulis, D., Walterfang, M., 2016. Social cognition in Huntington’s disease: a individuals with schizophrenia or obsessive-compulsive disorder and healthy
meta-analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 297, 131–140. controls in social cognition and mindfulness skills: a controlled study. PLoS ONE 14
Bozikas, V.P., Kosmidis, M.H., Giannakou, M., Saitis, M., Fokas, K., Garyfallos, G., 2009. (12), e0225608.
Emotion perception in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15 Markarian, Y., Larson, M.J., Aldea, M.A., Baldwin, S.A., Good, D., Berkeljon, A.,
(1), 148–153. Murphy, T.K., Storch, E.A., McKay, D., 2010. Multiple pathways to functional
Buhlmann, U., McNally, R.J., Etcoff, N.L., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Wilhelm, S., 2004. impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30 (1), 78–88.
Emotion recognition deficits in body dysmorphic disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 38 (2), Maurage, P., Lahaye, M., Grynberg, D., Jeanjean, A., Guettat, L., Verellen-Dumoulin, C.,
201–206. Halkin, S., Heeren, A., Billieux, J., Constant, E., 2016. Dissociating emotional and
Buhlmann, U., Wacker, R., Dziobek, I., 2015. Inferring other people’s states of mind: cognitive empathy in pre-clinical and clinical Huntington’s disease. Psychiatry Res.
comparison across social anxiety, body dysmorphic, and obsessive-compulsive 237, 103–108.
disorders. J. Anxiety Disord. 34, 107–113. Mavrogiorgou, P., Bethge, M., Luksnat, S., Nalato, F., Juckel, G., Brüne, M., 2016. Social
Bystritsky, A., Liberman, R.P., Hwang, S., Wallace, C.J., Vapnik, T., Maindment, K., cognition and metacognition in obsessive-compulsive disorder: an explorative pilot
Saxena, S., 2001. Social functioning and quality of life comparisons between study. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 266 (3), 209–216.
obsessive compulsive and schizophrenic disorders. Depress. Anxiety 14, 214–218. Menzies, L., Chamberlain, S.R., Laird, A.R., Thelen, S.M., Sahakian, B.J., Bullmore, E.T.,
Cain, N.M., Ansell, E.B., Simpson, H.B., Pinto, A., 2015. Interpersonal functioning in 2008. Integrating evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of
obsessive-compulsive personality disorder. J. Pers. Assess. 97 (1), 90–99. obsessive-compulsive disorder: the orbitofronto-striatal model revisited. Neurosci.
Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., Frith, C.D., 1995. Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social Biobehav. Rev. 32 (3), 525–549.
inference: investigating “Theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Mısır, E., Bora, E., Akdede, B.B., 2018. Relationship between social-cognitive and social-
Res. 17, 5–13. perceptual aspects of theory of mind and neurocognitive deficits, insight level and
Corcoran, K.M., Woody, S.R., Tolin, D.F., 2008. Recognition of facial expressions in schizotypal traits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Compr. Psychiatry. 83, 1–6.
obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Anxiety. Disord. 22 (1), 56–66. Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D.G., Group PRISMA, 2009. Preferred
Cox, C.L., Uddin, L.Q., Di Martino, A., Castellanos, F.X., Milham, M.P., Kelly, C., 2012. reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.
The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are The BMJ 339, b2535.
reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7 Montagne, B., de Geus, F., Kessels, R.P., Denys, D., de Haan, E.H., Westenberg, H.G.,
(6), 727–737. 2008. Perception of facial expressions in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a
Dalili, M.N., Penton-Voak, I.S., Harmer, C.J., Munafò, M.R., 2015. Meta-analysis of dimensional approach. Eur. Psychiatry. 23 (1), 26–28.
emotion recognition deficits in major depressive disorder. Psychol. Med. 45 (6), Olatunji, B.O., Kim, J., Cox, R.C., Ebesutani, C., 2019. Prospective associations between
1135–1144. disgust proneness and OCD symptoms: specificity to excessive washing compulsions.
Daros, A.R., Zakzanis, K.K., Rector, N.A., 2014. A quantitative analysis of facial emotion J. Anxiety. Disord. 65, 34–40.
recognition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 215 (3), 514–521. Parker, H.A., McNally, R.J., Nakayama, K., Wilhelm, S., 2004. No disgust recognition
Davis, M.H., 1980. A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. deficit in obsessive-compulsive disorder. J. Behav. Therapy Exp. Psychiatry. 35 (2),
JSAS Catalog Sel. Document. Psychol. 10, 85. 183–192.
Dziobek, I., Preissler, S., Grozdanovic, Z., Heuser, I., Heekeren, H.R., Roepke, S., 2011. Pino, M.C., De Berardis, D., Mariano, M., Vellante, F., Serroni, N., Valchera, A.,
Neuronal correlates of altered empathy and social cognition in borderline Valenti, M., Mazza, M., 2016. Two systems for empathy in obsessive-compulsive
personality disorder. Neuroimage 57, 539–548. disorder: mentalizing and experience sharing. Braz. Psychiatry 38 (4), 307–313.
Ekman, P., Friesen, W., 1976. Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychologists Press, Prikken, M., Konings, M.J., Lei, W.U., Begemann, M.J.H., Sommer, I.E.C., 2019. The
Palo Alto, CA. efficacy of computerized cognitive drill and practice training for patients with a
Fontenelle, L.F., Soares, I.D., Miele, F., Borges, M.C., Prazeres, A.M., Rangé, B.P., Moll, J., schizophrenia-spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis. Schizophr. Res. 204, 368–374.
2009. Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients with obsessive-compulsive Rasgon, A., Lee, W.H., Leibu, E., Laird, A., Glahn, D., Goodman, W., Frangou, S., 2017.
disorder. J. Psychiatr. Res. 43 (4), 455–463. Neural correlates of affective and non-affective cognition in obsessive compulsive
Goodman, W.K., Price, L.H., Rasmussen, S.A., Mazure, C., Fleischmann, R.L., Hill, C.L., disorder: a meta-analysis of functional imaging studies. Eur. Psychiatry. 46, 25–32.
Heninger, G.R., Charney, D.S., 1989. The Yale-brown obsessive compulsive scale. I. Ruscio, A.M., Stein, D.J., Chiu, W.T., Kessler, R.C., 2010. The epidemiology of obsessive-
Development, use, and reliability. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46 (11), 1006–1011. compulsive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Mol.
Griffiths, D.L., Farrell, L.J., Waters, A.M., White, S.W., 2017. ASD traits among youth Psychiatry 15, 53–63.
with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Child Psychiatry & Hum. Dev. 48 (6), 911–921. Sabbagh, M.A., 2004. Understanding orbitofrontal contributions to theory-of-mind
Harmsen, I.E., 2019. Empathy in Autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism. Dev. Disord. 49 reasoning: implications for autism. Brain Cogn. 55, 209–219.
(10), 3939–3955. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04087-w. Sabbagh, M.A., Moulson, M.C., Harkness, K.L., 2004. Neural correlates of mental state
Huppert, J.D., Simpson, H.B., Nissenson, K.J., Liebowitz, M.R., Foa, E.B., 2009. Quality decoding in human adults: an event-related potential study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16,
of life and functional impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison of 415–426.
patients with and without comorbidity, patients in remission, and healthy controls. Salazar Kämpf, M., Kanske, P., Kleiman, A., Haberkamp, A., Glombiewski, J., Exner, C.,
Depression Anxiety 26 (1), 39–45. 2022. Empathy, compassion, and theory of mind in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Ivarsson, T., Melin, K., 2008. Autism spectrum traits in children and adolescents with Psychol. Psychother 95 (1), 1–17.
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). J. Anxiety Disord. 22 (6), 969–978. Sayin, A., Oral, N., Utku, C., Baysak, E., Candansayar, S., 2010. Theory of mind in
Jansen, M., Overgaauw, S., De Bruijn, E.R.A., 2020. Social cognition and obsessive- obsessive-compulsive disorder: comparison with healthy controls. Eur. Psychiatry.
compulsive disorder: a review of subdomains of social functioning. Front. Psychiatry 25 (2), 116–122.
11, 118. Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G.H., Aan het Rot, M., 2013. Empathy in adults with clinical or
Jhung, K., Namkoong, K., Kang, J.I., Ha, R.Y., An, S.K., Kim, C.H., Kim, S.J., 2010. subclinical depressive symptoms. J. Affect. Disord. 150 (1), 1–16.
Perception bias of disgust in ambiguous facial expressions in obsessive-compulsive Shin, N.Y., Lee, T.Y., Kim, E., Kwon, J.S., 2014. Cognitive functioning in obsessive-
disorder. Psychiatry Res 178 (1), 126–131. compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 44 (6), 1121–1130.
Jolliffe, D., Farrington, D.P., 2006. Development and validation of the basic empathy Sprengelmeyer, R., Young, A.W., Pundt, I., Sprengelmeyer, A., Calder, A.J., Berrios, G.,
scale. J. Adolescence 29, 589–611. Winkel, R., Vollmöeller, W., Kuhn, W., Sartory, G., Przuntek, H., 1997. Disgust
Kang, J.I., Namkoong, K., Yoo, S.W., Jhung, K., Kim, S.J., 2012. Abnormalities of implicated in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Proc. R. Soc. B 264 (1389), 1767–1773.
emotional awareness and perception in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Stone, V.E., Baron-Cohen, S., Knight, R.T., 1998. Frontal lobe contributions to theory of
J. Affect. Disord. 141 (2–3), 286–293. mind. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 10 (5), 640–656.
Kerr-Gaffney, J., Harrison, A., Tchanturia, K., 2019. Cognitive and affective empathy in Thorsen, A.L., Hagland, P., Radua, J., Mataix-Cols, D., Kvale, G., Hansen, B., van den
eating disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychiatry 10, 102. Heuvel, O.A., 2018. Emotional processing in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a
Kim, H.W., Kang, J.I., Hwang, E.H., Kim, S.J., 2018. Association between glutamate systematic review and meta-analysis of 25 functional neuroimaging studies. Biol.
transporter gene polymorphisms and obsessive-compulsive disorder/trait empathy Psychiatry: Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimag. 3 (6), 563–571.
in a Korean population. PLoS ONE 13 (1), e0190593. Toh, W.L., Castle, D.J., Rossell, S.L., 2015. Facial affect recognition in body dysmorphic
Knowles, K.A., Jessup, S.C., Olatunji, B.O., 2018. Disgust in anxiety and obsessive- disorder versus obsessive-compulsive disorder: an eye-tracking study. J. Anxiety.
compulsive disorders: recent findings and future directions. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 20 Disord. 35, 49–59.
(9), 68. Tulacı, R.G., Cankurtaran, E.Ş., Özdel, K., Öztürk, N., Kuru, E., Özdemir, İ., 2018. The
Kornreich, C., Blairy, S., Philippot, P., Dan, B., Foisy, M., Hess, U., Le Bon, O., Pelc, I., relationship between theory of mind and insight in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Verbanck, P., 2001. Impaired emotional facial expression recognition in alcoholism Nord. J. Psychiatry. 72 (4), 273–280.

10
E. Bora Psychiatry Research 316 (2022) 114752

Viechtbauer, W., 2010. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J. Stat. outpatients with obsessive compulsive disorder in the UK. Int. J. Psychiatry. Clin.
Softwares 36, 1–48. Pract. 22 (1), 54–62.
Wikramanayake, W.N.M., Mandy, W., Shahper, S., Kaur, S., Kolli, S., Osman, S., Reid, J., Zhu, S., Zhu, K., Jiang, D., Shi, J., 2018. Social cognition and interaction training for
Jefferies-Sewell, K., Fineberg, N.A., 2018. Autism spectrum disorders in adult major depression: a preliminary study. Psychiatry Res. 270, 890–894.

11

You might also like