You are on page 1of 9

Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Asian Journal of Psychiatry


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ajp

Instinctual impulses in obsessive compulsive disorder: A neuropsychological


and psychoanalytic interface
Pritha Mukhopadhyay a,*, Sreemoyee Tarafder b, Dinaz D. Bilimoria c, Debika Paul d,
Gautam Bandyopadhyay e
a
Department of Psychology, University of Calcutta, India
b
Department of Psychology, West Bengal State University, India
c
Department of Psychology, Loreto College, University of Calcutta, India
d
Department of Psychology, University of Calcutta, India
e
Department of Psychiatry, R. G. Kar Medical College and Hospital, Kolkata, India

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Article history: Psychopathology of obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) that has been characterized by a conflict
Received 4 August 2009 between the ego and superego on one hand, and aggressive and sexual impulses emerging from the id on
Received in revised form 18 September 2010 the other, and employment of characteristic defenses to combat intense conflicts being connected with
Accepted 10 October 2010
ones’ biological disposition from the psychoanalytic school of thought now gets empirical foundation
from neuroimaging research. The findings disregard the psychological construct, exclusively establish-
Keywords: ing the neurobiology of the disorder. With the objective to study the impact of sexual and aggressive
Obsessive–compulsive disorder
impulses on the executive functions and processing speed in the patient group, 20 OCD patients (11
Sexual impulses
Aggressive impulses
males, 9 females) and 20 normal control subjects, matched for all relevant variables including age, sex,
Executive function educational level and handedness were studied. Sexual impulse and guilt was assessed on the Sex Guilt
Processing speed Rating Scale (SGRS), aggressive impulses were tested using State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory
Neuropsychology (STAXI), the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS) was used to assess symptom severity,
executive functions were assessed through Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), and processing speed
was assessed by employing the Processing Speed Index (PSI) – from WAIS III. It was found that the OCD
group differed significantly from the controls, attaining significantly lower percentiles on Processing
Speed Index and for all variables of WCST under consideration, namely, perseverative response,
perseverative error, non-perseverative error, conceptual level response and number of categories
completed. They reported higher scores on subscales of STAXI, specifically related to trait anger and
lower scores on anger expression. On the items of SGRS, the OCD group significantly differed with the
controls, expressing greater sexual inhibition. In conclusion, we propose an explanation of
psychopathology of OCD, which addresses instinctual impulses, executive functions and neural
substrates. Our findings contribute to understanding instinctual impulses from the neuropsychological
perspective. The findings have implications for better eclectic understanding of the pathogenesis of OCD.
ß 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction between the ego and superego on one hand, and aggressive and
sexual impulses emerging from the id on the other (Fenichel,
In 1909, psychoanalytic notions about obsessional neurosis 1945). The concept has received considerable attention in
(later known as obsessive compulsive disorder) were delineated psychoanalytic literature since a very early date (Abraham,
with Freud’s publication of the remarkable Rat Man case. Fenichel 1924; Freud, 1909/1955; Fenichel, 1945; Nagera, 1978) and
in 1945 concisely explained psychopathology of Obsessive continues to be relevant in the treatment of OCD from
Compulsive Disorder (OCD) to be characterized by a conflict psychodynamically oriented therapeutic approaches (Chessick,
2001; Esman, 1989, 2001; Gabbard, 2001; Salzman, 1980; Shapiro,
1965, 2001). The OCD symptom has been regarded as the
manifestation of the struggle of the ego against the intolerable
* Corresponding author. Mailing address: Department of Psychology, UCSTA, 92, impulses and a ‘‘severe’’, ‘‘unkind’’ (Freud, 1926), ‘‘archaic’’
A.P.C. Road, Kolkata 700009, West Bengal, India. Tel.: +91 943 333 1666.
E-mail addresses: pritha_m2@yahoo.com, prithamukhopadhyay@gmail.com
(Weissman, 1954) or ‘‘hypermoral’’ superego (Fenichel, 1945);
(P. Mukhopadhyay). Kempke and Luyten (2007) have aptly pointed out that the ensuing

1876-2018/$ – see front matter ß 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2010.10.002
178 P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185

conscientiousness and perfectionism in the obsessive character are context of empirical neuropsychological findings. It endeavours to
perhaps attempts to control the warded-off aggressive and sexual investigate the impact of instinctual impulses of sex and
wishes. aggression on performance of higher cognitive tasks, i.e., on
Classical psychoanalytic views also hypothesize basic ambiva- executive functions.
lence in obsessive–compulsive neurosis to be caused by a
tyrannical superego coupled with high levels of aggression 2. Methods
(Kempke and Luyten, 2007). Referring to relevant literature (Blatt
and Shichman, 1983; Kernberg, 1984; McWilliams, 1994) in their 2.1. Sample and study design
critically reviewed article, Kempke and Luyten (2007) have also
mentioned that the severe superego is thought to result from The study comprised of two groups and was cross-sectional
identification with critical and demanding significant others, while with a case–control design. A clinical sample (n = 20, male = 11,
the high levels of aggression are considered to be partly inborn and female = 9, age mean = 30.7 years, S.D.  7.98 years right handed,
partly the manifestation of an excessive repression of anger. urban, with at least Standard VIII education) of patients with a
Esman (2001) in his review considers Freud’s dynamic concept diagnosis of Obsessive–Compulsive Disorder without co-morbidity as
of obsession (1909), which involves the regression from unre- per ICD 10 Criteria (F42) onset of illness at 17 years or later, was
solved oedipal conflicts to pre-genital anal-sadistic level, when selected from psychiatric services of R. G. Kar Medical College and
genital sexual organization had to be relinquished. He further Hospital, Kolkata, India. A diagnosis of OCD was made through a
pointed out to the Freud’s (1913) proposition of pregenital detailed clinical examination by trained clinical psychologists, using
organization of the libido to be determined primarily by semi-structured case record format used at the Department of
constitutional rather than experiential factors which has later been Psychology, University of Calcutta. The diagnosis was independently
resonated by Freud (1966) as determinant of ‘‘intensity of the anal- confirmed by the consultant psychiatrist at R. G. Kar Medical College
sadistic tendencies’’, and the ‘‘preference’’ for the characteristic and Hospital.
defense mechanisms, as well. The emphasis on constitutional Patients with age of onset earlier than 12 years were excluded
factors links OCD to one’s biological disposition. to minimize the heterogeneity of the OCD sample, as childhood
Extensive progress made in the fields of neurobiology, neuro- onset OCD is purported to be a neurodevelopmental subtype of
psychology and neuroimagery, has not only revolutionalized the OCD (Geller et al., 2001; Jaisoorya et al., 2003) with different
understanding of OCD but has also led to the neurobiological neurobiological mechanisms from those with late onset OCD (Roth
conceptualization of the same (Chamberlain and Menzies, 2009). As et al., 2005). Upper age-limit of the patients was restricted to 45
a matter of fact, Freud’s depiction of obsessive–compulsive neurosis years, to avoid age related changes that may interfere with
has been found to have parallels in recent psychobiological theories assessment of cognitive functions. Patients with history of other
of OCD (Katz, 1991; Brito, 2002). Katz hypothesized that serotoner- anxiety disorders, psychosis, bipolar disorder, or current psycho-
gic processes acted as filters or suppressors of aggressive or libidinal active substance dependence, were excluded. A total of 20 patients
impulses, and since OCD patients and violent suicide attempters are fulfilled the study criteria. Patients with all varieties of obsessions
often reported to have reduced levels of serotonin in the central and compulsion were included with the exception of hoarding
nervous system, he suggested that Freudian repression is dependent (comprising subgroups with aggressive/somatic obsessions with
on serotonergic mechanisms. Brito (2002), however, disagreed with checking compulsions = 7; contamination obsession with wash-
the claim that serotonergic system in isolation could represent the ing/cleaning compulsions = 8; sexual and religious obsessions = 5).
neurobiological basis of repression, hypothesizing the operation of Exclusion criteria included lifetime diagnosis of schizophrenia,
the circuitry involving the frontal cortex, hippocampus, and the bipolar affective disorder, tic disorder, and substance misuse.
brainstem rather than only the brainstem (where serotonergic Patients with diagnosis of OCD with comorbid conditions other
neurons are located) as originally envisaged by Katz. According to than depression were excluded. Severity of depression was
the model proposed by Brito, as primitive forms of behaviour are assessed using clinician rated Hamilton Depression Rating Scale
thought to be organized in the brainstem, the neurobiological basis (HDRS) (Hamilton, 1960) and patients whose scores were greater
of regression, he claims, could involve an imbalance in the activity of than 8 were excluded. Organic brain disorder, neurological
cortico-subcortical processes such that activity in subcortical disorders, or any sensory impairment was not included. Handed-
structures overwhelms activity in cortical structures. Owing to ness, which is extremely relevant in terms of neuropsychological
the emotional salience of regressive phenomena, it presumably assessment, was determined with the 10-item Edinburgh Hand-
involves amygdalo-hippocampal circuitry in addition to cortical and edness Inventory (Oldfield, 1971) and only right-handed individ-
brainstem processes (Brito, 2002). uals were included. The demographic and clinical characteristics of
These theoretical hypotheses may well be corroborated with the participants are shown in Table 1.
neurobiological findings from the new brain imaging studies, A community sample (n = 20, age mean = 30.8 years, S.D.  7.17
particularly PET-scanning. Taken together, neuroimaging studies years) that matched the OCD patients in terms of age, sex, educational
indicate that OCD symptoms are mediated by hyperactivity in level, handedness, religion and mother tongue was selected through
orbitofrontalsubcortical circuits, which may be attributable to an the snowballing technique and screened on the General Health
imbalance of tone between direct and indirect striato-pallidal Questionnaire (GHQ; Goldberg and Miller, 1979). Participants scoring
pathways (Saxena et al., 1998a,b, 2001). The caudate nucleus, above the cut off value of 4 were excluded. Exclusion criteria also
although less consistently implicated, has been found in several included evidence of organic brain disorder/neurological disorder,
studies to demonstrate pre-treatment abnormalities (Baxter, and sensory impairment. They also had no family history of major
1992; Whiteside et al., 2004). Most recent studies now implicate psychiatric illnesses (psychosis, affective disorder, OCD, suicide or
the prefrontal cortex (orbitofrontal and cingulate), the basal alcohol and substance misuse) in a first-degree relative confirmed by
ganglia and the thalamus to the pathogenesis of OCD (Pena-Garijo history obtained from the control subject.
et al., 2010). Rauch and Jenike (1993), however, state that it is not The participants responded to Yale-Brown Obsessive Compul-
clear whether the observed neurobiological deviations cause or sive Scale, Sexual Guilt Rating Scale, State Trait Anger Expression
develop as a consequence of the disorder. Inventory, Wisconsin Card Sorting Test along with Symbol Search
The present paper is an attempt to converge the psychodynamic and Coding subtests from Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III that
and neurobiological approaches of understanding OCD in the comprised the Processing Speed Index. After describing the
P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185 179

Table 1 feelings. Items consist of 4-point scales that assess intensity of


Socio demographic description of the sample.
anger at a particular moment and the frequency of anger
Category Clinical sample Community control experience, expression, and control.
(n = 20) (n = 20)  Sex-Guilt Rating Scale (SGRS) developed by Paul (2004), is a 20-
Age (yrs) item Likert type scale which was used to determine feelings of
Mean 30.7 30.8 guilt and inhibition related to sexuality, reflecting either a
S.D. 7.98 7.17 mature or immature conceptualization of sex.
n % n %
Sex
Male 11 55 11 55 2.3. Statistical analysis
Female 9 45 9 45
Education (yrs) The data were tested for normative distribution using the
8–12 8 40 8 40 Levene’s test. Since most of the neuropsychological variables were
13–15 8 40 8 40
16–18 4 20 4 20
not normatively distributed, non-parametric analysis was carried
Occupation out. Mann–Whitney U test was used to assess the differences
Unemployed 2 10 0 between patients and their control counterparts. Spearman’s
Student 5 25 7 35 correlation analysis was employed to examine the relationship
House wife 6 30 6 30
between the scores on neuropsychological tests and variables
Service 5 25 5 25
Self-employed 2 10 2 10 related to aggression and sexuality, separately for the clinical
Marital status sample and the normative sample. Stepwise regression analysis
Single 9 45 11 55 was carried out to demarcate potential explanatory variables and
Married 11 55 9 45 draw conclusions about the impact of instinctual impulses on
executive functions. In this form of regression, a variable is added
as long as its addition contributes a positive increase in the R-
purpose of the study to the subjects, informed consent was square value of the model; i.e., as long as it meets the critical value
obtained. All measures were individually administered and the of the test. The critical value required for significance was set at
order of administration was kept constant. 0.05 level. Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version
13.0 was used for analyses and all reported p values are two-tailed.
2.2. Measures
3. Results
 Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Y-BOCS), a 10-item
clinician-administered scale, is a widely used rating scale to 3.1. Socio-demographic description of the sample
measure symptom severity of OCD designed by Goodman et al.
(1989). It is used extensively in research and clinical practice to The two groups were comparable in terms of age, sex, and years
determine both severity of OCD and to monitor improvement of education. The symptom severity in the clinical group, as
during treatment. This scale, which measures obsessions assessed on YBOCS, ranged from 10 to 40, with the mean being 22
separately from compulsions, specifically measures the severity which falls within moderate range. Duration of illness ranged from
of symptoms of OCD without being biased towards the type of 2 to 10 years. The socio-demographic details of the two groups are
obsessions or compulsions present. It has two general sections – provided below in Table 1.
an obsession rating scale and a compulsion rating scale.
 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) was originally developed to 3.2. Group differences between OCD and normal controls
assess abstract reasoning ability and the ability to shift cognitive
strategies in response to changing environment contingencies The clinical group scored significantly higher on YBOCS (u = 001;
(Berg, 1948). The WCST can be considered a measure of ‘executive p < 0.01). The OCD group attained significantly lower percentiles on
function’, requiring the ability to develop and maintain an PSI (u = 17.00; p < 0.01); and all variables of the WCST under
appropriate problem-solving strategy across changing stimulus consideration, namely, perseverative response (PR) (u = 0.40;
conditions in order to achieve a future goal (Luria, 1973; Shallice, p < 0.01); perseverative error (PE) (u = 7.50; p < 0.01); non-persev-
1982). Similar to other measures of executive function, the WCST erative error (NPE) (u = 9.50; p < 0.01); conceptual level response
requires strategic planning, organized searching, utilizing envi- (CLR) (u = 9.00; p < 0.01) and number of categories completed
ronmental feedback, shifting cognitive sets, directing behaviour (NOCC) (u = 95.00; p < 0.01). They reported higher scores on
toward achieving a goal and modulating impulsive responding. subscales of STAXI, specifically related to trait anger (T-Anger and
However, unlike other measures of abstract reasoning, the WCST T/r Anger) (u = 88.00; p < 0.01 and u = 73.00; p < 0.01); and lower
provides objective scores not only of overall success, but also for scores on anger expression (u = 129.00; p < 0.05). On the items of
specific sources of difficulty on the task (e.g., inefficient initial SGRS, the study group significantly differed with the controls on
conceptualization, failure to maintain cognitive set, perseveration three statements: SGRS1 (‘‘Sitting beside a person of opposite sex
and inefficient learning across stages of the test.) while travelling by bus is problematic’’), SGRS14 (‘‘One should not
 Processing Speed Index (PSI) from Wechsler Adult Intelligence think about physical intimacy’’) and SGRS20 (‘‘Boys and girls
Scale III (WAIS III) (Wechsler, 1997). The PSI includes two tests – should maintain a safe distance from each other’’), expressing
(i) Symbol Search: which assesses visual perception and speed greater sexual inhibition. The findings are elaborated in Table 2.
and (ii) Coding: which assesses visuo-motor co-ordination, and
motor and mental speed. 3.3. Correlations between instinctual impulses and executive
 State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI) developed by functions in clinical sample
Spielberger (1991) is a 44-item inventory which measures the
intensity of anger as an emotional state (State Anger) and the In the clinical sample, significant positive correlation was seen
disposition to experience angry feelings as a personality trait between symptom severity and anger control (AX CON on STAXI)
(Trait Anger). The instrument consists of six scales measuring the and negatively scored items reflecting inhibited sexuality, indicating
intensity of anger and the disposition to experience angry that excessive control of aggression and narrow and stereotypical
180 P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185

Table 2
Comparison of scores of the two groups on YBOCS, WCST, PSI, STAXI and SGRS.

Domains assessed Clinical (n = 20) Community control (n = 20) U-Value p


*
p < 0.05
**
Mean SD Mean SD p < 0.01

YBOCS 21.75 9.89 0.4 0.68 .001 .001**


PR 14.9 23.95 113.55 26.02 0.40 .001**
PE 15.45 25.86 112.1 31.76 7.50 .001**
NPE 36.55 30.51 102.4 10.21 9.50 .001**
CLR 18.9 24.13 92.4 20.71 9.00 .001**
NOCC 3.35 2.25 5.45 0.76 95.00 .001**
PSI 70.95 19.21 98.2 11.51 17.00 .001**
S-Anger 13.6 4.88 12.1 2.94 172.50 .461
T-Anger 22.95 5.93 16.4 5.08 88.00 .002**
T/t Anger 8.7 3.54 6.75 2.49 132.50 .068
T/r Anger 10.8 3.16 6.95 2.68 73.00 .001**
AX/IN 19.15 2.85 19.85 2.56 170.00 .429
AX/OUT 18.9 3.21 21.35 3.86 133.50 .072
AXCON 19.5 2.5 19.7 2.39 189.50 .779
AX/EX 34.55 3.35 37.4 4.26 129.00 .053*
SGRS 1 2.35 0.93 3 0.65 120.00 .030*
SGRS 2 3.1 0.79 3.35 0.49 170.50 .429
SGRS 3 2.6 0.99 2.35 0.93 168.50 .398
SGRS 4 3 1.08 3.35 0.60 144.50 .134
SGRS 5 2.5 0.99 1.95 0.22 188.00 .758
SGRS 6 2.2 0.89 2.05 0.22 187.00 .738
SGRS 7 3.1 0.85 3 0.92 186.00 .718
SGRS 8 2.7 1.13 2.9 0.79 180.00 .602
SGRS 9 2.3 1.13 2.6 0.68 154.00 .221
SGRS 10 2 0.97 1.9 0.55 193.00 .862
SGRS 11 2.2 1.06 1.7 0.47 151.00 .192
SGRS 12 2.15 0.99 1.65 0.59 144.00 .134
SGRS 13 3.35 0.99 3.65 0.49 179.50 .583
SGRS 14 2.7 1.08 1.65 0.93 92.50 .002**
SGRS 15 1.45 0.51 1.65 0.49 160.00 .289
SGRS 16 3.5 0.69 3.6 0.50 192.00 .841
SGRS 17 2.1 0.85 2.1 0.68 193.00 .862
SGRS 18 1.8 0.77 1.85 0.99 195.00 .904
SGRS 19 1.6 0.75 1.4 0.5 176.00 .529
SGRS 20 2.8 0.95 1.85 0.67 88.50 .002**
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

conceptualization of sexuality may be directly related to severity of obtained in the control group with respect to perseverative
OCD. Trait anger domains (T-Anger and T/t Anger) and anger control responses, which were found to be directly correlated to
as assessed on STAXI were found to be negatively correlated to stereotypical conceptualization of sexuality. Reverse correlation
conceptual level responses and perseverative errors, signifying that was seen between items reflecting inhibited sexuality and NPE,
trait anger and anger control are inversely correlated to set shifting. indicating that narrow and stereotypical conceptualization of
Items pertaining to sexual guilt were found to be inversely sexuality may be inversely correlated to random trial and error
correlated to set-shifting and cognitive flexibility (reflected through response pattern during set-shifting. Similar to the clinical group,
NOCC and NPE). Perseverative responses, on the other hand were the control group also reflected inverse correlation between items
found to be directly correlated to stereotypical conceptualization of pertaining to sexual guilt and cognitive flexibility, reflected
sexuality. The findings are detailed in Table 3 as follows. through categories completed on the WCST. The findings are
detailed in Table 4 as follows.
3.4. Correlations between instinctual impulses and executive
functions in control sample 3.5. Impact of instinctual impulses on executive functions in the
sample
In the control group, unlike the clinical sample, there was no
correlation observed between the neuropsychological test scores Step-wise Multiple Regression Analysis was computed sepa-
and domains of aggression. However, similar findings were rately for both the clinical and control group. In the clinical group it

Table 3
Correlations between scores on YBOCS and WCST and STAXI and SGRS in the clinical sample.

Correlated variables T anger T/t anger Ax con SGRS 2 SGRS 8 SGRS 11 SGRS 13 SGRS 16 SGRS 18 SGRS 19

YBOCS .600* .498* .531** .667**


PR .471*
PE .490*
NPE .591**
CLR .521* .449* .516*
NOCC .528** .492* .705**
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185 181

Table 4
Correlations between scores on YBOCS and WCST and STAXI and SGRS in the control group.

Correlated variables SGRS 2 SGRS 3 SGRS 4 SGRS 7 SGRS 8 SGRS 13 SGRS 18 SGRS 19
*
PR .471
NPE .539* .501* .453* .534*
* * **
NOCC .528 .492 .705
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.

Table 5
Step-wise Multiple Regression Analyses between the independent variables (instinctual impulses) and the dependent variables (neuropsychological domains) in both the
groups.

Sample Dependent variable Predictors Adjusted R2 Std. error of estimate

OCD group PSI SGRS16 0.216 17.0011


SGRS16 + SGRS20 0.363 15.3319
CLR AXCON 0.233 21.1353

Control group PR SGRS18 0.233 22.7836


NPE SGRS3 0.259 8.7907
SGRS3 + SGRS5 0.467 7.4545
SGRS3 + SGRS5 + T-Anger R/R 0.604 6.4222
SGRS3 + SGRS5 + T-Anger R/R + SGRS12 0.786 4.7232
CLR SGRS4 0.219 18.3094
NOCC SGRS3 0.165 0.6938

was observed that processing speed was predicted to the extent of performance is a result of utilization of feedback to one’s response
36% by combining two items of the SGRS, and aggression control when the stimulus – response task is based on trial and error
predicted conceptual responses by 23%. The nature of the mechanism (Mc.Donald and White, 1994). Increased NPE in the
predictions was quite different in the control group. Perseverative OCD, therefore, could also be explained as the error in feedback
responses, conceptual responses and categories completed were utilization, resulting in poor response selection, and culminating in
predicted to the extent of 23%, 22% and 16% respectively by items poor conceptual level response and poor category completion. Poor
of SGRS. Sexual guilt revealing statements in combination with a conceptual level response in OCD further signifies that their
domain of trait anger predicted NPEs to the extent of 79%. The psychopathology is most likely to prevent them from developing
findings are tabulated in Table 5. an effective strategy to deal with the task, which seems to be the
function of the Dorso Lateral Pre Frontal Cortex (Goldstein et al.,
4. Discussion 2004; Haut et al., 1996; Stuss et al., 2000) in conjunction with other
cortical, subcortical and cerebellar regions (Mukhopadhyay et al.,
4.1. Group differences between OCD and normal controls in terms of 2008) of the brain.
set-shifting capacity
4.2. Group differences between OCD and normal controls in terms of
Impairment in set-shifting capacity in the OCD, as is evident processing speed
from the results of perseveration (PE and PR; Table 2) and number
of category completion (NOCC; Table 2) on Wisconsin Card Sorting Taking into consideration the PSI, their slowness observed in
Test (WCST), indicates their poor feedback utilization capacity, this study could be attributed to their quest for perfectionism
which is in conformity with previous research reports (Boone et al., (Rhéaume et al., 1995; Lee et al., 2009). It is evident as well from
1991; Christensen et al., 1992; Lucey et al., 1997; Tarafder et al., our observation of their test behaviour during digit symbol task,
2006; Olley et al., 2007; Moritz et al., 2009). Their failure to attain where they took a lot of care in copying the symbols, concentrating
category completion is an indication of their difficulty in concept on accuracy, and compromising upon the speed aspect in order to
attainment, hypothesis testing and cognitive flexibility. Im- complete the task within the specified period of time. Impairment
pairment of set-shifting capacity in OCD is an index of their in processing speed in OCD is explicable with the psychopathology
difficulty in making decisions regarding response selection of obsessive slowness and may be attributed to their characteristic
(Aycicegi et al., 2003) and shifting of attention (Okasha et al., ambivalence (Fenichel, 1945). The ambivalence may be attribut-
2000; Veale et al., 1996), which may be considered to be an able to shallow processing of information regarding the given
increased function of underlying neural substrate of caudate stimulus, which causes a continual conflict in decision making
nucleus in consonance with anterior cingulate and orbito frontal (here, symbol to be selected as right or wrong) due to inability to
cortices (OFC) (Arciniegas and Beresford, 2001). Dysregulation in keep action plan in memory (Kuelz et al., 2004) which gets
OFC in OCD has been reported by Saxena et al. (1998b), Friedlander reflected in delayed response selection.
and Desrocher (2006). The accuracy aspect of the task dubbed as maladaptive
In contrast to normal controls, in whom strategy based on trial perfectionism (Moretz and McKay, 2009) was so prioritized by
and error method on WCST is a search for the way to shift between the OCD that their scanning was restricted to the task-demand of
sets utilizing the provided feedback, greater NPE in the OCD accuracy alone without consideration of the time factor. Their
indicates their restricted response selection which perhaps blocks overemphasis of the accuracy aspect perhaps symbolically
their way to select appropriate response to attain a new category. strengthens their schemata of compliance by giving extra effort
Inference may be drawn on the basis of the evidence that execution and time towards accuracy, which perhaps delays the fronto-
of a novel response as well as improvement of ongoing striatal function for motor execution of the task.
182 P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185

It may be inferred that non-conformity to rule would reflected in perseverative responses which makes the striatum
supposedly induce guilt in OCD, the deficit in the OCD to respond rigidly follow a specific modular pattern of neuronal activation
to feedback from the examiner regarding enhancement of speed based upon the information coming from within (Saka and
could be so guilt arousing that they might defensively deny the Graybiel, 2003).
time aspect of the instruction, which not only serves as an avenue
to release their aggression passively, but also reduces apparent 4.4. Group differences between OCD and normal controls in terms of
feelings of guilt in them. sexual instincts
This may be understood considering their setting of an internal
rule to which they conform, thereby, disregarding the external rule Regarding sexual impulse, their significantly higher scores on
is not appraised as non-conformity. Such rigidity and a strong need few items of the SGRS indicates the over-sensitivity of the OCD to
for control and autonomy result in anal character traits in them the items that contains even a mild indication of man–woman
(Blatt and Shichman, 1983; Shapiro, 1965). Since they prioritize the relationship which is within the range of social acceptability. This
bottom-up processing through assimilation, they accept it only can well be understood from the fact that forbidden thoughts
when that set of data is congruent with their existing cognitive regarding aggression and sexuality emerged as one of the four
structure or prevailing set. This, in turn, strengthens their set to act significant factors in a meta-analysis of the symptom structure of
upon conceptually driven but rigid top-down mechanism, ignoring OCD including 21 studies which involved 5124 participants (Bloch
the environmental demand or feedback, which ultimately becomes et al., 2008). Our findings are also in agreement with research
ego-syntonic in nature. Presumably, maintenance of their slow- reports of sexual obsessions being fairly common among
ness without any guilt in their life situation subsequently makes it individuals with OCD (Grant et al., 2006).
their character trait. The finding of slowness may be corroborated
with recent reports of bradykinesia to be correlated with mental 4.5. Association between symptom severity, instinctual impulses and
slowness and nonverbal performance impairment in OCD support- executive functions
ing the possibility of dysfunction of basal ganglia in OCD patients
(Pasquini et al., 2009). Symptom severity (as assessed on YBOCS) being significantly
The slowness also can be explained by their inability to change correlated with aggression control (AX Con on STAXI) and three
the focus of attention (Martı́nez-González and Piqueras-Rodrı́guez, statements related to sexuality (SGRS 16, SGRS 18, SGRS 19) (Table
2008), making it likely that they prioritize serial processing, 3), reiterates the high degree of association between maladaptive
evident from focusing their attention onto one aspect of the task control over both sexual and aggressive impulses exerted by the
(accuracy), with their failure to handle both the aspects of the task OCD group. The findings go hand in hand with previous reports of
(i.e., speed and accuracy) simultaneously, which again may positive correlations between contamination fear from sexual
underlie their cognitive inflexibility. activity and the severity of OCD symptoms (Abbey et al., 2007).
Association of the instinctual impulses with the perseverative
4.3. Group differences between OCD and normal controls in terms of response in the OCD group might be attributed to the ability of
aggressive instincts forbidden sexual and hostile thoughts to occupy Memory space,
with minimum chances of the same to get expressed or
With respect to the aggressive instincts, OCD shows higher acknowledged by the patient due to its capacity to arouse guilt.
predisposition of trait anger (T-Anger) with significantly higher They do not acknowledge their T-Anger because of a desire to
sensitivity for aggression and impulsivity (T/r Anger) and lower portray to others that they are docile by nature and do not lose
anger expression (AX/EX). Several studies have noted that control over anger. This is consistent with clinical reports that
individuals with both clinical and subclinical levels of OCD report individuals diagnosed with OCD are often preoccupied with how
elevated levels of anger compared with individuals without an others perceive them (Newth and Rachman, 2001).
anxiety disorder (Rubenstein et al., 1995; Spinella, 2005; White-
side and Abramowitz, 2005) which is consistent with our findings. 4.6. Impact of instinctual impulses on executive functions
The results may also be corroborated with research reports by
Radomsky et al. (2007) who found greater T-Anger but lower anger Impact of items pertaining to sexuality on processing speed in
expression, among OCD checkers than the student control group. the OCD group (Table 5) may be associated with Brito’s (2002)
The anger expression score in the OCD group under consideration assumption that the activity of cerebral structures associated with
in our study, being significantly lower than the controls coupled cortex and striatum overrides the activity of cerebral structures
with their equal status with normal on anger control (on STAXI), associated with limbic-subcortex. This regression of sexual
suggests their excessive exertion of anti-cathexis force to prevent impulses on the variance of conceptual responses of the OCD
the expression of anger. The finding gets strength from earlier substantiates the potency of the dysfunctional preoccupation to
observations that OCD patients have strongly repressed aggressive interfere with the executive function by occupying the working
impulses, while normal individuals develop less maladaptive ways memory-space and preventing the same from acting upon the
of dealing with these impulses (Kempke and Luyten, 2007). relevant task.
Due to fear of being aggressed upon by the authority figure, the
perception of environment as punitive (Fenichel, 1945) results in a 4.6.1. A neuropsychological and psychoanalytic interface
predisposition for narrowed response selection. It makes The preoccupation of the patients of OCD with unresolved ego-
responses, which would not generate any punitive consequences dystonic intrusive instinctual thoughts explains the dysexecution
that may be considered to be detrimental to one’s survival, highly in organizing and processing new thoughts giving rise to
predictable. Perhaps this apprehension causes one not to produce perseveration, as one’s attentional system is occupied by the
any novel, experimental or divergent responses which could aforementioned potent thoughts which necessitates the continu-
possibly lead to negative consequences, resulting in repetitive ous exertion of anticathexis force to ward off the thoughts from
cognitive or motor behaviour owing to deficit in learning of gating consciousness. This observation is in conformity with the previous
and recombining divergent information of intermediary level at an research report (Mukhopadhyay et al., 2006) that claims that
output layer (Graybiel and Saka, 2004; Parthsarathy and Graybiel, obsessive thoughts predict one’s deficit in the set shifting aspect of
1997). The pathology is restricted to inflexible thought or action- executive function. Psychopathology formulation of OCD from the
[()TD$FIG] P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185 183

Forbidden Thoughts of Inability to Modulate


Sexuality Hostile Thoughts

Unresolved Ego-Dystonic
Intrusive Instinctual
Thoughts

Continuous exertion of Anticathexis


Force to ward off the thoughts from
Consciousness.

Preoccupation of Attentional System


with Potent Instinctual Thoughts

Inadequate Resources available in


Working Memory Space

Rigid Response Selection

Dysexecution in Organising and


Processing new thoughts

Perseveration Slowness

Fig. 1.

dynamic perspective focuses upon the inability of the obsessional cognitive behaviours to meet the adaptive demand of the
subject to express their own will against superior hostile forces environment (Graybiel et al., 1994). Maintaining relevance to this
directly, making one learn to express hostility through stubborn- formulation, the narrow response selection in the OCD, owing to
ness (Fenichel, 1945). Although it apparently helps them to vent early experience of being dominated upon by powerful others,
out anger, this display of passive aggression perhaps does not help possibly acts through the striatum (Saka and Graybiel, 2003) in
to absolve the feeling of guilt in them. Consequently, this activating rest of the basal ganglia to participate strongly in the
resistance against superior forces, perhaps being guilt-arousing control of motor and cognitive behaviours, in accordance with the
in the unconscious, later results in a continual struggle with their adaptive demand of the prevailing environment of the OCD.
own superego (Fenichel, 1945). Since the conflict is significant, the The findings may be summarized schematically in Fig. 1 below
attentional resources, out of their own accord focus onto it, which
perhaps causes reverberation of the same in the available working
memory space. 5. Conclusion
Thus OCD, being the disorder of response selection, perhaps is
associated with increased activity in the caudate nucleus, cingulate Divergent information perhaps overwhelms the OCD subject
and orbitofrontal cortices (Graybiel and Rauch, 2000; Graybiel and with a plethora of response choices, which results in rigid
Saka, 2004), culminating in an imbalance between the direct cognition and motor response rather than them selecting a
striatopallidal pathway responsible for selection of a given response from number of available alternative options. It makes
response and concurrent suppression of unwanted response via them feel that they can exercise cognitive control only in this
indirect pathways (Arciniegas and Beresford, 2001), behaviourally manner, and this for them, is an index of ensuring their safety.
being manifested as ‘slowness’ in their pathology. Literature on empirical validation of theoretical psychoanalytic
Considering the psychopatholgy of OCD, it may be stated that constructs is limited. Our findings contribute to understanding
behaviour selection can be influenced by one’s own experience, instinctual impulses from the neuropsychological perspective. The
which being mediated by the striatum, activates the rest of the findings have implications for better eclectic understanding of the
basal ganglia to participate strongly in the control of motor and pathogenesis of obsessive–compulsive disorder.
184 P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185

Limitations Graybiel, A.M., Saka, E., 2004. The Basal Ganglia and the Control of Action, in the
Cognitive Neurosciences III Gazzaniga, M.S. (editor in chief). A Bradford Book.
The MIT Press, London, England.
A heterogeneous clinical sample (comprising OCD subgroups Graybiel, A.M., Asosaki, T., Flaherty, A.W., Kimura, M., 1994. The basal ganglia and
with aggressive and somatic obsessions with checking compul- adaptive motor control. Science 265, 1826–1831.
Hamilton, M., 1960. A rating scale for depression. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry
sions; contamination obsession with washing/cleaning compul- 23, 56–62.
sions; sexual and religious obsessions) and small sample size were Haut, M.W., Cahill, J., Cutlip, W.D., Stevenson, J.M., Makela, E.H., Bloomfield, S.M.,
some of the study’s major limitations. However the understanding 1996. On the nature of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizo-
phrenia. Psychiatry Res. 65, 15–22.
may be extended to a larger study with a homogenous sample. Jaisoorya, T.S., Janardhan Reddy, Y.C., Srinath, S., 2003. Is juvenile obsessive–
compulsive disorder a developmental subtype of the disorder? Findings from
an Indian study. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 12, 290–297.
Conflict of interest Katz, R.J., 1991. Neurobiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder – a serotonergic
basis of Freudian repression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 15, 375–381.
None. Kempke, S., Luyten, P., 2007. Psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral approaches
of obsessive–compulsive disorder: is it time to work through our ambivalence?
Bull. Menninger Clin. 71 (4), 291–311.
References Kernberg, O., 1984. Object-relations Theory and Clinical Psychoanalysis. Jason
Aronson, New York.
Kuelz, A.K., Hohagen, F., Voderholzer, U., 2004. Neuropsychological performance in
Abbey, R.D., Clopton, J.R., Humphreys, J.D., 2007. Obsessive–compulsive disorder obsessive–compulsive disorder: a critical review. Biol. Psychol. 65, 185–236.
and romantic functioning. J. Clin. Psychol. 63 (12), 1181–1192. Lee, J.C., Prado, H.S., Diniz, J.B., Borcato, S., da Silva, C.B., Hounie, A.G., Miguel, E.C.,
Abraham, K., 1924. A short study of the development of the libido in the light of Leckman, J.F., do Rosário, M.C., 2009. Perfectionism and sensory phenomena:
mental disorders. I. Melancholia and obsessional neurosis.In: Selected Papers. phenotypic components of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Compr. Psychiatry
Basic Books, New York, pp. 422–433, 1953. 431–436.
Arciniegas, D.B., Beresford, T.P., 2001. Neuropsychiatry: An Introductory Approach. Lucey, J.V., Burness, C.E., Costa, D.C., Gacinovic, S., Pilowsky, L.S., Ell, P.J., Marks, I.M.,
Cambridge University Press. Kerwin, R.W., 1997. Wisconsin card sorting task (WCST) errors and cerebral
Aycicegi, A., Dinn, W.M., Harris, C.L., Erkmen, H., 2003. Neuropsychological function blood flow in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Br. J. Med. Psychol. 70, 403–411.
in obsessive–compulsive disorder: effects of comorbid conditions on task Luria, A.R., 1973. The working brain. In: Heaton, R.K., Chelune, G.J., Talley, J.L., Kay,
performance. Eur. Psychiatry 18 (5), 241–248. G.G., Curtiss, G. (Eds.), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test manual. Basic Books, New
Baxter, L., 1992. Neuroimaging studies of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatr. York (Revised and expanded. New York: Psychological Assessment Resources,
Clin. N. Am. 15, 87–884. Inc., 1993).
Berg, E.A., 1948. A simple objective test for measuring flexibility in thinking. J. Gen. Martı́nez-González, A.E., Piqueras-Rodrı́guez, J.A., 2008. Neuropsychological update
Psychiatry 39, 15–22. on obsessive–compulsive disorder. Rev. Neurol. 46 (10), 618–625.
Blatt, S.J., Shichman, S., 1983. Two primary configurations of psychopathology. Mc.Donald, R.J., White, N.M., 1994. A triple dissociation of memory systems:
Psychoanal. Contemp. Thought 6, 187–254. hippocampus, amygdale and dorsal striatum. Behav. Neurosci. 107, 3–22.
Bloch, M.H., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., Rosario, M.C., Pittenger, C., Leckman, J.F., McWilliams, N., 1994. Psychoanalytic Diagnosis: Understanding Personality Struc-
2008. Meta-analysis of the symptom structure of obsessive–compulsive disor- ture in the Clinical Process. The Guilford Press, New York.
der. Am. J. Psychiatry 165 (12), 1532–1542. Moretz, M.W., McKay, D., 2009. The role of perfectionism in obsessive–compulsive
Boone, K.B., Ananth, J., Philpott, L., Kaur, A., Djenderedjian, A., 1991. Neuropsycho- symptoms: ‘‘not just right’’ experiences and checking compulsions. J. Anxiety
logical characteristics of non depressed adults with obsessive compulsive Disord. 23 (5), 640–644 (Epub 2009 Feb 10).
disorder. Neuropsychiatry, Neuropsychol. Behav. Neurol. 4, 96–109. Moritz, S., Hottenrott, B., Randjbar, S., Klinge, R., Von Eckstaedt, F.V., Lincoln, T.M.,
Brito, G.N.O., 2002. Mind from genes and neurons: a neurobiological model of Jelinek, L., 2009. Perseveration and not strategic deficits underlie delayed
Freudian psychology. Med. Hypotheses 59 (4), 438–445. alternation impairment in obsessive–compulsive disorder (OCD). Psychiatry
Chamberlain, S.R., Menzies, L., 2009. Endophenotypes of obsessive–compulsive Res. 170, 66–69.
disorder: rationale, evidence and future potential. Expert Rev. Neurother. 9 Mukhopadhyay, P., Das, S., Nandi, S., Bandopadhyay, G., 2006. Executive ability at
(8), 1133–1146. different levels of severity of obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Personality Clin.
Chessick, R.D., 2001. OCD, OCPD: acronyms do not make a disease. Psychoanal. Studies 22 (1&2), 7–11.
Inquiry 21, 183–207. Mukhopadhyay, P., Dutt, A., Das, S.K., Basu, A., Hazra, A., Dhibar, T., Roy, T., 2008.
Christensen, K., Kim, S.W., Dyksen, M.W., Hoover, K.M., 1992. Neuropsychological Identification of neuroanatomical substrates of set-shifting ability: evidence from
performance in obsessive compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 31, 4–18. patients with focal brain lesions. In: Progress in Brain Research Volume 168 Models
Esman, A., 1989. Psychoanalysis and general psychiatry: OCD as paradigm. J. Am. of Brain and Mind: Physical, Computational and Psychological Approaches Rahul
Psychoanal. Assoc. 37, 319–336. Banerjee and Bikas Chakrabarti (Editors). Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 95–104.
Esman, A.H., 2001. Obsessive–compulsie disorder: current views. Psychoanal. Inq. Nagera, H., 1978. Obsessional Neurosis: Developmental Psychopathology. Aronson,
21, 145–156. New York.
Fenichel, O., 1945. The Psychoanalytic Theory of Neurosis. 3 Volumes. W. W. Norton Newth, S., Rachman, S., 2001. The concealment of obsessions. Behav. Res. Ther. 39,
and Co., New York. 457–464.
Freud, A., 1966. Obsessional neurosis: a summary of psychoanalytic views as Okasha, A., Rafaat, M., Mahallawy, N., Nahas, G., Dawla, S.A., Sayed, M., Kholi, S.,
presented at the congress. Int. J. Psychoanal. 47, 116–122. 2000. Cognitive dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Acta Psychiatr.
Freud, S., 1909. Notes Upon a Case of Obsessional Neurosis. Hogarth Press, London, Scand. 101, 281–285.
Standard Edition, 10, 155–318, 1955. Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh
Freud, S., 1913. The Disposition to Obsessional Neurosis. Hogarth Press, London, inventory. Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.
Standard Edition, 12, 317–326, 1958. Olley, Malhi, Sachdev, 2007. Memory and executive functioning in obsessive
Freud, S., 1926. Inhibitions, Symptoms and Anxiety. Hogarth Press, London, Stan- compulsive disorder: a selective review. J. Affect. Disord. 104, 15–23.
dard Edition, 20, 87–175, 1959. Parthsarathy, H.B., Graybiel, A.M., 1997. Cortically driven immediate early-gene
Friedlander, L., Desrocher, M., 2006. Neuroimaging studies of obsessive–compulsive expression reflects modular influence of sensory motor cortex on identified
disorder in adults and children. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 26, 32–49. striatal neurons in the squirrel monkey (1997). J. Neorosci. 7, 2477–2491.
Gabbard, G.O., 2001. Psychoanalytically informed approaches to the treatment of Pasquini, M., Fabbrini, G., Moretti, G., Berardelli, I., Mandarelli, G., Delle Chiaie, R.,
obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychoanal. Inq. 21, 208–221. Leone, C., Biondi, M., Berardelli, A., 2009. Bradykinesia and mental slowness in
Geller, D.A., Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Agranat, A., Cradock, K., Hagermoser, L., Kim, patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Eur. Psychiatry 24 (Suppl. 1),
G., Frazier, J., Coffey, B.J., 2001. Developmental aspects of obsessive compulsive S1035.
disorder: findings in children, adolescents, and adults. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 189, Paul, D., 2004. The impact of instinctual impulses on executive function in Obses-
471–477. sive Compulsive Disorder. Unpublished Dissertation of Master’s in Medical
Goldberg, D.P., Miller, V.E., 1979. General Health Questionnaire: a scaled version of Psychology. Department of Psychology, University of Calcutta.
general health questionnaire. Psychol. Med. 9, 139–146. Pena-Garijo, J., Ruipérez-Rodrı́guez, M.A., Barros-Loscertales, A., 2010. THE neuro-
Goldstein, B., Obrzut, J.E., John, C., Ledakis, G., Armstrong, C.L., 2004. The impact of biology of obsessive–compulsive disorder: new findings from functional mag-
frontal and nonfrontal brain tumor on Wisconsin Card Sorting Test perform- netic resonance imaging (I). Rev. Neurol. 50 (8), 477–485.
ance. Brain Cogn. 54, 110–116. Radomsky, A.S., Ashbaugh, A.R., Gelfand, L.A., 2007. Relationships between anger,
Goodman, W.K., Price, L.H., Rasmussen, S.A., 1989. Yale-brown obsessive compul- symptoms, and cognitive factors in OCD checkers. Behav. Res. Ther. 45, 2712–
sive scale-I: development, use and reliability. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 46, 1006– 2725.
1010. Rauch, Jenike, M., 1993. Neurobiological models of obsessive–compulsive disorder.
Grant, J.E., Pinto, A., Gunnip, M., Mancebo, M.C., Eisen, J.L., Rasmussen, S.A., 2006. Psychosomatics 34, 20–32.
Sexual obsessions and clinical correlates in adults with obsessive–compulsive Rhéaume, J., Freeston, M.H., Dugas, M.J., Letarte, H., Ladouceur R, 1995. Perfection-
disorder. Compr. Psychiatry 47 (5), 325–329. ism, responsibility and obsessive–compulsive symptoms. Behav. Res. Ther. 33
Graybiel, A.M., Rauch, S.L., 2000. Towards a neurobiology of OCD. Neuron 28, 343– (7), 785–794.
347.
P. Mukhopadhyay et al. / Asian Journal of Psychiatry 3 (2010) 177–185 185

Roth, R.M., Milovan, D., Baribeau, J., O’Connor, K., 2005. Neuropsychological func- Spinella, M., 2005. Mood in relation to subclinical obsessive–compulsive symptoms.
tioning in early and late-onset obsessive–compulsive disorder. J. Neuropsychi- Int. J. Neurosci. 115, 433–443.
atry Clin. Neurosci. 17, 208–213. Stuss, D.T., Levine, B., Alexander, M.P., Hong, J., Palumbo, C., Hamer, L., Murphy, K.J.,
Rubenstein, C.S., Altemus, M., Pigott, T.A., Hess, A., Murphy, D.L., 1995. Symptom Izukawa, D., 2000. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in patients with
overlap between OCD and bulimia nervosa. J. Anxiety Disord. 9, 1–9. focal frontal and posterior brain damage: effects of lesion location and test
Saka, E., Graybiel, A.M., 2003. Pathophysiology of Tourette’s syndrome: striatal structure on separable cognitive processes. Neuropsychologia 38, 388–402.
pathways revisited. Brain Dev. 25 (Supply), S/55–S/519. Tarafder, S., Bhattacharya, P., Paul, D., Bandyopadhyay, G., Mukhopadhyay, P., 2006.
Salzman, L., 1980. Treatment of the Obsessive Personality. Jason Aronson, New York. Neuropsychological disposition and its impact on the executive functions and
Saxena, S., Brody, A.L., Schwartz, J.M., 1998a. Baxter LR Neuroimaging and frontal- cognitive style in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. Indian J. Psy-
subcortical circuitry in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. chiatry 48 (2), 102–106.
(35), 26–37. Veale, D.M., Sahakian, B.J., Owen, A.M., Marks, I.M., 1996. Specific cognitive deficits
Saxena, S., Brody, A.L., Schwartz, J.M., Baxter, L.R., 1998b. Neuroimaging and in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder.
frontalsubcortical circuitry in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Br. J. Psychiatry Psychol. Med. 26, 1261–1269.
Suppl. 35, 26–37. Wechsler, D., 1997. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 3rd ed. The Psychological
Saxena, S., Bota, R.G., Brody, A.L., 2001. Brain-behavior relationships in obsessive– Corporation, Harcourt Assessment Company, UK.
compulsive disorder. Semin. Clin. Neuropsychiatry 6 (2), 82–101. Weissman, P., 1954. Ego and superego in obsessional character neurosis. Psycho-
Shallice, T., 1982. Specific impairments in planning. In: Broadbent, D.E., Weiskar- anal. Quart. 23, 529–543.
antz, L. (Eds.), The Neuropsychology of Cognitive Function. The Royal Society, Whiteside, S.P., Port, J.D., Abramowitz, J.S., 2004. A meta-analysis of functional
London, pp. 199–209. neuroimaging in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 132 (1), 69–
Shapiro, D., 1965. Neurotic Styles. Basic Books, New York. 79.
Shapiro, D., 2001. OCD or obsessive–compulsive character? Psychoanal. Inq. 21, Whiteside, S.P., Abramowitz, J.S., 2005. The expression of anger and its relationship
242–252. to symptoms and cognitions in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Depress. Anxi-
Spielberger, C.D., 1991. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI). Revised ety 21, 106–111.
Research Edition Psychological Assessment and Resources, Inc., Florida.

You might also like