You are on page 1of 10

See

discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/275663298

Body satisfaction, thin-ideal internalization,


and perceived pressure to be thin among
Canadian women: The role of...

Article in Body image · April 2015


DOI: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.003 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

5 316

3 authors, including:

Zina Chaker Felicia Chang


University of Windsor University of Windsor
1 PUBLICATION 5 CITATIONS 6 PUBLICATIONS 16 CITATIONS

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Eating Behaviors and Emotion Regultation View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Zina Chaker on 19 January 2016.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Body Image
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Body satisfaction, thin-ideal internalization, and perceived pressure


to be thin among Canadian women: The role of acculturation and
religiosity
Zina Chaker ∗ , Felicia M. Chang, Julie Hakim-Larson
Department of Psychology, University of Windsor, 401 Sunset Avenue, Windsor, Ontario, Canada N9B 3P4

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: In an online study, 143 Canadian women of various religious backgrounds completed measures of accul-
Received 5 July 2014 turation, religiosity, body satisfaction, internalization of the thin ideal, perceived pressure from media,
Received in revised form 27 March 2015 and manner of dress. Heritage acculturation correlated with appearance satisfaction, but not weight
Accepted 4 April 2015
satisfaction. After accounting for BMI and social desirability, higher heritage acculturation and lower
mainstream acculturation were associated with lower perceived pressure from media. Thus, heritage
Keywords:
acculturation across religious denominations may serve as a buffer against appearance dissatisfaction
Appearance satisfaction
and perceived media pressure. Manner of dress among the Muslim subgroup and its relation to reli-
Thin-ideal internalization
Acculturation
giosity and acculturation were also assessed. Muslim women who dressed in greater accordance with
Religiosity Islamic principles reported lower heritage acculturation and greater religiosity. Thus, Muslim women’s
Muslim women manner of dress was related to their religiosity and the extent to which they embraced cultural values.
Hijab These findings are discussed in terms of the possible distinctions between heritage acculturation and
religiosity.
© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction Homan & Boyatzis, 2010). More specifically, church attendance and
religiosity are positively associated with body satisfaction among
Body dissatisfaction and internalization of the thin ideal pre- adults (Homan & Boyatzis, 2009) and negatively associated with
dict future depressive symptoms (Stice & Bearman, 2001) and the body dissatisfaction among female adolescents and adults with
development of eating disorders (e.g., Stice & Shaw, 2002) among bulimia (Smith, Richards, & Maglio, 2004). People who have more
female adolescents and young women. Understanding how vari- insecure attachments with God are more likely to report thin-ideal
ous factors may contribute to body image and internalization of internalization (Homan & Boyatzis, 2010). Experimental studies
the thin ideal is therefore critical in understanding eating disorders also suggest that the presence of religion may reduce one’s sus-
and depressive symptomatology. Recently, studies have attempted ceptibility to body dissatisfaction. For example, when women read
to assess how religious identity, religiosity, and culture relate to religious statements after being presented with an image of a thin
body image and internalization of the thin ideal. One’s religious model, they reported higher levels of body esteem (Boyatzis, Kline,
identity refers to the system of belief an individual identifies with, & Backof, 2007) and lower levels of body dissatisfaction (Bell, 2011)
while one’s religiosity refers to the level of devotion an individual compared to individuals who did not read religious statements. It is
has to his/her religion (Mattis & Jagers, 2001). Culture is defined of note that the aforementioned literature is based on samples that
as “systems of knowledge, concepts, rules, and practices that are are largely composed of individuals of Christian denominations.
learned and transmitted across generations” (American Psychiatric In terms of culture, a meta-analysis revealed that across
Association, 2013, p. 749). 15 studies, individuals of ethnic and/or racial minority groups
Existing research suggests that religiosity is a protective fac- reported lower levels of body dissatisfaction than did White
tor for body dissatisfaction and thin-ideal internalization (e.g., individuals (Wildes, Emery, & Simons, 2001). However, variations
in adherence to cultural values and practices among individuals
within ethnic and racial groups exist (Sue & Sue, 2008). Therefore,
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 519 562 1162. individuals’ levels of acculturation should be assessed rather
E-mail addresses: chakerz@uwindsor.ca (Z. Chaker), chang19@uwindsor.ca than conducting group comparisons, as was done in many of the
(F.M. Chang), hakim@uwindsor.ca (J. Hakim-Larson). studies included in the meta-analysis. Acculturation refers to the

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2015.04.003
1740-1445/© 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
86 Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93

process of psychological, sociocultural, political, and economic Moradi, 2011). Although this result may be confounded by reli-
changes in attitude and identity that occur when individuals gious identity, a study among solely Muslim women also found
from different cultures interact (Berry, 1997). Berry posited that that women who wore the hijab had significantly greater body sat-
immigrants’ acculturation strategies can be conceptualized using isfaction, lower internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived less
two orthogonal dimensions: maintenance of the heritage culture pressure to adhere to media’s beauty standards than women who
and adoption of the mainstream/host culture. Thus, in Canadian did not wear the hijab (Swami et al., 2013). Mixed findings also exist
immigrant populations, heritage acculturation refers to embracing among studies that do not assess acculturation in terms of the hijab.
the values and societal norms of the country from which the For example, Mussap (2009a) found that mainstream identifica-
individual immigrated, whereas mainstream acculturation refers to tion positively correlated with body dissatisfaction among Muslim
embracing the values and societal norms of the country to which women, whereas Abdollahi and Mann (2001) found no differences
the individual immigrated (i.e., Canadian values and practices). between Iranian women living in Tehran and Iranian women living
Mainstream acculturation is especially important to consider if in Los Angeles in terms of their ideal weight and shape concerns.
the host culture emphasizes a particular physical appearance as The aforementioned studies exemplify a tendency to indirectly
ideal, as do Western countries such as Canada. For example, eating assess religiosity and/or acculturation among Muslim women by
disorders have previously been described as Western culture- measuring the use of the hijab and/or the conservativeness of it (i.e.,
bound syndromes (Lai, 2000). Soh, Touyz, and Surgenor (2006) how much the headscarf covers), as opposed to focusing on other
have suggested that “ethnic minority groups in [Western] countries underlying factors. Fittingly, Swami et al. (2013, p. 9) have noted
have [a] lower risk of eating pathology [which] is possibly due to a that, “while use of the hijab itself may offer protection against nega-
lower level of acculturation to the mainstream society” (p. 56). In tive body image, it may also be a proxy for some other, unmeasured
support of these propositions, Ball and Kenardy (2002) found that protective factor.” Consistent with this proposition, the contribu-
after accounting for body mass index (BMI) and socioeconomic sta- tion of religiosity to body dissatisfaction among Muslim women
tus, mainstream acculturation, as measured by the number of years was mediated by the use of modest clothing (Mussap, 2009b). That
women had lived in Australia, significantly predicted body weight is, Muslim women who are more religious tend to dress more
dissatisfaction among a sample of over 10,000 women. Similarly, modestly which results in lower levels of body dissatisfaction, sug-
mainstream acculturation positively correlated with body dissat- gesting that clothing preferences may reflect Muslim women’s level
isfaction among Mexican American women (Poloskov & Tracey, of religiosity. As such, further research is necessary to determine if
2013). Women’s levels of acculturation also may be connected to these proxies are truly reflective of higher levels of religiosity. Over-
the extent to which they internalize and/or feel pressure to attain all, despite the use of proxies for religiosity in past studies, research
the thin ideal. For example, among ethnic minorities, women who on Muslim women appears to yield results that are consistent with
endorse mainstream acculturation reported higher levels of inter- findings among Christian samples. Thus, religiosity as opposed to
nalization of the thin ideal (Garcia-Rea, 2006). Indeed, Poloskov religious identity may play a key role in predicting body image and
and Tracey (2013) found a significant positive association between internalization of the thin ideal.
mainstream acculturation and thin-ideal internalization among
Mexican American women. These findings are not surprising when The Present Study
considering the proposition that internalization of the thin ideal
actually “represents a domain-specific manifestation of accultura- Generally, studies assessing acculturation suggest that adher-
tion to dominant culture” (Tolaymat & Moradi, 2011, p. 384). ence to either mainstream or heritage culture may have an
At present, there is limited literature addressing the role of reli- important relation to body satisfaction and internalization of the
giosity and acculturation on body satisfaction and internalization thin ideal. Much of the literature on acculturation conceptual-
of the thin ideal among Muslim women (e.g., Homan & Boyatzis, izes acculturation along a single continuum from being entirely
2009; Sussman, Truong, & Lim, 2007). It is important to include this accepting of the dominant culture to completely rejecting it (e.g.,
understudied population in research given that Islam is the fastest Garcia-Rea, 2006). Given that Berry (1997) conceptualized accul-
growing religion in Canada (Statistics Canada, 2011). Moreover, turation along two orthogonal dimensions, the extent to which one
the findings based on previous studies including Muslim partici- embraces his/her heritage should be assessed in addition to adher-
pants are mixed. For example, among Muslim women in Britain, the ence to the dominant culture. As well, there is a need for more
frequency and conservativeness with which women wore the reli- accurate measurement of acculturation as some studies use prox-
giously prescribed headscarf, referred to as the hijab, was positively ies (e.g., number of years living in a particular country) rather than
associated with body satisfaction, and negatively associated with validated measures of this construct. Thus, in the current study,
internalization of the thin ideal and perceived pressure to adhere to religiosity as well as both heritage and mainstream acculturation
media beauty standards (Swami, Miah, Noorani, & Taylor, 2013). In were examined. Our first aim was to assess potential associations
contrast, a study of Muslim women in the USA found no significant between (a) acculturation, religiosity, and religious identity and (b)
associations between the frequency of hijab use and internalization body esteem, internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived pres-
of the thin ideal (Tolaymat & Moradi, 2011). sure to adhere to beauty standards set by media. This aim was
While these studies conceptualized the hijab as an indicator investigated by administering validated measures of these con-
of religiosity, Dunkel, Davidson, and Qurashi (2010) noted that structs to first and second generation Canadian women between
the hijab also may reflect maintenance of one’s heritage culture. the ages of 18 and 25. Women of all religions were included in
Dunkel et al. (2010) found that women who wore non-Western the study given our interest in religiosity. Efforts were made to
clothing with a hijab reported less pressure to adhere to and inter- ensure Muslim women represented a substantial portion of the
nalize the thin ideal, compared to those who wore Western clothing sample since they are often neglected in this area of research. Extant
without a hijab, after controlling for BMI. However, in their cri- research has tended to examine the association between religios-
tique of this study, Tolaymat and Moradi (2011) noted that while ity and body image and internalization of the thin ideal among
all of the participants who indicated wearing non-Western cloth- Muslim and non-Muslim women independent of each other. How-
ing with a hijab were Muslim, the majority of participants who ever, given that Mussap (2009b) found no significant differences
indicated wearing Western clothing without the hijab were non- between the two groups’ levels of body dissatisfaction, the extent
Muslim. This calls into question whether the effects found were of one’s devotion to a particular religion could be assessed among
actually due to manner of dress or religious identity (Tolaymat & people of varying religions within one study. To demonstrate that
Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93 87

the associations between religiosity, body esteem, internalization Hypothesis 3. Manner of dress (e.g., the use of the hijab) among
of the thin ideal, and perceived pressure to adhere to media’s beauty Muslim Canadian women would differ depending on women’s lev-
standards do not depend on religious affiliation, we also assessed els of religiosity and both heritage and mainstream acculturation.
whether religiosity and religious identity (Muslim vs non-Muslim)
Thus, the purpose of this study was to enhance current under-
interact to predict the constructs of interest.
standing of the relations among religiosity and acculturation, and
Based on the aforementioned literature, we hypothesized the
body esteem, internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived pres-
following:
sure to adhere to media’s beauty standards. Given that religiosity
Hypothesis 1. Greater mainstream acculturation would con- and acculturation have often been assessed using proxies (e.g.,
tribute unique variance to lower levels of body esteem, greater whether one dresses in accordance with religious guidelines, years
internalization of the thin ideal, and greater perceived pressure to living in Western society), our use of validated measures of reli-
adhere to media’s beauty standards; conversely, greater heritage giosity and acculturation may clarify previous findings. As well,
acculturation would contribute unique variance to higher levels the portion of our study focusing on manner of dress among Mus-
of body esteem and lower internalization of the thin ideal and lim women may demonstrate whether past research’s use of the
perceived pressure. hijab as an indicator of religiosity and/or acculturation is indeed
accurate. Moreover, rather than simply examining whether or
Hypothesis 2. Greater religiosity would contribute unique vari- not Muslim women wear the hijab and its potential association
ance to greater body esteem, lower internalization of the thin ideal, with acculturation and religiosity, we included other categories
and lower perceived pressure. of dress that may better reflect increasing levels of adherence to
Islamic principles. Doing so allows for stronger distinctions to be
Although the interaction between religious identity and religios-
made in future between-group comparisons, and thus provides a
ity was investigated as a potential contributor of unique variance
better understanding of religiosity’s relation with body esteem.
to body esteem, internalization of the thin ideal, and pressure to
Additionally, our study focuses on women who are in emerging
adhere to beauty standards set by media, no hypotheses were
adulthood which is “an important, yet overlooked, age for estab-
developed.
lishing long-term health behaviour patterns” (Nelson, Story, Larson,
BMI has been shown to correlate with measures of body dissat-
Neumark-Sztainer, & Lytle, 2008, p. 2205), such as disordered eat-
isfaction (e.g., Tiggemann, 2005), and therefore BMI was controlled
ing. Lastly, our focus on Canadian women adds to the existing body
for in the analyses. As well, Dunkel et al. (2010) expressed concerns
of literature on acculturation given that most of this literature has
that having participants respond to questions about their level of
been conducted among U.S. samples, which may reflect a melt-
religiosity might lead them to respond to other questionnaires in a
ing pot rather than a mosaic culture (Peach, 2008). Consequently,
manner that would be more in line with the types of the answers
whether U.S. findings can be generalized to Canada, a country with
that they perceived as acceptable based on their religion. Thus, we
similar standards of beauty yet differing immigration attitudes, is
also considered social desirability as a covariate in our analyses.
unclear.
Our second aim was to determine whether manner of dress
among women who self-identify as Muslim differs based on their
levels of acculturation and religiosity. Muslims are provided guide- Method
lines on how to dress through the Qur’an, the central religious
text to Muslims, and the Hadeeth, preserved sayings and actions Participants
of the Prophet Muhammad. These guidelines require men and
women to wear loose clothing that covers certain areas of the body The data presented in this paper are based on the responses
when in public or in gender-mixed gatherings (al-Hashimi & al- of 143 Canadian women aged 18–25 (M = 20.92 years, SD = 1.97)
Khattab, 2010). For women, modest garments must cover the hair with an average BMI of 23.05 (SD = 4.48). BMI was calculated from
and the body, with the exception of the face and hands, in a man- women’s self-reported weight and height. Sixty-one of the 143
ner that does not attract unwarranted attention when non-relative participants characterized themselves as Muslim. Of these, 37.70%
men may be present (al-Hashimi & al-Khattab, 2010). Women may (n = 23) were born in North America, 26.23% (n = 16) in South Asia,
choose to cover their face through the niqab, a face veil with an 21.31% (n = 13) in the Middle East, 8.20% (n = 5) in North Africa,
opening for the eyes, but covering one’s face is not required and 3.28% in Europe (n = 2), and 3.28% in East Africa (n = 2). Of the
is believed to be a matter of personal choice by the majority of 82 non-Muslim participants, 75.60% (n = 62) were born in North
Muslims (Ruby, 2006). Dressing in accordance to Islamic guide- America, 8.54% (n = 7) in South Asia, 8.54% (n = 7) in Africa, 3.66%
lines varies across Muslim countries, where culture may also play a (n = 3) in Asia, and 3.66% (n = 3) in the Middle East. Non-Muslim
role in shaping one’s dress. For example, the jilbab or abayah, loose participants characterized themselves as Catholic (n = 33, 40.24%),
outer gowns covering the body, are predominantly worn with the Christian (n = 17, 20.73%), Agnostic (n = 12, 14.63%), Buddhist (n = 5,
hijab by Muslim women living in the Middle East, while Muslim 6.11%), Sikh (n = 4, 4.88%), Hindu (n = 4, 4.88%), Atheist (n = 1, 1.22%),
women living in South Asia may adhere to Islamic dress princi- and other (n = 6, 7.31%).
ples by wearing the hijab with salwar-kameez, a long tunic worn
over loose-fitting pants. Likewise, Muslim women living in North Measures
America may maintain a modest appearance while dressing accord-
ing to Western fashion, such as by wearing floor-length skirts and Brief Multidimensional Measurement of Religiousness/
long-sleeved shirts. In all of the aforementioned instances, Muslim Spirituality (BMMRS). The BMMRS (Fetzer Institute & The
women strive to adhere to religious dress guidelines, regardless of National Institute of Aging Working Group, 1999) contains 38
their specific dress. items that assess religiousness and spirituality over 11 domains:
While the hijab is usually perceived as a religious manifestation, daily spiritual experiences, values and beliefs, forgiveness, private
we chose to assess both acculturation and religiosity given that religious practices, religious/spiritual history, religious support,
some researchers, such as Dunkel et al. (2010), have considered commitment, organizational religiousness, religious and spiritual
the hijab to be a proxy for acculturation, and Mussap (2009b) has coping, religious preference, and overall self-ranking. Past studies
noted that religiosity is fundamental to one’s cultural identity. We have split religious and spiritual coping into two subscales to reflect
hypothesized that: positive and negative coping (e.g., Keefe et al., 2001) for a total of 12
88 Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93

subscales. The entire scale was administered in the present study; increases the risk of Type II error, and so the Attribution subscale
however, six items were not included when calculating partici- was not included in any analyses. Moreover, several Muslim par-
pants’ total scores due to having qualitative responses. Moreover, ticipants had contacted the primary investigator inquiring how to
some of these items inquired about participants’ financial con- respond to items such as “My looks help me to get dates” belong-
tribution to their religious institutions, and according to Masters ing to the BESAA Attribution subscale, given that they did not feel
et al. (2009) such items are not as relevant to college students. that they were applicable due to dating being discouraged in their
These items formed the Commitment, Religious/spiritual History, religion.
and Religious Preference subscales. The remaining 32 items such
as “I look to God for strength, support, and guidance” are subscales
on 4-, 5-, 6-, or 8-point Likert scales such that lower scores indi- Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-
cate greater levels of religiousness/spirituality. In the present study, 3 (SATAQ-3). The SATAQ-3 (Thompson, van den Berg, Roehrig,
scores were reversed such that higher scores indicated greater lev- Guarda, & Heinberg, 2004) contains 30 items such as “I would like
els of religiousness/spirituality for ease of interpretation. Given that my body to look like the models who appear in magazines” and is
items are responded to on different scales, all items were first con- comprised of four subscales. Only the General Internalization and
verted to z-scores as has been done in previous studies that have Perceived Pressures subscales were included in the present study.
used the BMMRS (e.g., Masters et al., 2009). The z-scores were then The General Internalization subscale measures general media influ-
averaged to create a composite score for the BMMRS. In the cur- ence and consists of nine items. The Perceived Pressures subscale
rent study, Cronbach’s alpha was .95 for the total score. Scores measures participants’ interpretations of pressure created from
for the subscales with quantitative responses (i.e., Daily Spiritual media exposure; it consists of seven items. All items are answered
Experiences, ˛ = .93; Values and Beliefs, ˛ = .45; Forgiveness, ˛ = .78; on a 5-point scale ranging from 1 (definitely disagree) to 5 (definitely
Private Religious Practices, ˛ = .88; Religious Support, ˛ = .76; Orga- agree) and summed such that higher scores reflect greater inter-
nizational Religiousness, ˛ = .83; Positive Religious and Spiritual nalization of the thin ideal and pressure to adhere to the standards
Coping, ˛ = .76; and Negative Religious and Spiritual Coping, ˛ = .46) for appearance set by the media. Both subscales included in this
were computed for post hoc analyses, with the exception of over- study are reported to have excellent internal consistency among
all self-ranking given that it is a 2-item summary of the measure female undergraduate students (˛ General = .96 and ˛ Perceived
rather than a specific aspect of religiosity. Reliability estimates for Pressures = .92; Thompson et al., 2004). In the current study, Cron-
the individual subscales have been upheld (e.g., Fetzer Institute & bach’s alphas were .85 and .91 for the General Internalization and
The National Institute of Aging Working Group, 1999; Knight et al., Perceived Pressures subscales, respectively.
2007).

Vancouver Index of Acculturation – Canadian (VIA-C). The


Canadian version of the Vancouver Index of Acculturation (Ryder, Marlowe Crowne Social Desirability Scale (MCSDS). The
Alden, & Paulhus, 2000) contains 20 items and is comprised of MCSDS (Crowne & Marlowe, 1960) contains 33 items assessing
two subscales. The Heritage subscale (VIA-Heritage) measures par- socially desirable responding such as “No matter who I’m listen-
ticipants’ levels of identification with their heritage culture and ing to, I’m always a good listener.” All items are answered with
contains 10 items such as “I often participate in my heritage cultural True or False which are scored as 0 or 1 depending on the item,
traditions.” The Mainstream subscale (VIA-Mainstream) measures and summed such that higher total scores indicate greater endorse-
participants’ levels of identification with Canadian culture and con- ment of socially desirable responses. The MCSDS is reported to have
tains 10 items such as “I often participate in mainstream Canadian good internal consistency among undergraduate students (˛ = .88;
cultural traditions.” All items are answered using a 9-point scale Crowne & Marlowe, 1960). In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha
ranging from 1 (disagree) to 9 (agree), and averaged such that higher was .80.
scores reflect greater levels of identification with the specified
culture (i.e., participants’ heritage culture and Canadian culture,
respectively). Both subscales are reported to have excellent internal Demographics questionnaire. Participants were directed to
reliability estimates among Canadian undergraduate students (˛ one of two demographics questionnaires depending on their reli-
Heritage = .90, ˛ Mainstream = .83; Asvat & Malcarne, 2008). In the gious affiliation. Both questionnaires inquired about background
current study, Cronbach’s alphas were .92 and .91 for the Heritage information such as the participant’s age, ethnicity, immigration
and Mainstream subscales, respectively. status, country of birth, and manner of dress, but the latter portion
varied between the two questionnaires. Women who identified
Body-Esteem Scale for Adolescents and Adults (BESAA). The themselves as Muslim were asked to rate how often they wear var-
BESAA (Mendelson, Mendelson, & White, 2001) contains 23 items ious types of clothing when in a public setting from 0 (never) to 4
such as “I like what I see when I look in the mirror” and is comprised (always). Dress options listed in the study were: Western dress (e.g.,
of three subscales. The Appearance subscale measures general t-shirts, jeans, etc.), non-North American traditional clothing (e.g.,
feelings about appearance and consists of 10 items. The Weight salwar-kameez and saris, garments worn by South Asian women)
subscale measures weight satisfaction and consists of eight items. without the hijab, long skirts and full-sleeved shirts without the
Lastly, the Attribution subscale measures how one thinks others hijab, Western dress with the hijab, non-North American tradi-
perceive his/her appearance and consists of five items. All items tional dress with the hijab, long skirts and full-sleeved shirts with
are answered on a 5-point scale from 0 (never) to 4 (always) and the hijab, the jilbab/abayah, and the niqab. Participants who did not
summed such that higher scores reflect greater body satisfaction. identify themselves as Muslim also were asked to rate how often
All three subscales are reported to have good-to-excellent inter- they wear various types of clothing when in a public setting from
nal reliability among girls and young women (˛ Appearance = .93, 0 (never) to 4 (always), to equalize groups and reduce any poten-
˛ Weight = .95, ˛ Attribution = .81; Mendelson et al., 2001). In tial response biases. Non-Muslim participants’ dress options only
the current study, Cronbach’s alphas were .93, .92, and .65 for included “Western dress” and “religious clothing.” Ratings were
the Appearance, Weight and Attribution subscales, respectively. used for the purpose of categorizing participants by the type of
According to Osborne and Waters (2002), using variables that clothing they reported wearing most frequently, as described in
are not reliable in correlational or multiple regression analyses the Results section.
Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93 89

Procedure predicted, higher levels of appearance satisfaction were associated


with greater endorsement of women’s heritage culture and greater
Participants were recruited for the present study via advertise- religiosity. However, appearance satisfaction was not associated
ments on an undergraduate psychology participant pool and on with lower levels of mainstream acculturation (i.e., Canadian cul-
social media websites (e.g., Facebook). Advertisements also were ture). Additionally, weight satisfaction and internalization of the
given to the leaders of student groups at the university and emailed thin ideal were not significantly correlated with religiosity or accul-
to members of a local mosque. The advertisements were circulated turation to either the mainstream or heritage culture. Perceived
in an urban area and indicated that participation in the study was pressure to adhere to media’s beauty standards was positively cor-
restricted to Canadian women between the ages of 18 and 25 years related with mainstream acculturation, as predicted.
old who were immigrants to Canada or the daughters of immigrant
parents. The latter criterion was included to ensure all participants
had a heritage culture, as acculturation refers to a process of cul- Hypothesis 1 and Hypothesis 2 Results
tural change. Overall, 224 individuals accessed the online study, 96
of whom were recruited from the participant pool and 128 from To determine whether religious identity, religiosity, main-
other recruitment methods (e.g., social media or student groups). stream and heritage acculturation, and the interaction between
However, only 143 met the aforementioned inclusion criteria, as religious identity and religiosity were associated with body esteem
well as completed all of the predictor measures and at least one (i.e., appearance satisfaction and weight satisfaction), thin-ideal
criterion measure. internalization, and perceived pressure to adhere to media’s beauty
After completing the appropriate demographic questionnaire, standards, four linear regressions with bootstrapping were con-
participants completed the remaining questionnaires in random- ducted. Covariates (i.e., BMI, social desirability) were entered into
ized order, and then reported their weight and height. Participants the first block. The second block contained the dummy codes for
who were registered in the participant pool were compensated religious identity, where 0 and 1 represented non-Muslim and Mus-
with 0.5 credits for their participation, and those who were not lim participants, respectively. Religiosity, heritage acculturation,
were entered in a raffle for a chance to win one of three $10 gift and mainstream acculturation also were included in the second
cards. This study was approved by the University’s Departmental block. Finally, the interaction between religious identity and reli-
Research Ethics Board. giosity was added into the third block. All predictors were centered
prior to being included in the regression analyses to avoid multi-
collinearity between the predictors and interaction (Field, 2009).
Results
The final regression models are presented in Tables 2 and 3. The
unstandardized regression coefficients and 95% confidence inter-
Preliminary Analyses
vals presented in these tables are based on 10,000 samples drawn
randomly with replacement from our dataset.
The assumptions of multiple regression first were assessed.
Table 2 provides a summary of the final regression model
The Kolmogorov–Smirnov test revealed that BMI, the MCSDS,
with appearance satisfaction as the criterion variable. Step 1
VIA-Heritage, VIA-mainstream, and BMMRS were not normally dis-
which controlled for BMI and social desirability was significant,
tributed (all ps < .01), although skewness and kurtosis were within
F(2, 140) = 11.87, p < .001, accounting for 14.5% of the variance in
±3 and ±10, respectively (Kline, 2010). Independence of residuals
appearance satisfaction and both variables significantly predicted
was assessed via the Durbin-Watson statistic, and all statistics were
appearance satisfaction. Adding in religious identity, religiosity,
close to the accepted value of 2, indicating that this assumption was
mainstream and heritage acculturation in Step 2 did not sig-
met. However, the assumptions of linearity and homoscedasticity
nificantly contribute to the variance in appearance satisfaction,
were not met. Thus, all regressions were conducted using boot-
F(4, 136) = 1.59, p = .181. Heritage acculturation was approaching
strapping, given that this technique can be employed as a strategy
significance in Step 2, t(136) = 2.16, p = .057, although it was non-
to obtain more accurate estimates when two or more assumptions
significant in Step 3 (p = .084) where the interaction between
are violated (Tavakol & Wilcox, 2013).
religious identity and religiosity was added. Step 3 did not signifi-
cantly contribute to the variance in appearance satisfaction, F(1,
Correlations 135) = 0.40, p = .529.
Table 2 also provides a summary of the final regression model
Table 1 contains the Pearson correlations, means, and standard with weight satisfaction as the criterion variable. Step 1 which
deviations of all the variables included in the present study. As controlled for BMI and social desirability was significant, F(2,

Table 1
Zero-order correlations between variables and covariates (N = 143).

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9

1. BMI 1.00 .01 −.29** −.59** .25** .32** .01 .11 −.13
2. Social desirability 1.00 .25** .00a −.28** −.27** .27** −.12 .26**
3. Appearance satisfaction 1.00 .54** −.59** −.50** .23* −.10 .19*
4. Weight satisfaction 1.00 −.44** −.44** −.11 −.10 .08
5. General internalization 1.00 .75** −.14 .15 −.09
6. Perceived pressure 1.00 −.16 .21** −.04
7. Heritage acculturation 1.00 .20* .21*
8. Mainstream acculturation 1.00 −.31**
9. Religiosity 1.00

M 23.05 17.67 22.78 16.10 18.97 15.28 6.14 6.88 0.00


(SD) 4.48 5.45 7.67 7.59 8.85 7.07 2.05 1.74 0.62
a
=.002.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
90 Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93

Table 2
Summary of final hierarchical regression analysis for BESAA subscales: appearance satisfaction and weight satisfaction (N = 143).

Variable Appearance satisfaction Weight satisfaction

b 95% CI SE b ˇ p sr2 b 95% CI SE b ˇ p sr2

BMI −0.481 −0.711, −0.195 0.131 −.281 .000 .077 −0.996 −1.352, −0.771 0.148 −.588 .000 .336
Social desirability 0.251 0.011, 0.481 0.121 .178 .039 .028 0.039 −0.144, 0.227 0.093 .028 .669 .001
Religious identitya 0.627 −2.597, 3.995 1.701 .041 .709 .001 −0.491 −3.677, 2.781 1.661 −.032 .766 .001
Religiosity 1.005 −2.031, 3.790 1.478 .081 .499 .003 0.536 −2.077, 2.929 1.280 .044 .680 .001
Heritage acculturation 0.654 −0.071, 1.384 0.376 .175 .084 .024 −0.422 −0.935, 0.120 0.271 −.114 .120 .010
Mainstream acculturation −0.268 −1.105, 0.575 0.430 −.061 .536 .003 −0.046 −0.797, 0.731 0.386 −.011 .907 <.001
Religiosity × Religious identitya −1.600 −7.488, 3.606 2.780 −.072 .559 .002 −0.183 −5.621, 5.108 2.733 −.008 .949 <.001
a
Muslim vs non-Muslim.

Table 3
Summary of final hierarchical regression analysis for SATAQ subscales: thin-ideal internalization and perceived pressure to be thin (N = 143).

Variable Thin-ideal internalization Perceived pressure to be thin

b 95% CI SE b ˇ p sr2
b 95% CI SE b ˇ p sr2

BMI 0.484 0.212, 0.759 0.139 .245 .001 .059 0.505 0.308, 0.766 0.116 .320 .000 .100
Social desirability −0.359 −0.590, −0.115 0.122 −.221 .005 .042 −0.307 −0.516, −0.090 0.108 −.236 .004 .048
Religious identitya −0.927 −4.690, 3.303 2.015 −.052 .636 .002 0.819 −1.964, 3.521 1.390 .057 .546 .002
Religiosity 0.427 −2.510, 3.776 1.587 .030 .786 <.001 1.173 −1.229, 3.558 1.207 .103 .333 .005
Heritage acculturation −0.498 −1.198, 0.202 0.360 −.115 .163 .010 −0.621 −1.182, −0.101 0.276 −.180 .022 .025
Mainstream acculturation 0.701 −0.271, 1.652 0.489 .138 .147 .014 1.061 0.445, 1.681 0.315 .261 .001 .050
Religiosity × Religious identitya 2.362 −4.593, 8.109 3.205 .092 .441 .004 1.425 −2.708, 5.829 2.188 .069 .508 .002
a
Muslim vs non-Muslim.

140) = 37.82, p < .001, accounting for 34.1% of the variance in weight Only subscales that had adequate reliability in the present study
satisfaction, but only BMI was a significant predictor, t(140) = −8.70, were included in the post hoc analyses (i.e., Daily Spiritual Expe-
p < .001. Adding in the dummy code for religious identity, reli- riences, Forgiveness, Private Religious Practices, Organizational
giosity, mainstream and heritage acculturation in Step 2 did not Religiousness, Religious Support, and Positive Religious and Spir-
significantly contribute to the variance in weight satisfaction, F(4, itual Coping). Thus, the original regressions were rerun with the
136) = .70, p = .592, nor did adding in the interaction term in Step 3, BMMRS total score being substituted for these six BMMRS sub-
F(1, 135) = 0.01, p = .934. scales.
Table 3 provides a summary of the final regression model with Adding in the BMMRS subscales along with religious identity
internalization of the thin ideal as the criterion variable. Step 1 and heritage and mainstream acculturation in Step 2 did not signif-
which controlled for BMI and social desirability was significant, icantly contribute to the variance in appearance satisfaction, F(9,
F(2, 140) = 9.96, p < .001, accounting for 11.2% of the variance in 131) = 1.20, p = .304. None of the BMMRS subscales emerged as sig-
internalization of the thin ideal with both variables significantly nificant contributors to the variance in appearance satisfaction.
predicting thin-ideal internalization. Adding in the dummy code Similarly, adding in the BMMRS subscales in Step 2 did not sig-
for religious identity, religiosity, mainstream, and heritage accul- nificantly contribute to the variance in weight satisfaction, F(9,
turation in Step 2 did not significantly contribute to the variance in 131) = 1.12, p = .350. However, organizational religiosity emerged
thin-ideal internalization, F(4, 136) = 1.03, p = .393, nor did adding as a significant contributor to the variance in weight satisfaction
in the interaction term in Step 3, F(1, 135) = 0.63, p = .430. in this step, t(131) = 1.98, p = .045, with no other subscales being
Table 3 also provides a summary of the final regression model significant.
with perceived pressures as the criterion variable. Step 1 which Adding in the BMMRS subscales along with religious identity
controlled for BMI and social desirability was significant, F(2, and heritage and mainstream acculturation in Step 2 did not signifi-
140) = 14.91, p < .001, accounting for 16.4% of the model with both cantly contribute to the variance in internalization of the thin ideal,
variables significantly predicting perceived pressures. Adding in F(9, 131) = 1.13, p = .349. None of the BMMRS subscales emerged
the dummy code for religious identity, religiosity, mainstream, as significant contributors to the variance in thin-ideal internal-
and heritage acculturation in Step 2 significantly contributed to ization. However, adding in the BMMRS subscales in Step 2 did
the variance in perceived pressure, F(4, 136) = 3.07, p = .019, significantly contribute to the variance in perceived pressure, F(9,
accounting for an additional 6.8% of the variance. Both mainstream, 131) = 2.22, p = .025, and accounted for an additional 10.9% of the
t(140) = 3.05, p = .001, and heritage acculturation, t(140) = −2.31, variance. Despite this, none of the BMMRS subscales emerged as
p = .012, emerged as significant predictors in this step. Adding the significant contributors to the variance in perceived pressure. As
interaction between religious identity and religiosity in Step 3 did with the original regression that utilized the total BMMRS score,
not significantly contribute to the variance in perceived pressure mainstream and heritage acculturation were significant contribu-
to adhere to media’s beauty standards, F(1, 135) = 0.40, p = .528. tors to the variance in perceived pressure.

Post Hoc Probing Hypothesis 3 Results

In the aforementioned regressions, religiosity did not signifi- Muslim participants’ manner of dress was categorized into one
cantly contribute to the variance in appearance satisfaction, weight of three categories based on which types of clothing they endorsed
satisfaction, internalization of the thin ideal, or perceived pres- wearing most frequently: (a) Western dress without the hijab
sure. Having used the total BMMRS scale score, post hoc analyses (n = 18), (b) Western dress with the hijab (n = 12), and (c) long
were conducted to determine whether the measure’s subscales skirts and full-sleeved shirts or jilbab/abayah with the hijab (n = 17).
contributed to the variance in any of the variables of interest. Because no participant endorsed traditional South Asian dress with
Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93 91

the hijab or long skirts and full-sleeved shirts or jilbab/abayah with- but not necessarily their weight. Given that non-Western cultures
out the hijab, these categories were excluded. Previous studies have tend to have more definitive roles for each gender (Dion & Dion,
inquired about the types of clothing participants wear most often 2001), women who maintain their heritage culture may act more
(e.g., Dunkel et al., 2010), and so this was deemed an appropriate in accordance with their cultures’ gender roles, and thereby feel
basis on which to categorize participants’ dress. more satisfied with their overall appearance but not specifically
Three one-way ANOVAs were conducted to determine whether how much they weigh. This finding suggests that adherence to
participants’ level of heritage acculturation, mainstream accultur- gender roles as defined by one’s culture could potentially mediate
ation, and religiosity varied based on how they dress most often. the relationship between heritage acculturation and appearance
There were main effects of manner of dress on heritage accultur- satisfaction.
ation F(2, 44) = 4.58, p = .02 and religiosity F(2, 44) = 7.84, p = .001. Religiosity as a composite score was significantly correlated
No significant main effect of manner of dress was found on main- with appearance satisfaction. However, religiosity was not a sig-
stream acculturation F(2, 44) = 2.64, p = .08. Post hoc analyses were nificant contributor to the variance of appearance and weight
performed using Scheffé tests to identify exactly where significant satisfaction after controlling for BMI and social desirability, con-
differences exist. The analyses revealed that Western dress with- trary to our second hypothesis. Post hoc analyses of specific
out hijab (M = 7.39, SD = 1.28) differed significantly from long skirts aspects of religiosity revealed that organizational religiosity (i.e.,
and full-sleeved shirts or jilbab/abayah with the hijab (M = 5.79, involvement with a religious centre such as a church or mosque)
SD = 1.80) on heritage acculturation (x̄a − x̄c = 1.60, 95% CI [0.61, significantly contributed to the variance in weight satisfaction.
2.67], d = 1.02). Western dress without hijab (M = 0.03, SD = 0.56) This is consistent with Homan and Boyatzis’ (2009) finding that
also differed significantly from long skirts and full-sleeved shirts church attendance was positively associated with body satisfaction
or jilbab/abayah with the hijab (M = 0.56, SD = 0.26) on religiosity among women. Given that other subscales assessed in the present
(x̄a − x̄c = −0.53, 95% CI [−0.89, −0.18], d = −1.22). As well, West- study (e.g., forgiveness, private religious practices, religious, and
ern dress without the hijab (M = 0.03, SD = 0.56) and Western dress spiritual coping) were not significant, it may be that there is an
with the hijab (M = 0.44, SD = 0.31) differed on religiosity (x̄a − x̄b = aspect of being part of a religious organization that contributes
−0.41, 95% CI [−0.80, −0.02], d = −0.91). to first and second generation women’s weight satisfaction. For
instance, it may be the case that being in an environment where
a woman can interact with other religious individuals makes her
Discussion feel more accepted and less judged about her body. Religious indi-
viduals are noted in literature to be “nicer” (i.e., more friendly and
We first examined the potential associations among religiosity cooperative; Brennan & London, 2001), especially when they are at
and acculturation on body esteem, internalization of the thin ideal, religious institutions where they are also more likely to engage in
and/or perceived pressure to adhere to media’s beauty standards pro-social behaviours (Malhotra, 2010). Taken together, organiza-
among Canadian women from a variety of religious denomina- tional religiosity may reflect a higher degree of engagement with
tions. Greater endorsement of heritage culture was associated with more accepting individuals, which can allow a woman to feel more
greater appearance, but not weight satisfaction. Moreover, after comfortable and satisfied with her weight. Should future research
accounting for BMI and social desirability, heritage acculturation find evidence for this proposition, it would suggest that motivating
remained as the only variable that emerged as a near signifi- people to choose supportive environments could help to enhance
cant contributor to the variance in appearance satisfaction. This weight satisfaction. This means of increasing weight satisfaction
was not the case for weight satisfaction, thus our first hypothesis can be applied to a wide range of people, and not just those who
was only partially confirmed. First and second generation women ascribe to a particular faith.
who adhere to their heritage culture may be more satisfied with In regards to internalization of the thin ideal, after accounting
how they look than women with lower heritage acculturation, but for BMI and social desirability, heritage acculturation, mainstream
their cultural adherence does not necessarily relate to their weight acculturation, and religiosity did not contribute to the variance,
satisfaction. In support of the latter, Abdollahi and Mann (2001) contrary to our hypotheses. Moreover, none of the religiosity
found no difference in participants’ weight and shape dissatisfac- subscales emerged as significant contributors to the variance in
tion between Muslim women living in Tehran and Muslim women the post hoc analyses. This is in contrast to previous literature
living in Los Angeles, who were assumed to be lower in heritage which has found significant associations between acculturation,
acculturation. Given that Abdollahi and Mann (2001) did not assess religiosity, and internalization of the thin ideal (e.g., Dunkel et al.,
general appearance satisfaction, it is unclear as to whether the 2010; Garcia-Rea, 2006). However, extant literature has focused
researchers would have found a significant difference in appear- on non-Canadian samples (e.g., U.S., Australian, British) and so it is
ance satisfaction between the two groups. Similar to Abdollahi and unclear as to whether the present study’s non-significant finding is
Mann’s (2001) study, most research has focused on body dissat- attributable to the Canadian sample. Future studies conducted on
isfaction rather than examining general appearance satisfaction Canadian samples would help to determine whether past findings
and weight satisfaction separately (e.g., Poloskov & Tracey, 2013; that acculturation and religiosity are related to internalization of
Swami et al., 2013). Thus, the results of the present study shed light the thin ideal can be generalized to Canadian women, who live in
on a distinction that may be useful to address in future research. a country with similar body ideals but different attitudes towards
Research on gender role adherence may help to explicate culture.
why heritage acculturation was associated with appearance but Concerning women’s perceived pressure to adhere to media’s
not weight satisfaction. Pozzebon, Visser, and Bogaert (2012) beauty standards, both mainstream and heritage acculturation
found that women whose personality and occupational prefer- emerged as significant contributors to the variance after account-
ences reflected more traditional gender roles rated themselves as ing for BMI and social desirability, in support of our hypothesis.
more physically attractive. Moreover, these women’s self-reported Specifically, women with greater levels of heritage culture feel less
ratings of appearance satisfaction were more highly correlated pressure to adhere to standards of beauty set by the media (e.g.,
with physical attractiveness than their ratings of weight satis- to be thin). Conversely, greater endorsement of mainstream (i.e.,
faction, and their compliance with traditional gender roles were Canadian) culture and values among women is associated with
not related to how they describe their weight. This suggests that greater perceived pressure to adhere to the media’s portrayal of
these women may have been more satisfied with their appearance beauty standards. This finding is consistent with past research
92 Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93

conducted outside of Canada (e.g., Ball & Kenardy, 2002; Garcia-Rea, Homan and Boyatzis (2010), where pressure to be thin and thin-
2006), and suggests that women high in mainstream acculturation ideal internalization predicted body dissatisfaction only for women
and low in heritage acculturation are more vulnerable to feeling with an anxious insecure attachment to God, while women who felt
pressure to adhere to the standards of beauty presented in the loved and accepted by God were buffered from these body issues.
media. Thus, prevention programs aimed at such women would Thus, future studies could include additional measures pertaining
be helpful. Conversely, being less acculturated to mainstream cul- to religion. Another limitation in the present study is that data were
ture, and placing greater value on one’s heritage culture may serve derived from questionnaires completed at one time. Thus, although
as a protective factor against perceived pressure. we found that heritage and mainstream acculturation are associ-
Overall, religious identity (i.e., Muslim vs non-Muslim) did not ated with perceived pressure, we do not know whether this is a
significantly contribute to the variance in appearance or weight sat- causal relationship, that is, whether a woman’s level of accultur-
isfaction, internalization of the thin ideal, and perceived pressure ation affects the extent to which she feels pressure to adhere to
to adhere to media’s beauty standards, suggesting that there are no media’s standard of beauty. Future quasi-experimental or longitu-
differences between Muslims and non-Muslims in terms of these dinal studies are necessary. Such studies should include measures
constructs. Additionally, the interaction between religiosity and of both weight and general appearance satisfaction, rather than
religious identity was not significant. This suggests that research general measures of body dissatisfaction as is common within the
can be conducted on samples comprised of individuals of varying existing literature (e.g., Mussap, 2009a). Lastly, although partici-
religious denominations. pants identified as being of a variety of religious denominations, not
Lastly, we assessed whether manner of dress was associated all religious groups were represented (e.g., no participants identi-
with varying levels of acculturation and religiosity. Religiosity and fied as Jewish). Thus, future studies could aim to include a larger
heritage acculturation differed based on Muslim women’s manner sample that is more representative of people of a variety of reli-
of dress. Moreover, rather than simply examining whether or not gions.
Muslim women wear the hijab and its potential association with In addition to some of the proposed future directions noted, it
acculturation and religiosity, we included other categories of dress may be helpful to validate the BESAA attribution subscale among
that may better reflect increasing levels of adherence to Islamic a sample of Muslim women, or validate a modified version of it.
principles. Doing so allows for stronger distinctions to be made A number of written concerns were received from Muslim partic-
in future between-group comparisons, and thus provides a better ipants during the study who were confused as to how to respond
understanding of religiosity’s relation with body esteem. to certain questions that they felt were inapplicable. Participants
As expected, women who reported greater levels of religiosity were encouraged to reply to the best of their ability which resulted
dressed in greater accordance with Islamic principles. Specifically, in a mixed response; some Muslim women responded by strongly
women who reported wearing Western dress without the hijab had disagreeing to these items while other Muslim women responded
lower levels of religiosity than women who reported wearing West- to the items by interpreting them within the context of permis-
ern dress with the hijab, as well as lower levels of religiosity than sible Islamic relations (e.g., “My looks help me to get marriage
women who reported wearing long skirts and full-sleeved shirts proposals”, “When I am not wearing my headscarf, other people
or jilbab/abayah with the hijab. These findings provide empirical consider me good looking”). This may explain why the Cronbach’s
support for Droogsma’s (2007) suggestion that modesty in a Mus- alpha internal consistency reliability score for the BESAA Attribu-
lim woman’s clothing may reflect how strongly she believes in her tion subscale was lower than what has typically been reported for
religion. this subscale.
In terms of acculturation, Muslim women who reported wearing In closing, the findings of the study contribute to a more compre-
Western dress without the hijab were more embracing of their her- hensive understanding of the roles of religiosity and acculturation
itage culture than Muslim women who reported wearing long skirts in appearance and weight satisfaction, internalization of the thin
and full-sleeved shirts or jilbab/abayah with the hijab. Although ideal, and perceived pressure to adhere to media beauty standards
this may seem counterintuitive, this is understandable given the among first and second generation Canadian women. The present
context of the cultural backgrounds the Muslim women come from. study also provides avenues for future research as mentioned
In many cases, culture and religion do not necessarily overlap. For throughout the discussion. For example, although the present study
example, when it comes to gender interaction, Arab, Middle East- was able to probe at more specific aspects of religiosity, further
ern, and South Asian culture may encourage free mixing between research could be conducted to understand what aspects of heritage
genders, particularly through frequent family and social gather- or mainstream acculturation specifically affect appearance satis-
ings. On the other hand, Islam proposes guidelines on how such faction and perceived pressure (e.g., adherence to gender roles).
interactions should be carried out (e.g., encouraging modest action Our findings regarding manner of dress also provide information
and dress between men and women, limiting idle conversation, more generally about Middle Eastern and/or South Asian culture
etc.). Given that Muslim women who wear Western dress with- and Islam; that is, Middle Eastern and/or South Asian culture and
out the hijab reported lower levels of religiosity, it follows that Islam are not synonymous, and thus individuals should note the
these women would be more embracing of their respective cul- potential distinctions when interacting with Muslim women.
tural practices than Muslim women who dress more modestly with
the hijab and reported greater levels of religiosity. Individuals who
are more religious may leave behind certain cultural practices that References
are discouraged in Islam, thus contributing to lower levels of her-
Abdollahi, P., & Mann, T. (2001). Eating disorder symptoms and body image con-
itage acculturation. This result is important in terms of highlighting
cerns in Iran: Comparisons between Iranian women in Iran and in America.
the difference between culture and religion, two notions that are International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 259–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/
commonly overlooked as one. eat.1083
al-Hashimi, M. A., & al-Khattab, N. (2010). The ideal Muslimah: The true Islamic person-
It is important to note that there were some limitations to the
ality of the Muslim woman as defined in the Qur’an and Sunnah (5th ed.). Riyadh,
present study, and that the results should be interpreted with these Saudi Arabia: International Islamic Publishing House.
in mind. First, although various forms of religiosity were accounted American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
for in the BMMRS, the presence of secure or insecure attachments disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Asvat, Y., & Malcarne, V. L. (2008). Acculturation and depressive symptoms in Muslim
with God was not measured. This may have an effect on perceived university students: Personal-family acculturation match. International Journal
pressure to be thin, as in the case with research conducted by of Psychology, 43, 114–124. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00207590601126668
Z. Chaker et al. / Body Image 14 (2015) 85–93 93

Ball, K., & Kenardy, J. (2002). Body weight, body image, and eating behaviours: Rela- Mussap, A. J. (2009b). Strength of faith and body image in Muslim and non-Muslim
tionships with ethnicity and acculturation in a community sample of young women. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 12, 121–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Australian women. Eating Behaviors, 3, 205–216. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ 1080/13674670802358190
S1471-0153(02)00062-4 Nelson, M. C., Story, M., Larson, N. I., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Lytle, L. A. (2008).
Bell, K. (2011). Religious coping and experience of body satisfaction among college Emerging adulthood and college-aged youth: An overlooked age for weight-
women (Master’s thesis). Retrieved from UNT Digital Library (UMI No. 1511475). related behavior change. Obesity, 16, 2205–2211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/oby.
Berry, J. W. (1997). Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Applied Psychology, 2008.365
46, 5–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1997.tb01087.x Osborne, J., & Waters, E. (2002). Four assumptions of multiple regression that
Boyatzis, C. J., Kline, S., & Backof, S. (2007). Experimental evidence that theistic- researchers should always test. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 8,
religious body affirmations improve women’s body image. Journal for the 1–9. Retrieved from http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=8&n=2
Scientific Study of Religion, 46, 553–564. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5906. Peach, C. (2008). The mosaic versus the melting pot: Canada and the USA. Scottish
2007.00377.x Geographical Journal, 121, 3–27. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00369220518737218
Brennan, K. M., & London, A. S. (2001). Are religious people nice people? Religiosity, Poloskov, E., & Tracey, T. J. (2013). Internalization of US female beauty standards as a
race, interview dynamics, and perceived cooperativeness. Sociological Inquiry, mediator of the relationship between Mexican American women’s acculturation
71, 129–144. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1475-682X.2001.tb01105.x and body dissatisfaction. Body Image, 10, 501–508. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
Crowne, D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent bodyim.2013.05.005
of psychopathology. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 24, 349–354. http://dx.doi. Pozzebon, J. A., Visser, B. A., & Bogaert, A. F. (2012). Do you think you’re sexy, tall,
org/10.1037/h0047358 and thin? The prediction of self-rated attractiveness, height, and weight. Journal
Dion, K. K., & Dion, K. L. (2001). Gender and cultural adaptation in immigrant families. of Applied Social Psychology, 42, 2671–2700. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-
Journal of Social Issues, 57, 511–521. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0022-4537.00226 1816.2012.00957.x
Droogsma, R. A. (2007). Redefining hijab: American Muslim women’s standpoints Ruby, T. F. (2006). Listening to the voices of hijab. Women’s Studies International
on veiling. Journal of Applied Communication Research, 35, 294–319. http://dx. Forum, 29, 54–66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.wsif.2005.10.006
doi.org/10.1080/0090988071434299 Ryder, A. G., Alden, L. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2000). Is acculturation unidimensional
Dunkel, T. M., Davidson, D., & Qurashi, S. (2010). Body satisfaction and pressure to be or bidimensional? A head-to-head comparison in the prediction of personality,
thin in younger and older Muslim and non-Muslim women: The role of Western self-identity, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
and non-Western dress preferences. Body Image, 7, 56–65. http://dx.doi.org/10. 49–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.49
1016/j.bodyim.2009.10.003 Smith, M. H., Richards, P. S., & Maglio, C. J. (2004). Examining the relationship
Fetzer Institute, & The National Institute of Aging Working Group. (1999). Multidi- between religious orientation and eating disturbances. Eating Behaviors, 5,
mensional measurement of religiousness/spirituality for use in health research: A 171–180. http://dx.doi.org/10.106/S1471-0153(03)00064-3
report of the Fetzer Institute/National Institute on Aging Working Group. Kalama- Soh, N. L., Touyz, S. W., & Surgenor, L. J. (2006). Eating and body image disturbances
zoo, MI: Fetzer Institute. across cultures: A review. European Eating Disorders Review, 14, 54–65. http://
Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publica- dx.doi.org/10.1002/erv.678
tions. Statistics Canada. (2011). Religion and ethnocultural diversity in Canada; religions
Garcia-Rea, E. A. (2006). Acculturation and sociocultural influences as predictors of in Canada, National Household Survey, 2011. [PDF document]. Retrieved from
family relationships and body image dissatisfaction in African American, Hispanic Statistics Canada: http://www12.statcan.gc.ca/nhs-enm/2011/as-sa/99-010-x/
American, and European American women (Doctoral dissertation). Retrieved from 99-010-x2011001-eng.pdf
UNT Digital Library (UMI No. 254185). Stice, E., & Bearman, S. K. (2001). Body-image and eating disturbances prospec-
Homan, K. J., & Boyatzis, C. J. (2009). Body image in older adults: Links with reli- tively predict increases in depressive symptoms in adolescent girls: A growth
gion and gender. Journal of Adult Development, 16, 230–238. http://dx.doi.org/ curve analysis. Developmental Psychology, 37, 597–607. http://dx.doi.org/10.
10.1007/s10804-009-9069-8 1037/0012-1649.37.5.597
Homan, K. J., & Boyatzis, C. J. (2010). The protective role of attachment to God Stice, E., & Shaw, H. E. (2002). Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and
against eating disorder risk factors: Concurrent and prospective evidence. Eating maintenance of eating pathology: A synthesis of research findings. Jour-
Disorders, 18, 239–258. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10640261003719534 nal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 985–993. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-
Keefe, F. J., Affleck, G., Lefebvre, J., Underwood, L., Caldwell, D. S., Drew, J.,. . . & Parga- 3999(02)00488-9
ment, K. (2001). Living with rheumatoid arthritis: The role of daily spirituality Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. W. (2008). Counseling the culturally diverse: Theory and practice.
and daily religious and spiritual coping. The Journal of Pain, 2, 101–110. http:// New York, NY: Wiley.
dx.doi.org/10.1054/jpai.2001.19296 Sussman, N. M., Truong, N., & Lim, J. (2007). Who experiences “America the beauti-
Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and practices of structural equation modeling (3rd ed.). ful?”: Ethnicity moderating the effect of acculturation on body image and risks
New York, NY: Guilford Press. for eating disorders among immigrant women. International Journal of Inter-
Knight, J. R., Sherritt, L., Harris, S. K., Holder, D. W., Kulig, J., Shrier, L. A.,. . .& cultural Relations, 31, 29–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijintrel.2006.03.003
Chang, G. (2007). Alcohol use and religiousness/spirituality among adoles- Swami, V., Miah, J., Noorani, N., & Taylor, D. (2013). Is the hijab protective? An
cents. Southern Medical Journal, 100, 349–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/SMJ. investigation of beauty image and related constructs among British Muslim
0b013e3180316a32 women. British Journal of Psychology, 105, 352–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
Lai, K. Y. (2000). Anorexia nervosa in Chinese adolescents: Does culture make a bjop/12045
difference? Journal of Adolescence, 23, 561–568. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jado. Tavakol, M., & Wilcox, R. R. (2013). Medical education research: The application of
2000.0343 robust statistical methods. International Journal of Medical Education, 4, 93–95.
Malhotra, D. K. (2010). (When) are religious people nicer? Religious salience and http://dx.doi.org/10.5116/ijme.5181.5fe8
the ‘Sunday effect’ on pro-social behavior. Judgment and Decision Making, 5, Thompson, J. K., van den Berg, P., Roehrig, M., Guarda, A. S., & Heinberg, L. J. (2004).
138–143. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1297275 The Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance Questionnaire-3 (SATAQ-
Masters, K. S., Carey, K. B., Maisto, S. A., Caldwell, P. E., Wolfe, T. V., Hackney, H. L.,. . .& 3): Development and validation. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35,
Himawan, L. (2009). Psychometric examination of the Brief Multidimensional 239–304. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/eat.10257
Measure of Religiousness/Spirituality among college students. The International Tiggemann, M. (2005). Body dissatisfaction and adolescent self-esteem: Prospective
Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 19, 106–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ findings. Body Image, 2, 129–135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.03.
10508610802711194 006
Mattis, J. S., & Jagers, R. J. (2001). A relational framework for the study of reli- Tolaymat, L. D., & Moradi, B. (2011). U.S. Muslim women and body image: Links
giosity and spirituality in the lives of African Americans. Journal of Community among objectification theory constructs and the hijab. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 29, 519–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jcop.1034 Psychology, 58, 383–392. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0023461
Mendelson, B. K., Mendelson, M. J., & White, D. R. (2001). Body-esteem scale for Wildes, J. E., Emery, R. E., & Simons, A. D. (2001). The roles of ethnicity and culture
adolescents and adults. Journal of Personality Assessment, 76, 90–106. http://dx. in the development of eating disturbance and body dissatisfaction: A meta-
doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7601 6 analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 521–551. http://dx.doi.org/10.
Mussap, A. J. (2009a). Acculturation, body image, and eating behaviours in Muslim- 1016/S0272-7358(99)00071-9
Australian women. Health & Place, 15, 532–539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.
healthplace.2008.08.008

View publication stats

You might also like