You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-020-04632-y

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Gap Between Cognition and Adaptive Behavior in Students


with Autism Spectrum Disorder: Implications for Social Anxiety
and the Moderating Effect of Autism Traits
Gil Zukerman1   · Gili Yahav1 · Esther Ben‑Itzchak2

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
The gap between cognitive ability and adaptive behavior has been thought to enhance psychopathology among people with
autism, particularly among those without intellectual disability. We examined this association by exploring the gap between
cognitive understanding of social behavior and socially adaptive behavior, and its impact on social anxiety symptoms,
obsessive–compulsive symptoms, and depressive symptoms, among 53 university students with autism (without intellectual
disability). A higher cognition—social adaptation discrepancy was associated with more social anxiety, but this effect was
moderated by autistic trait (AT) levels; a greater gap was associated with more avoidance symptoms of social anxiety only
among students with high AT. Cognitive flexibility and prosocial behavior may mitigate the effects of AT. Potential implica-
tions and interventions are discussed.

Keywords  Cognitive ability · Adaptive behavior · Autism traits · Social anxiety · Autism

Individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder exhibit In a typically developing population, adaptive skills are
a wide range of cognitive and verbal impairments (Ben- positively associated with cognitive ability (Harrison and
Itzchak et al. 2008, 2014; Boucher 2012; Grzadzinski et al. Oakland 2000; Sparrow et al. 1984). However, among indi-
2013; Lord and Pickles 2006; Tager-Flusberg et al. 2005). viduals with autism a different trajectory has been reported,
Along with these, adaptive behavior, which is essential for indicating a gap between cognitive and adaptive abilities
independent living and includes daily living skills, social in toddlers that increases through adolescence (Bradshaw
skills, and communication abilities (Harrison and Oakland et al. 2019; Volkmar 1987; Volkmar et al. 1993). This gap
2005), is reduced among this population (Lee and Park is more evident among those without intellectual disability
2007; Liss et al. 2001; Matthews et al. 2015; Pugliese et al. (Fenton et al. 2003; Kanne et al. 2011; Myers et al. 2018;
2015). This reduction appears even among individuals with Perry et al. 2009; Pugliese et al. 2015; Salomone et al. 2018;
autism and without intellectual disability (Kraper et al. 2017; Szatmari et al. 2009).
Zukerman et al. 2019a). The associations between adaptive behavior and intel-
lectual ability have theoretical and clinical implications
(Fisher et al. 2016). The two abilities are tapped into differ-
* Gil Zukerman ently—unlike intellectual ability that is measured through
gilzu@ariel.ac.il meticulous examination under controlled conditions (IQ
Gili Yahav testing), adaptive behavior is measured by a series of ques-
giliyhv@gmail.com tions regarding everyday functioning. It has been suggested
Esther Ben‑Itzchak that there is a correlational, but not causational, relation-
benitze@ariel.ac.il ship between intellectual functioning and adaptive behavior
1 (Tassé et al. 2016); although associated, the two constructs
Department of Communication Disorders, School of Health
Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel may be independent and reflect distinct domains of abilities.
2 One of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS,
Bruckner Center for Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Department of Communication Disorders, School of Health Wechsler 2008) subtests, the Comprehension subtest, is
Sciences, Ariel University, Ariel, Israel considered to tap into verbal IQ. The individual is asked

13
Vol.:(0123456789)
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

18 questions about social and other situations that focus on even when controlling for adaptive behavior levels (Gotham
“why we do things” or “how things work” in our society. et al. 2014). Among children with autism (without intel-
The subtest also includes two proverbs and demands intact lectual disability), higher full scale IQ predicted lower
language ability, verbal expression, and flexibility (Kauf- self-perceived social competence, which, in turn, predicted
man and Lichtenberger 2006), as well as executive func- higher levels of depressive symptomatology (Vickerstaff
tion (Arffa 2007; Thoma et al. 2009). Notably, individuals et al. 2006). Recently, a positive association between a full
with ASD (with and without intellectual disability) typically scale IQ—adaptive behavior gap and levels of depression
perform worse on the Comprehension subtest than on other and anxiety was reported among those with autism and with-
WAIS subtests, even those that tap into verbal comprehen- out intellectual disability (Kraper et al. 2017), although this
sion (Goldstein and Saklofaske 2010; Holdnack et al. 2011; finding was not replicated in a recent study of individuals
Koyama et al. 2006; Koyama et al. 2007; Mayes and Cal- with autism including those with intellectual disability (Till-
houn 2003; Nader et al. 2015; Oliveras-Rentas et al. 2012; mann et al. 2019). Although mostly conducted using a full
Rabiee et al. 2019; Wechsler 2003). Indeed, the Comprehen- scale IQ assessment, indicating a broad array of intellec-
sion subtest contains unique features that combine demands tual abilities, this finding may possibly suggest that, among
for language comprehension as well as abstract thinking; this people with autism, the perceived gap between cognitive
may constitute a significant obstacle for people with autism. ability, including understanding of social behavior (Compre-
Due to the social context of its various items, this subtest has hension subtest score) and adaptive behavior (social adaptive
been associated with knowledge, as well as understanding, behavior, in particular) might also lead to the development
of abstract social norms/conventions (Kaufman and Lichten- of depression and anxiety. Other conceptualizations could
berger 2006; Nestor et al. 2010; Suchy and Holdnack 2013) be suggested to account for the association between cogni-
and was conceptualized to tap into social intelligence or tion—adaptive behavior discrepancy and psychopathology.
social competence (Sipps et al. 1987). Among individuals Previous research has suggested that some individuals with
with ASD without intellectual disability, a significant cor- autism tend to conceal their social communication difficul-
relation between performance on this subtest and the ADOS ties by a conscious effort to use learned social communi-
Reciprocal Social Interaction domain (representing social cative behavior (imitations, gestures) while internal social
disability; Lord et al. 1999) was reported, leading the authors communicative understanding is still impaired (Lai et al.
to suggest that performance on this subtest relies, at least 2011). This process, referred to as camouflaging (Hull et al.
in part, on knowledge of community and social norms as 2017), is more evident among women (Lai et al. 2017) and
well as the ability to elaborate a socially related action to was conceptualized as the discrepancy between the person’s
a specific situation (Oliveras-Rentas et al. 2012). However, external behavioral presentation in social-interpersonal
it is possible that due to its unique features, performance of contexts and the person’s internal status (social cognitive
individuals with ASD without intellectual disability on this capacity). It was suggested that a constant effort to mimic
subtest indicates a more cognitive (scholastic) understand- conventional behaviors and “pretend to be neurotypical (nor-
ing of social behavior and less general IQ. This supposition mal)” may lead to exhaustion, elevated anxiety, and elevated
is supported by previous suggestions that high performance depression (Lai et al. 2011, 2017). Accordingly, depression
on this subtest does not necessarily indicate social adjust- and anxiety may be the result of constant effort to conceal
ment and should be corroborated by other measures, such as autistic features by employing socially acceptable behaviors
adaptive behavior inventories, in case of clinical evaluation (and not a direct consequence of elevated awareness of one’s
(Kaufman and Lichtenberger 2006). social disabilities).
Previous ASD research has reported a positive associa- The level of Autism traits could be associated with impor-
tion between full scale IQ scores and measures of anxi- tant capabilities that may impact the ability to cope with a
ety and depression, although verbal IQ was more strongly cognition/adaptive behavior discrepancy and its effects on
associated with this psychopathology (Mayes et al. 2011; one’s mental state and functioning. In neurotypical samples,
Mazurek and Kanne 2010). Research has also suggested that, autism traits were negativity associated with higher order
among individuals with ASD without intellectual disability, cognitive abilities such as cognitive flexibility (i.e., the abil-
higher cognitive ability is associated with more awareness ity to disengage from one situation, orientation, or task and
and social comparison, which may result in more awareness shift to another in order to adapt to a new situation; Albein-
of impairments in social performance and therefore lead Urios et al. 2018; Geurts et al. 2009). Cognitive flexibility
to more anxiety and depression. Hedley and Young (2006) has been reported to be reduced among individuals with
reported that depressive symptoms were indeed associated autism (Granader et al. 2014; Wallace et al. 2016). Accord-
with higher levels of social comparison in this population. ingly, one hypothesis is that this reduction, associated with an
An additional study reported that depression was associated autism-related impairment in executive function (Kenworthy
with greater self-perception of autism-related impairment, et al. 2008), may impair the ability to effectively cope with a

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

cognition/adaptive behavior discrepancy. Various social situ- Methods


ations may require adaptation to different ways of behaving
and those with low cognitive flexibility may stick to few rigid Participants
ways of responding, in spite of intact intellectual understand-
ing. This may lead to more frustration and enhance the devel- The study population included 53 first degree (Bachelor’s)
opment of psychopathology. students diagnosed with ASD) 4 females; Mage = 23.53,
The current study was designed to assess the association SD = 2.81), that were studying in a medium size (15,000
between cognition/adaptive behavior gaps and psychopathol- students) public university. Inclusion criteria for the study
ogy among university students with autism and without intel- involved being a full-time university student, a diagno-
lectual disability. A focus on the adaptation of young adults sis of ASD from a licensed neurologist or psychiatrist,
with autism to post-secondary education has drawn much and a score ≥ 32 on the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ;
attention in recent years, although actual research in this field Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). This score has been associated
is scarce and usually includes small numbers of participants or with clinically significant levels of autistic traits and is
case reports (Gelbar et al. 2014). Even in more recent studies, used as a screening tool for autism spectrum conditions
a moderate sample size (N = 30–50) has been reported (Bol- among the general population (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001).
ourain et al. 2018; Hillier et al. 2018). Additionally, research Participants were attending the university’s “integration
in people with autism in general (and with university students program” that guides students with autism through their
in particular) has encountered a number of methodological academic studies. All students had met the formal aca-
issues; researchers tend to use a cross-sectional, and, in many demic entry requirements of the university department into
cases, a single-case design (Gelbar et al. 2014). Most studies which he/she applied and had been pre-interviewed by the
use a factorial comparison when experimental manipulation university’s integration program to ensure their potential
is tested; however, since people with autism are characterized to adjust to on-campus living. Among the recruited cohort,
by high individual variability in performance, this approach two participants received a marginal score on the AQ [29
may be misleading (Bertollo et al. 2019). Previous research has and 31, respectively—lower than the suggested cutoff by
also suggested that people with autism are more compliant on Baron-Cohen et al. (2001) but higher than 26, the cutoff
both self-report and experimental tasks (Chandler et al. 2019) score suggested by others (Woodbury-Smith et al. 2005)
which can also affect research findings. and accordingly were reassessed using the Social Com-
Among university students with autism and without intel- munication Questionnaire- SCQ (Berument et al. 1999).
lectual disability, high academic demands and on-campus liv- Based on their high scores on the SCQ (19 and 21, respec-
ing may accentuate “gap awareness.” In this study, we were tively), they were included in the study; a score of 15 on
specifically interested to find out whether the gap between the SCQ is the commonly-used cutoff criterion for ASD
cognitive understanding of social behavior (as measured by (Barnard-Brak et al. 2016). Two additional participants
the Comprehension subtest score) and social adaptive behavior were excluded due to low AQ scores. Sixty-six percent
(e.g., everyday behaviors such as initiating and organizing joint of the students in the current sample were affiliated with
activities with a group of friends, listening to a friend talking the “exact sciences” fields of study (e.g., engineering and
about their problems, etc.) would be associated with internal- architecture), 24.5% with humanities, and about 9.5% with
ized psychopathology: anxiety (social anxiety, in particular) life/health sciences or other programs.
and depression. An additional study aim was to examine the
effect of the level of autistic traits on the severity of psycho-
pathological symptoms of this cohort. Based on previous Measures
research, we hypothesized that, among university students, the
discrepancy between cognitive understanding of social behav- Adaptive Behavior Assessment System (ABAS‑II)
ior and adaptive social behavior would be positively associated
with psychopathology indices (anxiety/depression). In addi- The ABAS-II (Harrison and Oakland 2003) provides
tion, we postulated that among students with higher levels of information on skills and adaptive behavior in the areas
autism traits, this association would be more robust. of Communication, Community Use, Functional Academ-
ics, Home Living, Health/Safety, Leisure, Self-Direction,
Social (skills), and Work (for those who work). Scores in
different areas are presented as norm–referenced scaled
scores (M = 10; SD = 3). A General Adaptive Compos-
ite score (GAC) that taps into all domains of adaptive
behavior is calculated. Additionally, composite scores are

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

produced relating to the three main facets of functioning: clinical and experimental use (Dehm 2010; Lenhard and
Conceptual (CON; Communication, Functional Academ- Lenhard 2014; Wright and Wright 2006).
ics, Self-Direction), Social (SOC; Leisure, Social), and
Practical (PR; Community Use, Home Living, Health/ Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS)
Safety, Self-Care). The composite scores are trans-
formed into a norm-referenced standard score (M = 100, The LSAS assesses social interaction and social perfor-
SD = 15). The adult self-report version was used. High mance anxiety and includes 24 Likert scale items (0–3) that
internal consistency, high test–retest reliability, and high are each rated for the experienced level of fear and avoidance
convergence validity with the Vineland Adaptive Behav- (Beard et al. 2011; Liebowitz 1987). A total score and fear
ior Scale (VABS) has been reported (Harrison and Oak- and avoidance subscale scores are produced (Beard et al.
land 2003). The ABAS-II has been used in several studies 2011). A total score of 35 in the self-report version of the
that examined adaptive behavior among individuals with questionnaire is considered an optimal cutoff point for social
ASD, indicating correlation between the ABAS-II GAC phobia/Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD; DSM-5, APA 2013)
and ASD symptomatology (Kenworthy et al. 2010; Lopata (Rytwinski et al. 2009; Santos et al. 2015; von Glischinski
et al. 2013). Among the current sample, the three ABAS-II et al. 2018). High reliability and validity were reported for
composite scores were in the 16th percentile reflecting low the self-report version used in this study (Baker et al. 2002).
average performance (Harrison and Oakland 2000) and
significantly lower than among a control group of typically Beck Depression Inventory‑II (BDI‑II)
developing students matched for academic field affiliation,
as reported in a previous study (Zukerman et al. 2019a). The BDI-II is a 21-item self-report measure assessing cogni-
tive, affective, and behavioral outcomes of depression (Beck
et al. 1996). The BDI-II is widely used and has high reli-
Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ)
ability and validity (Beck et al. 1996). A cutoff score of 13
was previously suggested to indicate at least mild dysphoria
The AQ is a self-report questionnaire that quantifies autistic
(Dozois et al. 1998).
traits among adults with average IQs (Baron-Cohen et al.
2001). Scores range from 0 to 50; the higher the score, the
Yale‑Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale‑ Second Edition
more autistic traits a person possesses. Participants rate to
(Y‑BOCS‑II)
what extent they agree with statements on a 4-point Likert
scale. Relatively good internal consistency was reported
The Y-BOCS-II measures symptom severity in obses-
(Baron-Cohen et al. 2001). In this study, as in previous stud-
sive–compulsive disorder (OCD) (Goodman et al. 1989).
ies (Golan et al. 2006), the criteria for participation included
The scale yields a total score (range 0–40) as well as sub-
AQ scores ≥ 32 (associated with clinically significant levels
scale scores for obsessions and compulsions (range 0–20
of autistic traits (Baron-Cohen et al. 2001) along with a for-
for each). Scores in the 0–13 range have been postulated to
mal ASD diagnosis.
indicate absent to mild symptoms, while scores of at least
14 to indicate moderate or greater symptomatology (Storch
Comprehension Subtest of the WAIS‑III et al. 2015). Previous research has demonstrated moderate
to high reliability and significant correlations with other
This subtest measures apprehension of conventional rules of scales of clinical symptoms related to OCD (Deacon and
behavior and social norms, and is associated with verbal IQ Abramowitz 2005).
(Wechsler 1997, 2008). Previous research has suggested that
performance on this subtest might be also associated with Procedure
language comprehension, abstract thinking. and executive
function (Arffa 2007). Acceptable split-half and test–retest University Ethics Committee approval was obtained prior
reliabilities were reported (0.84 and 0.81 respectively, Kauf- to study onset. Participants gave signed informed consent
man and Lichtenberger 1999). In the current study, the Com- to participate (including permission to access information
prehension subtest was taken from the WAIS–III, which has regarding their academic program affiliation. The self-
excellent psychometric properties, particularly in terms of its reported version of the ABAS-II, LSAS, BDI-II, and AQ
standardization and reliability (Kaufman and Lichtenberger were completed by the students themselves; a research
1999). Scaled scores for WAIS-III Comprehension subtest assistant was available, as necessary. The Comprehension
were transformed into IQ standard scores (M = 100, SD = 15) subtest of the WAIS-III and the Y-BOCS-II were adminis-
using the Norm Scale Calculator (Lenhard and Lenhard tered by a licensed clinical psychologist. Data was collected
2014) and in accordance with the expected procedure for from all 53 participants for the ABAS-II, LSAS, AQ, and

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Y-BOCS-II, from 47 participants for the WAIS-III Com- adaptive behavior gap (C-SOC) and level of autistic traits
prehension subtest, and from 42 participants for the BDI-II. (high/low AQ index) to the variance in severity of LSAS
(Distress and Avoidance), BDI-II, and Y-BOCS-II scores.
Data Analysis Regressions were conducted according to Dawson (2014).
Age was entered in the first step, the discrepancy between
Analyses were conducted using SPSS version 25 (Ark- Comprehension and social adaptive behavior (C-SOC) and
monk, N.Y.; IBM Corp., 2013). Missing data were com- the index level of autistic traits (high/low AQ index) in the
pleted for the ABAS-II, LSAS, and Y-BOCS-II. Imputation second step, and the interaction term C-SOC * AQ index
was conducted only when ≤ 20% of the data was missing, in the third step. The interaction was entered in a stepwise
used the “expectation maximization” technique, and only method.
when data was missing completely at random, by utilizing In addition, moderation was tested using computational
Little’s “missing completely at random” (MCAR) test. Nor- procedures for probing interaction as described by Hayes
mal distribution for each of the continuous dependent study and Matthes (2009). We investigated whether the associa-
variables was tested for skewness and kurtosis according tion between C-SOC and social anxiety is moderated by
to Kim (2013). A repeated measures analysis of variance autism traits (AQ index below/above median). Additionally,
was used to examine differences for the Comprehension we explored whether the association between C-SOC and
subtest and adaptive behavior domains across all partici- the level of autism traits (AQ score) would be moderated
pants. Pearson correlation analysis was conducted in order by anxiety (social anxiety index- LSAS score below/above
to examine the association of ABAS-II subdomain scores median).
(Conceptual [CON(], Social [SOC(], Practical [PR]) and the
psychopathology measures (LSAS, BDI-II, and Y-BOCS-II
scores). In addition, Pearson correlations were performed on Results
the difference between the WAIS-III Comprehension subtest
score and the scores of each of the three ABAS-II domains Skewness and kurtosis values indicated a normal distri-
of adaptive behavior (the gaps between these scores; there- bution of all continuous dependent variables (Z; p > 0.05;
fore, they are heretofore referred to as C-CON, C-SOC, and Kim 2013). Table 1 presents the characteristics of the study
C-PRA) and psychopathology. We also examined the cor- population. Low average scores on the Comprehension
relation between the AQ score and psychopathology indices subtest ­(19th percentile) as well as on the three domains
(LSAS, Y-BOCS-II, BDI-II). False Discovery Rate analysis
(FDR; Benjamini and Hochberg 1995) was used for correc-
tions of multiple correlations between those variables. Table 1  Characteristics of the study population
In order to study the effects of the gap between Com- Mean (SD) Range N
prehension and social adaptive behavior (C-SOC) and
autistic features (AQ) on psychopathology (LSAS/BDI- Age (years) 23.53 (2.81) 18–34 53
II/Y-BOCS-II), participants were divided according to ABAS
their median levels of: 1. AQ index (AQ < 46 = low AQ,  Conceptual 85.79 (14.43) 57–120 53
n = 26; AQ ≥ 46 = high AQ, n = 27); and 2. WAIS-III  Social 86.06 (16.62) 58–116 53
Comprehension score and social adaptive behavior index  Practical 85.16 (15.91) 48–120 53
gap (C-SOC < 5 = low gap, n = 23; C-COS ≥ 5 = high gap,  WAIS comprehension 87.97 (9.18) 70–120 47
n = 24). A 2 X 2 MANOVA (2 high/low AQ index X 2 high/  WAIS Comprehension—con- 3.70 (14.83) (–30)–37 47
ceptual
low C-SOC index) and follow-up ANOVAs with distress and
 WAIS Comprehension—social 2.81 (17.86) (–36)–48 47
avoidance LSAS subscales scores as dependent variables
 WAIS Comprehension—practi- 4.19 (16.50) (–34)–42.00 47
were then conducted. Additional 2 X 2 ANOVAs (2 high/low cal
AQ index X 2 high/low C-SOC index) were conducted with AQ 43.79 (4.50) 29–49 53
either depression level (BDI-II score) or obsessive–com- LSAS 44.64 (23.07) 3–108 53
pulsive symptom level (Y-BOCS-II score) as outcome vari- BDI II 9.59 (8.06) 0–28 42
ables. Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied as nec- YBOCS II 13.67 (5.87) 2–26 53
essary for violations of sphericity, and partial eta square ( 𝜂p2 )
served as an estimate for effect size of the ANOVAs. In all ABAS II Adaptive Behavior Assessment System II, AQ Autism Spec-
trum Quotient, Comprehension Comprehension subtest WAIS (III).
the analyses, Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used to examine
LSAS Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS), BDI II Beck Depres-
group differences. sion Inventory-II, YBOCS II Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive
Four linear regression analyses were conducted in order Scale II
to examine the contribution of the Comprehension/social Scores for the ABAS and Comprehension subtest are standard scores

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

of adaptive behavior (16–18th percentiles) were observed. Low C-SOC


Comparison of standard scores revealed no significant dif- High C-SOC
35

LSAS Avoidance score


ferences between the Comprehension subtest score and
adaptive behavior scores (or between adaptive behavior 30
domain scores themselves) (p = 0.21). The average reported 25
levels of social anxiety were beyond the cutoff point used 20
to differentiate individuals with and without Social Anxiety 15
Disorder—SAD (35), while a subclinical level of depres- 10
sion (BDI-II < 13) and a threshold level of OCD symptoms
5
(Y-BOCS-II < 14) were observed. Low AQ High AQ
A correlation analysis indicted that ABAS-II Social and
Practical composite scores were negatively correlated with
Fig. 1  LSAS Avoidance scores as function of the discrepancy
the LSAS (social anxiety) total score, while C-SOC and between cognition (WAIS-III Comprehension subtest score) and
C-PRA (comprehension Social/Practical behavior gaps) adaptive social behavior (ABAS-II Social composite score) below/
were positively correlated with the LSAS (social anxi- above the median, and autistic features (AQ scores below/above the
ety) total score (Table 2). No significant correlations were median). Standard errors are represented by the error bars attached to
each column. C-SOC Comprehension subtest score of the WAIS-III
observed between the AQ score and any of the psychopa- minus the Social composite score of the Adaptive Behavior Assess-
thology indices (LSAS, Y-BOCS II, BDI-II). ment System II, AQ Autism Spectrum Quotient, LSAS Liebowitz
Next, we compared scores on the psychopathology indi- Social Anxiety Scale
ces (LSAS, BDI-II, Y-BOCS-II) between the groups des-
ignated according to their AQ index (Low/High AQ index)
and C-SOC index (Low/High C-SOC index) using a 2 X 2 (Fig. 1) compared to the Low C-SOC group (F(1, 43) = 7.71,
MANOVA. For the LSAS scores, a significant C-SOC index p = 0.00, 𝜂p2 = 0.15). In addition, a significant interaction of
group effect was noted [Hotelling’s Trace; F(2, 42) = 4.70, the C-SOC index and AQ index was observed [Hotelling’s
p = 0.01, 𝜂p2 = 0.18]. Univariate analysis revealed that the Trace; F(2, 42) = 3.85, p = 0.03, 𝜂p2 = 0.16]. Univariate anal-
High C-SOC group reported a higher level of avoidance ysis indicated that this interaction was significant for the
LSAS Avoidance score (F(1, 41) = 4.50, p = 0.04, 𝜂p2 = 0.09].
Table 2  Pearson correlations between ABAS-II composite scores,
As presented in Fig. 1, among the High AQ group, those
Comprehension-ABAS-II composite scores, and psychopathology with a high C-SOC gap reported greater Avoidance com-
indices pared to those with low C-SOC gap. This pattern was not
LSAS BDI-II Y-BOCS-II found among those with low AQ scores. Since both BDI-II
r (n) r (n) r (n) (depression) and Y-BOCS-II (obsessive–compulsive symp-
toms) scores were at subclinical or subthreshold levels in all
CON ABAS-II − .18 (53) − .01 (42) − .27* (53) the designated groups, we did not compare group differences
SOC ABAS-II − .42**† (53) − .12 (42) − .22 (53) for these psychopathology indices.
PRA ABAS-II − .37*† (53) − .11 (42) − .24* (53) We then searched for predictors of the severity of social
C-CON .19 (47) − .05 (39) .12 (47) anxiety (fear and avoidance subscales), depression, and
C-SOC .40**† (47) − .10 (39) .04 (47) obsessive–compulsive symptoms, using linear regression
C-PRA .35**† (47) − .02 (39) .06 (47) models. Only the model with LSAS Avoidance as a depend-
ABAS-II Adaptive Behavior Assessment System II, BDI-II Beck
ent variable was significant (Table 3) and explained 32% of
Depression Inventory II, Comprehension Comprehension subtest the variance. The C-SOC gap and AQ index scores in the
of the WAIS-III, CON ABAS-II Conceptual composite score of the second step added 24% to the explained variance; however,
Adaptive Behavior Assessment System II, SOC ABAS-II Social com- only the C-SOC gap correlated positively and significantly
posite score of the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System II, PRA
ABAS-II Practical composite score of the Adaptive Behavior Assess-
with severity of LSAS Avoidance. The interaction of C-SOC
ment System II, C-CON Comprehension subtest score of the WAIS- * AQ scores in the third step added 8% to the explained
III minus the Conceptual composite score of the ABAS-II, C-SOC variance and was the only significant factor; among partici-
Comprehension subtest score of the WAIS-III minus the Social com- pants with a high AQ index, C-SOC levels predicted LSAS
posite score of the ABAS-II, C-PRA Comprehension subtest score
of the WAIS-III minus the Practical composite score of the ABAS-
Avoidance symptoms, while among those with a low AQ
II, LSAS Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale, Y-BOCS-II Yale-Brown index, this pattern was less robust. Using Hayes and Matthes
Obsessive Compulsive Scale II (2009) procedure, a significant moderation of autism traits
*p ≤ 0.05 on the association between C-SOC and avoidance symptoms
**p ≤ 0.01 of social anxiety (LSAS avoidance) was observed (p = 0.03).

 Survived FDR correction q < 0.05 However, the level of social anxiety (avoidance symptoms)

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Table 3  Linear regression Step Variable B SE β p R2 p R2Δ p


analysis for social anxiety
avoidance symptoms (LSAS 1 Age .18 .56 .04 .75 .00
Avoidance score, n = 47)
2 Age .20 .50 .05 .69 .24** .00 .24** .00
C-SOC 5.43** 1.48 .49 .00
AQind − 1.64 2.96 − .08 .58
3 Age .12 .48 .03 .80 .32** .00 .08* .03
C-SOC 1.77 2.21 .16 .43
AQind − 1.44 2.84 − .06 .61
C-SOC*AQind 6.30* 2.90 .43 .036

AQind Autism Spectrum Quotient by median (AQ < 46, AQ ≥ 46), C-SOC Comprehension subtest score
of the WAIS-III minus the Social composite score of the Adaptive Behavior Assessment System II; LSAS
Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale
*p ≤ 0.05 **p ≤ 0.01

did not moderate the association between S-COC and AQ family members that need to speak about their problems,"
score (p = 0.43). and "Shows sympathy when others are sad or depressed."
In contrast, the LSAS, which measures social anxiety, uses
many items that tap into proactive social behavior that might
Discussion lead to rejection or conflict such as "To express disagree-
ment with someone," "To appear in front of an audience,"
Among a cohort of university students with autism and and "To initiate a meeting with someone for romantic or
without intellectual disability, performance on the WAIS- sexual purposes." Accordingly, one possible interpretation
III Comprehension subtest (representing, among other con- of these findings is that awareness or acknowledgement of
structs, cognitive understanding of social behavior) was one’s failure to translate academic understanding of social
at the low-average range and comparable to the scores in behavior into everyday conventional social functioning
all domains of self-reported adaptive behavior (ABAS-II). leads to restriction of proactive social behaviors that might
Importantly, while previous research reported an average elicit negative reactions or rejection. Social anxiety, which
full scale IQ among autistic individuals without intellectual includes concerns about potential negative evaluation and
disability (Chiang et al. 2014; Kraper et al. 2017) that might scrutiny by others (DSM 5, APA 2013), has been reported as
relate to a broad array of intellectual abilities; our findings, the most frequently occurring anxiety disorder among adults
indicating lower than average performance on the Compre- and children with autism and without intellectual disability
hension subtest (which is associated more with cognitive (Bellini 2004; Cholemkery et al. 2014; Kuusikko et al. 2008;
understanding of social behavior than with IQ), are cor- Spain et al. 2016).
roborated by previous reports of impaired performance on Interestingly, although all the students in our study exhib-
this subtest among people with autism without intellectual ited clinically significant levels of autistic traits, the positive
disability (Goldstein and Saklofaske 2010; Holdnack et al. association between the cognition-adaptive behavior gap and
2011). The below average performance may be explained severity of social anxiety symptoms was evident only among
by the demands for intact executive function (problem solv- those with higher (above median) levels of autistic features.
ing, judgment) that completion of the comprehension subtest Several explanations can be suggested for this novel finding.
imposes. In the current study, the discrepancy between this One possible explanation is that relatively high/low autistic
subtest score (cognitive understanding of social behavior) traits are associated with different ways of coping with a
and functional social adaptive social behavior was positively cognition/adaptive behavior discrepancy among people with
associated with avoidance of social situations. This finding autism without intellectual disabilities. This difference in
corroborates our first hypothesis, albeit confined to only one coping might be related to cognitive flexibility, defined as
area of psychopathology—social anxiety. the ability to disengage from one task and to shift to another
A closer look at the instruments used to measure social in order to adapt to a new situation (Geurts et al. 2009). Cog-
function and anxiety may be useful to further understand nitive flexibility may be associated with social adaptation by
the findings. In the current study, we used the ABAS-II to enabling implementation of different behaviors depending
measure social functioning. Many of its items assess passive on the social context, and consequently leading to better
or receptive social behavior such as "Avoids saying things adjustment. Cognitive flexibility has also been negatively
that might hurt or embarrass others," "Listens to friends/ associated with autistic features (Albein-Urios et al. 2018).

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Thus, one postulation is that, among autistic people without these possible explanations. Moreover, it is important to note
intellectual disability and relatively elevated autistic traits that although we divided the study population into those
(including cognitive inflexibility), their intact academic with high and low levels of autistic traits, participants in
understanding of social behavior combined with awareness both groups reported high (> 29) AQ scores. Nevertheless,
of their social dysfunctionality may activate a rigid scenario according to the AQ norms for individuals with ASD estab-
that includes anticipation of rejection and scrutiny from oth- lished by Baron-Cohen et al. (2001; M = 35.08, SD = 6.5),
ers, and consequently lead to avoidance of social interac- the two groups’ standard scores represent the 7­ 4th and the
tions. Those with lesser autistic features (and, presumably, ­96th percentile. We believe that such significant differences
greater cognitive flexibility) might be able to conceive of in the levels of autistic traits (AQ score) may be expressed
more alternatives (negative as well as positive) for manag- in different ways of coping and, consequently, effect differ-
ing a social situation, in spite of their social skill inferior- ently the development of internalized psychopathology. This
ity. These hypotheses are corroborated by previous research possibility should be taken into consideration when devising
findings indicating a significant association between cogni- university programs for integration of young students with
tive inflexibility and anxiety among individuals with autism autism and without intellectual disability.
(Lawson et al. 2015; Wallace et al. 2016). The current study is novel in its attempt to understand the
Another possible explanation, derived from the social mental processes of young students with autism and with-
motivation hypothesis, postulates that high levels of autistic out intellectual disability who are acclimating to independ-
traits may lead to a reduction in positive rewards associated ent living and to campus life. Unlike previous research that
with social behavior and social interactions (Chevallier et al. assessed full scale IQ and overall adaptive skills (Kraper
2012). Previous research has demonstrated that high levels et al. 2017), the current study measured the gap between
of autistic traits attenuated the extent to which reward learn- cognitive understanding of social behavior (utilizing a spe-
ing for social stimuli (i.e., the association between reward cific IQ subtest—Comprehension) and real-world socially
and social stimuli) is transferred into prosocial behav- adaptive skills. We also used standardized instruments that
ior (Panasiti et al. 2016). Other researchers have reported tap into socially adaptive behavior. Previously, it has been
reduced activation of brain reward networks among people suggested that intact self-awareness among people with
with autism (Cox et al. 2015; Gossen et al. 2014). Among autism without intellectual disability leads to focusing on
neurotypical individuals in academic settings, more social the oftentimes frustrating gap between cognition and adap-
interaction and broader social networks has been associ- tive ability, and therefore to more depression and anxiety
ated with greater psychological gains (social and emotional (Hedley and Young 2006; Vickerstaff et al. 2007). In the cur-
support) and academic gains (participating in collaborative rent sample, the levels of depression and obsessive–compul-
learning) (Blansky et al. 2013; Crosnoe et al. 2003; Goguen sive symptoms were in the sub-clinical range, suggesting no
et al. 2010) leading to better academic achievements (Brook significant distress relating to these comorbidities. Instead,
and Willoughby 2015). However, such rewarding outcomes the identified cognition/adaptive skill gaps were associated
may be less stimulating for people with autism, a population with avoidance, but not distress, symptoms of social anxiety.
that is also characterized by diminished interest in collabora- Thus, our finding does not support the hypothesis that such
tive activities, social anhedonia, and more restricted social gaps are associated with generally elevated internalized psy-
networks, leading, possibly, to lower academic achievements chopathology; rather, it suggests a more specific effect, cen-
(Chevallier et al. 2012; van Asselt-Goverts et al. 2015; Zuk- tered around the anxiety elicited by actual social situations.
erman et al. 2019b). Accordingly, it is possible that, among The current study has important potential clinical impli-
students with greater autistic features, social interactions are cations. The high prevalence of anxiety disorders among
not perceived as likely to achieve favorable outcomes, and a people with autism (estimated to be as high as 50%; Spain
larger gap between cognitive understanding of social behav- et al. 2018) requires scientific exploration aimed at under-
ior and actual social behavior elicits more social avoidance. standing its genesis, course, and treatment (including con-
In contrast, among students with relatively lesser autistic struction of intervention programs). Recently, it has been
features, more social motivation and, possibly more antici- asserted that the impact of a pre-existing impairment in
pation of positive outcomes associated with social interac- the ability to behave in a flexible manner, among people
tion, may lead to less avoidance of social situations, despite with autism, may be magnified by an inherent tendency
awareness of social skill disparity. Another possibility is to avoid anxiety-provoking situations such as social inter-
that among those with high levels of autistic features, the actions (Rodgers and Ofield 2018). The current findings,
discrepancy between cognitive understanding of social indicating that the highest levels of social avoidance were
behavior and actual adaptive behavior leads to more pressure observed among those with more autistic features (and,
to camouflage one’s behavior and thus, to more stress and perhaps, less cognitive flexibility), also suggest a recipro-
avoidance (Lai et al. 2017). Further studies should examine cal association between those factors. Previous research

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

has demonstrated the efficacy of Cognitive Behavioral underlying the association between autism and social avoid-
Therapy (CBT) in reduction of anxiety among individuals ance and should be explored in future research.
with ASD (Ung et al. 2015), especially when such inter-
vention incorporates strategies for improving social inter-
actions, such as enhancing social skills, theory of mind, Author Contributions  GZ and EBY and GY planned and
and the ability to identify emotions in others (Attwood conceived the study. GY conducted the application of ques-
2004; Luxford et al. 2016). Although we did not directly tioners to the study participants. GZ and EBYconducted the
measure cognitive flexibility, one possible suggestion, statistical analysis as well as reviewed the paper. All authors
considering the current findings, is that teaching students have approved the current version of the manuscript.
with autism and without intellectual disability to think
about social situations in a more flexible and diverse man-
ner may reduce avoidance and facilitate better integration
into the social settings of post-secondary education. A
possible line of intervention among this population would
be to increase awareness of viable options to successfully
manage interactions with, and appropriately respond to, References
others, within social situations, as is frequently done when
implementing CBT (Niles et al. 2014). Albein-Urios, N., Youssef, G. J., Kirkovski, M., & Enticott, P. J.
(2018). Autism spectrum traits linked with reduced performance
Notwithstanding its strengths, the study findings are on self-report behavioural measures of cognitive flexibility. Jour-
limited by the fact that the indices of measuring psycho- nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(7), 2506–2515.
pathology (social anxiety, depressive symptoms, obsessive American Psychiatric Association (APA). (2013American). Diagnostic
compulsive symptoms), and adaptive behavior are based on and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington,
DC: American Psychiatric Association.
self-report questionnaires. Moreover, the Comprehension Arffa, S. (2007). The relationship of intelligence to executive function
subtest, conceptualized in the current study to tap into the and non-executive function measures in a sample of average,
understanding of social behavior, is also associated with above average, and gifted youth. Archives of Clinical Neuropsy-
other important abilities such as language comprehension, chology, 22, 969–978.
Attwood, T. (2004). Cognitive behaviour therapy for children and
abstract thinking, and executive function; this may have adults with Asperger’s syndrome. Behaviour Change, 21(3),
impacted our findings. Additionally, the male to female ratio 147–161. https​://doi.org/10.1375/bech.21.3.147.55995​.
in the current study (4 females of 53 participants) is lower Baker, S. L., Heinrichs, N., Kim, H. J., & Hofmann, S. G. (2002).
than the typically estimated prevalence ratio of 4:1 (Fom- The Liebowitz social anxiety scale as a self-report instrument:
A preliminary psychometric analysis. Behaviour Research and
bonne et al. 2011). Several studies have shown that ratio of Therapy, 40(6), 701–715.
males to females is higher among autistic people without Barnard-Brak, L., Brewer, A., Chesnut, S., Richman, D., & Schaeffer,
intellectual ability (Scott et al. 2002; Lai et al. 2015), but A. M. (2016). The sensitivity and specificity of the Social Com-
this was not supported in other studies (Idring et al. 2012; munication Questionnaire for autism spectrum with respect to
age. Autism Research, 9, 838–845.
Mattila et al. 2011). Accordingly, our finding might be more Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley,
applicable to men than to women. Future studies should E. (2001). The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from
attempt to use a more balanced male/female sample. Finally, Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females,
the relatively modest sample size of our cohort, although scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Develop-
mental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.
typical of similar studies of this subpopulation, recommends Beard, C., Rodriguez, B. F., Moitra, E., Sibrava, N. J., Bjornsson, A.,
caution when interpreting the current finding. Eventually, Weisberg, R. B., et al. (2011). Psychometric properties of the
the participants did not undergo a full psychological assess- Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale (LSAS) in a longitudinal study
ment. Future research should attempt to address these issues. of african Americans with anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders, 25(5), 722–726.
The current study was a pioneering attempt to exam- Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Brown, G. K. (1996). Manual for the
ine adaption to university life among young autistic adults Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological
without intellectual disability. As such, the study findings Corporation.
provide a platform for new hypotheses to understand the Bellini, S. (2004). Social skill deficits and anxiety in high-functioning
adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Focus Autism Dev
relationship between gaps in cognitive and adaptive abilities Dis, 19(2), 78–86. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10883​57604​01900​
and the adaptation of autistic students without intellectual 20201​.
disability to everyday functioning, particularly in the social Ben-Itzchak, E., Lahat, E., Burgin, R., & Zachor, A. D. (2008). Cogni-
domain. The current findings suggest that a cognitive/behav- tive, behavior and intervention outcome in young children with
autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29(5), 447–458.
ioral discrepancy leads to increased social avoidance among https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2007.08.003.
those with more autistic features. Cognitive flexibility and Ben-Itzchak, E., Watson, L. R., & Zachor, D. A. (2014). Cogni-
prosocial behavior have been suggested as possible factors tive ability is associated with different outcome trajectories in

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Develop- anxiety disorders. Behavior Therapy, 36(2), 139–145. https​://
mental Disorders, 44(9), 2221–2229. https​://doi.org/10.1007/ doi.org/10.1016/S0005​-7894(05)80062​-0.
s1080​3-014-2091-0. Dehm, M. (2010). Long term memory problems in children and ado-
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discov- lescents: Assessment, intervention and effective instruction.
ery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B (Methodologi- Dozois, D. J. A., Dobson, K. S., & Ahnberg, J. L. (1998). A psy-
cal), 57(1), 289–300. chometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory–
Bertollo, J. R., Strang, J. F., Anthony, L. G., Kenworthy, L., Wallace, II. Psychological Assessment, 10(2), 83–89. https ​ : //doi.
G. L., & Yerys, B. E. (2019). Adaptive behavior in youth with org/10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.83.
autism spectrum disorder: The role of flexibility. Journal of Fenton, G., D’Ardia, C., Valente, D., Del Vecchio, I., Fabrizi, A., &
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50, 42–50. https​://doi. Bernabei, P. (2003). Vineland adaptive behavior profiles in chil-
org/10.1007/s1080​3-019-04220​-9. dren with autism and moderate to severe developmental delay.
Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., & Bailey, A. Autism, 7(3), 269–287.
(1999). Autism screening questionnaire: Diagnostic validity. Fisher, M. H., Lense, M. D., & Dykens, E. M. (2016). Longitudinal
British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 444–451. trajectories of intellectual and adaptive functioning in adoles-
Blansky, D., Kavanaugh, C., Boothroyd, C., Benson, B., Gallagher, cents and adults with Williams syndrome. Journal of Intellectual
J., Endress, J., et al. (2013). Spread of academic success in a Disability Research, 60(10), 920–932.
high school social network. PLoS ONE, 8(2), e55944. https​:// Fombonne, E., Quirke, S., & Hagen, A. (2011). Epidemiology of perva-
doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.00559​44. sive developmental disorders. In D. G. Amaral, G. Dawson, & D.
Bolourian, Y., Zeedyk, S. M., & Blacher, J. (2018). Autism and the H. Geschwind (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders (pp. 90–111).
university experience: Narratives from students with neurode- New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
velopmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmen- Gelbar, N. W., Smith, I., & Reichow, B. (2014). Systematic review of
tal Disorders, 48, 3330–3343. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​ articles describing experience and supports of individuals with
3-018-3599-5. autism enrolled in college and university programs. Journal of
Boucher, J. (2012). Research review: Structural language in autis- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(10), 2593–2601. https​
tic spectrum disorder—Characteristics and causes. Journal of ://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-014-2135-5.
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(3), 219–233. https​://doi. Geurts, H. M., Corbett, B., & Solomon, M. (2009). The paradox of cog-
org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02508​.x. nitive flexibility in autism. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(2),
Bradshaw, J., Gillespie, S., Klaiman, C., Klin, A., & Saulnier, C. 74–82.
(2019). Early emergence of discrepancy in adaptive behavior Goguen, L. M. S., Hiester, M. A., & Nordstrom, A. H. (2010). Asso-
and cognitive skills in toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. ciations among Peer relationships, academic achievement, and
Autism, 23(6), 1485–1496. persistence in college. Journal of College Student Retention:
Brook, C. A., & Willoughby, T. (2015). The social ties that bind: Research, Theory & Practice, 12(3), 319–337.
Social anxiety and academic achievement across the university Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., Hill, J. J., & Golan, Y. (2006). The "read-
years. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 44(5), 1139–1152. ing the mind in films" task: Complex emotion recognition in
Chandler, R. J., Russell, A., & Maras, K. L. (2019). Compliance in adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. Social Neu-
autism: Self-report in action. Autism, 23(4), 1005–1017. https​ roscience, 1(2), 111–123. https​://doi.org/10.1080/17470​91060​
://doi.org/10.1177/13623​61318​79547​9. 09809​86.
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. Goldstein, G., & Saklofske, D. H. (2010). The Wechsler Intelligence
T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Scales in the assessment of psychopathology. In L. G. Weiss, D.
Cognitive Neurosciences, 16(4), 231–239. H. Saklofske, D. L. Coalson, & S. E. Raiford (Eds.), WAIS-IV
Chiang, H., Tsai, L. Y., Cheung, Y. K., Brown, A., & Li, H. (2014). A clinical use and interpretation practical resources for the mental
meta-analysis of differences in IQ profiles between individuals health professional (pp. 189–216). Oxford: Elsevier Academic
with Asperger’s disorder and high-functioning autism. Journal Press.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1577–1596. Goodman, W. K., Price, L. H., Rasmussen, S. A., Mazure, C., Delgado,
Cholemkery, H., Mojica, L., Rohrmann, S., Gensthaler, A., & Fre- P., et al. (1989). The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale
itag, C. M. (2014). Can autism spectrum disorders and social II. Validity. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 1012–1016.
anxiety disorders be differentiated by the social responsive- https​://doi.org/10.1001/archp​syc.1989.01810​11005​4008.
ness scale in children and adolescents? Journal of Autism Gossen, A., Groppe, S. E., Winkler, L., Kohls, G., Herrington, J.,
and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 1168–1182. https​://doi. Schultz, R. T., et al. (2014). Neural evidence for an associa-
org/10.1007/s1080​3-013-1979-4. tion between social proficiency and sensitivity to social reward.
Cox, A., Kohls, G., Naples, A. J., Mukerji, C. E., Coffman, M. C., Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(5), 661–670.
Rutherford, H. J. V., et al. (2015). Diminished social reward https​://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst03​3.
anticipation in the broad autism phenotype as revealed by Gotham, K., Bishop, S. L., Brunwasser, S., & Lord, C. (2014). Rumina-
event-related brain potentials. Social Cognitive and Affective tion and perceived impairment associated with depressive symp-
Neuroscience, 10(10), 1357–1364. https​: //doi.org/10.1093/ toms in a verbal adolescent-adult ASD sample. Autism Research,
scan/nsv02​4. 7(3), 381–391. https​://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1377.
Crosnoe, R., Cavanagh, S., & Elder, G. (2003). Adolescent friendships Granader, Y., Wallace, G. L., Hardy, K. K., Yerys, B. E., Lawson, R.
as academic resources: The intersection of friendship, race, and A., Rosenthal, M., et al. (2014). Characterizing the factor struc-
school disadvantage. Sociological Perspectives, 46(3), 331–352. ture of parent reported executive function in autism spectrum
https​://doi.org/10.1525/sop.2003.46.3.331. disorders: The impact of cognitive inflexibility. Journal of Autism
Dawson, J. F. (2014). Moderation in management research: What, and Developmental Disorders, 44(12), 3056–3062.
why, when, and how. Journal of Business and Psychology, 29(1), Grzadzinski, R., Huerta, M., & Lord, C. (2013). DSM-5 and autism
1–19. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1086​9-013-9308-7. spectrum disorders (ASDs): An opportunity for identify-
Deacon, B. J., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2005). Patients’ perceptions ing ASD subtypes. Molecular Autism, 4(1), 12. https​://doi.
of pharmacological and cognitive-behavioral treatment for org/10.1186/2040-2392-4-12.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2000). Adaptive behavior assessment high-functioning children and adolescents with Autism and
system. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2003). Adaptive behavior assessment Disorders, 38(9), 1697–1709. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​
system—2. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. 3-008-0555-9.
Harrison, P. L., & Oakland, T. (2005). Adaptive behaviour assess- Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Auyeung, B., Chakrabarti, B., & Baron-
ment system—Third edition manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Cohen, S. (2015). Sex/gender differences and autism: Setting the
Assessment. scene for future research. Journal of the American Academy of
Hayes, A. F., & Matthes, J. (2009). Computational procedures for prob- Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(1), 11–24.
ing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N., Chakrabarti,
implementations. Behavior Research Methods, 41(3), 924–936. B., Auyeung, B., et al. (2017). Quantifying and exploring camou-
Hedley, D., & Young, R. (2006). Social comparison processes and flaging in men and women with autism. Autism, 21(6), 690–702.
depressive symptoms in children and adolescents with asperger https​://doi.org/10.1177/13623​61316​67101​2.
syndrome. Autism, 10(2), 139–153. Lai, M. C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N., Wheelwright, S. J.,
Hillier, A., Goldstein, J., Murphy, D., Trietsch, R., Keeves, J., Mendes, Sadek, S. A., et al. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male
E., et al. (2018). Supporting university students with autism spec- and female adults with high functioning autism spectrum con-
trum disorder. Autism, 22(1), 20–28. ditions. PLoS ONE, 6(6), e20835. https​://doi.org/10.1371/journ​
Holdnack, J., Goldstein, G., & Drozdick, L. (2011). Social percep- al.pone.00208​35.
tion and WAIS-IV performance in adolescents and adults diag- Lawson, R. A., Papadakis, A. A., Higginson, C. I., Barnett, J. E., Wills,
nosed with Asperger’s syndrome and autism. Assessment, 18(2), M. C., Strang, J. F., et al. (2015). Everyday executive function
192–200. impairments predict comorbid psychopathology in autism spec-
Hull, L., Mandy, W., & Petrides, K. V. (2017). Behavioural and cog- trum and attention deficit hyperactivity disorders. Neuropsychol-
nitive sex/gender differences in autism spectrum condition and ogy, 29(3), 445–453.
typically developing males and females. Autism, 21(6), 706–727. Lee, H. J., & Park, H. R. (2007). An integrated literature Review on
https​://doi.org/10.1177/13623​61316​66908​7. the adaptive behavior of individuals with Asperger syndrome.
Idring, S., Rai, D., Dal, H., Dalman, C., Sturm, H., Zander, E., et al. Remedial and Special Education, 28(3), 132–139. https​://doi.
(2012). Autism spectrum disorders in the Stockholm youth org/10.1177/07419​32507​02800​30201​.
cohort: Design prevalence and Validity.. PLoS ONE, 7(7), Lenhard, W., & Lenhard, A. (2014). Norm scale calculator. Retrieved
e41280. https​://doi.org/10.1371/journ​al.pone.00412​80. from https​://www.psych​ometr​ica.de/normw​ertre​chner​_en.html.
Kanne, S. M., Gerber, A. J., Quirmbach, L. M., Sparrow, S. S., Cic- Bibergau: Psychometrica
chetti, D. V., & Saulnier, C. A. (2011). The role of adaptive Liebowitz, M. R. (1987). Social phobia. Modern Problems of Pharma-
behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for func- copsychiatry, 22, 141–173.
tional outcome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Feinstein, C., et al.
41(8), 1007–1018. (2001). Predictors and correlates of adaptive functioning in
Kaufman, A. S., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (1999). Essentials of WAIS-III children with developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and
assessment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Developmental Disorders, 31(2), 219–230.
Kaufman, A. S., & Lichtenberger, E. O. (2006). Assessing adolescent Lopata, C., Smith, R. A., Volker, M. A., Thomeer, M. L., Lee, G. K.,
and adult intelligence (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. & McDonald, C. A. (2013). Comparison of adaptive behavior
Kenworthy, L., Case, L., Harms, M. B., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. measures for children with HFASDs. Autism Research and Treat-
(2010). Adaptive behavior ratings correlate with symptomatol- ment. https​://doi.org/10.1155/2013/41598​9.
ogy and iq among individuals with high-functioning autism spec- Lord, C., & Pickles, A. (2006). Autism from 2 to 9 years of age.
trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(6), 694–701. https​://doi.
40(4), 416–423. org/10.1001/archp​syc.63.6.694.
Kenworthy, L., Yerys, B. E., Gutermuth-Anthony, L., & Wallace, G. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P. C., & Risi, S. (1999). Autism diag-
L. (2008). Understanding executive control in autism spectrum nostic observation schedule-WPS (ADOS-WPS). Los Angeles:
disorders in the lab and in the real world. Neuropsychological Western Psychological Services.
Review, 18(4), 320–338. Luxford, S., Hadwin, J. A., & Kovshoff, H. (2016). Evaluating the
Kim, H. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing nor- effectiveness of a school-based cognitive behavioural therapy
mal distribution using skewness and kurtosis. Restorative Den- intervention for anxiety in adolescents diagnosed with autism
tistry and Endodontics, 38(1), 52–54. https​://doi.org/10.5395/ spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis-
rde.2013.38.1.52. orders, 47(12), 3896–3908. https ​ : //doi.org/10.1007/s1080​
Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., & Kurita, H. (2006). Cogni- 3-016-2857-7.
tive and symptom profiles in high-functioning pervasive devel- Matthews, N. L., Smith, C. J., Pollard, E., Ober-Reynolds, S., Kir-
opmental disorder not otherwise specified and attention-deficit/ wan, J., & Malligo, A. (2015). Adaptive functioning in autism
hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental spectrum disorder during the transition to adulthood. Journal of
Disorders, 36(3), 373–380. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(8), 2349–2360. https​
Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., Takeda, T., & Kurita, H. (2007). ://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-015-2400-2.
Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and Mattila, M. J., Kielinen, M., Linna, S. L., Jussila, K., Ebeling, H.,
high-functioning autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, Bloigu, R., et al. (2011). Autism spectrum disorders according
61(1), 99–104. to DSM-IV-TR and comparison with DSM-5 Draft Criteria: An
Kraper, C. K., Kenworthy, L., Popal, H., Martin, A., & Wallace, G. L. epidemiological study. Journal of the American Academy of
(2017). The gap between adaptive behavior and intelligence in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(6), 583–592. https​://doi.
autism persists into young adulthood and is linked to psychiatrics org/10.1016/j.jaac.2011.04.001.
co-morbidities. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Mayes, S. D., & Calhoun, S. L. (2003). Analysis of WISC-III, Stanford-
47(10), 3007–3017. https:​ //doi.org/10.1007/s10803​ -017-3213-2. Binet:IV, and academic achievement test scores in children with
Kuusikko, S., Pollock-Wurman, R., Jussila, K., Carter, A. S., Mat- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3),
tila, M. L., Ebeling, H., et  al. (2008). Social anxiety in 329–341.

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Mayes, S. D., Calhoun, S. L., Murray, M. J., & Zahid, J. (2011). Vari- Santos, L. F., Loureiro, S. R., Crippa, J. A. S., & Osório, F. L. (2015).
ables associated with anxiety and depression in children with Can the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale—Self-report version be
autism. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, used to differentiate clinical and non-clinical SAD Groups among
23, 325–337. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1088​2-011-9231-7. brazilians? PLoS ONE, 10(3), e0121437.
Mazurek, M. O., & Kanne, S. M. (2010). Friendship and internal- Sipps, G. J., Berry, G. W., & Lynch, E. M. (1987). WAIS-R and social
izing symptoms among children and adolescents With ASD. intelligence: A test of established assumptions that uses the CPI.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(12), Journal of Clinical Psychology, 43(5), 499–504.
1512–1520. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s1080​3-010-1014-y. Scott, F. J., Baron- Cohen, S., Bolton, P., & Brayne, C. (2002). Brief
Myers, E., Stone, W. L., Bernier, R., Lendvay, T., Comstock, B., & report: Prevalence of autism spectrum conditions in children
Cowan, C. (2018). The diagnosis conundrum: Comparison of aged 5–11 years in Cambridgeshire UK. Autism, 6(231), 237.
crowdsourced and expert assessments of toddlers with high Spain, D., Happé, F., Johnston, P., Campbell, M., Sin, J., Daly, E.,
and low risk of autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, et al. (2016). Social anxiety in adult males with autism spectrum
11(12), 1629–1634. disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 32, 13–23.
Nader, A. M., Jelenic, P., & Soulieres, I. (2015). Discrepancy https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2016.08.002.
between WISC-III and WISC-IV cognitive profile in autism Spain, D., Sin, J., Linder, K. B., McMahon, J., & Happé, F. (2018).
spectrum: What does it reveal about autistic cognition? Social anxiety in autism spectrum disorder. A systematic review.
PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0144645. https​://doi.org/10.1371/journ​ Research in Autism Spectrum Disorder, 52, 51–68.
al.pone.01446​45. Sparrow, S., Balla, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). Vineland adaptive
Nestor, P., Niznikiewicz, M., & McCarley, R. W. (2010). Distinct con- behavior scales. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.
tribution of working memory and social comprehension failures Storch, E. A., De Nadai, A. S., do Rosário, M. C., Shavitt, R. G., Tor-
in neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia. The Journal res, A. R., Ferrão, Y. A., et al. (2015). Defining clinical severity
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 198(3), 206–212. https​://doi. in adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive
org/10.1097/NMD.0b013​e3181​d1414​3. Psychiatry, 63, 30–35.
Niles, A. N., Burklund, L. J., Arch, J. J., Lieberman, M. D., Saxbe, D., Suchy, Y., & Holdnack, J. A. (2013). Assessing Social Cognition Using
& Craske, M. G. (2014). Cognitive mediators of treatment for the ACS for WAIS–IV and WMS–IV. In J. A. Holdnack, L. W.
social anxiety disorder: Comparing acceptance and commitment Drozdick, L. G. Weiss, & G. L. Iverson (Eds.), WAIS-IV, WMS-
therapy and cognitive-behavioral therapy. Behavior Therapy, IV, and ACS advanced clinical interpretation (pp. 367–406).
45(5), 664–677. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.04.006. Oxford: Elsevier Academic Press.
Oliveras-Rentas, R. E., Kenworthy, L., Roberson, R. B., Martin, A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., Duku, E., Vaccarella, L., Zwaigenbaum,
& Wallace, G. L. (2012). WISC-IV profile in high-functioning L., Bennett, T., et al. (2009). Similar developmental trajecto-
autism spectrum disorders: Impaired processing speed is asso- ries in autism and Asperger syndrome: From early childhood to
ciated with increased autism communication symptoms and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and
decreased adaptive communication abilities. Journal of Autism Allied Disciplines, 50(12), 1459–1467. https​://doi.org/10.111
and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 655–664. 1/j.1469-7610.2009.02123​.x.
Panasiti, M. S., Puzzo, I., & Chakrabarti, B. (2016). Autistic traits mod- Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. E. (2005). Language and com-
erate the impact of reward learning on social behaviour. Autism munication in autism. In F. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. J.
Research, 9(4), 471–479. https​://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1523. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental
Perry, A., Flanagan, H. E., Geier, J. D., & Freeman, N. L. (2009). disorders: Diagnosis, development, neurobiology, and behavior
Brief report: The Vineland adaptive behavior scales in young (pp. 335–364). New York: Wiley.
children with autism spectrum disorders at different cognitive Tassé, M. J., Luckasson, R., & Schalock, R. L. (2016). The relation
levels. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), between intellectual functioning and adaptive behavior in the
1066–1078. diagnosis of intellectual disability. Intellectual and Developmen-
Pugliese, C. E., Anthony, L., Strang, J. F., Dudley, K., Wallace, G. tal Disabilities, 54(6), 381–390.
L., & Kenworthy, L. (2015). Increasing adaptive behavior skill Thoma, P., Huhnecker, M., Mandok, T., Wahner, A., Brune, G.,
deficits from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disor- Juckel, G., et al. (2009). Proverb comprehension impairments in
der: Role of executive function. Journal of Autism and Develop- schizophrenia are related to executive dysfunction. Psychiatry
mental Disorders, 45, 1579–1587. https:​ //doi.org/10.1007/s1080​ Research, 170, 132–179.
3-014-2309-1. Tillmann, J., Cáceres, A. S. J., Chatham, C. H., Crawley, D., Holt, R.,
Rabiee, A., Samadi, S. A., Vasaghi-Gharamaleki, B., Hosseini, S., Oakley, B., et al. (2019). Investigating the factors underlying
Seyedin, S., Keyhani, M., et al. (2019). The cognitive profile adaptive functioning in autism in the EU-AIMS Longitudinal
of people with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. European Autism Project. Autism Research, 12(4), 645–657.
Behavioral Sciences (Basel, Switzerland), 9(2), 20. https​://doi. https​://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2081.
org/10.3390/bs902​0020. Ung, D., Selles, R., Small, B. J., & Storch, E. A. (2015). A system-
Rodgers, J., & Ofield, A. (2018). Understanding, recognising and treat- atic review and meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioral therapy
ing co-occurring anxiety in autism. Current Developmental Dis- for anxiety in youth with high-functioning autism spectrum
orders Reports, 5, 58–64. disorders. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 46(4),
Rytwinski, N. K., Fresco, D. M., Heimberg, R. G., Coles, M. E., 533–547.
Liebowitz, M. R., Cissell, S., et al. (2009). Screening for social van Asselt-Goverts, A. E., Embregts, P. J., Hendriks, A. H., Weg-
anxiety disorder with the self-report version of the Liebowitz man, K. M., & Teunisse, J. P. (2015). Do social networks differ?
Social Anxiety Scale. Depression and Anxiety, 26(1), 34–38. Comparison of the social networks of people with intellectual
https​://doi.org/10.1002/da.20503​. disabilities, people with autism spectrum disorders and other
Salomone, E., Shephard, E., Milosavljevic, B., Johnson, B. H., Char- people living in the community. Journal of Autism and Devel-
man, T., & Team, B. A. S. I. S. (2018). Adaptive behaviour and opmental Disorders, 45(5), 1191–1203. https​://doi.org/10.1007/
cognitive skills: Stability and change from 7 months to 7 years in s1080​3-014-2279-3.
siblings at high familial risk of autism spectrum disorder. Jour- Vickerstaff, S., Heriot, S., Wong, M., Lopes, A., & Dossetor, D.
nal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(9), 2901–2911. (2006). Intellectual ability, self-perceived social competence, and

13
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

depressive symptomatology in children with high-functioning Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-WISC-
autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmen- IV; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio. Pearson Assess-
tal Disorders, 37, 1647–1664. ment: TX.
Vickerstaff, S., Heriot, S., Wong, M., Lopes, A., & Dossetor, D. (2007). Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (4th ed.). San
Intellectual ability, self-perceived social competence, and depres- Antonio, TX: Pearson Assessment.
sive symptomatology in children with high-functioning autistic Wright, P. W. D., & Wright, P. D. (2006). From emotion to advocacy.
spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Dis- Hartfield, VA: Harbor-House Law Press Inc.
orders, 37, 1647–1664. Woodbury-Smith, M. R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen,
Volkmar, F. R. (1987). Diagnostic issues in the pervasive develop- S. (2005). Screening adults for asperger syndrome using the AQ:
mental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, A preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical prac-
28(3), 365–369. tice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(3),
Volkmar, F. R., Carter, A., Sparrow, S. S., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1993). 331–335.
Quantifying social development in autism. Journal of the Ameri- Zukerman, G., Yahav, G., & Ben-Itzchak, E. (2019a). Increased psy-
can Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 32(3), 627–632. chiatric symptoms in university students with autism spectrum
von Glischinski, M., Willutzki, U., Stangier, U., Hiller, W., Hoyer, disorder are associated with reduced adaptive behavior. Psychi-
J., Leibing, E., et al. (2018). Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale atry Research, 273, 732–738. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.psych​
(LSAS): Optimal cut points for remission and response in a Ger- res.2019.01.098.
man sample. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 25(3), Zukerman, G., Yahav, G., & Ben-Itzchak, E. (2019b). Diametrically-
465–473. https​://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2179. opposed associations between academic achievement and social
Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., Popal, H. S., White, E. anxiety among university students with and without autism spec-
I., Brodsky, E., et al. (2016). Real-world executive functions in trum disorder. Autism Research, 12(9), 1376–1385.
adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of impairment and
associations with adaptive functioning and co-morbid anxiety Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to
and depression. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
46(3), 1071–1083.
Wechsler, D. (1997). wais-iii administration and scoring manual. San
Antonio, TX: The Psychological Association.

13

You might also like