You are on page 1of 2

PROFESSIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: RESEARCH AND PRACTICE 549

he wrote "Recall too that beyond a surprisingly limited num- Final Comment
ber of variables or amount of information, additional infor-
mation usually does not increase but may decrease predictive Faust overlooked many studies on clinical judgment. He
accuracy" (p. 427). He cited five studies, one of which does consistently underestimated the success that clinicians have
not even involve the presentation of information to clinicians when making judgments. A comment by Christensen-Szalan-
(Winch & More, 1956). His most recent reference was from ski and Beach (1984) seems relevant in this context: "It is our
1965. I recently reviewed the incremental validity literature hypothesis that the widely held belief in the hopelessness of
(Garb. 1984). I found 29 appropriate studies in which clini- human judgment and decision performance results in part
cians were given increasing amounts of information. Validity from the fact that only evidence to that effect gets much
generally increases when biographical, Minnesota Multiphasic attention. That is, evidence for poor performance is cited
Personality Inventory (MMPI), or neuropsychological test more frequently than is evidence for good performance" (p.
data are added to demographic or psychometric information. 75).
However, the findings for other types of assessment data were
not as positive; for example, the addition of videotapes of References
interviews to transcripts of interviews did not lead to an
increase in validity. Christensen-Szalanski, J. J. J., & Beach, L. R. (1984). The citation
bias: Fad and fashion in the judgment and decision literature.
American Psychologist, 39, 75-78.
Faust. D. (1986). Research on human judgment and its application
Confidence Ratings
to clinical practice. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice.
77,420-430.
Faust wrote. "A number of researchers have found that
Garb. H. N. (1984). The incremental validity of information used in
individuals who are very confident in their judgments are
personality assessment. Clinical Psychology Review, 4, 641-655.
often wrong" (p. 423). He cited several studies from the areas Garb. H. N. (1986). The appropriateness of confidence ratings in
of social and cognitive psychology. In none of the studies did clinical judgment. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 42. 190-197.
the researchers use mental health professionals as judges. I Goldberg, L. R. (1959). The effectiveness of clinicians' judgments:
reviewed studies in which clinical psychologists or psychia- The diagnosis of organic brain damage from the Bender-Gestalt
trists made judgments and then rated the degree of confidence Test. Journal of Consulting Psychology. 21, 25-33.
that they had in each of their judgments (Garb, 1986). Eight- Matarazzo. J. D. (1983). The reliability of psychiatric and psycholog-
een appropriate studies were found. Confidence ratings were ical diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review, 3, 103-145.
related positively to the validity of judgments in a number of Oskamp, S. (1965). Overconfidence in case-study judgments. Journal
of Consulting Psychology. 29. 261-265.
studies.
Winch. R. F.. & More, D. M. (1956). Does TAT add information to
interview? Statistical analysis of the increment. Journal of Clinical
Psychology. 72,316-321.
Level of Experience
HOWARD N. GARB
Faust wrote, "There is limited evidence—in fact, almost
Psychology Service
none—that experts or those with exhaustive experience per-
Highland Drive Veterans Administration Medical Center
form significantly better than 'regulars' or relative beginners"
Pittsburgh. Pennsylvania •
(p. 420). Research does indicate that the validity of judgments
is not greater for experienced clinicians than for inexperienced
judges. However, experienced clinicians tend to make more Response to Garb
appropriate confidence ratings than do inexperienced judges
as long as they are given information that is generally consid- Even if one accepts what I will show to be Garb's dubious
ered to be valid (e.g., neuropsychologica) data, MMPI results; criticisms, many core aspects of my original argument remain
Garb, 1986). These results were found in six studies. untouched. Garb does not dispute, for example, the superi-
ority of actuarial methods, problems in the assessment of
covariation and use of base rates, limits in configural judg-
Reliability of Diagnoses ment capacities, and so on.
Garb argues that 1 have misrepresented research on a num-
According to Faust, "Diagnostic reliability and validity are ber of topics. As stated in my article, coverage was necessarily
longstanding problems, and the judgment research may help skeletal and readers were encouraged to review original
clinicians and researchers to understand why this is so" (p. sources for themselves. However, I have not misrepresented
423). He also wrote. "Clinicians often have difficulty with overall findings. True, I did not cite some studies showing
broad diagnostic choices, to say nothing of more refined greater accuracy than that obtained in the studies I did cite,
assessment" (p. 426). Faust cited no studies to support his but I omitted an equal or greater number showing lower levels
claim that reliability and validity are low. Matarazzo (1983) of accuracy (e.g., Temerlin & Trousdale's 1969 study dem-
reviewed the literature on the reliability of psychiatric and onstrating a 100% error rate among professionals). Garb
psychological diagnosis. He concluded that "clinician-to-cli- contends that I also misrepresent the literature on incremental
nician diagnoses have now attained remarkably high levels of validity or performance in relation to amount of information.
reliability" (p. 103). However, these studies show that once one is given access to
550 PROFESSIONAL PSYCHOLOGY: RESEARCH AND PRACTICE

the two or three most valid variables, additional information peared in at least two journals). In regard to my claims about
produces little or no improvement, or even a decline, in experience and accuracy, if the strongest counterpoint is that
accuracy. Obviously, if one first provides minimally useful a few questionable studies show that greater experience may
data and then adds much more useful data, judgment can sometimes be associated with better calibration of confidence
hardly help but improve. The pertinent questions are, Does (including better awareness of inadequate performance), but
judgment improve when the clinician starts with the two or not with greater diagnostic or predictive accuracy, then what
three most valid variables and then receives further informa- of substance needs to be modified?
tion? and Given an array of data, do clinicians place greatest Garb seems to suggest that I view human judgment as
weight on the most useful information'.' Studies on cue utili- hopeless. I never said such a thing, and I certainly do not
zation, the continuing popularity of human figure drawings, believe it; if I did, I could find better uses of my time than
and disregard of base rates, to cite a few examples, tell an writing these papers. Obviously, human beings have devel-
obvious story. Although many clinicians assume that it is best oped and are capable of further developing decision aids and
to "integrate all of the data," there is absolutely no scientific technology. The main problem is convincing most clinicians
support for this policy; the value of exclusion is rarely recog- of the restrictions in unaided judgment, the pressing need for
nized. supplementary tools, and the means by which current limi-
Have 1 misrepresented research on confidence and accu- tations may eventually be overcome. There is no shame in
racy? In my article 1 merely stated that individuals quite recognizing the limits of unaided human capacity. After all, I
confident of their conclusions can often be wrong. Garb cites readily concede the need to fly to Paris rather than to swim
his own review (1986) on confidence and accuracy, in which there and do not think less of people because of this. As
he mainly seems to "count noses" and misses deeper mean- eloquently argued by Hayek (1955), it may be among the
ings. First, Garb notes that a number of studies show no most important and most difficult of tasks to grasp the limits
relationship between confidence and accuracy, but he argues of human intellect.
that low performance levels precluded significant relation-
ships. However, it should be very obvious that any clinician References
who is confident about invalid judgments is overconfident.
Second, these studies show that confidence generally increases American Psychiatric Association (1980). Diagnostic and statistical
as amount of information increases, and that as such the manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
relationship between confidence and accuracy will be deter- Garb, H. N. (1986). The appropriateness of confidence ratings in
mined by the relationship between additional information clinical judgment. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 42, 190-197.
Hayek, F. A. (1955). The counter-revolution of science. New York:
and accuracy. Add more information, and confidence will
Free Press of Glencoe.
increase on average. If the additional information is useful
Kutchins, H., & Kirk, S. A. (1986. Winter). The reliability of DSM-
and happens to increase accuracy (see previous paragraph), III: A critical review. Social Work Research & Abstracts, 3-12.
the relationship between confidence and accuracy will be Matarazzo, J. D. (1983). The reliability of psychiatric and psycholog-
positive. If the additional information does not increase ac- ical diagnosis. Clinical Psychology Review. 3, 103-145.
curacy, or decreases accuracy, there will be no relationship or Matarazzo, J. D. (1985). Clinical psychological test interpretations by
a negative one (as well illustrated by Wedding's 1983 study). computer: Hardware outpaces software. Computers in Human
Thus these studies provide virtually no evidence that confi- Behavior, 1, 235-253.
dence levels are appropriately calibrated. Temerlin, M. K., & Trousdale, W. W. (1969). The social psychology
Garb criticizes my failure to cite studies on the low relia- of clinical diagnosis. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, and Prac-
tice, 6, 24-29.
bility of psychological diagnosis. I thought that the readership
Wedding, D. (1983). Clinical and statistical prediction in neuropsy-
of Professional Psychology: Research and Practice hardly
chology. Clinical Neuropsychology. 5, 49-54.
required documentation of this claim. Garb counters with an
article by Matarazzo (1983) covering some earlier and seem- DAVID FAUST
ingly promising results, mainly as pertains to DSM-HI Division of Child and Family Psychiatry
(American Psychiatric Association, 1980) or related diag- Rhode Island Hospital •
noses. A more recent review by Kutchins and Kirk (1986)
showed that replication was not attained in later studies.
Indeed, the reliability of DSM-HI may be no better than What Do Counseling Psychologists Think About
DSM-II and, in absolute terms, remains abysmal for many Their Graduate Training? Some Positive and Negative
diagnostic categories. In a more recent article, Matarazzo Reflections
(1985) stated that extended research efforts have produced no
evidence that clinicians' interpretations are any more valid The issue of how to most effectively train psychologists has
than computerized interpretations, the latter of which he long been important in professional psychology. The impor-
characterized as having failed to meet even beginning tests of tance of this issue is reflected in the number of training
validity. I find it ironic that Garb repeatedly criticizes my conferences that have been held over the years (e.g., the
supposed selectivity, although he himself cites Matarazzo Chicago, Greyston, and Vail conferences), as well as in re-
(1983) but fails to cite subsequent studies that render this cently scheduled conferences (e.g., National Conference on
article's claims doubtful at best and even fails to cite Matar- Graduate Education in Psychology, National Conference on
azzo's own later statements about validity (which have ap- Internship Training in Psychology, and Conference on Spe-

You might also like