You are on page 1of 11

Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Nuclear Engineering and Design


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/nucengdes

Effect of user interface layout on the operators’ mental workload


in emergency operating procedures in nuclear power plants
Shengyuan Yan a, Cong Chi Tran a,⇑, Yu Chen b, Ke Tan c, Jean Luc Habiyaremye a
a
College of Mechanical and Electrical Engineering, Harbin Engineering University, 150001 Harbin, Heilongjiang, China
b
College of Mechanical Engineering, Heilongjiang University of Science & Technology, 150022 Harbin, Heilongjiang, China
c
China Nuclear Power Engineering Co Ltd, 518000 Shenzhen, Guangdong, China

h i g h l i g h t s

 Different interface design has a significant effect on the operators’ mental workload.
 Improving interface design based on human factor plays a significant role in reducing operator’s mental workload.
 NASA Task Load Index tool and eye movement technique are useful for assessing human – computer interface designs.

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The improved control interface designs in nuclear power plants (NPPs) have been constantly developed in
Received 30 March 2017 order to satisfy actual requirements. In this article, the effect of user interface layout on mental workload
Received in revised form 10 July 2017 (MWL) of operators in emergency operating procedures in NPPs is analyzed. The control circuit interface
Accepted 11 July 2017
of the Chemical and Volume Control system (RCV) was redesigned based on some human factor criterions
and it was compared to the original interface during the procedure of system boots. The multi-index eval-
uation method based on performance measures (i.e., time to complete and error rate), subjective rating
Keywords:
(i.e., NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX)) and physiological measure (i.e., eye movement) was used to
User interface
Mental workload
evaluate MWL of operators. Twenty-two engineering students tested both interfaces in randomized
Nuclear power plants order. The results showed that interface design has a significant effect on operators’ MWL. Specifically,
Eye-tracking the redesigned interface was rated significantly lower in overall workload, time to complete, and fixation
NASA-TLX indices, but higher blink rate. The results suggested that human factor criterions have a significant effect
on operators’ MWL so they should be considered during designing of interface, and using of NASA-TLX
tool and eye movement technique is useful for assessing of human-computer interface (HCI) designs
in NPPs.
Ó 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction time and to respond quickly with the decision, which easily leads
to generation of very high mental workload (MWL). Poor user
The interface design of control room is constantly improved in interfaces have effect on operators’ MWL, which further affects
order to make it more consistent with actual requirements because efficiency and reliability of the entire system. Statistics show that
poor interface design can cause user stress, lower work rates, human error is a causal factor in significant number of industrial
decreased job satisfaction and even cause absenteeism (Booth, accidents. Although, human error is rarely the sole cause of acci-
1989). In nuclear power plants (NPPs), user control interface is dents in NPPs, human error is major contributor of about 70–90%
often complex and has many emergency operating procedures of accidents (Isaac et al., 2002). Hitherto, the number of occurred
(EOPs) needed to process a large amount of information in a short nuclear incidents is relatively small, but if serious accident hap-
pens it will be a big disaster for people and environment. There-
⇑ Corresponding author at: College of Mechanical and Electrical Engineering, fore, operators have to be attentive all the time and good
Harbin Engineering University, NO. 145-1, Nantong Street, Nangang District, Harbin interface design will enable operators to accomplish their tasks
150001, China. efficiently and effectively with minimal errors (Ikuma et al., 2014).
E-mail addresses: yanshengyuan@hrbeu.edu.cn (S. Yan), trancongchi_bk@yahoo. Presently, there are no structured methods for all human-
com (C.C. Tran), chenyubeautiful@163.com (Y. Chen), tanke@cgnpc.com.cn (K. Tan),
computer interface (HCI) design. Reality shows that it is easy to find
habijealuc@yahoo.fr (J.L. Habiyaremye).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nucengdes.2017.07.012
0029-5493/Ó 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276 267

interface design defects when we have a physical interface to anal- Presently, operator performance in this domain necessitates mas-
ysis and evaluation. It is also important to separate design of the tering of all knowledge and capabilities accompanied by a great
interface from other components in the system, so defects in the deal of concentrated training and repetition to attain such mastery.
interface do not propagate faults through the system, yet this However, access to these experts is not readily available, hence this
method sometimes is complicated and expensive. Burns (1991) pro- study presents experiment with participants who were trained for
posed architecture to separate HCI from hard real-time systems, the task they were asked to do. Based on the results of these par-
which reduces complexity and timing constraints. Currently, one ticipants, we expect similar patterns exist for real operators, but
of the best available methods is iterative design. This methodology further study is needed to confirm it.
is based on a cyclic process of prototyping, testing, analyzing, evalu- Twenty-two man engineering students who were 22 ± 1.6
ating, and redesign. Based on these results many iterations of design, (mean ± SD) years old voluntarily participated in our experiment.
changes and refinements have been made recently. Namely, if we All participants were familiar with the computer operations, but
could perform efficient evaluations and correctly identify as many none of them was familiar with the operation of NPPs. They were
defects as possible, the interface would be significantly improved. also required to have good vision ability and to be right-handed.
One of the commonly used methods for interface evaluation is The experimental process was overseen by professors and experts
the evaluation based on operators’ MWL. This technique has become who are specialized in human factors and ergonomics. All partici-
an important measure for evaluation of HCI in work environment, pants completed and signed an informed consent form approved
and the number of studies on MWL measurement has been increas- by the university institutional review board and were compen-
ing since 1960s (Seker, 2014). Moray (1988) demonstrated that opti- sated with extra credit in extracurricular activities in their course.
mization of MWL allocation could reduce human errors, improve
system safety, and increase users’ satisfaction. Many of studies focus 2.2. Mental workload measures
on optimization of MWL by creating the appropriate designs and
interfaces (Hertzum and Holmegaard, 2013). There are many studies The MWL measurement methods can be divided into three
which show that a well-designed interface can provide a suitable main categories: performance measurement methods, physiologi-
operator MWL (Parasuraman et al., 2000). Niemelä and Saarinen cal measurement methods and subjective rating methods (Tsang
(2000) studied the effect of icons and spatial grouping on scanning and Vidulich, 2006). Performance measurement methods evaluate
speed in computer interface and results showed that design of synchronized data collected during primary tasks and secondary
graphics-based interfaces, such as effective arrangement of icons tasks in designed modules. Physiological measurement methods
in interface, had a significant positive effect on scanning speed, are based on indirect measurement of observed physiological
which means there is a change in user’s MWL. In addition, there changes, such as heartbeat, nasal tip temperature, eye movement,
are also studies related to spatial menu layout and color contrast body and face temperature, and so on, while subjective measures
of user interface and their impact on visual search performance, can be measured by asking users about their experience of
and their conclusion showed similar result (Michalski and demands imposed by current or recently completed task.
Grobelny, 2008; Van Schaik and Ling, 2001). Although there are many different ways to measure human
Interface evaluation is not only understanding of task, its envi- MWL, there is no universal method because each method has its
ronment and the way the user interacts with the system, but also disadvantage. For instance, psychophysiological signals are mea-
an appreciation of capabilities and cognitive demands of future sured using specialized equipment and that equipment could be
users. However, the evaluation of industrial control interface based costly because of capabilities of purchased system. Besides, when
on MWL still has some limitations, because the focus is mainly on in experiments some apparatuses are attached to user body that
impact of workload level, and there is a lack of studies on impact of constrains user movements and breaks naturalness of interaction.
interface design. Especially in NPPs, many information sources Physiological methods also can be influenced by physical and emo-
should be monitored, but the operators have limited capacity of tional factors. In subjective rating methods, rating scale results can
attention and memory (Ha and Seong, 2009). Since it is impossible be affected by characteristics of respondents, like biases, response
to monitor all information sources, the operators continuously sets, errors and protest attitudes (Dyer et al., 1976). Hence, use of
decide where to allocate their attention in order to select impor- workload rating scales is not recommended, when it is likely that
tant things and ignore irrelevant things. Particularly in emergency raters would fake results, have low motivation and prejudices,
situations, the provision of a set of well-designed procedures is guess randomly, etc. However, compared with performance mea-
necessary to reduce MWL of operators and to compensate degrada- surement and physiological measurement methods, subjective
tion of operations (Jimmieson and Terry, 1998; Wood et al., 1990). measurement methods are more efficient regarding the application
Therefore, user interfaces should be designed such that can achieve requirements and they are frequently used because of their large
suitable operator MWL. Thus, this study focuses on effect of user MWL measurement range, lower cost and less required effort. With
interface layout on operators’ MWL in emergency operating proce- the aim to reduce disadvantages of MWL measurement method
dures. The interface of control circuit in Chemical and Volume Con- and to improve the reliability of assessment results, this study
trol system (RCV) was redesigned after its limitations were applies the multi-index evaluation method, including performance
analyzed based on cognitive psychology principles in layout design measurement, physiological measurement and subjective ratings,
and then it was compared to original interface during operating the to evaluate operators’ MWL.
procedure of system boots. It was concluded that interface layout Performance measures can be classified into many categories
has a significant effect on MWL because redesigned interface had such as accuracy, task time, worst-case performance, and so on
lower operators’ MWL than original interface. (Gawron, 2008). In this study, time to complete (or speed of task)
and error rate are used as performance measures because of fol-
lowing reasons: (1) Operating time is an important criterion of
2. Methodology operating emergency procedures; in order to prevent incidents
during operation of these procedures, operators need to process
2.1. Participants information and to respond with the correct decision quickly. (2)
Operating errors involve risky behaviors that need to be under-
This study considers the effect of interface layout on operators’ stood in order to prevent incidents, because that directly relates
MWL during searching for information in NPP user interface. to the effectiveness and safety of operating of NPPs system.
268 S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

Additionally, many studies have shown that error rate is sensitive defined as a time interval between blinks and it is given in seconds
to differences in visual environment (Baker et al., 1960; Downing (Montés-Micó et al., 2005). Human pupil dilation can measure the
and Sanders, 1987; Hwang et al., 2008); and (3) time to complete average arousal underlying the cognitive tasks, and blink rate can
and error rate are easy to collect using video playback feature. In produce an opposite effect. Whereas MWL of cognitive tasks will
this study, time to complete (given in seconds) is defined as the increase blink rate, visual demand will inhibit it (Recarte et al.,
time that participant spend on each session of experiment, and 2008). Thus, many studies on relationship between average blink
error rate (%) is defined as the ratio of the number of incorrect rate and workload yield conflicting results. Nonetheless, some
operations and the number of all operations. researchers found that a decrease in blink rate is associated with
NASA-TLX method(Hart and Staveland, 1988) is used to evalu- an increase in task demands, particularly visual demands of tasks
ate operators’ subjective workload in this study, because it is one (Gao et al., 2013; Wilson, 2002), and other researchers failed to find
of the most widely used methods and it has provided solid results such a significant relationship (Casali and Wierwille, 1983).
in human factors research (Rubio et al., 2004). Namely, this tool Eye fixations are detected and their properties calculated by an
has been successfully used to measure MWL of HCI in various event-detection algorithm. Fixation duration is sometimes called
domains including automobile (Götze et al., 2015), aerospace (De ‘fixation time’, but also ‘dwell time’, or ‘dwell time of the fixation’
la Torre et al., 2016; Lehrer et al., 2010; Wei et al., 2014) and NPPs (Holmqvist et al., 2011). This index is extensively used measure
(Gao et al., 2013, 2015; Jou et al., 2009; Naderpour et al., 2016). and it is believed that it increases with increasing of mental task
This method is a multi-dimensional rating scale using six dimen- demands (Recarte and Nunes, 2000; Tole et al., 1982). Fixation rate
sions of workload to provide diagnostic information about the nat- is the number of fixations divided by certain time period, such as
ure and relative contribution of each dimension in influencing duration of a trial in seconds, giving the unit persecond. It is found
overall operator workload. Six dimensions to assess MWL includ- that it is negatively correlated to task difficulty, and it can be used
ing mental demand (MD), physical demand (PD), temporal demand as a measure of MWL (Van Orden et al., 2001).Recently,fixation
(TD), own performance (OP), effort (EF) and frustration (FR). Each indices have been investigated in a series of studies on MWL
of these is on a scale from 0 to 100. Each subscale has been pre- (Ahlstrom and Friedman-Berg, 2006; Di Stasi et al., 2013), assess
sented as a 12 cm line with a title and bipolar descriptors at each product design (Guo et al., 2016; Ho and Lu, 2014), assessing user
end. Numerical values have not been displayed, but values are interface (Mat Zain et al., 2011; Scott et al., 2002; Sharma et al.,
ranging from 1 to 100 have been assigned to scale positions during 2016), and these indices often show high reliability. In this study,
data analysis. The process of MWL evaluation through NASA-TLX the area of interest (AOI) is defined as a simulation screen. Total
involves three steps. At the first step, weight in each of the six sub- fixation duration (duration of all fixations within AOI) and fixation
scales is determined to reveal the priority of the six subscales. All rate were collected within the fixation lengths that varied from
subscales are self-evaluated and selected by the operator in a 100 ms to 600 ms.
paired form and 15 different comparisons, and then, each workload
dimension is scored 0–5. At the second step, to allocate the rating 2.3. Equipment
of workload, each of the six subscales is rated with the goal of
determination of each scale’s effect on the MWL. At this step, the 2.3.1. Eye tracking system
operator scores each of these six subscales from 1 to 100 based The participants’ eye movement data was recorded by the SMI
on his/her working condition. At the third and the last step, after HED of SensoMotoric Instruments (SMI). Software configuration
determination of weight and rating in previous steps, total MW has the video recording and the BeGaze 3.0 eye movement data
is calculated in the range 1–100 through the following formula analysis, sampling rate 50/60 Hz (optional 200 Hz), tracking reso-
‘‘Weighted Workload (WWL) is R (rating  weight)/15.” lution, pupil/CR < 0.1° (typ.), gaze position accuracy < 0.5°–1.0°
Physiological measurement based on eye movement is a tech- (typ.), tracking range ±30° horizontal and ±25° vertical. The exper-
nique that captures eye behavior as a response to visual stimulus, imental interface presented on the 17 inches LCD monitor, dis-
and this technique is widely used to analyze user behavior in mar- played resolution of 1024  768 pixels, 60 Hz of refresh rate.
keting, neuroscience, human-computer interaction, visualization Calibration was done before each using session.
research and user interface evaluation (Ikuma et al., 2014; Toker
et al., 2013). Recordings of user eye movements during interaction 2.3.2. RCV interface
(i.e., doing a task) with user interface can show where on the inter- The RCV system is essential for safe and reliable operation of
face user looks and for how long. The particular pattern of eye NPPs. Among its numerous functions, this system is designed to
movements can signal whether the user interface elicits usage maintain a continuous primary fluid letdown and charging flow
problems or facilitates efficient and effective task realization, via letdown line and charging line. The letdown line reduces the
which can help designers to evaluate changes in operator workload primary water to pressure and temperature conditions that are
during design and evaluation of complex systems (Ahlstrom and compatible with the purification/degassing systems. It is also
Friedman-Berg, 2006). designed to fulfill functions of injection into primary pump seals
Eye response parameters that are used as MWL measures and auxiliary spray. Failure or degradation of system components
include pupil dilation, blink rate and fixation. Human pupil dilation might significantly affect plant operations (reactivity and pressur-
can be used as a measure of psychological load because it is related izer level control). Failures which affect system non-safety-related
to the amount of cognitive control, attention, and cognitive pro- functions do not usually result in increase of plant risk.
cessing required for a given task (Moresi et al., 2008; Wierda The RCV interface used in this study is the interface of circuit
et al., 2012). In addition, it was previously shown that it correlates preparation for S-1-RCV-001, which is used in NPP in China,
with the cognitive workload, whereby increased frequency of dila- Fig. 1. The interface displays system and its different components.
tion is associated with increased degree of difficulty of a task The display also provides operators the information necessary to
(Hampson et al., 2010). Gao et al. (2013) also showed that in NPPs monitor the system state. Flow lines connect different system com-
operation the pupil size in a high complexity EOP is larger than in a ponents with arrows that indicate flow direction. Gauge values
low complexity EOP. adjacent to each of valves indicate flow rate, pressure, and temper-
Blink rate (‘frequency’) is defined as a number of blinks per sec- ature. When the interface of RCV system is used, operators must
ond or minute. Some researchers in ophthalmology prefer to use search and locate many valves, pumps place and set required
the inverse blink rate, called the ‘inter-blink interval’, which is parameters. Although the original design is used in practice, some
S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276 269

Fig. 1. Original interfaces of circuit preparation for S-1-RCV-001.

design characteristics suggest that consideration of avoiding designed intelligently, in order to adapt it to users’ cognitive pro-
human error and decreasing MWL of operators as much as possible cess dynamically.
should be addressed. Therefore, this study applies few layout prin- Principle 4. User interface in accordance with information orga-
ciples based on cognitive psychology, which are used for analysis nization law. The discrete stimulations in view could be organized
and redesign (Deng et al., 2016; Yunpeng, 2005). together forming a whole vision by certain relationship between
Principle 1. Cognition corresponds to objective. Using the inter- them, and this phenomenon is known as the visual organization
face in line with users’ experience and knowledge and adopting the features. Visual organizational principles mainly include proximity
appropriate processing method conform to the old habits and con- principle, similarity principle, and closeness principle, which have
cepts, could reduce learning time and memory time and avoid mis- a certain guiding significance for interface design.
takes. For instance, similar functions should be arranged in the After analysis, we found some limitations in this design that
same area, because operators would automatically search the might affect efficiency of operator performance in information
manipulator at the familiar area of interface. searching and processing, and they are labeled by 1–6 in Fig. 1.
Principle 2. Task flow design. The operating sequence design
can reduce users’ workload and improve working efficiency, also, (1) The RCV startup protocol interface does not specify task con-
the main tasks of operator that need to be done should be given tent required for completion. It also lacks low-level informa-
and execution process of tasks should be analyzed. Moreover, tion, high-level information, and links between reference
according to operators’ cognitive habits to merge or reduce unnec- information support and services, which might not be
essary actions or implement automation, the dialogue process important to experienced operators, but it is essential for
should be simplified, which would speed up information process- less experienced operators.
ing and reduce users’ cognitive load and time of information (2) Some critical parameters and variables in RCP001YFU,
processing. RCV001YCD, and RIS001YCD interface display unclearness
Principle 3. User interface matches with users’ cognitive strat- and inconsistency, so it is difficult for operators to read the
egy. User interface is very difficult to adapt to the dynamic cogni- information. For instance, the data of 120MT and 109MT
tive differences of all kinds of users. So, based on users’ knowledge are the same type as the data of 110MT, 112MT, 113MT.
level, cognitive ability, and habits, and by judging the different However, 120MT and 109MT are located on the right side
levels of users’ cognitive strategy, user interface should be of the main control window, while the indicators that
270 S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

display 110MT, 112MT, 113MT are located on the left side of (6) Some display information is not necessary; namely, the sur-
the screen. The lack of grouping reduces perception that data plus information is easily disturbed to the operator. For
are related to a common task. Furthermore, visual searching instance, in RCV001YCD interface, displayed information of
time increases during manipulation of flow controllers RCP007MN, RCP008MN and RCP011MN is not necessary,
because operators’ gaze must switch between flow con- so it is possible to hide them using certain display modes
trollers and indicators. in order to make interface neater.
(3) Information flows in CP001YFU interface are not prominent,
namely they are thin and poorly visible. The more salient According to the above analysis, the interface is redesigned
solid line depicting active flow serves as an emergent feature based on some standards of NUREG (O’Hara et al., 2002). The rede-
that can quickly provide operators the information they signed interface is shown in Fig. 2. Specifically, navigational links
need. Thus their display should be changed in term of color to and from high-level and lower-level information and to refer-
or flow size to make the flows clearly displayed. ence and supporting information that should be provided when
(4) Some of display information does not distinguish clearly needed for operators’ tasks are added using NUREG-07001.1-27
information type, thus it is easy to be confused and delayed criterion (Navigational Links to Related Information). The criterion
in searching of information. Therefore, an explicit mapping of grouping of related information (NUREG-07001.1-28) is used to
between system characteristics and functions should be rep- suggest some groups and relevant information with the aim to
resented and features should be clearly displayed. For minimize user distraction, which minimizes disadvantages of
instance, RCP001PO, RCP002PO, and RCP003PO are not dis- divided attention and number of attention shifts within a display
played clearly. Their physical form and displayed functions page and between them. For instance, based on this criterion,
should be explicitly tied to their meaning with respect to 120MT and 109MT data are moved and grouped with 110MT,
their functions. 112MT, 113MT on the left side of the screen. NUREG-07001.2.8-6
(5) The information in the interface needs to be compared by (Line Coding) is used to design important display information
operators’ psychological integration; the information to be flows, and NUREG-07001.1-5 (Correspondence Mapping) is used
compared should be arranged close to each other or aligned to display information that does not distinguish clearly between
if possible. For instance, in RCV001YCD interface, 310VP CL/ other types. The information that needs to be compare by opera-
OPEN and 036KGMANU/AUTO need to be compared with tors’ psychological integration in interface is designed based on
409KU, 409KG, 413KU in 013VP data. NUREG-07001.1-29 (Spatial Proximity for Related Information).

Fig. 2. Redesigned interface of circuit preparation for S-1-RCV-001.


S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276 271

This criterion proposes that all information that must be compared ticipant repeated steps (2) to (4) using the other interface. Each
or mentally integrated should be presented in close spatial proxim- session (steps (2) to (4)) lasted approximately 40 min.
ity. If possible, the information items should be contained in the
same display page and grouped together. Spatial proximity can 3. Results
be also achieved by presenting display pages in adjacent display
windows or on adjacent display devices that can be viewed In all cases, a level of 0.05 was used to determine statistical sig-
together. For instance, for convenience of comparing data of nificance. The observed significance level (p-value) for a specific
310VPCL and 036KG with 409KU, 409KG and 413KU of 013VP data, statistical test is the probability of observing a value of the test
in redesigned interface they are horizontally aligned and have the statistic. One rejects the null hypothesis if the p-value is smaller
same design shape. Finally, NUREG-07001.5-7 (Redundancy) is than or equal to a level. If the level is 0.05, then the results are only
applied to display the information that is not necessary. 5% likely to be as extraordinary as just seen, given that the null
The processes of interface analysis, evaluation and redesigning hypothesis is true. The statistical analysis was conducted using
were conducted carefully. In addition to designing based on cogni- SPSS software, version 20.
tive principles, interface layout must also meet the requirements of
technology and system functions. Therefore, interface analysis and
3.1. Performance data
redesigning process were combined with comments from some
professors and experts who are specialized in human factors and
The performance data of participants were analyzed to distin-
NPP technology.
guish the differences between two interfaces, and results are sum-
marized in Table 1. The results showed that average time to
2.4. Experimental task
complete of two interfaces is significantly different. The partici-
pants were slower using the original interface than redesigned
The main objective of this study is to consider the effect of
interface (t = 1.97; p = 0.043). However, the error rate was not sig-
interface layout on operators’ MWL during searching for informa-
nificant different (t = 0.27; p = 0.792). The obtained performances
tion in complex user interface. To accomplish this, the participants
are presented graphically in Fig. 4.
were asked to execute the tasks of operation of system boots for S-
1-RCV-001 using two interfaces, the original interface that is used
3.2. Subjective workload
in NPP and the redesigned interface. Task steps and actions to be
performed, such as search and locate valve/pump, set parameters,
Subjective workload data obtained from participants using
and turn on/off valve/pump, are presented in Fig. 3 and partici-
NASA-TLX method were statistically evaluated, and results are
pants had to perform these actions following the required order.
summarized in Table 2. There was a significant difference between
In order to use RCV procedure, 011MN and 012MN parameters
WWL in operating with original interface and redesigned interface
were set to less than 3 m. Participants started the system boot with
(t = 3.11; p = 0.005). Comparing six different dimensions of NASA-
opening the RIS012VP valve and completed the procedure when
TLX, significant differences were also found for MD (t = 2.81;
RIS013VP valve closed. The process did not have any automatic
p = 0.022) and FR (t = 2.52; p = 0.042). NASA-TLX dimensions and
controllers and had to be monitored and controlled by operators.
WWL score of two interfaces are presented graphically in Fig. 5.
2.5. Procedures
3.3. Physiological measures
Each participant tested both mentioned interfaces in random-
ized order. All participants were asked to complete task seriously, The mean value of all participants’ eye movement data of two
and no information was given about any specific hypotheses under interfaces is illustrated in Table 3. The results of t-Test indicate that
investigation. The procedure of experiment was as follows. (1) there are significant differences in average blink rate (t = -2.37;
Firstly, all participants got about 30 min of training. During the p = 0.027), total fixation duration (t = 2.86; p = 0.009) and fixation
training, the participants read and listened to the electronic lec- rate (t = 2.01; p = 0.044). The eye movement indices of two inter-
ture, which provided background information about location and faces are plotted in Fig. 6.
functioning of system components. They were taught how to use
the interface simulators, eye tracking equipment and NASA-TLX 4. Discussion
questionnaire. (2) Then, each participant received the list of oper-
ations and practiced the procedures. The user interface for practice This study analyzes effect of user interface layout on operators’
was the same as that in the formal experiment except that there MWL in emergency operating procedures in NPPs. Two interfaces
was no pressure on time. The practice ended when participant of RCV control were used to analyze the interface effect based on
was sure that he understood all procedures. Moreover, each partic- performance measures, subjective rating and physiological mea-
ipant was interviewed by a control room expert to ensure that he sures. Time to complete and error rate of task were used as perfor-
was adequately prepared for the task. After that, the participant mance measure indices, because they directly relate to
took a five minutes rest. (3) Afterwards, each participant was pro- effectiveness and safety of system operation. The obtained results
vided with eye-tracker and started the first session on one of two have shown that participants spent less time to finish task on rede-
interfaces. In the experiment, participant was allowed to use the signed interface but there was no significant difference in error
list of operations if he needed it. (4) After first session, the partic- rate. This result can be explained by the fact that human perfor-
ipant filled out NASA-TLX pairwise comparisons and completed mance should be considered in terms of both MWL and situation
NASA-TLX questionnaire, and then rested for 5 min before he awareness. Theoretically, if interface design could reduce MWL
started to use the other interface. It should be noted that, to and enhance situation awareness, error rate would be decreased
improve the reliability of subjective rating results, the NASA-TLX (Salvucci and Bogunovich, 2010). However, error rate is often not
questionnaire was conducted after each completed experiment sensitive to workload in a short experimental time (Hwang et al.,
session. Certain studies indicated that evaluation results are signif- 2008; Lanzetta et al., 1987). Additionally, in experimental condi-
icantly different when ratings is done before each condition and tions, participants often do not have psychological comfort because
when it is done after all conditions (Ikuma et al., 2014). (5) The par- they suffer the consequences of their mistakes when the operation
272 S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

Fig. 3. Operation/check list of system boots for S-1-RCV-001.

Table 1 ing to the workload of participant for both interfaces. This partially
t-Test of performance data between two interfaces. confirms the research problem statement that operation of NPPs is
Original interface Redesign p-value
mentally demanding job. In addition to MD, TD also received high
(Mean ± SD) (Mean ± SD) rating in contributing to the workload of operator for both HCIs.
Time to complete (s) 275.6 ± 96.8 231.9 ± 81.4 0.043*
Namely, TD is influenced by parameters such as time pressure
Error rate (%) 10.2 ± 8.2 9.6 ± 7.1 0.792 and task speed. Due to the emergency operating procedures and
*
fact that each participant was asked to complete task seriously
p  0.05.
and hence provides higher ratings of workload. EF and FR dimen-
sions were also found as important parameters that contribute to
fails or does not fulfill task requirements which might affect oper- operator workload, because all persons who tested the interfaces
ation performance discussed in this study. were novices, and they did not have relevant experience with oper-
The results of subjective rating have shown that operators’ ating of NPPs and they had to complete task in a short time, so the
MWL for different interfaces is different. The WWL of NASA-TLX amount of required effort was significantly increased. Additionally,
of redesigned interface was reduced for 9.8% in comparison to the frustration originated from searching and processing of infor-
the original interface, which indicates that interface design has sig- mation in the task. Namely, the RCV interface had many icons
nificant effect on operators’ MWL. The results of NASA-TLX score and data which operator needs to distinguish, which makes oper-
have shown that MD dimension received high ratings in contribut- ator to feel difficult to complete the task.
S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276 273

Fig. 4. Performance data of two interfaces. Vertical bars denote the mean standard error.

Table 2 significant differences in pupil dilation between two interfaces,


t-Test of NASA-TLX between two interfaces. but there were significant differences in the mean blink rate and
Original interface Redesign p-value fixations.
(Mean ± SD) (Mean ± SD) Several studies associated the blink rate with MWL (Wolkoff
Mental demand (MD) 68.4 ± 9.8 58.9 ± 13.2 0.022* et al., 2005). Van Orden et al. (2000) found that blink rate increases
Physical demand (PD) 47.9 ± 12.2 43.5 ± 11.4 0.202 as a function of tracking error in human factors tracking task. How-
Temporal demand (TD) 61.7 ± 14.6 58.6 ± 13.4 0.495 ever, Veltman and Gaillard (1998) concluded that blinking is inde-
Own Performance (OP) 46.6 ± 12.8 48.9 ± 13.0 0.845
pendent of MWL. These results indicate that more research is
Efforts (EF) 53.0 ± 12.1 47.3 ± 14.3 0.081
Frustration (FR) 59.8 ± 12.5 53.5 ± 10.1 0.042* required before blink rate could be used as a measure of MWL
Overall workload (WWL) 58.0 ± 7.1 52.3 ± 5.6 0.005** (Kramer, 1991). In this study, we found a significant difference in
* the mean blink rate between two interfaces. Compared to rede-
p  0.05.
**
p  0.01. signed interface, the average blink rate of original interface was
higher for 14.8%. Therefore, operator had to process more visual
information in original interface, so duration of blinks was lower.
Although, the eye movement method has certain limitations,
such as the fact that eye is easily interfered by lighting environ-
ment and impact of eye-tracking camera’s ability to record eye Table 3
t-Test of eye movement between two interfaces.
movements, eye movement is useful measure to determine the
areas of interface that draw user’s attention, whereas the other Original interface Redesign p-value
tools only provide information on interface as a whole (Lehrer (Mean ± SD) (Mean ± SD)
et al., 2010). In addition, many indexes of this method directly Pupil dilation (pixel) 53.1 ± 6.9 51.5 ± 5.7 0.330
relate to MWL and they are commonly used in assessment of Blink rate (times/min) 18.5 ± 5.2 21.6 ± 6.2 0.027*
Total fixation duration (s) 87.9 ± 29.7 64.5 ± 21.5 0.009**
MWL. The size of human eye pupil is often used as a measure
Fixation rate (times/s) 1.57 ± 0.92 1.14 ± 0.64 0.044*
of mental effort because it is assumed that pupil size is related
*
to the amount of cognitive control, attention, and cognitive pro- p  0.05.
**
p  0.01.
cessing required by a given task. This experiment did not found

Fig. 5. NASA-TLX scores of two interfaces. Vertical bars denote the mean standard error.
274 S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

Fig. 6. Eye movement indices of two interfaces. Vertical bars denote the mean standard error.

It complies with many researches, wherein it was suggested that improved interface design based on human factor criterions has a
blink rate decrease reflects a strategy to maximize the time avail- significant effect on operator MWL. However, this is just an assess-
able for visual processing when visual demands are the highest. ment of user interface effect based on ergonomics, thus we are not
Further, it is possible that differences in blink rate and mental/ able to conclude that the redesigned interface presented in this
visual workload reflect different types of task (Veltman and study is the best design. Rather, this interface design can be useful
Gaillard, 1998). for interface designers in the future.
Recently, numerous parameters that characterize eye fixation
have been studied in relation with human’s MWL. Many research-
4.1. Limitations
ers, such as Tole et al. (1982), found an increase in fixation duration
when MWL increases, and Goldberg and Kotval (1999) found a
There are several limitations of this study. First, a small number
negative correlation between fixation time and performance. In
of samples was used to test interface designs in experiments.
this study, in the task of searching and processing of information
Moreover, a small size of samples reduced statistical power, partic-
in user interface, there were significant differences between two
ularly for interface factor evaluation. Additionally, all participants
interfaces in terms of fixation duration and fixation rate. These
were students, who cannot represent the real operators of NPPs.
results have indicated that redesigned interface is more effective
Although after training and practice they had adequate knowledge
in information searching because higher fixation rate on a particu-
and skills for solving the problem used in the experiment, their
lar area can be indicative of greater interest in the target, or it can
experience might be very different from experience of real opera-
be a sign that the target is complex in some way and more difficult
tors who have been trained for years. Thus, further validation with
to be searched (Nakayama et al., 2002).
professional operators or experts is needed before applying these
The goal of user interface design is to produce a user interface
results in the industry. Also, the experiment was not performed
which makes it easy to use, efficient, and friendly to operate the
in real environment (real environment would make the study more
system in the way which produces the desired result and improv-
realistic and reliable) which may cause significant differences
ing the interface layout often gains significant efficiency (Niemelä
among the outcomes, thus results have limited reliability. Despite
and Saarinen, 2000). Some similar experiments with slightly differ-
these limitations, we believe this study contributes to understand-
ent icons were also reported in other studies on evaluation of user
ing of interface layout design effect on operators’ MWL during the
interface system, wherein an eye-tracking device was used for data
operation of EOPs.
gathering, and analysis of layout and its impact on visual search
performance (Murata and Furukawa, 2005; Van Schaik and Ling,
2001). They demonstrated that speed of task execution is signifi- 5. Conclusions
cantly higher for good interface layout. Comparative evaluation
results of this study show that new interface is more effective in Reality shows that HCI design greatly influences information
information searching and processing, which means that the acquisition in operator control, time of feedback and final results,
S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276 275

leading to possible human factor accidents. Therefore, the Holmqvist, K., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., Van de Weijer,
J., 2011. Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. OUP
improvement of HCI design is essential, especially in the field of
Oxford.
NPPs. This paper reports the effect of user interface layout on oper- Hwang, S.-L., Yau, Y.-J., Lin, Y.-T., Chen, J.-H., Huang, T.-H., Yenn, T.-C., Hsu, C.-C., 2008.
ators’ MWL based on human performances, NASA-TLX rating and Predicting work performance in nuclear power plants. Saf. Sci. 46, 1115–1124.
eye movement. The original design is used in practice, but some Ikuma, L.H., Harvey, C., Taylor, C.F., Handal, C., 2014. A guide for assessing control
room operator performance using speed and accuracy, perceived workload,
design characteristics suggest that consideration of avoiding situation awareness, and eye tracking. J. Loss Prev. Process Ind. 32, 454–465.
human error and decreasing MWL of operators as much as possible Isaac, A., Shorrock, S.T., Kirwan, B., 2002. Human error in European air traffic
should be addressed. According to the obtained results, the rede- management: the HERA project. Reliab. Eng. Syst. Saf. 75, 257–272.
Jimmieson, N.L., Terry, D.J., 998. An experimental study of the effects of work stress,
signed interface is more effective than the original interface in both work control, and task information on adjustment. Appl. Psychol. 47, 343–369.
information searching and processing, and some evaluation tools Jou, Y.-T., Yenn, T.-C., Lin, C.J., Yang, C.-W., Chiang, C.-C., 2009. Evaluation of
are sensitive to differences between interface designs. Although operators’ mental workload of human–system interface automation in the
advanced nuclear power plants. Nucl. Eng. Des. 239, 2537–2542.
this is just an assessment of user interface effect based on ergo- Kramer, A.F., 1991. Physiological metrics of mental workload: a review of recent
nomics, the results of this study can be a real basis for reference progress. Multiple-Task Perform., 279–328
and development of new interface designs in the future. Lanzetta, T.M., Dember, W.N., Warm, J.S., Berch, D.B., 1987. Effects of task type and
stimulus heterogeneity on the event rate function in sustained attention. Hum.
Fact.: J. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. 29, 625–633.
Lehrer, P., Karavidas, M., Lu, S.E., Vaschillo, E., Vaschillo, B., Cheng, A., 2010. Cardiac
Acknowledgements data increase association between self-report and both expert ratings of task
load and task performance in flight simulator tasks: an exploratory study. Int. J.
Psychophysiol. 76, 80–87.
This study was conducted in the Lab of the College of Mechan- Mat Zain, N., Abdul Razak, F., Jaafar, A., Zulkipli, M., 2011. Eye tracking in
ical Engineering at Harbin Engineering University, Harbin, Hei- educational games environment: evaluating user interface design through eye
tracking patterns. Visual Informatics: Sustaining Research and Innovations, pp.
longjiang, China. The authors would like to thank the reviewers 64-73.
for their valuable remarks and comments. Also, the authors thank Michalski, R., Grobelny, J., 2008. The role of colour preattentive processing in
the experts and students who helped conduct this research. human–computer interaction task efficiency: a preliminary study. Int. J. Ind.
Ergon. 38, 321–332.
Montés-Micó, R., Alió, J.L., Charman, W.N., 2005. Dynamic changes in the tear film in
dry eyes. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 46, 1615–1619.
References Moray, N., 1988. Mental workload since 1979. Int. Rev. Ergon. 2, 123–150.
Moresi, S., Adam, J.J., Rijcken, J., Van Gerven, P.W., Kuipers, H., Jolles, J., 2008. Pupil
dilation in response preparation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 67, 124–130.
Ahlstrom, U., Friedman-Berg, F.J., 2006. Using eye movement activity as a correlate
Murata, A., Furukawa, N., 2005. Relationships among display features, eye
of cognitive workload. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 36, 623–636.
movement characteristics, and reaction time in visual search. Hum. Fact. 47,
Baker, C.A., Morris, D.F., Steedman, W.C., 1960. Target recognition on complex
598–612.
displays. Hum. Fact.: J. Hum. Fact. Ergon. Soc. 2, 51–60.
Naderpour, M., Lu, J., Zhang, G., 2016. A safety-critical decision support system
Booth, P., 1989. An Introduction to Human Computer Interaction. Lawrence
evaluation using situation awareness and workload measures. Reliab. Eng. Syst.
Erlbaum Associates Ltd, Hills dale.
Saf. 150, 147–159.
Burns, A., 1991. The HCI component of dependable real-time systems. Softw. Eng. J.
Nakayama, M., Takahashi, K., Shimizu, Y., 2002. The act of task difficulty and eye-
6, 168–174.
movement frequency for the’Oculo-motor indices’, Proceedings of the 2002
Casali, J.G., Wierwille, W.W., 1983. A comparison of rating scale, secondary-task,
symposium on Eye tracking research & applications. ACM, pp.37–42.
physiological, and primary-task workload estimation techniques in a simulated
Niemelä, M., Saarinen, J., 2000. Visual search for grouped versus ungrouped icons in
flight task emphasizing communications load. Hum. Fact. 25, 623–641.
a computer interface. Hum. Fact. 42, 630–635.
De la Torre, G.G., Ramallo, M.A., Cervantes, E., 2016. Workload perception in drone
O’Hara, J., Brown, W., Lewis, P., Persensky, J., 2002. Human-System Interface Design
flight training simulators. Comput. Hum. Behav. 64, 449–454.
Review Guidelines (NUREG-0700, Rev 2). US Nuclear Regulatory Commission,
Deng, L., Wang, G., Yu, S., 2016. Layout design of human-machine interaction
Washington, DC, p. 12.
interface of cabin based on cognitive ergonomics and GA-ACA. Comput. Intell.
Parasuraman, R., Sheridan, T.B., Wickens, C.D., 2000. A model for types and levels of
Neurosci. 2016, 2.
human interaction with automation. IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybernetics-Part A:
Di Stasi, L.L., Antolí, A., Cañas, J.J., 2013. Evaluating mental workload while
Syst. Hum. 30, 286–297.
interacting with computer-generated artificial environments. Entertain.
Recarte, M.A., Nunes, L.M., 2000. Effects of verbal and spatial-imagery tasks on eye
Comput. 4, 63–69.
fixations while driving. J. Experim. Psychol. Appl. 6, 31.
Downing, J.V., Sanders, M.S., 1987. The effects of panel arrangement and locus of
Recarte, M.Á., Pérez, E., Conchillo, Á., Nunes, L.M., 2008. Mental workload and visual
attention on performance. Hum. Fact. 29, 551–562.
impairment: differences between pupil, blink, and subjective rating. Spanish J.
Dyer, R.F., Matthews, J.J., Wright, C.E., Yudowitch, K.L., 1976. Questionnaire
Psychol. 11, 374–385.
construction manual. DTIC Document.
Rubio, S., Díaz, E., Martín, J., Puente, J.M., 2004. Evaluation of subjective mental
Gao, Q., Wang, Y., Song, F., Li, Z., Dong, X., 2013. Mental workload measurement for
workload: a comparison of SWAT, NASA-TLX, and workload profile methods.
emergency operating procedures in digital nuclear power plants. Ergonomics
Appl. Psychol. 53, 61–86.
56, 1070–1085.
Salvucci, D.D., Bogunovich, P., 2010. Multitasking and monotasking: the effects of
Gao, Q., Yu, W., Jiang, X., Song, F., Pan, J., Li, Z., 2015. An integrated computer-based
mental workload on deferred task interruptions, Proceedings of the SIGCHI
procedure for teamwork in digital nuclear power plants. Ergonomics 58, 1303–
conference on human factors in computing systems. ACM, pp. 85–88.
1313.
Scott, I.U., Feuer, W.J., Jacko, J.A., 2002. Impact of graphical user interface screen
Gawron, V.J., 2008. Human Performance, Workload, and Situational Awareness
features on computer task accuracy and speed in a cohort of patients with age-
Measures Handbook. CRC Press.
related macular degeneration. Am. J. Ophthalmol. 134, 857–862.
Goldberg, J.H., Kotval, X.P., 999. Computer interface evaluation using eye
Seker, A., 2014. Using outputs of NASA-TLX for building a mental workload expert
movements: methods and constructs. Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 24, 631–645.
system. Gazi Univ. J. Sci. 27, 1131–1142.
Götze, M., Ruff, F., Bengler, K., 2015. Optimal information output in urban traffic
Sharma, C., Bhavsar, P., Srinivasan, B., Srinivasan, R., 2016. Eye gaze movement
scenarios: an evaluation of different HMI concepts. Procedia Manuf. 3, 739–746.
studies of control room operators: a novel approach to improve process safety.
Guo, F., Ding, Y., Liu, W., Liu, C., Zhang, X., 2016. Can eye-tracking data be measured
Comput. Chem. Eng. 85, 43–57.
to assess product design?: visual attention mechanism should be considered.
Toker, D., Conati, C., Steichen, B., Carenini, G., 2013. Individual user characteristics
Int. J. Ind. Ergon. 53, 229–235.
and information visualization: connecting the dots through eye tracking,
Ha, J.S., Seong, P.H., 2009. A human–machine interface evaluation method: a
Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing
difficulty evaluation method in information searching (DEMIS). Reliab. Eng.
systems, Paris, France, pp. 295–304.
Syst. Saf. 94, 1557–1567.
Tole, J., Stephens, A.T., Harris, R., Ephrath, A., 1982. Visual scanning behavior and
Hampson, R., Opris, I., Deadwyler, S., 2010. Neural correlates of fast pupil dilation in
mental workload in aircraft pilots. Aviation, Space, and EnvironmentalMedicine.
nonhuman primates: relation to behavioral performance and cognitive
Tsang, P.S., Vidulich, M.A., 2006. Mental Workload and Situation Awareness. Wiley,
workload. Behav. Brain Res. 212, 1–11.
New York, NY.
Hart, S., Staveland, L., 1988. Development of NASA-TLX (Task Load Index): results of
Van Orden, K.F., Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., 2000. Combined eye activity measures
empirical and theoretical research. Hum. Mental Workload, 139–183.
accurately estimatechanges in sustained visual task performance. Biol. Psychol.
Hertzum, M., Holmegaard, K.D., 2013. Perceived time as a measure of mental
52, 221–240.
workload: effects of time constraints and task success. Int. J. Hum. Comput.
Van Orden, K.F., Limbert, W., Makeig, S., Jung, T.-P., 2001. Eye activity correlates of
Interact. 29, 26–39.
workload during a visuospatial memory task. Hum. Fact.: J. Hum. Fact. Ergon.
Ho, C.-H., Lu, Y.-N., 2014. Can pupil size be measured to assess design products? Int.
Soc. 43, 111–121.
J. Ind. Ergon. 44, 436–441.
276 S. Yan et al. / Nuclear Engineering and Design 322 (2017) 266–276

Van Schaik, P., Ling, J., 2001. The effects of frame layout and differential background Wilson, G.F., 2002. An analysis of mental workload in pilots during flight using
contrast on visual search performance in web pages. Interact. Comput. 13, 513– multiple psychophysiological measures. Int. J. Aviat. Psychol. 12, 3–18.
525. Wolkoff, P., Nøjgaard, J.K., Troiano, P., Piccoli, B., 2005. Eye complaints in the office
Veltman, J., Gaillard, A., 1998. Physiological workload reactions to increasing levels environment: precorneal tear film integrity influenced by eye blinking
of task difficulty. Ergonomics 41, 656–669. efficiency. Occup. Environ. Med. 62, 4–12.
Wei, Z., Zhuang, D., Wanyan, X., Liu, C., Zhuang, H., 2014. A model for discrimination Wood, R., Bandura, A., Bailey, T., 1990. Mechanisms governing organizational
and prediction of mental workload of aircraft cockpit display interface. Chin. J. performance in complex decision-making environments. Organ. Behav. Hum.
Aeronaut. 27, 1070–1077. Decis. Process. 46, 181–201.
Wierda, S.M., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N.A., Martens, S., 2012. Pupil dilation Yunpeng, Z., 2005. Human-computer interface design based on knowledge of
deconvolution reveals the dynamics of attention at high temporal resolution. cognitive psychology. Comput. Eng. Appl. 30, 034.
P. Natl. A. Sci. 109, 8456–8460.

You might also like