You are on page 1of 14

Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 Ž1998.

321–334

Effects of waiting on the satisfaction with the service:


Beyond objective time measures 1
Ad Pruyn ) , Ale Smidts
Rotterdam School of Management, Erasmus UniÕersity, P.O. Box 1738, 3000 DR Rotterdam, Netherlands
Received 7 November 1996; accepted 24 March 1998

Abstract

A major concern for service managers is to counteract negative effects of waiting. In this study, the effects of objective
waiting time and waiting environment on satisfaction with the service were investigated. Two elements of the waiting
environment were distinguished: the attractiveness of the waiting room and the presence of television ŽTV. as an explicit
distracter. The mediating role of three subjective variables Žperceived waiting time, acceptable waiting time and the
Žcognitive and affective. appraisal of the wait. was explored. Waiting appears to influence satisfaction quite strongly. The
adverse effects of waiting can be soothed more effectively by improving the attractiveness of the waiting environment than
by shortening the objective waiting time. Objective waiting time influences satisfaction mainly via a cognitiÕe route:
through perceived waiting time Žin minutes. and the longrshort judgment of the wait. Perceived attractiveness of the waiting
environment operates mainly through affect, and thus serves as a mood inducer. The acceptable waiting time appears to be a
critical point of reference, since surpassing it provokes strong affective responses. Although the presence of TV did not
result in the expected effect of distraction, the tendency to watch it was found to be dependent on the length of the wait Žand
thus, boredom.. q 1998 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Time perception; Waiting environment; Waiting tolerance; Cognition; Affect; Distraction

1. Introduction management techniques Žsee, e.g., Buffa and Sarin,


1987. are helpful in this respect. Mere reduction of
Waiting for service is a pervasive and often un- objective waiting times, however, is not always fea-
avoidable experience for customers and appears to sible, because of the lack of control over the queuing
be a strong determinant of overall satisfaction with situation ŽTaylor, 1994. or due to cost considera-
the service and customer loyalty ŽPruyn and Smidts, tions.
1993; Taylor, 1994, 1995; Hui and Tse, 1996.. Ser- As customers’ satisfaction with the service will be
vice marketers often attempt to counteract negative affected by the perception of waiting Žtime., an
effects of waiting by shortening queues. Operations alternative approach to queuing is to find ways to
make time pass as quickly and pleasantly as possible.
)
Corresponding author. Tel.: q31-10-4081960; fax: q31-10-
One may thus reduce the negative effects of waiting
2120544; e-mail: a.pruyn@fac.fbk.eur.nl. even when objective waiting times cannot be short-
1
Both authors contributed equally to this article. ened. Maister Ž1985. suggested a number of condi-

0167-8116r98r$19.00 q 1998 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved.


PII: S 0 1 6 7 - 8 1 1 6 Ž 9 8 . 0 0 0 0 8 - 1
322 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

tions, such as information about the reasons for A time perception management approach is pur-
andror length of the delay, the degree to which the sued in this study by investigating how objective
waiting time is filled and the perceived fairness of waiting time and waiting environment influence sat-
the queue setup, that influence time perception dur- isfaction with the service via subjective mediators.
ing waiting. Since then, there has been an increase in To this end, we developed an integrative model,
research on how customers react to such waiting partly based on the wait experience model of Taylor
conditions ŽLarson, 1987; Clemmer and Schneider, Ž1994.. Although Taylor did not pay attention to the
1989; Dube´ et al., 1989, 1991; Hui and Tse, 1996.. waiting environment, she did assess the effects of
Probably one of the easiest conditions for man- objective waiting time on the evaluation of the ser-
agement to manipulate is one that has received sur- vice and the mediating role of affect. In modeling
prisingly little attention, namely the waiting environ- that process, however, she did not include the cus-
ment. Recently, Baker and Cameron Ž1996. have tomer’s acceptable waiting time Žor the level of
proposed how specific elements in the environment, tolerance for waiting.. Such a level of tolerance may
such as lighting, color and spatial layout, may influ- act as a subjective point of reference proven to be
ence time perception during waiting. To date, how- important in the evaluation of service quality and
ever, empirical research on this topic has been non- satisfaction with the service Žsee, e.g., Zeithaml et
existent. It is therefore our purpose to study as to al., 1993.. We expect that this variable has a strong
what extent Žand how. the waiting environment af- impact on affective responses to the wait. Moreover,
fects the experience of the wait, and eventually, one may question as to what extent the acceptable
overall satisfaction with the service. waiting time is affected by the attractiveness of the
In this study, we distinguish two aspects of the waiting environment. The potential role of accept-
waiting environment: Ž1. the attractiveness of the able waiting time will therefore also be investigated
waiting environment based on physical attributes in this study.
such as design and decoration, and Ž2. specific stim-
uli, such as television ŽTV., purposely introduced to
the waiting environment to distract customers’ atten- 2. Conceptual background
tion from waiting. Both aspects of the waiting envi-
ronment are seen as means to positively influence In Fig. 1, the conceptual model is shown. Both
the appraisal of the wait and satisfaction with the the objective waiting time and the waiting environ-
service. The underlying process, however, may differ ment are seen as instrumental variables that affect
between these two aspects of the waiting environ- the satisfaction with the service through wait-related
ment in terms of cognitions and affects. Pruyn and subjective variables.
Smidts Ž1993. found that an attractive waiting envi- A central variable of interest is the customers’
ronment directly and positively influences satisfac- appraisal of the wait. Two components of the ap-
tion with the service. Thus, the design element of the praisal are distinguished: Ža. a cognitive component
waiting environment may induce a positive mood which reflects the perception of the time span in
which influences the appraisal of the wait without terms of long or short; and Žb. an affective compo-
necessarily influencing the perceived duration of the nent which consists of emotional responses to wait-
wait. In contrast, explicit Žor foreground. distracters ing, such as irritation, boredom, stress, etc. As noted
in the waiting environment are supposed to affect the by, e.g., Bagozzi Ž1996., the causal order of affect
cognitive timer Žinternal clock. by means of distract- and cognition is a topic of controversy. Whereas
ing the attention from the passage of time itself Lazarus Ž1982. argues that affect is the product of
ŽZakay, 1989.. This would result in shorter perceived cognitive processes, others claim that affect may
durations and thus, a more positive appraisal of the precede cognitions Že.g., Zajonc, 1980.. Thus, the
wait. As both the design element and explicit dis- experience of a lengthy wait may negatively affect
tracters may influence satisfaction, it is important to the subject’s emotions, but alternatively, the affec-
investigate differences in effect and process of these tive state Žsuch as irritation because of waiting. may
two characteristics of the waiting environment. also influence the time perception ŽHornik, 1993..
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 323

Fig. 1. A model of customer’s reactions to waiting.

The causal order is therefore not stated in advance Moreover, we expect that the perceived duration
and left open for empirical testing. of the wait will particularly affect the cognitive
component of the appraisal thereof, because an esti-
2.1. Waiting time mation of a time span in terms of minutes will be
more closely related to a longrshort judgment than
An important factor that will affect customers’ to the emotional response that may eventually fol-
appraisal of the wait is the length of the wait Žwait- low. Hence we hypothesize,
ing time.. As already suggested by Hornik Ž1984.,
H2: The longer the perceived waiting time, the more
waiting time has to be distinguished in objective
Ži.e., the actual or clocked. waiting time and the negative the appraisal of the wait. The cognitive
component will be affected more strongly than the
perceived duration. The latter variable is defined as
affective component.
the subjective estimate of the waiting time in min-
utes. Of course, perceived waiting time will depend In line with the expectancy–disconfirmation
on the objective waiting time ŽAllan, 1979.. It has paradigm in consumer satisfaction literature ŽOliver,
been suggested, however, that customers’ reactions 1980; Tse and Wilton, 1988. and in service quality
to waiting are more strongly affected by perceived literature ŽZeithaml et al., 1993., we assume that
than by objective waiting time ŽHornik, 1984; Pruyn subjects employ a point of reference from which
and Smidts, 1993.. This would imply that perceived they judge the waiting experience: the acceptable
waiting time acts as a mediator. Thus, waiting time. The acceptable waiting time is defined
as the maximum number of minutes tolerated in a
H1: The effect of objective waiting time on the specific waiting situation. It should be regarded as
appraisal of the wait is mediated by the perceived the minimum level of service the customer expects.
waiting time. As such, the concept of acceptable waiting time is
324 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

treated in a similar way as the definition of service attentional model and segmentation model is offered
adequacy level ŽZeithaml et al., 1993.. The model by empirical research which has shown that time
asserts that the acceptable waiting time will serve as estimates are dependent on whether subjects have to
a standard with which the perceived time is com- perform a prospective or a retrospective time estima-
pared, resulting in a Žpositive or negative. appraisal tion task ŽBlock, 1989.. In a prospective task, sub-
of the wait. A significant disconfirmation of the jects are requested to make a time judgment before
acceptable waiting time will thus affect the appraisal they experience the event Žtime is made salient to
of the wait. As research has shown that disconfirma- them.. In a retrospective task, subjects have to make
tion influences emotional responses in service en- time judgments about an interval without being in-
counters ŽWoodruff et al., 1983; Bolton and Drew, formed beforehand that they will be requested to do
1994., we propose that particularly the affective so. Zakay Ž1989. suggests that the attentional model
component of the appraisal of the wait will be explains prospective time estimates, whereas retro-
affected. By surpassing the acceptable level, waits spective time estimates are explained more accu-
will not just be experienced as long, but as intolera- rately by the contextual change model. Since for
bly long; shorter perceived waiting times can be a subjects in waiting situations time can be considered
relief, instead. We therefore hypothesize, to be a salient attribute Žespecially when they have
an appointment., for them time estimation thus re-
H3: Differences between the acceptable waiting time sembles a prospective rather than a retrospective
and the perceived waiting time will affect the ap- task. Consequently, we propose that in waiting situa-
praisal of the wait. The affective component will be tions, the effect of an explicit distracter on perceived
affected more strongly than the cognitive component. duration is explained best by the attentional model.
Hence,
2.2. Waiting enÕironment
H4: Waiting customers exposed to an explicit dis-
Waiting environments consist of physical design tracter will estimate the waiting time to be shorter as
elements Žsuch as architecture, layout and lighting, compared with customers who are not explicitly
which create attractiveness in terms of, e.g., comfort, distracted.
spaciousness and atmosphere., and elements of ex-
plicit distraction Žsuch as reading material and TV.. Another element of the waiting environment con-
According to the attentional model of time percep- cerns its perceived attractiveness Žas related to the
tion ŽThomas and Weaver, 1975., explicit distraction physical design of the waiting room.. Whereas a
leads to shorter perceived durations of waiting. This pleasant environment can be expected to increase
is due to the fact that attention is drawn away from their tolerance for waiting, customers may find, e.g.,
the internal clock responsible for the perception of a cold and noisy waiting room with uncomfortable
time passage ŽZakay, 1989.. Thus, watching TV in a seating less acceptable. Thus,
waiting room, for example, would cause customers
to be less occupied with time since their cognitive H5: The greater the perceived attractiveness of the
systems are processing Žnon-temporal. external stim- waiting environment, the longer customers are will-
uli. ing to wait.
The contextual change Žor segmentation. model of
time perception ŽPoynter, 1983; Block, 1990. alter- We also propose that an attractive environment
natively predicts that the more changes there are in directly influences customers’ moods ŽMehrabian and
the environment during an interval, the longer its Russell, 1974; Bitner, 1992; Baker and Cameron,
perceived duration. This would imply that a dis- 1996. and thus the affective component of the ap-
tracter that segments the time interval into easily praisal of the wait. Moreover, there is ample evi-
remembered chunks of information Žsuch as a TV dence that mood states may bias overall evaluations
with various programs. increases the perceived dura- and judgments Žsuch as satisfaction with the service
tion of waiting. A reconciliation of the conflicting and evaluations of quality. in mood-congruent direc-
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 325

tions ŽGardner, 1985; Mayer et al., 1992.. Thus, we clinics: gynaecology, dermatology, ophthalmology,
expect that customers perceiving the waiting envi- laryngology, or internal medicine. Subjects’ ages
ronment as attractive are brought into a positive varied between 16 and 89 Žmean age: 46.. All sub-
mood, which will set the tone for a favorable ap- jects had an appointment. The sample consists of a
praisal of the wait. A positive mood will also directly demographically and socio-economically diverse
increase the overall satisfaction with the service. group of subjects with all sorts of medical com-
Therefore we expect that, plaints Žexcept cardiac illnesses and cancer, which
were purposely left out..
H6: The greater the perceived attractiveness of the
waiting environment, the more positive the appraisal 3.2. Experimental design and procedure
of the wait. The affective component will be affected
more strongly than the cognitive component. The study was conducted on six consecutive
working days. In each hospital, observations took
H7: The greater the perceived attractiveness of the place in three randomly selected waiting rooms. The
waiting environment, the greater the satisfaction with waiting rooms differed Žboth within and between the
the service. hospitals. with respect to layout, atmosphere, decora-
tion and furniture Žarrangement.. The TV set was
Finally, a number of studies have shown that the always located in one of the corners of the waiting
appraisal of the wait will influence Ždis.satisfaction room so that each visitor was able to watch the
with the service process Že.g., Pruyn and Smidts, screen. Each day was divided into four blocks of 2 h.
1993; Taylor, 1994, 1995; Hui and Tse, 1996.. Hence Per block, it was randomly decided whether the TV
we hypothesize, set was turned on or off. Of the subjects, 53.4% Ž64
men; 116 women. were exposed to a variety of light
H8: The more negative the appraisal of the wait, the entertainment Žsports, nature, medical information,
less satisfaction with the service. etc... In between these programs, small blocks of
commercials were broadcast. The remaining subjects
It will be explored as to what extent this effect is Ž63 men; 94 women. were waiting under the same
to be attributed to either the cognitive or the affec- conditions but with the TV set turned off and con-
tive component of the appraisal of the wait. cealed behind doors.
Observers, trained graduate students, sat incon-
spicuously in the waiting rooms to watch the sub-
3. Method jects. A systematic random sample was drawn by
selecting every tenth visitor for observation. The
3.1. General setting and subjects total time spent in the waiting room was objectively
assessed by registering the time of entrance and the
The study involved waiting in Dutch hospital time the subject was called into the physician’s
polyclinics. In almost one third of the polyclinics in room. After consultation, subjects were intercepted
the Netherlands, an advertising agency has installed by an interviewer and asked to fill out a question-
video equipment and TV monitors. This service, naire. Response rate was 70.4% Ž337 of 479 subjects
already operative for 4 yr, is being exploited under observed in the waiting rooms..
the argument that by watching the programmed in-
formation, visitors to polyclinics would be distracted 3.3. Measures
from waiting and thus be less aggravated by an
eventual delay. The service is being paid for by The questionnaire started by asking for the time
advertisers who buy commercial time on the medium. of the appointment. Thus, the objective waiting time
Subjects were 127 men and 210 women visiting a could be calculated by taking the difference between
polyclinic in one of three hospitals. In each hospital, the appointment time and the Žregistered. time the
subjects were visiting one of the following poly- subject was called into the physician’s room. Two
326 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

perceived duration variables were subsequently as- perceived attractiveness of the waiting environment
sessed. Perceived time spent in the waiting room was assessed by means of four attributes: atmo-
was measured by asking the subject to estimate Žin sphere, cleanliness, spaciousness and climate. This
minutes. the total time spent in the waiting room Žthe scale also appeared to be one-dimensional Ž x 2 Ž2. s
time span between arrival, sometimes amply before 6.01, p s 0.05; GFI s 0.99; TLI s 0.91; RMSEAs
the appointment and consultation with the doctor.. 0.084; CFI s 0.97., although lower in reliability
Perceived waiting time was assessed by asking the ŽCronbach a s 0.62.. Again, a composite scale was
subject to give an estimation Žin minutes. of the time formed. The nine waiting rooms differed signifi-
that passed from the moment of appointment until cantly in perceived attractiveness Ž F8,323 s 3.48, p -
the start of the consultation. Next, overall satisfac- 0.0007.. The average attractiveness is 4.17 and ranges
tion with the service in the polyclinic was measured from 3.91 to 4.54.
Žon a 10-point scale ranging from ‘ very dissatisfied’ Finally, the subject was asked to indicate the
Ž1. to ‘ very satisfied’ Ž10... maximum acceptable waiting time Žin minutes. in
Then the appraisal of the wait was assessed. The such a polyclinic. Subjects in the TV condition were
cognitive component of the appraisal was measured posed an additional question about the intensity of
on a five-point scale ranging from ‘ very short’ Ž1. to watching TV Žfive-point scale.. A number of demo-
‘ very long’ Ž5.. The affective component consisted graphic variables Žsex, age, education. were also
of five semantic differential items Žfive-point scales. measured for descriptive purposes.
on which subjects were asked to rate the irritation,
fairness, annoyance, boredom and stress experienced
during waiting. A confirmatory factor analysis 4. Results
ŽSteenkamp and van Trijp, 1991. showed that this
affect measurement is one-dimensional Ž x 2 Ž5. s
9.30, p s 0.10; GFI s 0.99; TLI s 0.98; RMSEAs 4.1. Characterization of the wait
0.055; CFI s 0.99. and sufficiently reliable ŽCron-
bach a s 0.80.. A composite scale was formed by On average, patients of the polyclinics appear to
averaging these items. Also on five-point scales, the have spent 23 min in the waiting room Žsee Table 1.;

Table 1
Summary statistics Žmeans. of the waiting experience and effects of the TV as an explicit distracter Žtime is presented in minutes.
Total No TV TV Significancea Do not Watch Significanceb
p- watch TV TV p-
n 337 157 180 51 110
Objective time spent 23.1 23.7 22.5 ns 19.6 24.5 0.039
Perceived time spent 22.6 23.8 21.6 ns 15.0 25.4 0.001
Objective waiting time 15.0 15.7 14.5 ns 10.2 16.7 0.004
Perceived waiting time 16.1 17.4 14.9 ns 9.4 18.2 0.003
Acceptable waiting time 23.1 23.5 22.8 ns 22.9 22.8 ns

Appraisal of the wait


(on fiÕe-point scales)c
Longrshort judgment 2.5 2.6 2.4 ns 2.0 2.7 0.006
Affective response 2.0 2.0 2.1 ns 1.9 2.2 0.064
Satisfaction with the service 7.9 7.9 7.9 ns 8.3 7.8 0.025
Ž10-point scale.
a
t-tests Ž df s 335.; ns denotes p ) 0.10 Žtwo-tailed..
b
t-tests Ž df s 159.; ns denotes p ) 0.10 Žtwo-tailed..
c
High scores represent negative responses Žlong, annoying, irritating, boring, unfair, stressful..
Note: performing nonparametric tests yields an identical pattern of results.
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 327

28% spent more than half an hour. Since most through four of Table 1. The results show that no
patients arrive earlier than their scheduled appoint- significant difference between the experimental con-
ment, the mean waiting time is shorter: 15 min Ži.e., ditions appears for the perceived waiting time Ž t s
about two-thirds of the time spent in the waiting 1.19, p s 0.24.. Neither do we find a significant
room is to be classified as waiting time.. Although effect of the TV on the cognitive component Žthe
the mean objective waiting time appears to be rela- longrshort judgment: t s 1.20, p s 0.24. or on af-
tively short, the variation is quite substantial Žstan- fective responses to the wait Ž t s 0.27, p s 0.78..
dard deviation is 13.5 min.. Whereas 31% of the This would lead to the conclusion that the presence
patients were served within 5 min of their appoint- of a TV does not serve as a distracter in the waiting
ment time Ž10% were served exactly on time or even room as it does not result in any change in the
sooner., 15% of the patients had to wait longer than waiting experience.
half an hour. An explanation for the absence of effects might
Patients are quite competent at estimating the be that patients in the waiting room simply do not
waiting time. The correlation between actual and watch TV because they are talking to one another,
perceived waiting time is 0.73 Ž p - 0.001.. In addi- reading magazines, etc. In the questionnaire, the
tion, when the objective and perceived waiting time respondents reported their watching intensity. About
are compared per indiÕidual, there is a tendency that 33% stated not to have watched TV at all; 59%
overestimation occurs especially when patients have reported to have watched ‘every now and then’ and
to wait for a considerable length of time. Underesti- 8% reported to have watched Žalmost. continuously.
mation, however, seems to be the dominant response One may expect that effects of the TV on the
when the patients’ wait is relatively short. perceived waiting time Žand appraisal of the wait.
The mean acceptable waiting time is 23 min would particularly occur for people who in fact
ŽSD s 12.7.. Only 10% of the patients are willing to watched it. To investigate this, in Table 1, the expe-
accept more than 30 min of waiting and nobody rimental group that is exposed to the TV is split up
appears to accept waiting times of more than an on the basis of the reported watching behavior. This
hour. The maximum acceptable waiting time for the reveals a number of interesting Žalthough unex-
majority of patients is clearly between 11 and 30 pected. findings Žcolumns five through seven in Table
min. In comparing objective waiting time with the 1.. For the majority of variables, a significant differ-
acceptable waiting time, it appears that for three- ence appears between viewers and non-viewers. Sub-
quarters of the patients, the actual waiting time jects who watch TV spend more time in the waiting
remains within the tolerance level set by the patient room, they have to wait longer, perceive the wait as
Žthe same percentage of patients does not perceiÕe longer, feel more negative about the wait and are less
any surpassing of the acceptable waiting time either.. satisfied with the service. However, corrected for
However, despite the fairly high mean acceptance objective waiting timerobjective time spent Žin anal-
level, for 8.5% of the patients, actual waiting times yses of covariance., these significant differences be-
appear to exceed their set level by more than 15 min. tween viewers and non-viewers disappear for all
Table 1 further shows that, on average, patients variables except for perceived time spent Ž F1,156 s
consider the wait as somewhat lengthy. Their affec- 4.30, p - 0.04.: viewers overestimate the time spent
tive response Žin terms of irritation, boredom, fair- in the waiting room, whereas non-viewers underesti-
ness, etc.. is less negative. Satisfaction with the mate this time.
service is high Ža mean of 7.9 on a 10-point scale.. From these findings, we cannot confirm our
proposition that watching TV distracts customers
from the wait and, as a consequence, makes the time
4.2. The effect of TV as a source of distraction in the pass quicker. On the contrary, it seems that the
waiting room causal order is the other way around: patients who
have to wait longer and are thus more annoyed, seem
The experimental effects of TV in the waiting to be more eager for distraction and start watching
room ŽHypothesis H4. are presented in columns two TV.
328 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

Fig. 2. Estimated path model Žstandardized regression coefficients, t-values in brackets..

4.3. Test of the model: Procedure specifying a path from perceived waiting time to
‘disconfirmation of the acceptable waiting time’. 2
In order to test the eight hypotheses underlying Third, in the theoretical model, the causal order of
the theoretical model presented in Fig. 1, the direct the cognitive and affective component of the ap-
and indirect effects were estimated by means of path praisal of the wait was not specified. Therefore, in
analysis Žusing LISREL, Joreskog¨ ¨
and Sorbom, the empirical model, we specified reciprocal effects
1989.. In Fig. 2, the results of the analysis are between cognitive and affective components: both
shown. Before the results are discussed, we describe components are expected to influence one another.
the procedure of analysis. First, as no effects of the This specification of a non-recursive model has con-
explicit distracter Žthe TV. were found ŽHypothesis sequences for the test of Hypotheses H2, H3 and H6,
H4., we did not include this variable in the estima- in which effects of, respectively, perceived waiting
tion. Second, for the test of Hypothesis H3, we time, disconfirmation of the acceptable waiting time
introduced the variable ‘disconfirmation of the ac- and perceived attractiveness of the waiting environ-
ceptable waiting time’ which represents the differ- ment on both components of the appraisal of the wait
ence Žin minutes. between the acceptable waiting were proposed Ža total of six paths.. In order for the
time and the perceived waiting time. A negative non-recursive model to be identified, at least one of
score on this variable means that the subject per- ¨
these six paths has to be constrained to zero ŽJore-
ceives the waiting time to be longer than acceptable ¨
skog and Sorbom, 1989.. Consequently, Hypotheses
Žnegative disconfirmation.. A positive Ždisconfirma-
tion. score means that the perceived waiting time 2
In contrast to Mittal et al. Ž1998. who found that negative
stays within the zone of tolerance. Since this variable disconfirmation had a greater impact on satisfaction than positive
is a difference score, it is correlated with its compo- disconfirmation, we found that the effect of disconfirmation is
nents. This relationship is taken into account by symmetrical Žand also linear..
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 329

H2, H3 and H6 cannot be tested in one run. In the relaxing the path from ‘disconfirmation of the ac-
analysis, we proceeded as follows. First, a model ceptable waiting time’ to ‘longrshort judgment’; the
was fit in which three paths were constrained to zero other effects are even smaller.
Ži.e,. the paths from ‘perceived waiting time’ to
4.4. Test of the model: Findings
‘affective response’, from ‘disconfirmation of the
acceptable waiting time’ to ‘longrshort judgment’, In Fig. 2, the standardized regression coefficients
and from ‘perceived attractiveness of the waiting of the model are presented together with t-values in
environment’ to ‘longrshort judgment’.. Next, by brackets. The covariance matrix was analyzed and
relaxing these constraints one by one and performing ML estimates are presented. The coefficient of rela-
a chi-square difference test, it could be tested whether tive multivariate kurtosis was 1.13, indicating that
any of these effects was significant. Note that the the assumption of multivariate normality is tenable.
three effects constrained to zero deal with the paths Moreover, sample size is substantial Ž n s 294. and
that are proposed to have the smallest effects. For the ratio of sample size to the number of free param-
example, in Hypothesis H2, the effect of perceived eters is 16:1, which is amply beyond the required
waiting time is expected to be larger on the 10:1 ratio ŽBaumgartner and Homburg, 1996. for
longrshort judgment than on the affective response; trustworthy parameter estimates and valid tests of
here, the stronger version of that hypothesis is tested: significance. The fit of the model is very good
perceived waiting time affects the longrshort judg- Ž x 2 Ž10. s 14.25, p s 0.16; GFI s 0.99; TLI s 0.99;
ment and not the affective response. The results of RMSEAs 0.038; CFI s 1.00..
this test procedure unequivocally showed that the Hypothesis H1 concerns the mediating role of
parameters of none of the three constrained paths is perceived waiting time. As can be seen in Table 2,
significantly different from zero. The maximum in- perceived waiting time affects the appraisal of the
crease in fit Ž xd2 Ž1. s 0.47, p s 0.49. is obtained by wait more strongly than objective waiting time. Also,

Table 2
Pearson correlations between waiting time variables and the longrshort judgment, affective response, and satisfaction with the service
Ž n s 296.
Appraisal of the wait Satisfaction with
Longrshort judgment Affective response the service
Žhigh s long. Žhigh s positive.
Affective response y0.62 y
Satisfaction with the service y0.47 0.39 y
Objective time spent Žminutes. 0.38 y0.19 y0.24
Perceived time spent Žminutes. 0.55 y0.35 y0.40
Objective waiting time Žminutes. 0.42 y0.26 y0.32
Perceived waiting time Žminutes. 0.51 y0.38 y0.36
Acceptable waiting time Žminutes. ns 0.18 a ns
Disconfirmation of acceptable y0.37 0.34 0.31
waiting time Žobjective. b
Disconfirmation of acceptable y0.48 0.43 0.36
waiting time Žperceived. b
Perceived waiting environment ns 0.19 0.29
Žhigh s attractive.
a
p - 0.01.
All other correlations: p - 0.001.
ns denotes p ) 0.05 Žtwo-tailed..
b
Disconfirmation of the acceptable waiting time represents the difference Žin minutes. between the acceptable waiting time and the
Žobjective or perceived. waiting time: a positive score means that the waiting time stays within the zone of tolerance Žpositive
disconfirmation., a negative score implies that waiting time exceeds the level of tolerance Žnegative disconfirmation..
330 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

there appears to be a significant and strong, positive satisfaction with the service Ž b s 0.23., but no sig-
relationship between objective and perceived waiting nificant direct effect exists on the longrshort judg-
time Žsee Fig. 2, b s 0.73.. By including the per- ment. These findings support Hypotheses H6 and
ceived waiting time as a mediator in the model, the H7.
direct effect of objective waiting time on the ap- In Hypothesis H8, we proposed a relationship
praisal of the wait disappears. Thus, Hypothesis H1 between the appraisal of the wait and satisfaction
is supported. with the service. Both cognitive and affective com-
Perceived waiting time has a significant effect on ponents indeed have a significant impact on satisfac-
the longrshort judgment. The direct effect of per- tion. Although it seems that the cognitive component
ceived waiting time on the affective response is not Žthe longrshort judgment. is the stronger predictor
significant. The indirect effect of perceived waiting of satisfaction Ž b s y0.37 vs. b s 0.12., a chi-
time on the affective response Žthrough the disconfir- square difference test on the equality of the parame-
mation of the acceptable waiting time. is smaller ters does not confirm this Ž xd2 Ž1. s 1.67, p s 0.19..
Žy0.81 = 0.34 s y0.28. than the direct effect on The cognitive and affective component of the
the longrshort judgment Ž b s 0.37.. These results appraisal of the wait affect one another reciprocally.
support Hypothesis H2. The effect of cognitive response Žlongrshort judg-
In Hypothesis H3, it was proposed that differ- ment. on affective response to the wait seems some-
ences between the perceived waiting time and the what smaller Ž b s y0.16. than the effect of affect
acceptable waiting time affect the appraisal of the on cognition Ž b s y0.39.. Testing the equality of
wait Žin particular, the affective component.. As can the parameters shows that there is only a tendency of
be seen in Fig. 2, disconfirmation of the acceptable difference between them Ž xd2 Ž1. s 2.78, p s 0.096..
waiting time indeed affects the affective response to Therefore, these findings corroborate the reported
the wait Ž b s 0.34.; no significant direct effect on mood effects on time perception of Hornik Ž1993.,
the longrshort judgment appears, only an indirect but only marginally.
effect through the affective component Ž0.13.. Thus, Modification indices suggested that the fit of the
Hypothesis H3 is also supported: the affective com- model could be improved only by adding one, not
ponent fully mediates the effect of disconfirmation hypothesized, direct effect from ‘disconfirmation of
of the acceptable level of waiting on the longrshort the acceptable waiting time’ to satisfaction Ž b s
judgment. 3 0.14, t s 2.46.. This effect seems to make sense
In Hypothesis H5, an effect of the perceived when one realizes that, e.g., violation of the level of
attractiveness of the waiting environment on the tolerance will not only affect subjects’ moods with
acceptable waiting time was proposed. As the corre- respect to the waiting itself, but will also have more
lation between the two variables is not significant general repercussions in terms of the overall evalua-
Ž r s 0.05, p s 0.39., no support is obtained for this tion of the service encounter and the service provider.
hypothesis. Finally, considering that satisfaction with the ser-
In Fig. 2, it can be seen that the perceived attrac- vice in a hospital clinic depends on more factors than
tiveness of the waiting environment does have an only time-related aspects, the relationship between
effect on the affective response Ž b s 0.14., and on satisfaction and the variables related to waiting is
quite substantial Ž R 2 s 0.29..
3
Note that the acceptable waiting time itself hardly influences
the appraisal of the wait Žsee Table 2: acceptable waiting time is 5. Discussion and conclusions
not significantly related to the longrshort judgment and correlates
only r s 0.18 with affective response.. Instead, it is rather the Managing customers’ waiting is relevant because
difference between the objective Žand even more so, the perceived waiting appears to have a strong effect on overall
waiting time. and the level of acceptance that affects the satisfaction with the service. In particular, this study
longrshort judgment Ž r sy0.37; r sy0.48. and the affective
response Ž r s 0.34; r s 0.43.. These findings show that the ac-
indicates the value of heeding the waiting environ-
ceptable waiting time serves as a notable reference point for the ment. The waiting environment proves to be a
appraisal of the wait. stronger determinant of service satisfaction than ob-
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 331

jective waiting time Žthe total standardized effect of waiting minutes; Žb. the transformation of perceived
the waiting environment on satisfaction is 0.27, waiting time into a longrshort judgment; and Žc. the
whereas the effect of objective waiting time is evaluation of any difference between perceived and
y0.17.. Therefore, service managers can be advised acceptable waiting time Žor the Žaffective. response
to be less focused on shortening waiting times, but to the disconfirmation process.. Queue managers
pay special attention to their customers’ waiting may interfere in any of these processes. For example,
conditions instead. information about actual waiting times can be pro-
The study also addresses the psychological pro- vided, which consequently positively influences both
cesses involved in waiting. Understanding these pro- perceived waiting time Žand the longrshort judg-
cesses is essential in order to influence the waiting ment. and the outcome of the disconfirmation pro-
experience more effectively. The empirical model cess. Of course, such a strategy will be especially
shows how objective waiting time and waiting envi- effective when customers tend to overestimate their
ronment affect satisfaction with the service. It can be waiting time in the queue.
concluded that objective waiting time affects satis- Secondly, paying attention to the waiting enÕiron-
faction through both a cognitive and an affective ment is certainly one of the easiest and perhaps one
route, whereas the most influential variable, the wait- of the most effective means of improving customers’
ing environment, operates through affects only. Spe- appraisal of the wait and satisfaction with the ser-
cific conclusions are stated below. vice. The waiting environment clearly influences the
Firstly, the total effect of objectiÕe waiting time affective response to the wait Žstandardized effect:
on satisfaction Žy0.17. can be partitioned in an 0.14.. It also affects satisfaction: there appears to be
effect through perceived waiting time and the a strong direct effect Ž0.23. and a Žmuch smaller.
longrshort judgment Žy0.112. and in an effect indirect effect through the appraisal of the wait
through the disconfirmation of the acceptable wait- Ž0.04.. As there is no significant direct effect of the
ing time and affective response Žy0.055.. The cog- waiting environment on cognitive responses Žneither
nitive route thus appears to be stronger than the on perceived waiting time nor on the longrshort
affective route. With respect to the cognitive route, judgment., it can be concluded that an attractive
the results indicate that it is not so much the per- waiting environment predominantly serves as a mood
ceived number of minutes that a customer has to inducer which colors the evaluation of the wait and
wait which affects satisfaction as it is the subjective the overall service process ŽGardner, 1985.. An at-
transformation of these minutes into a longrshort tractive waiting environment can thus prevent cus-
judgment. In the longrshort judgment, customers tomers from becoming irritated or bored very quickly
use their own frame of reference from which they even though they may find the wait long.
appraise the waiting situation. Perceived attractiveness of the waiting environ-
Affective responses are dependent on the differ- ment did not affect the acceptable waiting time. It
ence between the perceived waiting time and the may be the case that the variation in quality of the
acceptable waiting time. A disconfirmation process different waiting rooms in this study was not sub-
thus appears to be operative in customers’ assess- stantial enough to evoke the proposed effect on
ments of waiting situations. The acceptable waiting acceptable waiting time Žnote that even the worst
time is used as a critical point of reference: if, for waiting room was still rated quite positively Ž3.91 on
example, the perceived waiting time exceeds the a five-point scale... Also, acceptable waiting time
level of tolerance, waits become ‘too long’ Žand may be more strongly determined by a number of
thus, aggravating.. other situation-specific variables Že.g., being in a
In summary, objective waiting time by itself can hurry, having information about the reasons for the
only explain about 10% of the variance in satisfac- delay. andror trait-like properties Že.g., impatience,
tion with the service Žsee Table 2.. Three intermedi- neuroticism.. 4
ary processes determine the impact of objective wait-
ing time on satisfaction: Ža. the Žimperfect. estimate
4
of the objective waiting time in terms of perceived We thank an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.
332 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

In this study, the presence of TV as a source of objective waiting times as is the case in operations
explicit distraction in the waiting environment did management. In designing effective service opera-
not lead to shorter perceived waiting times. Appar- tions, marketing and operations management should
ently, the TV does not distract the attention from the thus combine their insights into queuing phenomena.
passage of time Žas the attentional model ŽZakay, Further research is required to confirm and en-
1989. would predict. and does not serve to reduce hance our findings and model. As the waiting envi-
wait-related boredom. Our results would rather seem ronment appears to be an influential factor in cus-
to indicate that it is sooner the length of the wait tomers’ reactions to waiting, research into the rele-
Žand thus, boredom. that induces people to start vant factors which constitute an attractive waiting
watching. Presumably, people start to watch TV only environment is required. 5 Baker and Cameron Ž1996.
after some time, usually after they have finished have suggested a number of such factors. A frame-
reading or flicking through the magazines or have work for studying these factors is provided by Bitner
exhausted the ‘small talk’ with others in the waiting Ž1992.. Research is also required to detect manageri-
room. This would imply that, while waiting, self- ally relevant instruments to influence the subjective
paced means of distraction Že.g., reading material. waiting variables. For example, service managers
are preferred, and only after a while does one start to may benefit from information about how perceived
seek distraction elsewhere Žvia, e.g., television.. Even waiting time and the longrshort judgment are influ-
with subjects that reported to have watched televi- enced by, e.g., queue configurations and information
sion intensively, we did not find effects of this about the length and reasons of the delay. Some
distracter in line with the attentional model. On the research findings in this respect already exist Žsee,
contrary, there is an indication that viewers sooner e.g., Larson, 1987; Clemmer and Schneider, 1989;
overestimate the time spent in the waiting room. A Hui and Tse, 1996.. A number of social psychologi-
similar finding has been reported by Hui et al. cal and time perception theories that may help to
Ž1997., who used music as a background Žand there- explain these relationships have been suggested by
fore less explicit. distracter. Taking into account that Smidts and Pruyn Ž1993.. Factors that affect the
in our study the TV programming consisted of short, acceptable waiting time Žsuch as those identified by
varied items interrupted by commercials, the ten- Zeithaml et al., 1993. also warrant serious attention
dency to overestimate is more in line with the con- in further queuing research.
textual change model of time perception ŽZakay and Another topic for further research concerns the
Hornik, 1992.. This model predicts that the more a question as to how customers can be distracted dur-
time interval is segmented, the longer its perceived ing waiting. In this study, no such effects were
duration. In waiting rooms, it might therefore be found. It should be noted, however, that the explicit
wiser to show longer and less varied programs. distracter was operationalized in a distinctive manner
Ži.e., exposure to a commercially exploited TV. and
studied in a specific setting Žwaiting in a hospital..
6. Implications and further research These factors may hinder the generalizability of the
results. As patients might well enter the setting with
In this study, a perception management approach a certain level of uneasiness, perhaps television does
to queuing was presented. As waiting appears to not perform the same function as it might in environ-
affect customers’ satisfaction and thus the competi- ments where customers are usually less anxious.
tive advantage of a company, marketers must deal
explicitly with influential waiting factors. This study
suggests that the perceived waiting environment, the
5
perceived waiting time, the acceptable waiting time In situations where queues and waiting are really inevitable
Že.g., airport lounges, amusement parks, hospital polyclinics.,
and the appraisal of the wait should be considered
creating an attractive environment may even consist of finding
beyond objective waiting time. For queue manage- ways to benefit from customers’ waiting time Žby means of shops
ment, this implies that the performance of a service and restaurant facilities, vending products along the queue, adver-
system should not be evaluated solely on the basis of tising relevant information, etc...
A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334 333

That is to say, patients might be so strongly inter- Bagozzi, R.P., 1996. The role of arousal in the creation and
nally focused that they are ‘immune’ to environmen- control of the halo effect in attitude models. Psychology and
Marketing 13, 235–264.
tal distracters. Further research must therefore deter- Baker, J., Cameron, M., 1996. The effects of the service environ-
mine the conditions in which explicit distracters may ment on affect and consumer perception of waiting time: an
be effective means to distract customers during the integrative review and research propositions. Journal of the
wait. Academy of Marketing Science 24, 338–349.
Besides studying the Žmanagerial. instruments that Baumgartner, H., Homburg, C., 1996. Applications of structural
equation modeling in marketing and consumer research: A
can be implemented to manipulate the subjective review. International Journal of Research in Marketing 13,
waiting variables, more insight is needed with regard 139–161.
to the processes involved in the appraisal of waiting Bitner, M.J., 1992. Servicescapes: the impact of physical sur-
situations. One venue is to study the possible moder- roundings on customers and employees. Journal of Marketing
ating role of personal characteristics on several rela- 56, 57–71.
Block, R.A., 1989. Experiencing and remembering time: Affor-
tionships in the model. For example, the effect of the dances, context, and cognition. In: Levin, I., Zakay, D. ŽEds..,
waiting environment on the appraisal of the wait Time and Human Cognition: A Life Span Prospective. North-
may be moderated by customer characteristics ŽBi- Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 333–363.
tner, 1992.. Although in the present study no such Block, R.A., 1990. Models of psychological time. In: Block, R.A.
ŽEd.., Cognitive Models of Psychological Time. Lawrence
moderating effects of sex, age, education or per-
Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1–35.
ceived seriousness of the medical complaint were Bolton, R.N., Drew, J.H., 1994. Linking customer satisfaction to
found, one may expect that a number of variables service operations and outcomes. In: Rust, R.T., Oliver, R.L.
specifically related to impatience Žsuch as Type ArB ŽEds.., Service Quality: New Directions in Theory and Prac-
coronary personality., need for control or orientation tice. Sage, London, pp. 173–200.
towards time Žpast, present or future. are of influ- Buffa, E.S., Sarin, R.K., 1987. Modern ProductionrOperations
Management. Wiley, New York.
ence. Clemmer, E.C., Schneider, B., 1989. Toward understanding and
Finally, the topic of waiting has so far been controlling customer dissatisfaction with waiting, Report No.
confined to consumers waiting for service. Insights 89–115. The Marketing Science Institute, Cambridge, MA.
gained in this area can also be introduced to the Dube,´ L., Schmitt, B.H., Leclerc, F., 1989. Consumers’ reactions
realm of, for example, business-to-business market- to waiting: When delays affect the perception of service
quality. In: Srull, T.S. ŽEd.., Advances in Consumer Research,
ing and studies on the performance of distribution Vol. 16. Association Consumer Research, Provo, UT, pp.
channels so as to be able to compete more effec- 59–63.
tively in a world in which adequate management of Dube,´ L., Schmitt, B.H., Leclerc, F., 1991. Consumers’ affective
time Žperception. is becoming increasingly important response to delays at different phases of a service delivery.
for companies’ survival ŽKotler and Stonich, 1991.. Journal of Applied Psychology 21, 810–820.
Gardner, M.P., 1985. Mood states and consumer behavior: A
critical review. Journal of Consumer Research 12, 281–300.
Hornik, J., 1984. Subjective vs. objective time measures: A note
Acknowledgements on the perception of time in consumer behavior. Journal of
Consumer Research 11, 614–618.
The authors are grateful to Gerrit van Bruggen, Hornik, J., 1993. The role of affect in consumers’ temporal
judgments. Psychology and Marketing 10, 239–255.
Fred van Raaij, Diane Ricketts, and Berend Wierenga
Hui, M.K., Tse, D.K., 1996. What to tell consumers in waits of
for helpful suggestions on early drafts of this paper. different lengths: An integrative model of service evaluation.
We are also indebted to the editors, John Saunders Journal of Marketing 60, 81–90.
and Jan-Benedict Steenkamp and to two anonymous Hui, M.K., Dube, ´ L., Chebat, J.-C., 1997. The impact of music on
reviewers for their valuable comments. consumers’ reactions to waiting for services. Journal of Retail-
ing 73, 87–104.
¨
Joreskog, ¨
K.G., Sorbom, D., 1989. LISREL 7: A Guide to the
Program and Applications. SPSS, Chicago, IL.
References Kotler, P., Stonich, P.J., 1991. Turbo marketing through time
compression. Journal of Business Strategy ŽSept.rOct.. 24–29.
Allan, L.G., 1979. The perception of time. Perception and Psy- Larson, R.C., 1987. Perspectives on queues: Social justice and the
chophysics 26 Ž5., 340–354. psychology of queuing. Operations Research 35, 895–905.
334 A. Pruyn, A. Smidtsr Intern. J. of Research in Marketing 15 (1998) 321–334

Lazarus, R.S., 1982. Thoughts on the relations between emotion LISREL in validating marketing constructs. International Jour-
and cognition. American Psychologist 37, 1019–1024. nal of Research in Marketing 8, 283–299.
Maister, D.H., 1985. The psychology of waiting lines. In: Czepiel, Taylor, S., 1994. Waiting for service: The relationship between
J.A., Solomon, M.R., Surprenant, C.F. ŽEds.., The Service delays and evaluations of service. Journal of Marketing 58,
Encounter. Lexington Books, Lexington, MA, pp. 113–123. 56–69.
Mayer, J.D., Gaschke, Y.N., Braverman, D.L., Evans, T.W., Taylor, S., 1995. The effects of filled waiting time and service
1992. Mood congruent judgment is a general effect. Journal of provider control over the delay on evaluations of service.
Personality and Social Psychology 63, 119–132. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science 23, 38–48.
Mehrabian, A., Russell, J.A., 1974. An Approach to Environmen- Thomas, E.A., Weaver, W.B., 1975. Cognitive processing and
tal Psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. time perception. Perception and Psychophysics 17, 363–376.
Mittal, V., Ross, W.T., Baldasare, P.M., 1998. The asymmetric Tse, D.K., Wilton, P.T., 1988. Models of consumer satisfaction
impact of negative and positive attribute-level performance on formation: an extension. Journal of Marketing Research 25,
overall satisfaction and repurchase intentions. Journal of Mar- 204–212.
keting 62, 33–47. Woodruff, R.B., Cadotte, E.R., Jenkins, R.L., 1983. Modelling
Oliver, R.L., 1980. A cognitive model of the antecedents and customer satisfaction processes using experience-based norms.
consequences of satisfaction decisions. Journal of Marketing Journal of Marketing Research 20, 296–304.
Research 17, 460–469. Zajonc, R.B., 1980. Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
Poynter, D.G., 1983. Duration judgment and the segmentation of inferences. American Psychologist 35, 151–175.
experience. Memory & Cognition 11, 77–82. Zakay, D., 1989. An integrated model of time estimation. In:
Pruyn, A.T.H., Smidts, A., 1993. Customers’ evaluations of Levin, I., Zakay, D. ŽEds.., Time and Human Cognition: A
queues: Three exploratory studies. In: van Raaij, W.F., Life Span Perspective. North-Holland, Amsterdam, pp. 365–
Bamossy, G. ŽEds.., European Advances in Consumer Re- 397.
search, Vol. 1. Association of Consumer Research, Provo, UT, Zakay, D., Hornik, J., 1992. How much time did you wait in line?
pp. 371–382. A time perception perspective. Working paper, University of
Smidts, A., Pruyn, A.T.H., 1993. Customers’ reactions to queues: Tel Aviv, Israel.
´ J., Sureda, J.
Towards a theory of waiting and delay. In: Chıas, Zeithaml, V.A., Berry, L.L., Parasuraman, A., 1993. The nature
ŽEds.., Marketing for the New Europe: Dealing with Complex- and determinants of customer expectations of service. Journal
ity, Vol. 2. ESADE, Barcelona, pp. 1383–1402. of the Academy of Marketing Science 21, 1–12.
Steenkamp, J.-B.E.M., van Trijp, H.C.M., 1991. The use of

You might also like