You are on page 1of 6

Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Appetite
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/appet

Research report

Agreement between paper and pen visual analogue scales


and a wristwatch-based electronic appetite rating system (PRO-DiaryÓ),
for continuous monitoring of free-living subjective appetite sensations
in 7–10 year old children
P.L.S. Rumbold ⇑, C.J. Dodd-Reynolds, E. Stevenson
Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, Department of Sport, Exercise and Rehabilitation, Northumbria University, Northumberland Building, Newcastle upon Tyne NE1 8ST, UK

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Electronic capture of free-living subjective appetite data can provide a more reliable alternative to tradi-
Received 19 February 2013 tional pen and paper visual analogue scales (P&P VAS), whilst reducing researcher workload. Conse-
Received in revised form 1 May 2013 quently, the aim of this study was to explore the agreement between P&P VAS and a wristwatch-
Accepted 5 June 2013
based electronic appetite rating system known as the PRO-DiaryÓ technique, for monitoring free-living
Available online 14 June 2013
appetite sensations in 7–10 year old children. On one occasion, using a within-subject design, the 12 chil-
dren (n = 6 boys; n = 6 girls) recorded their subjective appetite (hunger, prospective food consumption,
Keywords:
and fullness), at two time points before lunch (11:30 and 12:00) and every 60 min thereafter until
Children
Subjective appetite
21:00. The agreement between the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ technique was explored using 95% limits
Pen and paper visual analogue scales of agreement and 95% confidence intervals (95% CI) calculated using the Bland and Altman (1986) tech-
PRO-DiaryÓ nique. For hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness, the 95% limits of agreement were
1 ± 25 mm (95% CI: lower limit 8 mm; upper limit +6 mm), 0 ± 21 mm (95% CI: lower limit 6 mm;
upper limit +6 mm) and 6 ± 24 mm (95% CI: lower limit 14 mm; upper limit +1 mm), respectively.
Given the advantages associated with electronic data capture (inexpensive; integrated alarm; data easily
downloaded), we conclude that the PRO-DiaryÓ technique is an equivalent method to employ when con-
tinuously monitoring free-living appetite sensations in 7–10 year old children, but should not be used
interchangeably with P&P VAS.
Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Introduction line (Hill & Blundell, 1982). This technique requires individuals to
respond to a question by placing a vertical mark between the an-
Visual analogue scales (VAS) have been successfully and exten- chor phrases. Visual analogue scales therefore require individuals
sively used to assess subjective appetite sensations (in laboratory to extrapolate a subjective sensory experience into a linear format,
and free-living settings) such as hunger, prospective food con- which then enables the subjective sensation to be measured and
sumption and fullness in children (Dodd, Welsman, & Armstrong, quantified (Gift, 1989).
2008; Moore, Dodd, Welsman, & Armstrong, 2004; Rumbold, Traditionally VAS are administered using pen and paper (P&P)
Dodd-Reynolds, & Stevenson, in press) and adolescent populations and therefore are convenient and quick to complete (Gibbons, Cau-
(Bellissimo, Thomas, Goode, & Anderson, 2007; Bozinovski et al., dwell, Finlayson, King, & Blundell, 2011; Wewers & Lowe, 1990).
2009; Rumbold & Dodd, 2007; Rumbold, St Clair Gibson, Allsop, Conversely however, the measurement and recording process is
Stevenson, & Dodd-Reynolds, 2011; Rumbold et al., 2013; Thivel, extensive, time consuming and susceptible to human error, given
Isacco, Rousset, et al., 2011; Thivel et al., 2012; Thivel, Isacco, Tail- that the horizontal lines require hand measurement and data entry
lardat, et al., 2011). Thus, VAS provide an invaluable means of mon- (Gibbons et al., 2011). Moreover, P&P VAS often require individuals
itoring free-living subjective appetite sensations in these to complete such scales unsupervised in their natural environment,
population groups. Typically in paediatric exercise and appetite which increases the likelihood of questions being omitted, com-
regulatory research, VAS take the form of 100–150 mm horizontal pleted incorrectly and not at the correct time points stipulated
lines, with extreme subjective phrases placed at either end of the by the researcher (Stratton et al., 1998).
Various electronic devices which address the limitations inher-
ent when administering P&P VAS in free-living environments have
⇑ Corresponding author. been developed. These include the Psion hand-held personal digital
E-mail address: penny.rumbold@northumbria.ac.uk (P.L.S. Rumbold).

0195-6663/$ - see front matter Ó 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.06.005
P.L.S. Rumbold et al. / Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185 181

assistant Electronic Appetite Ratings System (EARS), (ÓUniversity in young children. One 1-d test session took place in a free-living
of Leeds, UK), the Apple Newton Message Pad EARS (ÓR.J. Stubbs environment (primary school and home setting).
and M. Elia), the EARS II [ÓQueensland University of Technology The study was approved by the Faculty of Health and Life Sci-
(QUT)] and more recently the PRO-Diary (ÓCamNtech Ltd. and ences Research Ethics Committee at the University of Northumbria.
CamNtech Inc.). Such portable devices are relatively inexpensive Written informed consent was obtained from both the children
and incorporate an integrated alarm to alert participants when to and parents or guardians prior to data collection. The children were
complete the VAS questions (Gibbons et al., 2011). In addition, re- notified that the study was based on general appetite measures,
searcher error associated with the manual measurement of P&P but not told that it was concerned with the agreement between
VAS is reduced, given that data from electronic devices can be two methods of subjective appetite assessment.
downloaded directly into appropriate software programmes for
subsequent analysis (Stubbs et al., 2001). These practicalities have Participants
resulted in compliance rates of P90% being reported in the litera-
ture (Hufford & Shields, 2002). The agreement of these devices The sample size of 12 was designed to have more than 90%
compared to traditional P&P VAS have been extensively explored power to detect statistical significance at an alpha level of 0.05.
in adult populations in both laboratory-based (Delargy, Lawton, Our calculations are based on previous data which identified that
Smith, King, & Blundell, 1996; Gibbons et al., 2011; Stubbs et al., n = 11, n < 8 and n < 8 participants are required to show detectable
1997; Stubbs et al., 2001; Whybrow, Stephen, & Stubbs, 2006; Za- differences of 10 mm in subjective hunger, fullness and prospec-
bel, Ash, Bauer, & King, 2009) and free-living settings (Stratton tive food consumption respectively (Flint, Raben, Blundell, & Ast-
et al., 1998). Consensual findings in adults indicate that the EARS rup, 2000). Therefore, 12 children (n = 6 boys; n = 6 girls) aged
provide reproducible results relative to P&P VAS (Stubbs et al., (mean ± SD) 9.0 ± 1.0 years; stature 1.40 ± 0.09 m; mass 30.8 ±
2000). However, the cited authors also indicate that individuals 9.7 kg; BMI 16.6 ± 3.1 kg/m2; maturity offset 2.9 ± 1.3 years and
tend to use EARS in a more constrained manner with regards to dietary restraint 2.1 ± 0.6 took part in the study. All children were
the range of responses provided between the extreme anchor classified as normal weight (Cole, Bellizzi, Flegal, & Dietz, 2000).
phrases. Therefore in adults, it is advised that P&P VAS and EARS
methodologies should not be used interchangeably (Stubbs et al., Preliminary measures
2000).
With regards to young people however, there are a lack of stud- During a preliminary testing session at the school, the children’s
ies which have explored the agreement between free-living appe- stature and seated height were measured to the nearest 0.01 m
tite sensations assessed using P&P VAS and alternative electronic using a Harpeden Portable Stadiometer (Holtain Limited, Pembs,
devices. When working with young people in free-living environ- UK) and body mass to the nearest 0.1 kg using Avery Balance scales
ments and considering both the disadvantages associated with (Avery Berkel Ltd., West Midlands, UK). These measures were used
the administration of P&P VAS (time consuming; susceptible to hu- to calculate BMI (kg/m2) and maturity offset (Mirwald, Baxter-
man error both participants and researchers) and the inherent Jones, Bailey, & Beunen, 2002), with the latter providing an indica-
advantages of using electronic VAS (inexpensive; integrated alarm; tion of years to/from peak height velocity and thus maturation. In
high compliance rates; data easily downloaded), the use of devices the presence of the research team, the children completed the
such as the PRO-DiaryÓ technique to capture appetite sensations Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire for children (DEBQ-C) (van
electronically would be extremely useful. More recently, we have Strien & Oosterveld, 2008) to provide an indication of restrained
adopted a more representative, free-living and ecologically valid eating prevalence within the sample.
approach when conceiving paediatric exercise and appetite re- Chambers and Craig (1998) suggest that young children tend to
search study designs (Rumbold, Dodd-Reynolds, et al., 2013; Rum- respond to appetite rating scales in an extreme manner. Conse-
bold, St Clair Gibson, Allsop, et al., 2011; Rumbold et al., 2013). quently, the children were familiarised with the VAS diaries and
Thus we strongly advocate the use of robust methodological tools PRO-DiaryÓ (CamNtech Ltd. and CamNtech Inc., Cambridge, UK)
which accurately assess both free-living energy intake and appetite devices and provided with careful instructions on how to use both
sensations in paediatric populations. Indeed, we have developed a methods by trained research assistants (Hanet, Salah, & Lluch,
combined dietary data collection technique to employ with female 2010). For the P&P VAS the participants were instructed to mark
adolescents when quantifying free-living energy intake (Rumbold, a vertical line on the 100 mm VAS to correspond with what they
St. Clair Gibson, Stevenson, & Dodd-Reynolds, 2011). At the time of were subjectively feeling at the time the VAS were administered.
writing, we are unaware of any studies which have investigated With regards to the PRO-DiaryÓ device, participants were asked
the agreement between P&P VAS and alternative electronic devices (when alerted by the integrated alarm) to respond using their fin-
such as the PRO-DiaryÓ technique, for assessing free-living appe- ger via the touch sensitive ‘slider’ function at the bottom of the
tite sensations in young people. PRO-DiaryÓ interface. The touch sensitive ‘slider’ enabled the chil-
Therefore, the aim of this study was to explore the agreement dren to move a vertical cursor along the horizontal VAS and once in
between the P&P VAS method and the PRO-DiaryÓ technique for the desired place, were instructed to press the ‘O’ button to con-
assessing free-living continuous subjective appetite sensations firm their response. Subsequently, the problems encountered with
(hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness) in 7–10 year regards to extreme responses are believed to be eliminated (Price,
old children. McGrath, Rafii, & Buckingham, 1983).

Protocol
Methods
The 1-d test session took place on a day that did not involve the
Design children participating in physical education or any other active
curriculum lessons (dance/drama, etc.), as we have previously
On one occasion, using a within-subject design, the agreement demonstrated the potential for exercise to influence subjective
between the P&P VAS method and PRO-DiaryÓ technique for appetite in children and adolescents (Dodd et al., 2008; Moore
assessing free-living continuous subjective appetite sensations et al., 2004; Rumbold, St Clair Gibson, Allsop, et al., 2011). The
(hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness) was explored organisation of the 1-d test session followed the typical timings
182 P.L.S. Rumbold et al. / Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185

of a school day to ensure the study day was representative of the P&P VAS ratings. Furthermore, converting the values from decimals
children’s free-living routine. All 12 children attended school as to millimetres provided more ecologically valid results.
normal from 08:55 to 15:15 and continued with their typical
free-living routine for the remainder of the day up until 21:00.
The children recorded their subjective appetite (hunger, prospec- Analysis
tive food consumption and fullness), at two time points before
lunch (11:30 and 12:00) and every 60 min thereafter until 21:00 Microsoft Excel was used for all data analysis. Mean ± SD were
(13:00, 14:00, 15:00, 16:00, 17:00, 18:00, 19:00, 20:00 and calculated for all data. Visual analogue scale ratings for subjective
21:00). The children completed the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ appetite (hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness) for
scales immediately after one another, but were instructed to com- each method (P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ) were calculated as
plete these in a randomised order. Between 12:00 and 13:00 the time-averaged area under the curve (AUC) for the study period
children consumed school dinner or packed lunch according to (11:30–21:00; 9.5 h). Data were checked for heteroscedasticity,
their own preference. No food or drink was consumed between to enable the appropriate statistical technique to be employed.
lunchtime and the end of school (13:00–15:15), however following Subsequently, the agreement between the P&P VAS method and
this period the children were free to eat as they pleased up until PRO-DiaryÓ technique for assessing free-living continuous subjec-
21:00 (i.e. snacking, evening meals). As per the methods described tive appetite sensations (hunger, prospective food consumption
in one of our previous studies (Rumbold, Dodd-Reynolds, et al., and fullness) was explored using Pearson product-moment corre-
2013) children’s school dinner consumption was covertly weighed lation coefficients, 95% limits of agreement and 95% confidence
to the nearest gram by the research team, whilst parents/guardians intervals calculated using the Bland–Altman method (Bland & Alt-
were asked to weigh and record in a food diary their child’s packed man, 1986). Statistical significance was accepted at p < 0.05 for all
lunch intake and their child’s food and drink consumption for the analyses.
period between the children left school and the end of the study
period (15:15–21:00).
Results
P&P VAS
Hunger
The P&P VAS were presented to the children individually on
separate pages within small A5 booklets. Subjective appetite re- Mean ± SD for hunger (mm), for the P&P VAS and PRO-Diary
sponses were rated on 100 mm horizontal lines, to each of the fol- technique over the study period (11:30–21:00; 9.5 h) are pre-
lowing questions: ‘How hungry do you feel now?’ anchored by very sented in Fig. 1a. Correlation coefficients between the P&P VAS
hungry (100) and not at all hungry (0); ‘How full do you feel now?’ and PRO-Diary ratings for hunger were significant (r = 0.71;
anchored by very full (100) and not full at all (0); prospective food p = 0.009) (Fig. 1b). Mean time-averaged AUC hunger for the P&P
consumption and ‘‘How much would you like to eat now?’’ an- VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ technique were 45 ± 18 and 44 ± 13 mm,
chored by a lot (100) and nothing at all (0). The P&P VAS method in- respectively. The 95% limits of agreement were 1 ± 25 mm
volved the children marking a vertical line with a pencil at a point (Fig. 1c). This indicated that 95% of the population will have differ-
along the length of the 100 mm horizontal lines, which they felt ences between their hunger ratings of 26 to +24 mm, when as-
represented their subjective feeling at the time with regards to sessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ techniques. However,
hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness. All P&P VAS the 95% confidence interval for the mean bias indicated that the
were measured by hand by one researcher, from the minimum true difference in hunger, assessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-
score of 0 mm to the maximum score of 100 mm and entered into DiaryÓ technique lies between 8 and +6 mm. Given that the
an Excel spreadsheet. 95% confidence interval contains zero, this indicates that there is
no statistically significant difference in mean time-averaged AUC
PRO-DiaryÓ hunger ratings when assessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ
technique.
The PRO-DiaryÓ device took the form of a ‘watch like’ com-
puter, which due to its compact dimensions (51  34  8 mm)
was worn on the children’s wrist. One advantage of the PRO-Dia- Prospective food consumption
ryÓ over the P&P VAS method is that an alarm (at given time points
set by the researcher) notifies the user to respond to the questions Mean ± SD for prospective food consumption (mm), for the P&P
onscreen. The same subjective appetite questions and anchors VAS and PRO-Diary technique over the study period (11:30–21:00;
used for the P&P VAS were uploaded onto the PRO-DiaryÓ devices 9.5 h) are presented in Fig. 2a. Correlation coefficients between the
and appeared as separate VAS on the screen (24  13 mm) one at a P&P VAS and PRO-Diary ratings for prospective food consumption
time, in the same order of appearance as the P&P VAS (i.e. hunger, were significant (r = 0.75; p = 0.005) (Fig. 2b). Mean time-averaged
prospective food consumption, and fullness). The children were AUC prospective food consumption for the P&P VAS and PRO-Dia-
asked to read each question carefully and respond using their fin- ryÓ technique were 43 ± 16 and 43 ± 13 mm, respectively. The 95%
ger via the touch sensitive ‘slider’ (39  5 mm) function at the bot- limits of agreement were 0 ± 21 mm (Fig. 2c). This indicated that
tom of the PRO-DiaryÓ interface. The touch sensitive ‘slider’ 95% of the population will have differences between their prospec-
enabled the children to move a vertical cursor along the horizontal tive food consumption ratings of 21 to +21 mm, when assessed
VAS. Once the vertical cursor was in place, the children were in- using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ techniques. However, the
structed to press the ‘O’ button to confirm their response. Using 95% confidence interval for the mean bias indicated that the true
the micro USB provided with the PRO-DiaryÓ devices the data difference in prospective food consumption, assessed using the
was downloaded and subsequently imported into Microsoft Excel. P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ technique lies between 6 and +6 mm.
With regards to the unit of measurement for the PRO-DiaryÓ the Given that the 95% confidence interval contains zero, this indicates
value was given as a decimal between 0.00 and 1.00. Given that that there is no statistically significant difference in mean
the P&P VAS were rated on 100 mm horizontal lines, the PRO-Dia- time-averaged AUC prospective food consumption ratings when
ryÓ values were appropriately converted in accordance with the assessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ technique.
P.L.S. Rumbold et al. / Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185 183

(a)
100 100
(a)

Prospective food consumption (mm)


90 90
80 80
70 70
Hunger (mm)

60 60
50 50
40 40
30 30
20 20
10 10
0 0
11:30 12:00 13:00 14:00 15:00 16:00 17:00 18:00 19:00 20:00 21:00 11:30 12:00 13:00 14:00 15:00 16:00 17:00 18:00 19:00 20:00 21:00
Time (h) Time (h)
P&P VAS PRO-Diary P&P VAS PRO-Diary

P&P VAS prospective food consumption


70 70
(b) (b)
60 60
P&P VAS hunger (mm)

50 50

40

(mm)
40

30
30
20
20
10
10
0
0 0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 PRO-Diary prospective food consumption (mm)
PRO-Diary hunger (mm)
Difference in time-averaged AUC prospective food

(c)
consumption (PRO-Diary - P&P VAS; mm)

25
(c)
30 Mean+1.96SD
20
Differecne in time-averaged AUC hunger

Mean+1.96SD

20 15
(PRO-Diary - P&P VAS; mm)

10
10 5
Mean bias
0
Mean bias
0 -5
-10
-10
-15
Mean-1.96SD
-20
-20
Mean-1.96SD -25
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70
-30 Mean time-averaged AUC prospective food consumption
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 (PRO-Diary and P&P VAS; mm)
Mean time-averaged AUC hunger
(PRO-Diary and P&P VAS; mm) Fig. 2. (a) Mean ± SD for prospective food consumption ratings (mm) derived from
the P&P VAS and PRO-Diary technique over the study period (9.5 h). (b) Correlation
Fig. 1. (a) Mean ± SD for hunger ratings (mm) derived from the P&P VAS and PRO- coefficients for prospective food consumption ratings were significant (r = 0.75;
Diary technique over the study period (9.5 h). (b) Correlation coefficients for hunger p = 0.005). (c) Bland and Altman plot for time-averaged AUC for prospective food
ratings were significant (r = 0.71; p = 0.009). (c) Bland and Altman plot for time- consumption ratings. AUC, area under the curve; VAS, visual analogue scales.
averaged AUC for hunger ratings. AUC, area under the curve; VAS, visual analogue
scales.

95% confidence interval contains zero, this indicates that there is


no statistically significant difference in mean time-averaged AUC
Fullness fullness ratings when assessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ
technique.
Mean ± SD for fullness (mm), for the P&P VAS and PRO-Diary
technique over the study period (11:30–21:00; 9.5 h) are pre-
sented in Fig. 2a. Correlation coefficients between the P&P VAS
and PRO-Diary ratings for fullness were significant (r = 0.65; Discussion
p = 0.023) (Fig. 3b). Mean time-averaged AUC fullness for the P&P
VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ technique were 55 ± 16 and 48 ± 12 mm, This study was the first to investigate the agreement between
respectively. The 95% limits of agreement were 6 ± 24 mm the P&P VAS method and the PRO-DiaryÓ technique, for assessing
(Fig. 3c). This indicated that 95% of the population will have differ- free-living continuous subjective appetite sensations (hunger, pro-
ences between their fullness ratings of 31 to +18 mm, when as- spective food consumption and fullness) in 7–10 year old children.
sessed using the P&P VAS and PRO-DiaryÓ techniques. However, Consequently, there are a very limited number of directly compa-
the 95% confidence interval for the mean bias indicated that the rable studies in young people which can be used to explore these
true difference in fullness, assessed using the P&P VAS and PRO- data, therefore findings from adult studies will be used for the pur-
DiaryÓ technique lies between 14 and +1 mm. Given that the pose of this discussion.
184 P.L.S. Rumbold et al. / Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185

ger = 1.7 mm; prospective food consumption = 0.6 mm) and


(a)
120

100
indeed are more favourable compared to data presented by Zabel
et al. (2009) (hunger = 6.2 mm; prospective food consump-
Fullness (mm)

80 tion = 5 mm). Subjective fullness seemed to be the appetite


60
parameter that disagreed with the current literature, with a mean
difference between techniques of 6 mm in the present study com-
40 pared to 1.6 mm (Stratton et al., 1998) and 2.6 mm (Zabel et al.,
20 2009). Based on the power analysis (as previously described), it
suggests that the sample size of n = 12 in the present study was
0
11:30 12:00 13:00 14:00 15:00 16:00 17:00 18:00 19:00 20:00 21:00
sufficient enough to detect any within-subject differences in sub-
Time (h) jective hunger, fullness and prospective food consumption (Flint
P&P VAS PRO-Diary
et al., 2000). The mean difference values for hunger, fullness and
prospective food consumption, between techniques (P&P VAS;
80 PRO-DiaryÓ technique) all lay within the acceptable level of

70
(b) 10 mm (10%). Thus, indicating that there was no meaningful
difference in the free-living monitoring of these subjective appetite
P&P VAS fullness (mm)

60 variables in 7–10 year old children. Moreover, it is imperative to


50 explore the variability of the ratings (standard deviations) and
limits of agreement. In the present study the standard deviations
40
were high, resulting in large limits of agreement (hun-
30 ger = 1 ± 25 mm; prospective food consumption = 0 ± 21 mm;
20 fullness = 6 ± 24 mm). Thus as with the majority of adult litera-
ture, despite low mean differences between techniques (Delargy
10
et al., 1996; Stratton et al., 1998; Stubbs et al., 1997; Stubbs et al.,
0 2001; Whybrow et al., 2006; Zabel et al., 2009), electronic appetite
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
PRO-Diary fullness (mm)
monitoring devices and P&P VAS should not be used interchange-
ably, due to the large limits of agreement values. Subsequently,
Mean+1.96SD the findings from the present study confirm that this is also the rec-
Difference in time-averaged AUC fullness

20
ommendation when monitoring free-living, continuous subjective
15
(c)
(PRO-Diary - P&P VAS; mm)

10 appetite sensations (hunger, prospective food consumption and


5 Mean bias
fullness) in 7–10 year old children. Furthermore, consideration
0 regarding the inherent advantages, disadvantages, practicality of
-5 each technique (PRO-DiaryÓ and P&P VAS), research setting (exper-
-10 imental laboratory or free-living) and research question should be
-15 considered prior to the commencement of appetite and energy reg-
-20 Mean-1.96SD ulation research in young children.
-25 This was the first study to explore the agreement between the
-30 P&P VAS method and the PRO-DiaryÓ technique, for assessing
-35
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 free-living continuous subjective appetite sensations (hunger, pro-
Mean time-averaged AUC fullness spective food consumption and fullness) in 7–10 year old children.
(PRO-Diary and P&P VAS; mm) Subsequently, we believe this study to have a high degree of eco-
logical validity, given that the agreement between the two appetite
Fig. 3. (a) Mean ± SD for fullness ratings (mm) derived from the P&P VAS and PRO-
Diary technique over the study period (9.5 h). (b) Correlation coefficients for monitoring techniques were explored in a free-living, representa-
fullness ratings were significant (r = 0.65; p = 0.023). (c) Bland and Altman plot for tive environment. Despite this strength however, we did not ex-
time-averaged AUC for fullness ratings. AUC, area under the curve; VAS, visual plore the agreement between the PRO-DiaryÓ and P&P VAS
analogue scales. under a more experimental laboratory setting (i.e. fixed intakes
at fixed mealtimes) or indeed the test–retest reproducibility of
the PRO-DiaryÓ under free-living conditions. Thus taken together,
Despite being administered simultaneously, there was a slight these shortcomings suggest that these findings should only be ap-
bias for lower scores to be reported using the PRO-DiaryÓ tech- plied to the specific sample population in question (7–10 year old
nique compared to the P&P VAS for subjective hunger and fullness children), whilst appetite and energy regulation research is being
(mean differences of 1 mm and 6 mm, respectively). However, conducted in a free-living, naturalist environment. Consequently,
the 95% confidence intervals for hunger ( 8 to +6 mm) and fullness this work has provided a baseline for further work on two counts.
( 14 to +1 mm) demonstrated a lack of statistical difference in Firstly, the agreement between the P&P VAS method and the PRO-
mean time-averaged AUC hunger and fullness ratings. Similarly, DiaryÓ technique, for assessing subjective appetite sensations
for prospective food consumption there was no difference (0 mm) (hunger, prospective food consumption and fullness) in children
between the PRO-DiaryÓ technique and P&P VAS, which was also should be explored (a) in a more experimental laboratory setting
reinforced by the 95% confidence interval ( 6 to +6 mm). Such con- and (b) in various population groups (overweight and/or obese
sistently lower values (although not statistically significant) de- children and adolescents). Moreover, it must be recognised that
rived from electronic appetite rating scales compared to P&P VAS there is no ‘gold standard’ for assessing subjective appetite in en-
is a consistent finding within the available adult literature (Delargy ergy regulation studies (in both adults and children), therefore
et al., 1996; Stratton et al., 1998; Zabel et al., 2009). More specifi- the aim of the present study was not to validate the use of the
cally, the findings in the present study with regards to mean differ- PRO-DiaryÓ technique in young children (i.e. indicate that one
ences between electronic appetite ratings and P&P VAS for hunger method is better than the other). Indeed in adults, saliva samples
and prospective food consumption ( 1 mm and =0 mm, respec- provide a viable and alternative measure for exploring hormonal
tively), agree with data presented by Stratton et al. (1998) (hun- appetite responses (ghrelin) (Cetinkaya et al., 2009), however
P.L.S. Rumbold et al. / Appetite 69 (2013) 180–185 185

contamination is likely to impinge on hormonal concentrations if Moore, M. S., Dodd, C. J., Welsman, J. R., & Armstrong, N. (2004). Short-term appetite
and energy intake following imposed exercise in 9–10-year-old girls. Appetite,
nutritional interventions are implemented in energy regulation
43, 127–134.
studies. Thus, researchers should seek to explore the level of agree- Price, D. D., McGrath, P. A., Rafii, A., & Buckingham, B. (1983). The validation of
ment between simultaneous venous and arterialized finger prick visual analogue scales as ratio scale measures for chronic and experimental
blood samples for the expression of appetite hormones. An ability pain. Pain, 17, 45–56.
Rumbold, P. L. S., & Dodd, C. J. (2007). Energy intake and appetite responses of
to collect hormonal appetite data via finger prick in children and adolescent girls following a netball specific exercise protocol. Proceedings of the
adolescent populations would potentially provide an accepted Nutrition Society, 66(38A).
means for exploring hormonal appetite responses in paediatric en- Rumbold, P. L. S., Dodd-Reynolds, C. J., & Stevenson, E. J. (2013). Informing primary
school nutritional policy: Effects of mid-morning snacks on appetite and energy
ergy regulation studies. control. Food and Nutrition Sciences, 4(5), 529–537.
In conclusion, the PRO-DiaryÓ technique is an effective method Rumbold, P. L. S., St Clair Gibson, A., Allsop, S., Stevenson, E., & Dodd-Reynolds, C. J.
to employ when continuously monitoring free-living appetite sen- (2011). Energy intake and appetite following netball exercise over 5 days in
trained 13–15 year old girls. Appetite, 56(3), 621–628.
sations in 7–10 year old children, but should not be used inter- Rumbold, P. L. S., St Clair Gibson, A., Stevenson, E., & Dodd-Reynolds, C. J. (2011).
changeably with traditional P&P VAS. Agreement between two methods of dietary data collection in female
adolescent netball players. Appetite, 57, 443–447.
Rumbold, P. L. S., St Clair Gibson, A., Stevenson, E. J., King, J. A., Stensel, D. J., & Dodd-
References Reynolds, C. J. (2013). Influence of netball-based exercise on energy intake,
subjective appetite and plasma acylated ghrelin in adolescent girls. Applied
Bellissimo, N., Thomas, S. G., Goode, R. C., & Anderson, G. H. (2007). Effect of short- Physiology, Nutrition and Metabolism, 38, 1–8.
duration physical activity and ventilation threshold on subjective appetite and Stratton, R. J., Stubbs, R. J., Hughes, D. A., King, N. A., Blundell, J. E., & Elia, M. (1998).
short-term energy intake in boys. Appetite, 49(3), 644–651. Comparison of the traditional paper visual analogue scale questionnaire with an
Bland, J. M., & Altman, D. G. (1986). Statistical methods for assessing agreement Apple Newton electronic appetite rating system (EARS) in free living subjects
between two methods of clinical measurement. Lancet, i, 307–310. feeding ad libitum. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 52, 737–741.
Bozinovski, N. C., Bellissimo, N., Thomas, S. G., Pencharz, P. B., Goode, R. C., & Stubbs, R. J., Hughes, D. A., Johnstone, A. M., Rowley, E., Ferris, S., Elia, M., et al.
Anderson, G. H. (2009). The effect of duration of exercise at the ventilation (2001). Description and evaluation of a Newton-based electronic appetite rating
threshold on subjective appetite and short-term food intake in 9–14 year old system for temporal tracking of appetite in human subjects. Physiology and
boys and girls. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, Behaviour, 72(4), 615–619.
6, 66–76. Stubbs, R. J., Hughes, D. A., Johnstone, A. M., Rowley, E., Reid, C. A., Elia, M., et al.
Cetinkaya, Z., Aydin, S., Cerrahoglu, Y. Z., Ayten, R., Erman, F., & Aygen, E. (2009). (2000). The use of visual analogue scales to assess motivation to eat in human
Changes in appetite hormone (ghrelin) levels of saliva and serum in acute subjects. A review of their reliability and validity with an evaluation of new
appendicitis cases before and after operation. Appetite, 52(1), 104–107. hand-held computerized systems for temporal tracking of appetite ratings.
Chambers, C. T., & Craig, K. D. (1998). An intrusive impact of anchors in children’s British Journal of Nutrition, 84, 405–415.
faces pain scales. Pain, 78, 27–37. Stubbs, R. J., Rowley, E., Hughes, D. A., Johnstone, A. M., Reid, C. A., King, N., et al.
Cole, T. J., Bellizzi, M. C., Flegal, K. M., & Dietz, W. H. (2000). Establishing a standard (1997). Evaluating a new electronic appetite rating system (EARS). International
definition for child overweight and obesity worldwide. International survey. Journal of Obesity, 21, 391.
British Journal of Nutrition, 320, 1240–1246. Thivel, D., Isacco, L., Montaurier, C., Boirie, Y., Duché, P., & Morio, B. (2012). The 24-h
Delargy, H., Lawton, C. L., Smith, F. C., King, N., & Blundell, J. E. (1996). Electronic energy intake of obese adolescents is spontaneoulsy reduced after intense
appetite rating system (EARS). Validation of continuous automated monitoring exercise. A randomized controlled trial in calometric chambers. PLoS One, 7(1),
if motivation to eat. International Journal of Obesity, 20(4), 104. 1–7.
Dodd, C. J., Welsman, J. R., & Armstrong, N. (2008). Short-term energy intake and Thivel, D., Isacco, L., Rousset, S., Boirie, Y., Morio, B., & Duche, P. (2011). Intensive
appetite responses to exercise in lean and overweight girls. Appetite, 51, exercise. A remedy for childhood obesity? Physiology and Behavior, 102,
482–488. 132–136.
Flint, A., Raben, A., Blundell, J. E., & Astrup, A. (2000). Reproducibility, power and Thivel, D., Isacco, L., Taillardat, M., Rousset, S., Boirie, Y., Morio, B., et al. (2011).
validity of visual analogue scales in assessment of appetite sensations in single Gender effect on exercise-induced energy intake modification among obese
test meal studies. International Journal of Obesity, 24, 38–48. adolescents. Appetite, 56(3), 658–661.
Gibbons, C., Caudwell, P., Finlayson, G., King, N., & Blundell, J. E. (2011). Validation of van Strien, T., & Oosterveld, P. (2008). The children’s DEBQ for assessment of
a new hand-held electronic data capture methods for the continuous restrained, emotional, and external eating in 7–12-year-old children.
monitoring of subjective appetite sensations. International Journal of International Journal of Eating Disorders, 41(1), 72–81.
Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 8(57), 1–7. Wewers, M. E., & Lowe, N. K. (1990). A critical review of visual analogue scales in the
Gift, A. G. (1989). Visual analogue scales. Measurement of subjective phenomena. measurement of clinical phenomena. Research in Nursing and Health, 13,
Nursing Research, 286–288. 227–236.
Hanet, N., Salah, S., & Lluch, A. (2010). A training session to rate appetite feelings Whybrow, S., Stephen, J. R., & Stubbs, R. J. (2006). The evaluation of an electronic
increases the robustness of methodology. Appetite, 55(1), 168. visual analogue scale system for appetite and mood. European Journal of Clinical
Hill, A. J., & Blundell, J. E. (1982). Nutrients and behaviour. Research strategies for Nutrition, 60, 558–560.
the investigation of taste characteristics, food preferences, hunger sensations Zabel, R., Ash, S., Bauer, J. D., & King, N. A. (2009). Assessment of subjective appetite
and eating patterns in man. Journal of Psychological Research, 17, 203–212. sensations in hemodialysis patients. Agreement and feasibility between
Hufford, M., & Shields, A. (2002). Electronic subject diaries. An examination of traditional paper and pen and novel electronic appetite rating system.
applications and what in the field. Applied Clinical Trials, 11, 46–56. Appetite, 52(2), 525–527.
Mirwald, R. L., Baxter-Jones, A. D., Bailey, D. A., & Beunen, G. P. (2002). An
assessment of maturity from anthropometric measurements. Medicine and
Science in Sports and Exercise, 34(4), 689–694.

You might also like