You are on page 1of 29

t.

~~

r
; In L · A. Pervin (Ed.) .
New York~ Guilford.
Handb_?ok__.9_f_ _P_e_!_s_o_n_a_~i_t_y_: __ J_h_E'.:_o_r_y_a_nd Rese_a_r_c_~ (pp. 609· 63 7) .

Chapter 23
Emotion and Adaptation
Craig A. Smith
Vanderbilt University
Richard S. Lazarus (1990)
University of Califcrrnia-Berkeley

Subjectively, there are few psychological phe, intertwined. Many of the trait words people use
nomena that compare with emotion. Emotions to describe others' personalities (e.g., •:hostile,"
punctuate almost all the significant events in "t1m1
. 'd , " (( spite
. ful , " "c hee rfu 1, ,, "aggressive,
. "
our lives: We feel proud when we receive a "cautious," etc.) refer directly to the persons'
promotion; we become angry when we learn tendencies to respond to diverse situations
that our homes have been burglarized, we are with characteristic emotions (see Plutchik,
joyful at the births of our children; and we 1980).
experience profound grief at the death of some, Given the central position that we cede to
one we love. Furthermore, the emotions we emotions in our personal lives and the pro,
experience seem to strongly influence how we minence of emotion in literary studies of the
act in response to these events: The joy and human condition, one might expect emotion
pride encourage renewed commitment to ad, to serve as a central, organizing construct in
vance and protect career and family; the anger scientific psychology, and especially in a psy,
motivates us to seek justice and retribution; and chology of personality. If to this we add the
the sadness pushes us to seek aid and comfort widespread-and no doubt justified-belief
while coming to terms with our loss. among professionals and laypersons that emo,
The centrality of emotion in human exist, tions have a major impact on our subjective
ence is no secret in the arts. Good drama is well,being, our physical health, our social
directed toward evoking emotion in the audi, functioning, and our problem,solving perform,
ence (Scheff, 1979), and thus serves as a study ance, then understanding the emotions ought
of the affective power of various social cir, to be a major agenda for the social and biologi,
cumstances. In a complementary fashion, au, cal sciences. Historically, however, the study of
thors use emotional reactions as important emotion in psychology has been severely neg,
clues to their characters' true motivations and lected. Emotion has been considered an irrele~
personalities, revealing a pervasive assumption vantepiphenomenon (e.g., Skinner, 1953), or
that emotions and personality are inextricably has been used as a convenient chapter heading
610 RESEARCH TOPICS

for a loosely organized collection of material regarded as emotions, and that certain others
not easily covered elsewhere (see Bolles, 1974; (e.g., hunger and thirst) should not, there are
Lazarus, 1966; Tomkins, 1962). many other states (e.g., startle, interest, guilt)
This neglect, however, is currently showing about which there is little consensus (cf. Ek-
healthy signs of dissipating. Psychologists from man, 1984; Ekman, Friesen, & Simons, 1985;
all subfields profess interest in emotional pro- Izard, 1977; Ortony, 1987; Plutchik, 1980;
cesses, and research on emotion-related topics Tomkins, 1980). The lack of consensus occurs
is burgeoning. A number of volumes (e.g., because there is no absolute agreement on the
Izard, Kagan, & Zajonc, 1984; Plutchik & criteria that should be used to distinguish emo-
Kellerman, 1980; Scherer & Ekman, 1984; tion from nonemotion. The "defining" criteria
Shaver, 1984), and even new journals (e.g., have been based on specific behaviors believed
Cognition and Emotion), devoted to the study of to be produced by the emotions (e.g., Watson,
emotion have recently appeared. The same can 1919), linguistic properties of the English
be found in sociology, anthropology, and the words used to denote various states (e.g.,
neurosciences. Ortony, 1987; Ortony, Clore, & Foss, 1987),
What we think has happened is this: First, and distinctive patterns of physiological activ-
there was a loosening of the restrictive ity, such as characteristic facial expressions
epistemology of behaviorism, which allowed (e.g., Ekman, 1984; Izard, 1977; Tomkins,
investigators once again to examine thoughts 1980). An examination of previous definitional
about one's plight as factors in adaptation and attempts might lead to this conclusion: "Every-
emotion. Second, the cognitive revolution one knows what an emotion is, until asked to
allowed researchers to center attention on emo- give a definition. Then, it seems, no one
tion in common-sense or folk psychology knows" (Fehr & Russell, 1984, p. 464).
terms, to recognize the dependence of our emo- In any definition we need to distinguish be-
tional lives on motivation, and to focus atten- tween what can be said about emotion in
tion on the individual differences in what is general, and what can be said about specific
important to the person. Although heartened emotions such as anger, fear, guilt, shame,
by these developments, we maintain an uneasy pride, love, and so forth. The most common
sense that, with a few exceptions (e.g., Thoits, solution, historically, has been to base the defi-
1984), much of this work still fails to appreciate nition on descriptive characteristics of the
emotion's rightful place as a central and general reaction, which, Hillman ( 1960) has
organizing construct within psychology. In- suggested, provides substantial agreement in
stead, there is a tendency to treat it as yet the abstract. Hillman quotes the following from
another interesting, isolated subtopic. Drever's (1952, pp. 80-81) Dictionary of Psy-
We begin by addressing the question of what chology:
an emotion is. Next, we describe our own re-
cent work directed at illuminating what we see Emotion: differently described and explained by
as one of the important issues in emotion different psychologists, but all agree that it is a
theory-the role of cognitive appraisal. We complex state of the organism, involving bodily
embed this work in a general model of emotion, changes of a widespread character-in breathing,
which identifies the key variables and processes pulse, gland secretion, etc.-and, on the mental
within a systems framework emphasizing per- side, a state of excitement or perturbation,
son-environment relationships and cognitive marked by strong feeling, and usually an impulse
mediation. In presenting our model, we illus- toward a definitive form of behavior. If the emo-
tion is intense there is some disturbance of the
trate how emotion theory makes firm contact
intellectual functions, a measure of dissociation,
with a variety of topics currently being pursued and a tendency towards action . . . .
across diverse psychological disciplines, es-
pe.cially personality and social psychology. Although this definition expresses some con-
sensus at the descriptive level, it does not go far
toward settling disputes over distinctions be-
DEFINITIONAL ISSUES: THE tween emotion and nonemotion, or th~ specific
NATURE OF EMOTION reaction states that should be considered true
emotions. Is surprise an emotion? Excitement?
Unfortunately, although there is considerable Relief? Love? How should we treat the so-
agreement that certain psychophysiological called "aesthetic" emotions? Nbr does it reveal
states (e.g., anger, fear, and sadness) should be much about the properties of specific emotions,
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 611

or help us specify the processes and variables species achieve solutions determined by both
involved in the generation of emotion. the environmental pressures they face and their
In seeking to distinguish emotion from biological potential.
nonemotion, the seemingly irreconcilable dif, Like many contemporary and recent theo,
ferences among various definitions suggest to rists (e.g., Arnold, 1960; Ekman, 1984;
us, as they have to others {e.g., Fehr & Russell, Ellsworth & Smith, l 988a; Epstein, 1984;
1984; Shaver, Schwartz, Kirson, & O'Connor, Izard, 1977; Lazarus, 1968; Lazarus, Kanner, &
1987), that emotion may not be readily amen, Folkman, 1980; Lazarus & Smith, 1988; Leep,
able to classical definition. Instead, it may be er, 1948, 1965; Leventhal, 1980; Plutchik,
better to think of a set of prototype definitions 1980; Roseman, 1984; Scherer, 1984b; Tom,
that explicitly acknowledges that "emotion" kins, 1962, 1980), we believe that emotions
may be an inherently fuzzy set, and perhaps to represent one class of solutions to these adapta,
replace traditionally strict necessary and suf, tional problems. Each emotion expresses a per,
ficient conditions with defining features that son's appraisal of a person-environment rela,
tend to be shared by most emotions and absent tionship involving a particular kind of harm or
from most nonemotions (Fehr & Russell, 1984; benefit. The appraisal is based on antecedent
Leeper, 1965; Rosch, 1978; Shaver et al., motivational and belief variables that confront
1987). The very notion of a "fuzzy" set in, (interact with) a set of environmental de,
dicates the possible existence of borderline mands, constraints, and resources, and it
phenomena that have some characteristics pro, generates action tendencies relevant to the
totypical of an emotion and other characteris, specific conditions of harm or benefit con,
tics prototypical of a nonemotion, making the fronted-tendencies that are embodied and ex,
full category of "emotion" very difficult to de, pressed in a particular physiological pattern.
fine and many borderline states difficult to clas, However, emotions are not the only entities
sify with assurance. that serve adaptive functions; both within and
Wisdom suggests that in this chapter we across species, similar functions are also served
should not try to resolve the question of which by reflexes (e.g., startle) and physiological
states are borderline states or genuine emo, drives (e.g., hunger and thirst). Reflexes, phys,
tions, since the arguments are complicated and iological drives, and emotions all stimulate
would require much space, and we are not (motivate) the organism to behave in ways that
totally confident about our own solutions." In, enhance its potential to survive and flourish. In
stead, we compare emotions to related psy, other words, each is a different "adaptational
chophysiological phenomena in order to iden, subsystem. "2 .
tify the most important distinguishing charac, Although serving similar general functions
teristics of emotion. We draw upon these char, (i.e., promoting survival), the three adaptational
acteristics to describe the specific variables and subsystems are, in principle, distinguishable in
processes involved in emotion, since delineat, an evolutionary sense. It is a reasonable in,
ing these specifics and their interrelationships ference that emotions evolved from simpler and
in each kind of emotion represents the most more rigid adaptational systems such as reflexes
pressing agenda for emotion theory and re, and physiological drives (see Ellsworth &
search. Smith, 1988a, 1988b; Epstein, 1984; Leeper,
1948, 1965; Scherer, 1984b; Tomkins, 1962).
Undoubtedly, the most important evolutionary
THE ADAPTATIONAL PROBLEM AND change was the movement away from specific
THE EVOLUTION OF EMOTION built,in responses elicited by specific environ,
mental stimuli toward increasing variability
As Plutchik (1980) has cogently argued, each and complexity that decoupled the behavioral
species faces a number of fundamental adapta, response from the environmental input. Along
tional problems that it must adequately address with this increasing variability and loss of be,
in order to survive. The survival issues faced by havioral rigidity, there simultaneously evolved
most animal species include, among others, an increasing dependence on intelligence and
adequate nourishment, reproduction, and pro, learning. As more complicated species evolved,
tection from external and internal threats to they became less dependent on hard,wired re,
well,being. 1 Through natural selection, sue, flexes, and a gap developed between environ,
cessful species develop mechanisms that enable mental demand and action. This gap was filled
them to meet these problems, and different increasingly by thought and judgment, as is
612 RESEARCH TOPICS

most evident in humans (Piaget, 1952; Wer, need is reliably signaled by a very specific cue or
ner, 1948). Instead of surviving and flourishing set of cues, and can be met by performing a
because of a built,in program of adaptive reac, specific behavior. It is this very specific linkage
tions for every specific environmental condi, that innate reflexes accomplish, and the hall,
tion, more advanced species survived by learn, mark of the reflex is its stimulus specificity and
ing how to deal with their environments and response rigidity (Ekman, 1984; Ekman et al.,
mobilizing accordingly. Increasingly, judgment 1985). Very specific patterns of stimulation
took over from innate reflexes, and emotions- ("releasing stimuli") elicit very specific patterns
drawing upon both motives and thought-have of behavior ("fixed action patterns") that en,
become the key adaptational process interven, sure the need signalled by the releasing stimulus
ing between environmental challenges and ac, is met. These characteristic properties of re,
tions (Tomkins, 1962). In short, innate re, flexes are summarized in the first column of
flexes were once the simplest solutions to the Table 23. l.
adaptational problem of getting along in the Reflexes (at one time, the term "instincts"
world, but in more complex creatures these might have been used) constitute an effective
evolved into emotional patterns. adaptational system for organisms that can
In considering the role of the emotions in afford to interact with their environments in
adaptation, one must remember that the fun, highly stereotyped ways. However, their sim,
damental adaptational task is to mobilize the plicity-the rigid pairing of a specific stimulus
most efficacious behavior in the face of the with a specific response--has high costs, parti,
biological and social requirements of living. cularly as organisms and their environmental
Remember, too, that in order to effectively interactions become more complicated. For an
produce contingent behavior, the organism organism dependent on reflex, each new mode
must meet two fundamental conditions: First, it of interaction with the environment requires
needs to reliably detect when environmental the development of a new reflex, and at some
circumstances are relevant to one or another of relatively modest level of complexity this
its survival needs; second, this detection must requirement becomes highly disadvantageous.
result in behavior that increases the likelihood As the survival issues themselves become more
of satisfying the need. In our view, reflexes, complex, they become increasingly difficult to
physiological drives, and.emotions all represent address through the performance of rigid be,
mechanisms connecting the detection of sur, havioral sequences, and more flexible, context,
vival,relevant conditions with the production sensitive responses become necessary. Further,
of survival,enhancing behavior, but they more, it becomes increasingly unlikely that
achieve this connection in different ways. specific survival issues will be reliably signaled
by single stimuli. Thus, with increasing com,
plexity there is increasing selective pressure to
Reflexes surmount the behavioral rigidity inherent in
reflexes and to decouple specific stimuli from
The task of pairing adaptive behaviors with specific responses {see Epstein, 1984; Leeper,
survival,relevant conditions is easiest when a 1965; Scherer, 1984b; Tomkins, 1962). There

TABLE 23.1. Comparison of Reflexes, Physiological Drives, and Emotions

Physiological
Property Reflex drive Emotion

Stimulus source Internal or external Internal tissue deficit Internal or external


event (real) (real) event (real or im,
agined)
Periodicity Reactive Cyclical Reactive
Stimulus specificity High Moderate-high Low
Response flexibility Low Moderate High

Examples Startle, eye blink Hunger, thirst Anger, sadness, guilt


Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 613

is still a need for mechanisms that both alert something that guides the organism toward
the organism when it faces survival,relevant specific appropriate behaviors. Thus, with hun,
circumstances and compel it to respond adap, ger, appropriate strategies for obtaining food
tively to those circumstances. However, there must come from somewhere, and the organism
is much to be gained if the organism is some, must have some means of identifying suitable
how able to equate distinct stimuli that signal foods.
functionally similar conditions, and/or to re, The apparent evolutionary solution to this
spond to those conditions with a degree of tradeoff has been to make the degree of be,
behavioral flexibility. havioral flexibility associated with drives de,
pendent upon the species' capacity for learning.
The ability to draw upon past experience to
Physiological Drives guide present behavior seems to be a prerequi,
site for response flexibility (cf. Bolles, 197 4;
Physiological drives, such as hunger and thirst, Ellsworth & Smith, 1988a; Epstein, 1984;
evolved in the service of particular internal, Scherer, 1984b). Across species, organisms
homeostatic needs. For instance, hunger serves that demonstrate the most highly developed
to ensure that the organism's nutritional needs learning capabilities tend to be the ones that
are met, and thirst ensures that the organism have acquired the greatest behavioral latitude
maintains an adequate fluid balance. These in responding to specific physiological drives.
drives have tended to remain stimulus,specific For instance, human food preferences are de,
even in the most complicated species, presum, pendent on culture and the individual's life
ably because the homeostatic needs they serve experience (Rozin & Fallon, 1987).
can be reliably anticipated on the basis of Drives display an additional characteristic,
specific internal cues, resulting in little selec, "periodicity, that further distinguishes them
JI

rive pressure to abandon the specificity. For from reflexes (and emotions). Unless anti,
example, in many animal species (including cipated, the homeostatic needs, and hence the
humans), an impending need for nourishment drives that serve them, arise with great regular,
can be predicted quite reliably from specific ity. For example, after an extended period
internal cues, such as the level of sugar in the without nourishment (or fluid) an organism will
bloodstream, and these cues elicit hunger (e.g., become hungry (or thirsty) etc.) in a very pre,
Thompson & Campbell, 1977). dictable manner. In contrast, many reflexes,
Physiological drives are distinguished from and all emotions, are "reactive." They arise in
reflexes by a moderate degree of response response to appropriate signals whenever those
flexibility. In most higher animals drives tend signals occur, and if the signal never arises then
to motivate specific da.sses of behavior, but the the reflex or emotion may never be ex,
specific behavioral sequences within these perienced. As summarized in the second col,
classes are not determined by the drive itself. umn of Table 23.1, physiological drives display
For instance, hunger motivates the organism to periodicity, stimulus specificity, and moderate
eat something, but for many species (including response flexibility, and they serve homeostatic
humans), the hunger itself does not determine needs.
either the specific behaviors to be performed to Physiological drives-which are innate in all
obtain and prepare the food, or the identity of animal species, including humans-are not the
appropriate foodstuffs. This response flexibility only motivational forces to which complicated
provides considerable adaptational advantages, species respond. In humans, for example, there
but also entails considerable cost. appear to be strong needs to explore, achieve,
The major advantage is that the behavioral and gain mastery over the environment, as well
flexibility enables the organism to adjust its as to maintain contact and form social bonds
behaviors sensitively to its specific environ, with others. Whether one refers to these needs
mental contingencies. Thus, in hunger, if one as learned or acquired drives, or "social mo,
strategy for obtaining food fails, the organism is rives, the development of adaptational systems
JI

relatively free to try another; if a favorite food to satisfy them has depended on a powerful and
becomes scarce, the organism is able to seek an abstract intelligence. Advanced intelligence
alternative; and so on. The major cost is that also made possible the complex patterns of so,
this flexibility makes the drive in some sense cial organization that dominate the behavior of
incomplete: It must be supplemented with advanced species, and that are as important to
614 RESEARCH TOPICS

survival and prosperity as meeting physiological recognition of threat is further complicated by


needs. the fact that a predator is often not dangerous
This is not the place to argue about the unless aggravated or hungry. Thus, the signifi,
extent to which social motives are innate or cance of any given signal may vary considerably
acquired or developmentally dependent on across divergent contexts. In modem human
conditioning in the presence of the subsidence existence, the danger from others may consist
of physiological drives such as hunger and of subtle and concealed disapproval, patroniz,
thirst. Human functioning and adaptation are ing statements that barely reveal a true attitude
heavily dependent upon the fate of social mo, and require considerable social experience and
tives, and in our view, emotions are part of an intelligence to interpret, or poor matches be,
evolutionary solution for ensuring their satis, tween performance demands (e~g .. at work)
faction. An understanding of human emotions and the abilities and knowledge possessed by a
would be impossible without reference to a person for meeting those demands. As a further
motivational principle that identifies what is complication, threats represent only one of
regarded by the individual as important or un, several classes of significant events, each of
important to personal well,being. A cognitive- which can take a variety of forms and be sig,
relational theory of emotions, such as the one naled in a multitude of ways.
we propose below, cannot depend on the fate of In place of the unwieldy adaptational solu,
innate physiological needs alone, but rests on tion of developing a different reflex in response
the premise of individual differences in motiva, to every signal of every potentially significant
tional patterns-patterns that set the stage for event in all contexts, more complicated species
defining harm and benefit for each individual. have to stake their security on the capacity to
evaluate the significance of what is happening.
They need to be responsive to a wide variety of
Emotions cues signaling a particular kind of significant
event, and they need to be sensitive to the
As we have said, emotions emerged in com, context in which these cues are encountered.
plicated species to meet the need for high de, In humans it is easy to demonstrate that, under
grees of response flexibility to the often com, the appropriate circumstances, just about any
plex and subtle conditions of life that could stimulus event can produce just about any emo,
generate harms and benefits. They developed tion, and no single stimulus will always elicit a
in ways that differentiate them from both given emotion under all conditions (Ekman,
reflexes and drives in flexibility, variability, 1984; Frijda, 1986).
richness, and dependence on intelligence. The suggestion that emotions lack stimulus
As indicated in the last column of Table 23.1, specificity does not imply that they are random
emotions not only expanded the response response states. On the contrary, we see each
flexibility that distinguishes drives from re, distinct emotion as a response to a particular
flexes, but also lost the stimulus specificity that kind of significant event-a particular kind of
characterizes both reflexes and drives. harm or benefit (see Lazarus, 1968, 1982;
Unlike physiological needs, which are in, Lazarus & Smith, 1988)-that motivates cop,
temal and reliably signaled by specific stimulus ing activity. However, because there is no sim,
conditions, adaptationally significant external ple mapping between objective stimulus prop,
events present themselves to complex species erties and adaptive significance, the task of
in a variety of guises. For instance, one class of detecting significant events becomes quite
events with which the organism must be pre, formidable, and to accomplish it the organism
pared to cope in order to survive is that of must be able to somehow classify what is being
threats to its well,being. All threats share the confronted into a relatively small number of
property of having the potential of resulting in categories, corresponding to the various kinds
harm if they are not avoided or neutralized. of harm or benefit it may face. Above all, the
However, these dangers can take a variety of emotional response is not a reaction to a stimu,
forms, and each can be signaled by a wide array lus, but to an organism (person)-environment
of conditions. The danger may be any one of relationship. Given the properties of the stimu,
several predators, whose presence may be sig, lus context and the organism's pattern of
naled by diverse stimuli (an odor, a sound, the motivation, what must be detected is that the
sudden movement of a shadow, etc.). ·The convergence of these two sets of characteristics
·,.

·~
,:;j,

Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 615

results in harm or benefit. This is what it means 1970; Nomikos, Opton, Averill, & Lazarus,
to speak of a "relational" approach to emotion. 1968). Even purely imaginary experiences,
Moreover, with the adaptational responses which the person in no way expects to take
having become less innate, more flexible, more place, are quite effective at evoking low,level
variable, and more dependent upon the species' emotional reactions, as a long tradition of im,
cognitive capabilities, emotions are not only agery,based research will attest (e.g., Carroll,
reactions to ongoing relationships with the en, Marzillier, & Merian, 1982; Lang, 1979;
vironment but are also cognitive. Schwartz, Fair, Salt, Mandel, & Klerman,
However, the adaptive solution has not been 1976; Smith, 1989; Smith, McHugo, & Lan,
merely to produce a purely cold cognitive proc, zetta, 1986).
ess of detection and evaluation. Instead, it That emotions are reactions to abstract
comprises a complex psychobiological reaction meanings conveyed by just about any set of
that fuses intelligence with motivational pat, circumstances implies an emotion process that
terns, action impulses, and physiological is extraordinarily complex, variable, and flex,
changes that signify to both the actor and ible. Whereas any given drive can only be satis,
observer that something of significance for fied by performing a particular class of be,
well,being is at stake in the encounter with the haviors (e.g., eating something in hunger,
environment. We call this psychobiological drinking something in thirst), this does not
reaction an "emotion.,, It is a very complex appear to be the case for emotions, perhaps
reaction that simultaneously encompasses mo, because the diverse range of circumstances that
tives and cognitive evaluations of the adapta, can elicit a given emotion cannot be effectively
tional requirements of the encounter, and, if addressed by any single class of behavior. For
the encounter is evaluated as having important example, anxiety arises when we perceive our,
consequences for personal well,being, it results selves to be in a potentially dangerous situa,
in organismic involvement. Therefore, in place tion, and we become motivated to avoid or
of "emotion" we often use the expression escape the threat. But a wide variety of be,
"cognitive-motivational-emotive configura, haviors that eliminate or reduce the threat can
tion." satisfy this motivation-fleeing the situation,
The divorce of emotional response from remaining in the situation but increasing vigi,
specific stimuli and its replacement with a lance, or even mounting a pre,emptory attack
cognitive evaluation of the significance of the to eliminate the source of threat. Thus, an
organism-environment relationship is the cen, emotion provides the motivation to react to the
terpiece of the emotion process in humans. By situation in an ill,defined way-in this case, to
centering on the person's interpretation or avoid the perceived threat-but it does not

I evaluation of what an encounter signifies for its


well,being, the effective stimulus for emotion
has shifted from a concrete event to an abstract
greatly constrain the specific behaviors pro,
duced.
Finally, the dependence on meaning lends
I meaning. In becoming meaning,centered, emo,
tions have achieved a flexibility and adapta,
emotion a dynamic fluidity that allows the re,
sponse to sensitively track the changing adapta,

I
I
tional power that is simply not possible for
stimulus,centered adaptational systems such as
drives and reflexes.
tional significance of the person-environment
relationship as an encounter unfolds. Thus, if
the anxious person's attempts to avoid the
From this point of view, anything that implies threat prove successful, and the perceived clan,
harm or benefit to the person can produce an ger is eliminated, the person's anxiety will be
emotion. Thus, pain, hunger, or even emo, transformed into relief,' and vigilance abates. If
tional .reactions themselves (e.g., anger) can the threat materializes, and there is recognition
evoke fear, guilt, shame, or some other emo, of an irremedial harm, the anxiety wilL be
tion-even a positive one such as happiness or transformed into sadness or despair, and the
love-if they are interpreted as somehow being psychophysiological and behavioral pattern will
a harm or threat, or a benefit. Just as signifi, look quite different.
candy, the critical event-be it internal, ex, The idea that the adaptational power and
ternal, or a combination of both-need not flexibility of emotion depend upon the organ,
have actually occurred. Anticipated circum, ism's cognitive capabilities provides the basis
stances can be as emotionally arousing as the for Plutchik's (1984) assertion that cognition
actual occurrence, if not more so (e.g., Folkins, evolved in the service of emotion, and has also
616 RESEARCH TOPICS

been invoked to explain why human beings, Appraisal and Knowledge


the most cognitive of creatures, also appear to
be the most emotional (e.g., Hebb, 1949; Although emotions are evoked as a result of
Scherer, 1984b). Given the analysis above, it is cognitive activity, not all cognitive activity is
not surprising that recent efforts to understand relevant to emotion, and even relevant cogni,
emotion have focused on the role of cognition, tive activities are not all equally relevant. The
and in particular cognitive appraisal, in eliciting task of interpreting the adaptational signifi-
emotion. cance of our circumstances draws upon a highly
complicated and only partially reliable arrange,
ment of cues to determine what, if anything,
the relationship to the environment implies for
APPRAISAL THEORY personal well,being. There appear to be at least
two distinct types of cognition involved in this
process.
How a given individual reacts emotionally to First, there must be a well,developed repre-
an encounter depends on an evaluation of what sentation of one's circumstances. Much social,
the encounter implies for personal well,being, psychological and personality research has been
which is what "appraisal" means in our usage. devoted to describing a vast array of attribu,
A fundamental proposition is that the evalua, tional and inferential strategies that people use
tion causes the emotional response in accor, to go beyond the often paltry data directly
dance with a set of psychobiological laws, available and construct rich representations of
which we spell out later. That is, if we know what is happening (e.g., Bruner, 1957; Heider,
how a person evaluates the relationship with 1958; Jones et al., 1971; Lewin, 1936; Nisbett
the environment, we can predict that person's & Ross, 1980; Ross, 1977, 1987; Shaver,
emotional reaction. In order to develop this 1977). These representations, which reflect
position into a full,scale theory, and to make knowledge or beliefs about what is happening,
clear its utility, it is important to specify the are relevant to emotion because they are the
causally relevant aspects of the appraisal proc, data that the person evaluates with respect to
ess for each emotion. A large portion of our their adaptational significance. These know!,
own recent collaboration (e.g., Lazarus & edge,centered representations, or "situational
Smith, 1988; Smith, Lazarus, & Novacek, construals," however, do not directly produce
1990) has been directed at developing a system emotions.
of thought that specifies what a person must Instead, it is how these representations are
want and think in order to experience each appraised with respect to their significance for
kind of emotional response. personal well,being-the second type of cogni,
In developing the theory we wanted to inte, tion-that directly determines the emotional
grate recent theoretical and empirical work state (see Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus &
relating specific types of cognitive activity to Smith, 1988). Appraisals are strongly in,
specific emotions (e.g., Ellsworth & Smith, fluenced by personality variables. Two in,
1988a, 1988b; Frijda, 1986; Roseman, 1984; dividuals can construe their situations quite
Scherer, 1984b; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, similarly (agree on all the facts), and yet react
1987; Weiner, 1985) with the more general with very different emotions, because they
theory of appraisal, stress, and coping de, have appraised the adaptational significance of
veloped by Lazarus and colleagues (e.g., Lazar, those facts differently. This derives from the
us, 1966, 1968; Lazarns, Averill, & Opton, relational nature of emotions, in which the
•'
1970; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). We also confluence of both an environmental con,
.;
wanted to clarify and refine the construct of figuration and personality traits is required to
"appraisal" so that it would refer only to the have a particular bearing on subjective well,
cognitive activities directly related to emotion. being in the eyes of each individual.
Finally, we hoped that by specifying the The distinction between knowledge and
appraisals that produce individual emotions, appraisal can be understood as the difference
the resulting theory would clarify how emotions between distal cognitive variables that in,
motivate the organism to cope effectively with fluence emotions only indirectly, and proximal
the adaptational demands confronting it. ones that have direct causal influences (see
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 617

House, 1981; Jessor, 1981). The appraisal con, vironment in terms of a particular type of harm
struct encompasses the most proximal cognitive or benefit. For example, an ambiguous danger
variables (Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus & Folkman, or threat produces anxiety; loss and helpless-
1984), and in formulating our specific appraisal ness produce sadness; offense to oneself or those
model, we have been quite restrictive about one identifies with produces anger-much as
what we include as appraisal. We have not Aristotle suggested in his Rhetoric-and so on
included a number of cognitive variables pre, for each emotion (see Abramson, Seligman, &
viously found to be relevant to emotion, be, Teasdale, 1978; Plutchik, 1980).
cause upon close inspection they reflect either This molar level of description provides an
the more distal, knowledge,based cognitive ac, economical summary of the appraised meaning
tivities discussed above (e.g., locus of causality/ leading to each distinct emotion. However, by
control; Roseman, 1984; Smith & Ellsorth, itself, it is incomplete because it does not reveal
1985; Weiner, 1985) or the subjective proper, the specific evaluations leading to the core re-
ties of the emotional response itself (e.g., sub- lational theme. For example, knowing that an
jective pleasantness; Scherer, 1984b; Smith & appraisal of "ambiguous danger" produces anxi-
Ellswoth, 1985), rather than being evaluative ety indicates very little about the specific
appraisals (see Lazarus & Smith, 1988, for a cognitive decisions made in evaluating the
fuller account of this distinction). situation as dangerous.
Weiner (1985, p. 564), the foremost advo- Therefore, the molar level of analysis must
cate of an attributional analysis of emotion, has be supplemented by a more molecular one,
himself acknowledged that knowledge or beliefs which attempts to describe the specific apprais-
about how things work are more or less in- al questions and answers that result in each
determinate with respect to their emotional core relational theme. Knowledge of the details
consequences: "A word of caution . . . is of appraisal would make it possible to describe
needed. . . . Given a causal ascription, the and understand the details of the linkage be,
linked emotion does not necessarily follow ... tween the core relational themes and the emo-
[Attributional] dimension-affect relations are tions that flow from them, as well as the sim-
not invariant, but are quite prevalent in our ilarities and differences among the various
culture, and perhaps in many others as well." themes and emotions (see Smith & Ellsworth,
What is needed to make the analysis more 1985). For example, knowing about the corn,
determinate is to add appraisal of the personal ponent evaluations that combine to define un-
significance of what is happening for well, certain threat and irretrievable loss, respective-
being. ly, might suggest why subjective experiences of
anxiety and sadness seem in many respects
Core Relational Themes and Appraisal quite similar, why anxiety and sadness often
Components co,occur in the same situation, and yet why
they are so different as well (Ellsworth &
The appraisal task for the person is to evaluate Smith, l 988a).
perceived circumstances in terms of a relatively We have made an effort to identify the major
small number of categories of adaptational sig- dimensional components of appraisal-that is,
nificance, corresponding to different types of the specific questions evaluated in appraisal.
benefit or harm, each with different implica, The answers to these questions are then com-
tions for coping. A key proposition of a cogni- bined to produce the molar personal meanings
tive-relational theory of emotion is that the that directly result in specific emotions (see
appraisal process results in the identification of Lazarus & Smith, 1988). To do this,--we have
a molar person-environment relationship, or drawn on a number of recent proposals at,
what w'e call a "core relational theme," and tempting to identify the specific cognitions associ-
that ·each distinct theme results in a distinct ated with particular emotions (e.g., Frijda, 1986;
emotion (Lazarus & Smith, 1988). As in- Roseman, 1984; Scherer, 1984b; Smith &
dicated earlier, we think of this as a psy- Ellsworth, 1985, 1987; Weiner, 1985).
chobiological law. It is useful, we think, to view each appraisal
For each different emotion, one should be component as addressing one of the two global
able to identify the core relational theme that appraisal issues originally proposed by Lazarus
summarizes the person's relationship to the en- and his colleagues as relevant to well-being
I
-.iJ
! 618 RESEARCH TOPICS

(e.g., Lazarus, 1966; Lazarus et al., 1970; ditions of harm, people who are considered the
Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). "Primary appraisal" locus of causality will be held less accountable
concerns whether and how the encounter is to the extent that their harmful actions are
relevant to the person's well,being, and "secon, perceived as unintentional, just, and/or un,
dary appraisal" concerns the person's resources avoidable (Pastore, 1952; Shaver, 1985; Wein,
and options for coping with the encounter. 3 er, Amirkhan, Folkes, & Verette, 1987). The
Both of these issues can be further subdivided, attribution of causality is "cold," with no neces,
and at present we have identified a total of six sary motivational consequences, whereas a de,
appraisal components-two for primary ap, termination of blame or credit is "hot" because
praisal, and four for secondary appraisal. it not only implies personal involvement but
Consistent with the doctrine that emotion also implies that one's subsequent emotion and
depends on antecedent motivations that are coping efforts should be directed toward the
part of personality, the two components of pri, target of that judgment.
mary appraisal are motivational relevance (or Often what makes the difference between an
importance) and motivational congruence or attribution of mere locus of causality and an
incongruence. "Motivational relevance" is an appraisal of accountability, and hence blame, is
evaluation of the extent to which an encounter a judgment of imputed control by the other
touches upon personal goals and concerns-in person. In other words, if the other person who
other words, the extent to which there are has caused the harm could have done other,
issues in the encounter about which the person wise, as when he or she has acted maliciously or
cares or in which he or she has a stake. This has treated us too lightly and hence demeaned
appraisal component is also included in the us, there will be accountability, blame, and
theoretical systems of Frijda (1986), Scherer anger. If, however, the other person could not
(1984b), and Smith and Ellsworth (1987). have controlled what was done, then there will
"Motivational congruence or incongruence" be an attribution of causal locus without
refers to the extent to which a transaction is accountability or blame. In this case, anger will
consistent or inconsistent with what one not occur, or it will be directed at other sources
wants-that is, the extent to which it either of blame on the basis of complex social judg,
,-
-J thwarts or facilitates personal goals. This corre, ments about the accountability, say, of the au,
sponds closely to Roseman's (1984) concept of thorities, or the system, or the like. The differ,
"motive consistency," Scherer's (1984b) "goal ent motivational dynamics of locus of causality
conduciveness," and Smith and Ellsworth's and accountability can often be observed when
(1985) "perceived obstacle." in the course of their jobs people must inflict
The four components of secondary appraisal harm on others, and even while acknowledging
are accountability, problem,focused coping being the locus of causality they try to deflect
potential, emotion,focused coping potential, the accountability and blame to their social
and future expectancy. "Accountability" pro, role in the hope that the other person's anger
vides direction and focus to the emotional re, will be similarly deflected (e.g., 'Tm sorry, I
I sponse and the coping efforts motivated by it. It
determines who (oneself or someone else) is to
really hate to do this, but I have to--it's my
job"). !
i receive the credit (if the encounter is motiva,
tionally congruent) or the blame (if it is moti,
vationally incongruent) for the harm or bene,
The remaining three components of secon,
dary appraisal all have to do with evaluation of
the potential for improving an undesirable
!
I

I
·~·
fj.
fit. It is also closely related to locus of causality
(Ellsworth & Smith, 1988a; Weiner, Graham,
& Chandler, 1982), which is an attributional
or knowledge factor, but differs from it in ways
that highlight the earlier stated difference be,
tween knowledge and appraisal.
situation or maintaining a desirable one. The
two subvarieties of coping potential correspond
to one's evaluations of tbe ability to engage in
the two major types of coping identified by
Folkman and Lazarus (1980, 1985; Folkman,
Lazarus, Dunkel,Schetter, Delongis, & Gru,
i
f
t
-f
t
Accountability is a more proximal construct en, 1986; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). "Pro, I

than locus of causality, intentionality, legiti, blem,focused coping potential" reflects evalua,
macy, and controllability, which are often tions of one's ability to act directly upon the
combined in evaluating accountability-that situation to manage the demands of the
is, who gets the credit or blame (McGraw, encounter and actualize the personal com,
1987; Shaver, 1985). For example, under con, mitments that are brought to it. This evalua,
',..'
f.
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 619

tion is closely related to the concept of power as Primary appraisal is involved in every emo,
discussed by Roseman ( 1984), and control and tional encounter. The evaluation of motiva-
power as discussed by Scherer (1984b). "Emo- tional relevance is necessary for emotion, since
tion-focused coping potential" refers to the per- it defines the most elemental aspect of a per,
ceived prospects of adjusting psychologically to son's level of affective involvement by indicat-
the encounter-in other words, of regulating ing whether there is any personal stake in the
the emotional state that harmful or threatening encounter. In the absence of motivational rele-
consequences generate. This evaluation is vance, the person's state of mind is likely to be
closely related to Scherer's concept of "the one of indifference or passive tranquility (cf.
potential for adjustment to the final outcome Ellsworth & Smith, 1988b). Motivational con,
via internal restructuring" (Scherer, 1984a, p. gruence or incongruence combines with rele-
39). "Future expectancy" refers to the per- vance to define the encounter as beneficial or
ceived possibilities, for any reason (i.e., in, harmful, actually or potentially (Lazarus et al.,
dependent of whether the individual plays a 1980).
role), for changes in the psychological situation Motivational relevance and motivational
that could make the encounter more or less congruence or incongruence are not sufficient
motivationally congruent. to shape the kind of emotion that will be ex,
perienced. The components of secondary
appraisal are also needed to determine whether
Appraisals for Each Emotion one will experience happiness, relief, pride,
gratitude, hope, or the like on the positive side,
The six appraisal components noted above, or anger, guilt, shame, anxiety, sadness, envy,
which combine into core relational themes, or the like on the negative side.
provide the conceptual machinery needed to Table 23. 2 combines the appraisal com,
generate hypotheses about the specific apprais- ponents with core relational themes, and de,
als responsible for every emotion. One task is to picts the specific appraisals for illustrative
identify the core relational theme and its emotions. These hypotheses are generally
specific harm or benefit, which is necessary and consistent with the findings of a number of
sufficient to produce each emotion. A second studies that have examined the relationships
task is to describe this theme in terms of a between cognitive activities and emotions
particular combination of the six appraisal (e.g., Ellsworth & Smith, 1988a, 1988b; Frijda,
components. 1987;. Roseman, 1984; Scherer, Wallbott, &

TABLE 23.2. Functional Analysis of Some Illustrative Emotions

Proposed Core Important


Emotion adaptive function relational theme appraisal components

Anger Remove source of harm from Other-blame 1. Motivationally relevant


environment and undo harm 2. Motivationally incongruent
3. Other-accountability
Guilt Make reparation for harm to Self-blame 1. Motivationally relevant
others/motivate socially re- 2. Motivationally incongruent
sponsible behavior 3. Self-accountability
Anxiety Avoid potential harm Ambiguous danger/ 1. Motivationally relevant
threat 2. Motivationally incongruent
3. Low/uncertain (emotion-focused) coping
potential
Sadness Get help and support in the Irrevocable 1. Motivationally relevant
face of harm/disengage from a loss 2. Motivationally incongruent
lost commitment 3. Low (problem-focused) coping potential
4. Low future expectancy
Hope Sustain commitment and Possibility of ame- I. Motivationally relevant
coping lioration/success 2. Motivationally incongruent
3. High future expectancy
620 RESEARCH TOPICS

Summerfield, 1986; Smith & Ellsworth, 1985, tional consequences. The blame in self,
1987; Weiner et al., 1982), even though these directed anger is quite literally "other,blame
studies have not always examined the relevant directed at the self." That is, the person
appraisals directly (see Lazarus & Smith, observes himself or herself behaving undesir,
1988). The hypotheses have recently received ably and holds the observed person (who hap,
further direct support in an initial study ex, pens to be the self) accountable. This blaming
plicitly designed to test them (Smith, Lazarus, process does not necessarily implicate one's
& Novacek, 1990). self,concept or feelings of self,worth. In con,
For each emotion in the table, we have listed trast, the self,blame in guilt calls into question
the adaptive function for that emotion. Then one's self,worth. The distinction is expressed in
we have listed the core relational theme that the internal dialogue that often accompanies
corresponds to the particular relationship with these two forms of blame, the blame associated
the environment in which that function is like, with self,directed anger expressed in the second
ly to be appropriate. Finally, we have listed the person and that associated with guilt in the first
major appraisal components that combine to person (e.g., "You idiot, what did you do that
define that core relational theme. For example, for?" vs. "What have I done t'). This example
anger motivates the person to eliminate, neu, highlights how seemingly small cognitive dif,
tralize, or undo a source of harm (Cannon, ferences can lead to large differences in the
';
.'f;
1929; Ellsworth & Smith, 1988a; Izard, 1977; nature of the emotional reaction.
Plutchik, 1980; Tomkins, 1963). The core re, Whereas accountability or blame is of central
lational theme that defines the relevant cir, importance in differentiating anger from guilt,
cumstances for this function is "other,blame." other appraisal components are more important
In other words, anger arises when someone else in differentiating anxiety from sadness. Both
is being blamed for a harmful situation, anxiety and sadness are associated with harmful
although if the "other person" being blamed is situations in which the prospects for ameliora,
the self one could speak of anger at the self. tion are uncertain or poor, and these similari,
Since anger motivates the person to do some, ties may explain why these emotions are often
thing to remove the source of harm, the assign, evoked in conjunction with each other. Never,
ment of accountability or blame provides a tar, theless, there are distinct motivational func,
get for these coping efforts, which is crucial for tions for these emotions; their hypothesized
its subjective and behavioral characteristics. core relational themes, as well as the appraisal
Guilt motivates the individual to make repa, components that define them, reflect these dis,
rations for harm he or she has caused to others, tinct functions.
and generally to engage in socially responsible Anxiety motivates the person to avoid
behavior (Ellsworth & Smith, 1988a; Izard, potential harm (Cannon, 1929; Izard, 1977;
1977). Consistent with these functions, Plutchik, 1980; Tomkins, 1963), the core re,
the core relational theme producing guilt is lational theme being an appraisal of uncertain
"self, blame," which means holding oneself ac, "dangee' or "threat." The component of secon,
countable for an important, motivationally dary appraisal is poor coping potential, which
incongruent situation. Like anger, guilt moti, derives from the inevitable uncertainty in anxi,
vates the person to do something to remove the ety about what will happen and when. If it
source of harm, but because the focus is on arises from symbolic and existential threats-
oneself, it takes the form of a desire to make which is one of the major conceptualizations of
reparations for any harm the person has caused · anxiety (see Lazarus & Averill, 1972)-the
(e.g., Carlsmith & Gross, 1969; Freedman, danger to self is obviously vague .and ambigu,
Wallington, & Bless, 1967). In addition, guilt ous; this translates into a condition of poor
is painful and therefore self,punishing (Wal, coping potential, since one cannot know what
lington, 1973), which reduces the probability to do about danger of this kind. Emotion,
..
·that the person will continue to engage in the focused coping potential may be especially im, X.'
harmful behavior in the future. portant in anxiety. The sense of danger, and ~'.

We consider the blame in sel£,directed anger hence anxiety, will be particularly acute when,
to be qualitatively distinct from the self,blame beyond seeing potential or actual harm in the
associated with guilt; accordingly, we hold that situation, one believes that this harm-say, a
feeling guilty and feeling angry at oneself are loss of self or meaningfulness--cannot be toler,
different emotional states with distinct motiva, ated emotionally if it occurs (or has occurred).
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 621

On the other hand, sadness promotes dis, toire, but are illustrative of some of the most
engagement from commitments that have been important. They help us demonstrate how a
lost and motivates the person to get help (Izard, more complete cognitive-relational theory of
1977; Klinger, 1975; Plutchik, 1980). The core emotion might look. The richness of our En,
relational theme producing this emotion is glish vocabulary of emotions (see Averill,
"irrevocable loss" or "helplessness" (Abramson 1975; Ortony et al., 1987; Shaver et al., 1987)
et al., 1978). Accordingly, the components of suggests that there are many more emotional
secondary appraisal that distinguish this theme states, each produced by distinctive appraisals,
from anxiety are a combination of negative than the few we have considered. For example,
future expectancy and poor coping potential. we have not analyzed a number of positive
In sadness, one is totally pessimistic about emotions, including happiness, pride, relief,
amelioration, whereas in anxiety there is main, and gratitude, that arise under various con,
ly uncertainty. And whereas emotion,focused ditions of appraised benefit (Ellsworth &
coping potential is salient for anxiety, problem, Smith, 1988b; Lazarus et al., 1980). A full
focused coping potential is particularly: salient theoretical statement must address positive or
in sadness; in a condition of irrevocable loss, benefiHelated emotions as well as those flow,
nothing that can be done seems capable of ing from conditions of harm, and decisions
restoring the prior status. must be made about which states should be
Thus far, we have considered only "negative" considered bona fide emotions, nonemotions, ·
emotions--those arising under conditions of or marginal instances.
harm or threat. However, an exclusive focus on Second, in discussing the primary appraisal
harm,related emotions does a disservice to the of motivational relevance that gives rise to
role of emotion in adaptation, because there is emotion, we have not considered the potential
also motivational incongruence when a person role of particular goals or stakes in providing
perceives the absence of potential benefits and emotional differentiation beyond what we have
gains. A voiding or ameliorating harm is, of depicted. Stake,specific differentiation can
course, a factor in survival. However, striving sometimes occur between broad emotional
for gain enables the person (and the species) to categories such as guilt and shame. For in,
grow and flourish. Accordingly, human adapta, stance, in guilt the stake is a moral value, while
tional subsystems also include hope, which sus, in shame it is an ego ideal. Moreover, con,
tains positive striving toward mastery and gain sideration of particular stakes is likely to be
(Ellsworth & Smith, 1988b; Lazarus et al., especially important when attempting to differ,
1980). It seems wise here to again recognize the entiate among affective states within the broad
difficulty of deciding what are genuine emo, emotion categories we have outlined here. For
tions by begging the question of whether hope instance, many forms of anger specifically in,
should be regarded as an emotion or a border, volve some sort of insult to one's personal
line state. identity, while others, such as annoyance, may
As depicted in Table 23. 2, the core relation, be less stake,specific. Similarly, feelings of
al theme for hope combines an appraisal that abandonment appear to involve a particular
existing conditions are not yet the way the type of loss involving one's relationships with
person wants them to be (importance, motiva, others, while the broader category of sadness is ·
tional incongruence) with a future expectation not specific with regard to the particular stake
that these conditions could become or be made that has been lost or irreparably harmed (cf.
motivationally more congruent. Hope can be Ortony et al., 1987).
maintained as long as there is some (however Third, although we have given a relatively
slight) potential for improvement in an other, static description of the structural relations be,
wise bleak set of conditions, as when we "hope tween appraisal and emotion, we do not mini,
f against all hope." Thus hope springs from the mize the importance of thinking about and
i conviction, which may well be a characteristic studying emotion as a dynamic process (see

I of some personalities, that even under dire cir,


cumstances there is still a chance that things
could get better (Lazarus et al., 1980).
Folkman & Lazarus, 1988b; Lazarus, 1989b).
The theory of appraisal indicates how at any
given moment the person's specific appraisals

I
I
i
i
The analyses above require several quali,
fications. First, the emotions examined do not
include all the emotions in the human reper,
will produce a particular emotional state.
Knowing these structural relations is, we be,
lieve, a crucial first step to unde~standing the
~
622 RESEARCH TOPICS

emotion process in cognitive-relational terms. ical changes, and subjective experiences. In


However, as an encounter unfolds-as the per, support of this view, proponents cite evidence
son attempts to cope with the adaptive im, for cross,culturally universal associations be,
J' plictions of the circumstances and the envi, tween particular facial expressions and auto,
ronment reacts to those coping efforts-the nomic nervous system and hormonal response
adaptive significance of the encounter is likely patterns for each emotion (e.g., Ekman &
to shift, and as the appraisal shifts so will the Friesen, 1971; Ekman, Sorenson, & Friesen,
t
'J
emotional state. 1969; Izard, 1971, Levenson, 1988).
Fourth and last, emotion is a much richer Proponents of the cultural position regard
and broader construct than stress, and should emotions as socially defined phenomena fol,
supersede stress in the study of coping and lowing conventional rules, or scripts, that vary
adaptation (see Lazarus, 1968, in press; Lazarus widely across cultures (e.g., Averill, 1968,
& Folkman, 1984; Lazarus & Launier, 1978). 1980; Hochschild, 1979; Sarbin, 1985). Evi,
The concept of stress is largely unidimensional, dence for this position typically includes
and expresses little beyond the idea that the observations of considerable cross,cultural di,
person-environment relationship is adapta, versity in both the conditions giving rise to
tionally significant and motivationally in, particular emotions and the expressive and in,
congruent. In contrast, emotion is a multi, strumental coping behaviors accompanying
dimensional construct that reveals a wealth of emotions that have been evoked.
;
information about the adaptational encounter, Our view of emotion occupies a middle
- ,_ the reaction to it, and the personality of the ground between these extremes. By tracing its
individual. Thus, the observation of anger in evolution to the sensorimotor reflex we have
contrast with anxiety, guilt, shame, and so on assumed a substantial biological influence on
tells us much more than merely knowing that a the emotion process. Yet by emphasizing the
person is undergoing stress. Although the dis, loosening of reflexive ties between stimulus and
tinction among harm, threat, and challenge reaction, and the importance of both cognitive
(Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), or between eu, activity and sociocultural learning factors, we
stress and distress (Selye, 1974), modestly en, have left much room for the influence of per,
larges the scope of stress beyond its traditional sonality in emotion, which in turn is partially a
unidimensional character, even this usage pales product of developmental experience with the
in richness and clinical significance compared sociocultural environment (see Ryff, 1987;
with emotion. Shweder & LeVine, 1984). Emotion theory
must go beyond the banal assertion that there is
merit to both perspectives by offering specific
PERSONALITY, SOCIETY, AND proposals about the respective contributions of
BIOLOGY IN EMOTION biology and the society.

A general theory of emotion must take into


account the respective contributions of per, The Biological Core of Human Emotion
sonality, culture, social structure, and biology
to the emotional process. Most theories take Figure 23 .1 depicts our overall theoretical mod,
one of two extreme positions, considering emo, el. The emotional response is at the innate
tions to be either largely innate-that is, fun, biological center of the cognitive-motivation,
damentally fixed products of our biological al-emotive system. We assume that human
heritage and subject to only modest cultural beings (and, we believe, animals too) are con,
influences--or largely socioculturally defined. structed biologically to be constantly engaged
Many proponents of the biological position in appraisals of ongoing and changing rela,
speak of an innate "affect program" for each tionships with the environment. These rela,
emotion, which organizes the emotion process tionships are evaluated in terms of a relatively
(e.g., Ekman, 1984; Ekman & Friesen, 1975; small set of specific, innately determined
Izard, 1977; Tomkins, 1962, 1963). When the appraisal issues, which we have identified
appropriate eliciting conditions for a particular above. Appraisals promote the detection and
affect program are present the program fires evaluation of adaptationally relevant conditions
reflexively and runs its course, which includes requiring action. They determine the emotional
preprogrammed action tendencies, physiolog, state, which prepares and motivates one
.· ~~~
·~-";"
,,._
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 623
.':''
to cope with the adaptational implications of These response components are systematical,
what is happening. ly organized around the adaptive implications
If a person appraises the conditions being evaluated in the appraisal, and appear to have
confronted in a manner that corresponds to a evolved to serve the two general functions of
particular core relational theme of harm or social communication and coping (Lazarus,
benefit, the preprogrammed emotion is auto, 1968; Scherer, 1984a; Smith, 1989). The
matically generated as a feature of our biologi, motor-physiological changes are, in part, de,
cal heritage. Although the appraisal is itself a tectable by observation (e.g., changes in facial
continuing component of the emotional re, expression, posture, vocal tone, etc.; see Ek,
sponse, it is by no means the entire response. man, 1984; Riskind, 1984; Scherer, 1986), and
As indicated in Figure 23.1, additional com, they communicate important information to
ponents include a distinctive subjective feeling others in the social environment about ap,
state, the urge to respond behaviorally to the praisal and possible actions (Scherer, 1982,
situation in a particular manner (e.g., action 1984a; Smith, 1989). The motor-physiolog,
tendency; see Frijda, 1986; Scherer, 1984b), ical changes in posture, muscle tone, hormonal
and a patterned physiological response consist, activity, and autonomic activity prepare the
ing of facial muscle, postural, and neurohumor, person physi9logically to engage in and sustain
al activity associated with the action tendency the coping activities motivated by the action
and coping process. tendency, which itself directly reflects the

Intra-Individual Factors Situational Factors

Personality

Needs/Commitments/Goals Situational ~-+---t Objective


Construal Conditions
Knowledge/ Attitudes/Beliefs

Appraisal Process

Emotional Response

Subjective
Action Physiological
Experience
Tendencies Response
or "Affect"

Translation of Action
Tendencies to Coping Ac ti vi ty

Emotion-Focused Coping Problem-Focused


Coping Activities Coping

FIGURE 23.1. A model of the cognitive-motivational-emotive system.


624 RESEARCH TOPICS ·

adaptational demands implied by the continu, that personality and culture intersect with biol,
ing appraisal (Frijda, 1986; Lazarus, 1968; ogy and play fundamental roles in the function,
Smith, 1989). They also reflect changes in the ing of the cognitive-motivational-emotive
organismic state resulting from a changed adap, system. These points of intersection give emo,
tational condition, as in relief or contentment tion the flexibility that differentiates it from
after a threat has been removed. reflexes and physiological drives and provides it
The correlations between the appraisal and with much of its adaptational power.
these response components appear to have two One way in which the sociocultural and
distinct levels of organization corresponding to biological points of intersection can be clarified
the distinction we have drawn between core is to make a statement with the following for,
relational themes and appraisal components. mal character: If a person appraises his or her
The molar level of organization consists, as we relationship to the environment in a particular way 1

have said, of core relational themes, and is then a specific emotion that is tied to the appraisal
parallel to the concept of affect programs. Par, always results. Furthermore, if two individuals
ticular action tendencies are probably emotion, make the same appraisals they will experience the
specific and linked to specific relational same emotion, regardless of the actual circu.m,
themes. For example, "other,blame" generates stances.
anger and the impulse to attack the blamewor, Personality factors arising in the course of
thy agent, whereas an ambiguous threat gener, psychological development, as well as environ,
.1 ates anxiety and the impulse to avoid or escape mental variables (e.g., the immediate social
I? the threat.
In addition, some of the innately determined
structure), combine to influence the molecular
appraisals-in effect, the specific meanings--
motor-physiological consequences in emotion that result in each core relational theme. These
may be tied to molecular appraisal components. influences shape the "if' of the formal state,
For example, Smith (1989) has provided evi, ment above. "If" means, in effect, that different
dence that activity of the corrugator supercilii individuals can appraise their relationships
muscles to pull the eyebrows together and down with the environment differently, or that the
into a frown is associated with appraisals of same individual can do so at different times or
motivational incongruence, and this associa, occasions. However, once a given appraisal
tion may extend over a broad range of emo, pattern with its core relational theme has taken
tions. It is possible that secondary appraisals place, a particular emotion, with its subjective
having direct implications for subsequent cop, feeling state, action tendency, and motor-
ing (e.g., evaluations of coping potential) may physiological response pattern, is generated as
have direct autonomic and postural effects con, a biological principle. Each core relational theme
sistent with the coping requirements. has its own universal biological emotional out,
The aspects of the emotional response de, come, which is invariant as long as the in,
scribed so far-the appraisals that define adap, dividual continues to appraise what is happen,

..,
tationally significant core relational themes,
and the subjective, physiological, and motiva,
ing in a given way. The appraisal can, of
course, change (1) as the· person-environment

I
tional consequences initiated by these evalua, relationship changes; (2) in consequence of
. tions-are, in our view, universal in our spe, self,protective coping activity (e.g., emotion,
cies. In considering this innate organization, focused coping); (3) in consequence of chang,

I
which has yet to be detailed and demonstrated, ing social structures and culturally based values
.'
.I
. it is important to consider what we have not
included. The biologically fixed portion of the
and meanings; or (4) when personality
changes, as when goals or beliefs are abandoned
.
emotion system starts with the appraisal pattern as unserviceable .
~ ~.;
arid ends with the action tendency, leaving Stated in a slightly different way, the "if' in
..
-~
, considerable flexibility and biological indeter,
minacy as to which stimulus configurations will
the formula above provides for the flexibility
and complexity made possible by intelligence
,,-;t
result in which appraisals, and which actions and culture; the "then" provides the biological
.,/l (as opposed to action tendencies) will follow universal linking cognition to the emotional
any given cognitive-motivational-emotive response. Change the "if' and the response
configuration. It is precisely at these two configuration is also changed. Personality and
points--the process of appraisal and the environmental variables are the antecedents
translation of emotion into coping- in this model, their emotional consequences
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 625
"'f..:_
;:
·:. being mediated by appraisals (influenced recur, tions underlying observed events (see Jones et
sively by coping and its effects) whose biologi, al., 1971; Lewis, 1935, 1936; Nisbett & Ross,
cally determined consequences constitute the 1980; Ross, 1977, 1987; Shaver, 1977). With,
emotional response. in the social,psychological and personality
literatures, there is ample documentation that
these constructive, inferential processes are sys,
Knowledge, Appraisal, Culture, and tematically influenced by the motivations,
Personality knowledge, and expectations the person brings
into an encounter.
Goals often have an important role in de,
It will now be useful to examine more closely termining the aspects of the situation that are
some of the intersections implied above be, noticed, encoded, and emphasizep; one is likely
tween personality, culture, and biology. The to look for and notice things that are motiva,
upper portion of Figure 23.1 depicts some ?er, tionally relevant (cf. the "New Look" percep,
sonality factors that contribute to the emotion, tion research movement of the 1940s and
al response at two levels. First, they influence 1950s; e.g., Postman, Bruner, & McGinnies,
the cognitive representation, or knowledge 1948). For example, partisans on both sides of a
about the person-environment relationship rough football game will tend disproportion,
being appraised, which is identified as a situa,
ately to notice penalties committed by the op,
tional construal in the figure; second, they posing team, thereby strengthening their view
make contributions to the appraisal process it, of the other team as consisting of undeserving
r self. The figure identifies two distinct types of cheaters (Hastorf & Cantril, 1954). Similarly,
personality factors. One consists of motivation, pro,Arab and pro,Israeli viewers watching the
al characteristics, which include the values, very same news coverage of the 1982 Beirut
goals, and commitments that a person brings massacre came away convinced that the other
into every encounter. These characteristics side received a greater number of favorable re,
.·;
have parallels in other concepts, such· as "cur,
ferences and a smaller number of negative ones
rent concerns" (Klinger, 197 5), "personal pro,
than their side did, in support of their view of
jects" (Little, 1983; Palys & Little, 1983), and
the media as biased against them (Ross, 1987;
"personal strivings" (Emmons, 1986). The
Vallone, Ross, & Lepper, 1985).
second factor consists of the person's knowl,
Even when strong motivations are not in,
edge base, which includes generalized beliefs,
valved, prior knowledge and expectations in,
both concrete and abstract, about the way fluence the interpretation and encoding-of sub,
things are, how they work, the nature of the
sequent information. Thus, initial information
world, and the person's place in it. It also
about a person can produce a "halo effect" that
includes attitudes, expectations, and intuitive
influences how subsequent information about
theories about the self (including self,concept)
that person is interpreted (Asch, 1946), and
and the world (see Epstein, 1983; Lazarus &
r Smith, 1988; Lewis & Michalson, 1983; Ross,
facts and events consistent with one's "schema"
( or mental model of an episode are likely to be
1977). We suggest that the two personality
.f assumed to be present in the encounter, and to
factors have distinctive but interactive in,
be incorrectly remembered subsequently as
fluences on both the way a person construes
having been directly observed (e.g., Bower,
what is happening and the appraisal of that
Black, & Turner, 1979; Owens, Bower, &
construal.
y Black, 1979).
j Although they have seldom outlined the
Personality Contributions to Knowledge about specific beliefs and motivations underlying
the Encounter them, cllnical and personality researchers have
y documented the existence of relatively stable
t! Cognitive representations of our relationships individual differences in characteristic ways of
l with the environment often go far beyond the construing certain types of encounters, often
l perceptual data directly available. ·Certain referred to as "attributional biases" (e.g., Dodge
aspects of the encounter are ignored; others are & Coie, 1987; Nasby, Hayden, & dePaulo,
l emphasized; missing information is filled in; 1979) or "attributional" or "explanatory styles"
s and any number of inferences are made regard, (e.g., Peterson & Barrett, 198 7; Peterson et
ing the possible causes, intentions, and motiva, al., 1982). These differences have been reliably
626 RESEARCH TOPICS

associated with individual differences in coping l 989b; Lazarus & Folkman, 1984; Lazarus &
and mood, presumably through the construal's Smith, 1988).
influences on appraisal and emotional re- The theoretical relationship between goal
sponse. Recently it has been pointed out commitments and primary appraisal suggests
(Lazarus, 1989a) that emotion can be studied that motivational measures-such as those de-
both as a personality trait (which is the domi, veloped by Little (1983) and Emmons (1986),
nant interest of clinical workers treating chron- as well as our own recent efforts-become
ically dysfunctional emotional patterns), and as necessary tools for predicting and understand-
a state that is generated by particular encount- ing individual differences in emotional re-
ers with the environment but that does not sponse, and make it possible to identify who
necessarily represent recurrent adaptational will react to a particular situation with strong
problems. A full approach to emotion requires emotion and the specific encounters to which a
both of these perspectives. particular individual is especially responsive
For example, chronically aggressive chil- emotionally (see Pervin, 1983).
dren, especially children whose aggression A number of studies, both old and new,
usually takes the form of angry reactions to illustrate the promise of motivational measures
perceived provocations, have been shown to in the prediction of emotional reactions. For
have stronger tendencies than less aggressive example, Vogel, Raymond, and Lazarus (1959)
children to attribute hostile intentions to the showed that subjects having strong achieve-
.;,. .
ambiguous actions of others (e.g., Dodge, ment goals and weak affiliation goals reacted to
1980; Dodge & Coie, 1987; Dodge, Murphy, & experimentally produced achievement-center-
Buchsbaum, 1984 ). Similarly, in adults the ed threats with more psychophysiological stress
tendency to attribute negative events to in, than to affiliation-centered threats; the reverse
ternal, global, and stable causes, which is hy- pattern was found for subjects with strong
pothesized to promote appraisals of helplessness affiliation goals and weak achievement goals.
and hence sadness and depression (Abramson Similarly, Bergman and Magnusson ( 1979)
et al., 1978), has been prospectively associated demonstrated that Swedish male high school
with enduring depression following poor per- overachievers, rated by their teachers as ex-
formance on an exam (Meralsky, Halberstadt, tremely ambitious, secreted more adrenaline in
& Abramson, 1987), relatively poor academic an achievement demanding encounter than
performance during the first year of college other boys in the same class.
(Peterson & Barrett, 198 7), and success and A study centered on health-related variables
productivity of life insurance sales agents by Kasl, Evans, and Niederman (1979) showed
(Seligman & Schulman, 1986). that a combination of high academic achieve- -
ment motivation and poor performance pre-
dicted risk of infectious mononucleosis among
Personality Contributions to Appraisal West Point cadets. Hammen, Marks, Mayol,
and deMayo (1985) have also reported evi-
In addition to affecting emotion indirectly by dence that students for whom interpersonal
systematically influencing the contents of issues were especially important were more like-
knowledge a person draws upon in appraisal, ly to experience depression in relation to stress-
personality contributes directly to the appraisal ful events involving interpersonal relationships
process itself. Primary appraisal makes sense than they were to stressful events involving
only when one's relationship to the environ- achievement concerns, while the reverse
ment is considered in relation to needs, desires, tended to be true for students with strong
or what one cares about-in effect, the goal achievement concerns.
hierarchy characteristic of a person as it in- Finally, Gruen, Folkman, and Lazarus
tersects with the demands, constraints, and re- (1989) found that some day-to-day "hassles"
sources of the encounter. Without an analysis and upsets were identified by respondents as
of what is (potentially) at stake in an encounter being more important and central to their con-
for that person, it is impossible to evaluate the cerns than others. The contents of these "cen,
appraisal components of motivational rele, tral hassles" varied considerably from individual
vance and congruence or incongruence. If to individual, presumably reflecting different
nothing the person cares about is at stake, then patterns of commitment. Moreover, the central
little or no emotion will result (see Lazarus, hassles were more strongly associated with
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 627

symptoms of psychological dysfunction than assortment of one-sentence descriptions of


the peripheral hassles. hypothetical situations. However, instead of
In addition to motivation, which is most asking about causal attributions in each situa-
closely tied to primary appraisal, a second type tion, we ask about their appraisals along each of
of personality factor-beliefs and expecta- our six appraisal components, from which we
tions-is crucial for emotional differentiation hope to derive stable measures of an in-
and acts as an antecedent of secondary apprais- dividual's characteristic appraisal style for each
al. For example, beliefs about what is norma- appraisal component. We conceive of these
tively appropriate, feasible, legitimate, or ex- measures as reflecting the individual differences
cusable in a given situation should strongly most proximal to the appraisal process, and
influence whether and to what extent an they should enable predictions about the con-
appraisal of accountability for a noxious event textual appraisals that directly produce the
will be made and result in anger, say, instead of emotional state. Research by others (e.g., Re-
sadness. petti, 1987; Solomon, Mikulincer, & Hobfoll,
Beliefs also affect expectations about the 1987) has demonstrated that proximal mea-
probable effectiveness of various courses of ac- sures involving subjective appraisals are better
tion and one's ability to perform those actions, predictors of emotional reactions than are (dis-
which contribute to judgments of self-efficacy tal) objective measures.
(Bandura, 1977, 1982; Maddux, Norton, &
Stoltenberg, 1987), and therefore to evalua-
tions of coping potential and future ex- Cultural Contributions to Appraisal
pectancy. Evaluations of efficacy partially de-
termine whether an encounter will be appraised We have emphasized the contributions of per-
as a harm, threat, or potential gain, and in sonality to appraisal because emotions are re-
consequence contribute to anxiety, sadness, or sponses of an individual person. Individuals,
hope (see also the research and analyses of not cultures, perceive, construe, and appraise.
Antonovsky, 1987, and Scheier & Carver, Moreover, an individual's personal goals and
1987). Expanding on his well-known studies of beliefs should be important in shaping apprais-
self-efficacy as a factor in performance, persis- als and their consequent emotions. Culture,
tence, and the emotional reaction, Bandura (in however, significantly shapes an individual's
press) has also recently provided a rich over- beliefs and motivations over the course of per-
view of the role of self-efficacy beliefs in the sonality development (see Ryff, 1987;: Shweder
development of competence and incompe- & Le Vine, 1984) by providing culturally shared
tence. meanings about what is socially important,
Knowledge and beliefs can also contribute to what various circumstances imply for personal
primary appraisal by helping us define what is well-being, and therefore which emotions are
relevant to our goal commitments and what appropriate under those circumstances (see,
constitutes harm or benefit. For example, be- e.g., Hochschild, 1979).
liefs and expectations about a necessary but It is common to contrast two broad forms of
aversive encounter (e.g., how much pain it is social influence: the living culture into which a
normal to experience during a particular dental person is born, and the social structure. "Cul-
procedure and the gains that result from un- ture" provides a set of meanings and symbols,
dergoing it) can significantly influence the de- many of which are internalized and carried with
gree to which an encounter is appraised as the person into transactions with the social and
motivationally incongruent, and also influence physical environment. The "social structure"
appraisal~ of coping potential and future ex- produces a set of immediate demands, con-
pectancy. straints, and resources that operate contempo-
In· our own recent work (Smith, Novacek, raneously in adaptive transactions, though they
Lazarus, & Pope, 1990), we have been at- can also be internalized and become part of an
tempting to develop measures that reflect stable individual's personality. This contrast is well
individual differences in "appraisal style." We drawn by Schneider (1976, pp. 202-203):
have used the measurement strategy employed
by Peterson et al. (1982) in the Attributional Culture contrasts with norms in that norms are
Style Questionnaire. Respondents are asked oriented to patterns of action, whereas culture
to report their probable reactions to an constitutes a body of definitions, premises, state-
'tf
628 RESEARCH TOPICS
..... _

ments, postulates, presumptions, propositions, used traditionally in stress theory and research
and perceptions about the nature of the universe and not in emotion (see Folkman & Lazarus,
and man's place in it. Where norms tell the actor 1988b). Although specific action tendencies
how to play the scene, culture tells the actor how
are almost universally assumed to flow from
the scene is set and what it all means. Where
norms tell the actor how to behave in the pres,
certain emotions such as anger and fear, biolog,
ence of ghosts, gods, and human beings, culture ically based action tendencies in coping and
tells the actor what ghosts, gods, and human be, the consequences of beliefs for the coping pro,
ings are and what they are all about. cess have received little research attention.
Averill ( 1983) has even argued from his data
on college students that attack is relatively un,
Coping and Emotion common in anger encounters, despite the usual
expectation that it is a biologically generated
Just as the top part of the model in Figure 23 .1 action tendency. A basic unanswered question
depicts personality factors as influencing is this: What happens when the person copes in
appraisal, the bottom portion depicts them as ways that run directly counter to the specific
determinants of coping. The emotional re, thrust of the action tendency itself? This would
sponse includes an action tendency-that is, be the case when the impulse is to attack, but it
an urge to respond to the encounter in a par, is inhibited and perhaps even responded· to by
ticular way: to attack in anger, cry in sadness, denial or suppression. Studies of the role of this
flee or avoid in anxiety, and so on. Neverthe, pattern in stress,related disorders such as
less, at all.but the most extreme levels of emo, hypertension have been common but inconclu,
tional arousal, people have the ability to sup, sive.
press the action tendency and select from a The model portrayed in Figure 23.1 does not
wide array of coping options; this illustrates the stop at coping, but is continuous (see Lazarus,
flexibility of the emotion process. 1968, 1989b), and depicts coping as influenc,
For example, we are free to engage in any of ing subsequent appraisal and emotion by at
a number of problem,focused coping activities least two types of mechanisms: First, problem,
that reflect active attempts to influence the focused coping consists of active attempts to
person-environment relationship and to main, alter the existing problematic relationship
tain or increase its degree of motivational con, (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). If the coping at,
gruence. We are also free to engage in any of a tempts are effective, and harm or threat is
number of emotion,focused coping strategies alleviated or removed, the change is likely to
that attempt to regulate the emotional response be reflected in subsequent appraisals, with con,
itself (cf. Folkman & Lazarus, 1980, 1985; sequent changes in emotion away from distress
Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). We are not con, and toward positive states (see Folkman &
strained to a single coping strategy, and under Lazarus, 1989a). Ineffective attempts can in,
stressful circumstances it appears that people fluence subsequent appraisal as well, as when a
most often engage in a ·combination of many nonresponsive environment alters the person's
problem,focused and emotion,focused strat, beliefs and expectations about both the nature
egies (Folkman & Lazarus, 1980; Lazarus & or type of an encounter and the future sense of
Folkman, 1984). Of the personality factors efficacy. Encounters originally appraised as sub,
identified and discussed above, we suspect that ject to beneficial change can be reappraised as
beliefs are especially influential in affecting the irremedial harms, producing corresponding
actual coping activities to be engaged in, parti, emotional changes from hope to sadness or res,
cularly beliefs about the coping options avail, ignation.
able and their probable effectiveness. Beliefs Second, emotion,focused coping consists of
about the social appropriateness of the actions, managing distressing emotions that arise in any
which are often culturally defined-for ex, given encounter when the circumstances are
ample, the display rules about when and how it refractory to change. Some forms of emotion,
is appropriate to express an emotional state focused coping alter the emotional response
openly or to mask it behind some other expres, directly without changing the meaning of what
sion (Ekman, 1984)-undoubtedly play a role is happening (e.g., by affecting autonomic
too. arousal through relaxation or exercise, or
Explicit research on coping in the context of avoiding thinking about the appraisal, etc.).
emotion theory is a neglected area of research, Other forms alter the appraised meaning of the
verhaos because the concept of coping has been encounter (e.g., by denial or distancing).
Chapcer 23. Emotion and Adapcation 629

Even though cognitive dissonance encom, this content. Unless we are clear about this,
passes a particular, limited type of motivational there is a danger that we will be interpreted as
incongruence, many of the changes produced implying that appraisal is a conscious, volition,
by emotion,focused coping overlap those iden, al, verbally accessible process that requires de,
tified in a long tradition of research into cogni, liberation and considerable time. On the con,
tive dissonance (e.g., Festinger, 1957; Wick, trary, we have been consistent in maintaining
lund & Brehm, 1976). For example, one can that appraisal can be automatic (even primi,
reconstrue the nature of the situation, such as tive) and instantaneous, and can occur outside
by deciding that a perceived offense was really of consciousness (see Lazarus, 1966, 1968,
unintentional or unavoidable, or that an in, 1982, 1984; Lazarus & Smith, 1988).
ferred event did not actually occur. Or one can In this connection, it is useful to maintain a
alter personal beliefs about the meaning of the distinction advanced by Leventhal (1980,
encounter, and hence its implications for well, 1984; Leventhal & Scherer, 1987) between
being. In the face of a seemingly intractable "schematic" and "conceptual" processing,
unpleasant person-environment relationship, which has also been discussed by others, in,
one can also give up cherished personal goals eluding Lazarus (1982, 1984), in discussions of
and values so that the encounter is no longer cognition-emotion relationships. In combina,
appraised as relevant to well,being, and it no tion these two qualitatively distinct forms of
longer has the power to evoke strong emotion cognition give the emotion system the ability
(see Klinger, 1975). to react nearly instantaneously to adaptational,
Although emotion,focused coping alters the ly significant events, and yet to draw fully upon
person instead of the environment, often by the power and flexibility of human cognitive
distorting reality, and although Western psy, capacities.
chologists tend to assume (incorrectly, we Through "schematic processing," the per,
think) that changing things by action is more sonal significance of an encounter is appraised
adaptive than merely changing the way things automatically and nearly instantaneously on
are construed, emotion,focused coping is not the basis of past experiences with similar
inherently less adaptive than problem,focused encounters. That is, the appraisal can act much
coping (cf. Collins, Baum, & Singer, 1983; like the "social affordances" described by Baron
Lazarus, 1983; Strentz & Auerbach, 1988). On (1988; Baron & Boudreau, 1987), with the
the contrary, both forms of coping have an adaptational implications of the environment
important place in human adaptation. The two leaping automatically and without deliberation
functions of coping (problem, and emotion, into the person's mind, so to speak.
focused) are major strategies for achieving a One way in which the operation of schema,
better fit between persons and their environ, tic processing can be understood is by using the
mental circumstances, and, in the long term, concepts of activation and associative networks
adaptive functioning requires maintaining a commonly invoked in the study of memory
delicate balance between the two. (e.g., Anderson & Bower, 1973), although we
need not commit ourselves to this idea and use
it only to illustrate the point about rapid pro,
OTHER ISSUES cessing of complicated material. When a per,
son becomes involved in an encounter similar
In this section we address briefly three topics of
to some in the past, memories of these past
importance to emotion theory that have not
encounters are likely to become quickly acti,
yet been considered-namely, the characteris,
vated. Personal meanings strongly associated
tics of the appraisal process, the maladaptive
aspects of emotion, and emotional develop, with those previous encounters are likely to be
ment. Each of these also has relevance to per, activated and available as contributors to the
sonality and social psychology. person's current emotional state. In this way,
complicated and involved appraisals, drawing
heavily on the person's knowledge and past
The Character of the Appraisal Process experiences, can be arrived at quickly and auto,
matically. In considering this type of mech,
In discussing appraisal and its role in emotion, anism, it is not necessary to think of the
we have focused primarily on the contents of appraisal process as following a fixed or pre,
appraisal, but have been relatively silent about defined sequence (as, e.g., Scherer, 1984b,
the formal cognitive processes that underlie does in his concept of "evaluation checks"),
630 RESEARCH TOPICS

since the full appraised meaning associated lationship with the environment, and suggests
with the past experience(s) can be activated in a number of specific points for possible in,
a single step. tervention. Anger indicates that important
Automatic or schematic processing, as we personal goals are being threatened, and also
have described it, is quite passive, and it is that this person tends to blame someone else
important not to lose sight of the fact that for this, perhaps because of a vulnerable self,
humans are sentient, problem,solving beings esteem that leads to assumptions of malevo,
who actively seek to understand the world and lence or insulting attitudes on the part of oth,
their reactions to it. Thus, schematic process, ers. The clinician will be prompted to explore
ing is accompanied by what Leventhal ( 1984) the circumstances giving rise to the anger, as
has termed "conceptual processing"-a set of well as the client's motivational patterns and
more abstract, conscious, and deliberate cogni, beliefs, in order to understand whether and
tive processes-through which the person is why the client is misconstruing what is happen,
able to evaluate the adaptational significance of ing interpersonally. Why does the client react
the encounter more actively. Although con, with anger as opposed to anxiety, guilt, or
ceptual processing of appraisal components envy? And what is it about the client that leads
~:-
could perhaps follow predefined sequences, as to aggression rather than to a more productive
Scherer ( 1984b) has suggested, we are wary of a coping process?
...~.-
stage theory, since whatever issues and aspects The answers to these questions may suggest
of the encounter seem especially salient may the most appropriate points for intervention.
well pre,empt attention at any given moment. For example, the client may be correctly
Conceptual processing is very important in appraising what is happening-there may in,
much appraisal, as it permits the evaluation of deed be malevolence in those toward whom
the adaptational significance (hence the emo, anger is experienced-but the coping response
tional response) and the availability of coping to this. may be counterproductive. The best
options to be finely tuned to the specific re, intervention may be to try to inhibit or suppress
quirements of the encounter as it unfolds. It the aggressive reactions, and instead to evolve
can also draw on highly complex, symbolic more effective coping options. Alternatively,

'
I
meanings, which we believe often underlie our analysis of the problem may suggest that the
garden, variety emotions. To the extent that appraisal of other,blame, and hence the anger,
they become associated with the encounter in is inappropriate to the social conditions; per,
memory, the results of conceptual processing haps this is the result of incorrect or "irrational"
become available for subsequent schematic pro, assumptions or beliefs that should be changed
cessing and are an important aspect of emotion, (see Ellis & Bernard, 1985). Many programs of
al development. In any case, appraisal is a com, cognitive therapy are predicated on this latter
plex process that can occur on more than one type of analysis of dysfunctional emotions.
level of cognitive processing.

Emotional Development
Emotion and Dysfunction
The theory of emotion we have been describing
Our focus has been on the functional, adaptive has been cast in terms of adult human experi,
nature of emotion, the guiding thesis being that ence. However, the analysis is also intended to
emotions evolved to ensure that the person apply to human infants and other complex
responds effectively to the adaptational chal, mammalian species (see Lazarus, 1982, 1984).
lenges that arise throughout the life course. Yet by emphasizing intelligence, personality,
However, emotions are often dysfunctional or and culture (as values and meanings) in the
maladaptive in individual cases. As such we emotion process, we imply that emotion in the
can learn much about faulty appraisal and cop, newborn infant will not be exactly the same as
ing processes, and their personality determi, in the adult. After all, the appraisal dimensions
nants, from an examination of a person's emo, we have proposed (e.g., future expectancy,
tional patterns. accountability) require cognitive capacities,
For example, knowing that a person fre, skills, social motives, and understandings that
quently reacts with high levels of anger and the newborn simply does not yet possess.
aggression reveals much about a troubled re".' As we see it, the emotion system develops in
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 631

two distinct ways--one primarily reflecting a the ability to maintain a rudimentary self,
biologically determined maturational process, concept, and perhaps the ability to make a
and the other reflecting socioculturally based more sophisticated accountability judgment
learning, which must eventually influence the (involving notions of responsibility as well as
personality variables shaping appraisal and causality) than might be required for, say, an,
emotion in adaptational encounters. ger. Or perhaps a rudimentary self is a sine qua
In the largely biological maturational process non of true emotions, as distinguished from
the components of appraisal become in, undifferentiated contentment and distress.
creasingly differentiated as the infant acquires Leaming and culture interact with this bio,
the formal cognitive capacities (d la Piaget) logically determined unfolding of cognitive
necessary to make the various evaluations of abilities to give the cognit1ve-motiva,
the significance of what is happening for per, tional-emotive system the full flexibility and
sonal well,being. Therefore, consistent with power of which it is capable. Throughout the
the observations of numerous developmental, lifespan both the person's knowledge base and
ists (e.g., Bridges, 1932; Emde, 1980; Izard, motivational hierarchy continue to change.
1977; Lewis & Michalson, 1983; Piaget, 1981; Therefore, as the person's cognitive capacities
Sroufe, 1979; Stein & Levine, 1987), we would and knowledge base increase, we should expect
expect the infant to demonstrate increasing to see increasing sophistication and flexibility
emotional differentiation as it matures. The in both coping activities and the evidence used
developing child should not experience a par, to make evaluations along the various appraisal
ticular emotion until it is able, at least in dimensions.
rudimentary form, to make the appraisals that In the newborn, appraisal of motivational
together comprise the core relational theme for congruence or incongruence may be based pri,
that emotion (see Scherer, 1984b). The de, marily (and almost reflexively) upon perceptual
velopmental research task is to delineate the data, with pleasant sensations indicating moti,
unfolding of the appraisal process and the vational congruence, and unpleasant ones
appreciation by the child of its environmental (e.g., physical discomfort, pain) indicating
and motivational components in the case of motivational incongruence (cf. Emde, 1980;
each emotion as it emerges. Leventhal, 1980, 1984; Piaget, 1981; Sroufe,
For example, the newborn may only be capa, 1979). How.ever, by adulthood, the evaluation
ble of rudimentary appraisals along the two of motivational congruence or incongruence is
components of primary appraisal, motivational far more complex, involving- subtle im,
relevance and motivational congruence or in, plications about the person's relationship with
congruence. This will restrict the newbom's the environment with respect to personal needs
emotional range to states of interested aware, and desires, and strategies of self,control. By
ness, generalized pleasure, and generalized dis, adulthood, low to moderate sensations of
tress. Anger, as differentiated from generalized physical discomfort should no longer be reliable
distress, should not appear until the infant is indicators of motivational incongruence, and
capable of some form of rudimentary account, under the right circumstances may actively be
ability judgment, perhaps involving little more sought, as when discomfort signals to the
than the most primitive notion of causality. athlete that training is progressing as desired
The differentiation between fear and sadness (see Lazarus & Smith, 1988). In a similar man,
should not appear until the infant is capable of ner, the earliest appraisals of accountability
assessing coping potential/future expectancy, may consist of little more than the primitive
which would seem to require, at minimum, the identification of a causal agent (e.g., identify,
ability to anticipate and form expectations ing the direct source of undesired physical re,
about" future events and perhaps even about straint), whereas by adulthood accountability is
one's own competence to influence outcomes. a highly complicated social judgment that com,

I In all likelihood, the relatively fine,grained dis,


tinctions among emotion,focused coping
potential, problem,focused coping potential,
bines causal information with beliefs about in,
tentionality, justifiability, foreseeability, and so
on.

II
and future expectancy emerge even later, from Finally, we should see a similar development
a more general evaluation of coping potential. in the complexity and flexibility of the rela,
J
Emotions implicating the self, such as pride, tionship between emotion and coping. Early on
shame, and guilt, would seem to require we would expect the emotionally pro,
!
632 RESEARCH TOPICS

duced action tendencies arising in an encoun, and challenges {see Pervin, 1983 )-ought to
ter to be acted on in a rather direct, impulsive, concentrate on the emotional life. They are
almost reflexive manner. Thus, in the infant, likely to find the personality variables most
distress reliably produces crying, and in young relevant to emotion to be a rich starting point
children anger is very likely to produce overt for this synthetic {rather than analytic) per,
aggression. However, as the child matures the spective on persons. Of all the personality char,
capacity for behavioral control is much in, acteristics one could use to measure individual
creased; in addition, through direct and differences and to describe functioning persons,
vicarious experience, children acquire and can those that we have identified as being most
use complex knowledge of what is effective and relevant to emotion-the persons' goals and
normatively appropriate under various cir, commitments, and knowledge and beliefs about
cumstances in choosing the coping activities self and the world relevant to avoiding harm
that are acted on in an encounter. Although and achieving those goals and commitments-
largely limited to Western culture, research on are the very variables most likely to give rise to
the development of children's knowledge of a coherent picture of personality. In other
emotions provides an important step in the words; if one wants to understand whole per,
direction of studying the development of the sons and· how they function in nature, what
emotion process (e.g., Gnepp, Klayman, & better place to begin than with a consideration
Trabasso, 1982; Harris, 1985; Stein & Levine, of how the persons are equipped to handle the
1987). challenges, opportunities, and problems of liv,
ing? This is, indeed, what emotions are all
about.
CONCLUDING THOUGHTS

We have begun this chapter with an expression


of regret that emotion has not served-as we NOTES
think it should-as an integrating concept in
psychology. In our discussion of emotion and
dysfunction, we have intimated that emotions 1. Although we find Plutchik's ( 1980) analysis to be
are instructive about persons because both important and thought,provoking, we disagree
emotions and the personality are organized with one of his basic assumptions--namely, that
around the problem of surviving, getting along, there are eight survival issues universal to all
and flourishing over the life course. Our con, animal species, and that the "basic" emotions for
clusion returns to this theme. any species reflect that species' solutions to these
We have been saying that emotions are the specific issues. We see this assumption as simulta,
product of transactions or relationships between neously being too constraining and too broad. It
the person and the environment {Lazarus & is too constraining because, by imposing a con,
Folkman, 1987; Lazarus & Launier, 1978). stant set of survival issues across species, it over,
This suggests one resolution to the person- estimates the number of distinct issues facing very
situation debate {e.g., Bern & Funder, 1978;
Endler & Magnusson, 1976; Epstein, 1979,
simple organisms and underestimates the number
facing more complex species. As species and their L
1983; Mischel, 1968; Mischel & Peake, 1982) interactions with the environment become more
and provides some statements about how per,
sonality and situational variables interact {see
Figure 23.1).
Since the emotion process serves adaptation,
theorists and researchers who would like to put
complex, they often face new, emergent pro,
blems, fundamental to their survival but irrele,
vant to simpler species (see Frijda, 1986, p. 86).
For example, social beings, like humans, must
find solutions to a number of fundamental issues
I
l
ij
the· "person" back into personality research surrounding the coordination of cooperative and t
,.
I-
(e.g., Carlson, 1984)-that is, to move from competitive behavior among conspecifics--issues i
t. ·-
-
,_

the study of a disparate, seemingly random col, that need not be addressed by species whose t
t
lection of "traits" to the study of an organized, members tend to lead their lives in isolation. At
coherent being who responds to the environ,
ment in ways that are intended to realize valued
goals and to promote survival and personal
growth in the face of potential harms, threats,
the same time, the assumption is too broad be,
cause it equates emotion with any solution a spec,
ies has evolved to contend with a survival issue.
lt
By contrast, we view emotion as being one of
- · I''
i
~
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adzzptation 633 -

several types of solution (including physiological Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health.
drives and reflexes) that species have evolved to San Francisco: Jessey-Bass.
Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality (2 vols.).
foster adaptation.
New York: Columbia University Press.
2. We are referring to emotions, reflexes, and drives Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality.
as "adaptational subsystems" rather than as Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 258-
"motivations" or "motives" in order to maintain a 290.
clear distinction between the urges (or tenden- Averill, J. R. (1968). Grief: Its nature and significance.
Psychological Bulletin, 70, 721- 748.
cies) to behave in particular ways produced by Averill, J. R. (1975). A semantic atlas of emotional con-
emotions, drives, and reflexes, and the underly- cepts. ]SAS: Catalogue of Selected Documents in Psy-
ing goals or needs those urges serve. In the past, chology, 5, 330. (Ms. No. 421)
"motivation" has been used rather indiscrimi- Averill,]. R. (1980). A constructivist view of emotion. In
R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory,
nately to refer to the underlying needs, the be-
research, and experience. Vol. I. Theories of emotion (pp.
havioral urges, and the processes that give rise to 305-339). New York: Academic Press.
the urges in response to the needs. We believe Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression:
that a clear understanding of emotions and their Implications for theories of emotion. American Psy-
role in adaptation depends upon the ability to chologist, 38, 1145-1160.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying
distinguish among these aspects of "motivation," theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84,
and we have tried to select our language accor- 191-215.
dingly. Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human
3. In referring to appraisal as "primary" or "second- agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122-147.
Bandura, A. (in press). Reflections on nonability determi-
ary" we are not referring sequential properties and nants of competence. In J. Kolligan, Jr., & R. J.
implying that primary appraisal necessarily pre- Sternberg (Eds.), Competence considered: Perceptions of
cedes secondary appraisal in time. As we discuss competence and incompetence across the lifespan. New
in a later section of this chapter, whether and Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Baron, R. M. (1988). An ecological framework for es-
under what conditions appraisal may follow a
tablishing a dual-mode theory of social knowing. In D.
sequential process are important and open issues Bar-Tal&A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychol-
for further research. Instead, we consider primary ogy of knowing (pp. 48-82). New York: Cambridge
appraisal "primary" because it establishes the per- University Press.
sonal relevance of the encounter, and this rele- Baron, R. M., & Boudreau, L. A. (1987). An ecological
perspective on integrating personality and social psy-
vance is hypothesized to be a prerequisite for chology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
strong emotion. That is, primary appraisal is 53, 1222-1228.
responsible for the degree of emotional "heat" in Bern, D. ]., & Funder, D. C. (1978). Predicting more of
a transaction. If the encounter is appraised as not the people more of the time: Assessing the personality
relevant to well-being, then secondary appraisal of situations. Psychological Review, 85, 485-501.
Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1979). Overachieve-
is relatively unimportant because there will be ment and catecholamine excretion in an achieve-
little emotion of any kind. However, if primary ment-demanding situation. 'Psychosomatic Medicine,
appraisal indicates that the situation is relevant 41 ' 181 - 188.
to well-being, then secondary appraisal plays a Bolles, R. C. (1974). Cognition and motivation: Some histo-
rical trends. In B. Weiner (Ed.), Cognitive views of
vital role in differentiating the emotional experi- human motivation (pp. 1- 20). New York: Academic
ence. Thus, secondary appraisal is "secondary" Press.
because its role in differentiating the emotional Bower, G. H., Black,]. B., & Turner, T. ]. (1979). Scripts
response is highly dependent on the outcome of in text comprehension and memory. Cognitive Psychol-
primary appraisal (see Lazarus, 1968). ogy, 11, 177-220.
Bridges, K. M. B. (1932). Emotional development in early
infancy. Child Development, 4, 36-49.
Bruner, J. S. (1957). Going beyond the information given.
In H. Gruber, K. R. Hammond, & R. Jesser (Eds.),
Contemporary approaches to cognition (pp. 41-69).
REFERENCES Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cannon, W. B. (1929). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear,
and rage (2nd ed.). New York: Appleton-Century.
Carlsmith, ]. M., & Gross, A. E. (1969). Some effects of
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. guilt on compliance. Journal of Personality and Social
(1978). Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and Psychology, 11, 23 2- 239.
reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49- Carlson, R. (1984). What's social about social psychology?
74. Where's the person in personality research? Journal of
Anderson, J. R., & Bower, G. H. (1973). Human associa- Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1304-1309.
tive memory. Washington, DC: Hemisphere. Carroll, D., Marzillier, J. S., & Merian, S. (1982). Psy-
634 RESEARCH TOPICS
.( .

chophysiological changes accompanying different Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stan-
types of arousing and relaxing imagery. Psychophysiolo- ford, CA: Stanford University Press.
gy, 19, 75-82. Folkins, C. H. (1970). Temporal factors and the cognitive
Collins, D. L., Baum, A., & Singer, J. E. (1983). Coping mediators of stress reaction. Journal of Personality and
with chronic stress at Three Mile Island: Psychological Social Psychology, 14, 173 - 184.
and biochemical evidence. Healch Psychology, 2, 149- Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1980). An analysis of
166. coping in a middle-aged community sample. Journal of
Dodge, K. A. (1980). Social cognition and children's Health and Social Behavior, 21, 219- 239.
aggressive behavior. Chil.d Development, 51, 162- 170. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. ( 1985). If it changes it must
Dodge, K. A., & Core, ] . D. (1987). Social-information- be a process: Study of emotion and coping during three
processing factors in reactive and proactive aggression stages of a college examination. Journal of Personality
in children's peer groups. Journal of Personality and and Social Psychology, 48, 150-170.
Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158. Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (l 988a). Coping as a
Dodge, K. A., Murphy, R.R., & Buchsbaum, K. (1984). mediator of emotion. Journal of Persona/icy and Social
The assessment of intention-cue detection skills in Psychology, 54, 466-475.
children: Implications for developmental psychopa- Folkman, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1988b). The relationship
thology. Chil.d Development, 55, 163-173. between· coping and emotion: Implications for
Drever, J. (1952). A dictionary of psychology. Harmonds- theory and research. Social Science in Medicine, 26,
worth, England: Penguin. 309-317.

I
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and the nature of emotion. Folkman, S., Lazarus, R. S., Dunkel-Schecter, C., De-
In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to longis; A., & Gruen, R. J. ( 1986). The dynamics of a
emotion (pp. 329-343). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. stressful encounter: Cognitive appraisal, coping, and
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across encounter outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social
cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality Psychology, 50, 992-1003.

I
and Social Psychology, 17, 124- 129. Freedman, J. L., Wallington, S. A., & Bless, E. (1967).
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the face: Compliance without pressure: The effect of guilt. Jour-
A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Engle- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7. 117 -124.
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York: Cambridge
Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Simons, R. C. (1985). Is University Press. r
the startle reaction an emotion? Journal of Personality Frijda, N. H. (1987). Emotion, cognitive structure, and r
i
and Social Psychology, 49, 1416-1426. action tendency. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 115-143.
Ekman, P., Sorenson, E. R., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). Gnepp, ]., Klayman, J., & Trabasso, T. (1982). A hierar-
Pan-cultural elements in facial displays of emotions. chy of information sources for inferring emotional
Science, 164, 86-88. reactions. Journal of Experimencal Chil.d Psychology, 33,
Ellis, A., & Bernard, M. E. (1985). What is rational- 111-123.
emotive therapy (RET)? In A. Ellis & M. E. Bernard Gruen, R. ]., Folkman, S., & La:arus, R. S. (1989).
(Eds.), Clinical applications of racional-emocive therapy Centrality and individual differences in the meaning
(pp. 1-30). New York: Plenum. of daily hassles. Journal of Personality, 56, 743- 762.
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988a). From appraisal Hammen, C. L., Marks, T., Mayol, A., & deMayo, A. R.
to emo•ion: Differences among unpleasant feelings. (1985). Depressive self-schemas, life stress, and vul-
Motivation and Emotion, 12, 271-302. nerability to depression. Journal of Abnormal Psycholo-
Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988b). Shades of joy: gy, 94, 308-319.
Patterns of appraisal differentiating pleasant emotions. Harris, P. L. (1985). What children know about the situa-
Cognition and Emotion, 2, 301-331. tions that provoke emotions. In M. Lewis & C. Saami
Emde, R. N. (1980). Levels of meaning for infant develop- (Eds.), The socialization of affect (pp. 161-186). New
ment. In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium York: Plenum. -"
on Chil.d Psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 1-37). Hillsdale, Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A
NJ: Erlbaum. case study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,
Emmons, R. A. ( 1986). Personal strivings: An approach to 49, 129-134.
personality and subjective well-being. Journal of Per- Hebb, D. 0. (1949). The organization of behavior. New
sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 1058-1068. York: Wiley.
Endler, N. S., & Magnusson, D. (Eds.). (1976). In- Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations.
teractional psychology and personality. New York: Wiley. New York: Wiley.
Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predict- Hillman,]. (1960). Emotion: A comprehensive phenomenolo-
ing most of the people much of the time. Journal of gy of theories and their meanings for therapy. Evanston,
Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097-1126. IL: Northwestern University Press.
Epstein, S. ( 1983). A research paradigm for the study of Hochschild, A. R. (1979). Emotion work, feeling rules,
personality and emotions. In M. M. Page (Ed.), Ne- and social structure. American Journal of Sociology, 85,
braska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 30, pp. 91- 551-575.
154). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. House, J. S. (1981). Social structure and personality. In M.
Epstein, S. (1984). Controversial issues in emotion theory. Rosenberg & R. H. T umer (Eds.), Social psychology:
In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of pers0nalicy and social Sociological perspectives (pp. 525-561). New York:
psychology:· Vol. 5. Emotions, relationships, and health Basic Books.
(pp. 64-88). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Ap-
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion pleton-Century-Crofts.
viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Ex- Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.
perimental Psychology: General, 113, 464-486. Izard, C. E., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.). (1984).
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 635

Emotions, cognition, and behavior. New York: Cam- appraisal in the cog01t1on - emotion relationship.
bridge University Press. Cognition and Emotion, 2, 281-300.
Jessor, R. (1981). The perceived environment in psycho- Leeper, R. W. (1948). A motivational theory of emotion
logical theory and research. In 0. Magnusson (Ed.), to replace "emotion as disorganized response." Psycho-
Toward a psychology of situations: An interactional per- logical Review, 55, 5 - 21.
spective (pp. 297 -317). Hiilsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Leeper, R. W. (1965). Some needed developments in the
Jones, E. E., Kanouse, 0. E., Kelley, H. H., Nisbett, R. motivational theory of emotions. In 0. Levine (Ed.),
E., Valins, S., & Weiner, B. (Eds.). (1971). Attribu- Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 13, pp. 25-
tion: Perceiving the causes of behavior. Morristown, NJ: 122). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
General Leaming Press. Levenson, R. W. (1988). Emotion and the autonomic
Kasl, S. V., Evans, A. S., & N iederman, J. C. (1979). nervous system: a prospectus for research on autonom-
Psychosocial risk factors in the development of in- ic specificity. In H. L. Wagner (Ed.), Social psy-
fectious mononucleosis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 41, chophysiology and emotion: Theory and clinical applica-
445-466. tions (pp. 17-42). New York: Wiley.
Klinger, E. (1975). Consequences of commitment to and Leventhal, H. (1980). Toward a comprehensive theory of
disengagement from incentives. Psychological Review, emotion. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in ex~
81, 1-25. perimental social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 139-207).
Lang, P. J. (1979). A bio-informational theory of emotion- New York: AcademiC Press.
al imagery. Psychophysiology, 16, 495 -512. Leventhal, H. ( 1984). A perceptual motor theory of emo-
Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping tion. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches
process. New York: McGraw-Hill. to emotion (pp. 271-291). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Lazarus, R. S. (1968). Emotions and adaptation: Con- Leventhal, H., & Scherer, K. R. (1987). The relationship
ceptual and empirical relations. In W. J. Arnold of emotion to cognition: A functional approach to a
(Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 16, pp. semantic controversy. Cognition and Emotion, I, 3-
175-266). Lincoln: University.of Nebraska Press. 28.
Lazarus, R. S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New
emotion and cognition. American Psychologist, 37, York: McGraw-Hill.
1019-1024. Lewis, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New
Lazarus, R. S. (1983). The costs and benefits of denial. In York: McGraw-Hill.
S. Breznitz (Ed.), The denial of stress (pp. 1-30). New Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Children's emotions and
York: International Universities Press. mood: Developmental theory and measurement. New
Lazarus, R. S. (1984). On the primacy of cognition. Amer- York: Plenum.
ican Psychologist, 39, 124-129. Little, B. R. (1983). Personal projects: A rationale and
Lazarus, R. S. (1989a). Cognition and emotion from the method for investigation. Environment and Behavior,
RET viewpoint. In M. E. Bernard & R. OiGiuseppe 15, 273-309.
(Eds.), Inside rational-emotive therapy (pp. 47-68). Maddux,]., Norton, l., & Stoltenberg, C. (1986). Self-
Orlando, FL: Academic Press. efficacy expectancy, outcome expectancy, and out-
Lazarus, R. S. (1989b). Constructs of the mind in mental come value: Relative effects on behavioral intentions.
health and psychotherapy. In A. Freeman, K. M. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 783-
Simon, L. E. Beutler, & H. Arkowitz (Eds.), Com- 789.
prehensive handbook of cognitive therapy (pp. 99- 121). McGraw, K. M. (1987). Guilt following transgression: An
New York: Plenum. attribution of responsibility approach. Journal of Per-
Lazarus, R. S. (in press). Theory-based stress measurement. sonality and Social Psychology, 53, 24 7-256.
Psychological Inquiry. Metalsky, G. I., Halberstadt, L. J., & Abramson, L. Y.
Lazarus, R. S., & Averill, J. R. (1972). Emotion and (1987). Vulnerability to depressive mood reactions:
cognition: With special reference to anxiety. In C. 0. Toward a more powerful test of the diathesis-stress
Spiel berger (Ed.), AnXiety and behavior (2nd ed., pp. and causal mediation components of the reformulated
242-283). New York: Academic Press. theory of depression. Journal of Personality and Social
Lazarus, R. S., Averill, J. R., & Opton, J. R., Jr. (1970). Psychology, 52, 386-393.
T award a cognitive theory of emotion. In M. B. Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York:
Arnold (Ed.), Feelings and emotions: The Loyola Sym- Wiley.
posium (pp. 207-232). New York: Academic Press. Mischel, W., & Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond deja vu in
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal, and the search for cross-situational consistency. Psycholog-
coping. New York: Springer. ical Review, 89, 730- 755.
Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1987). Transactional Nasby, W., Hayden, B., & dePaulo, B. M. (1979). Attri-
theory and research on emotions and coping. European butional bias among aggressive boys to interpret un-
Journal of Personality, 1, 141-169. ambiguous social stimuli as displays of hostility. Journal
Lazarus, R. S., Kanner, A. D., & Folkman, S. (1980). of Abnormal Psychology, 89, 459-468.
Emotions: A cognitive-phenomenological analysis. Nisbett, R., & Ross, L. (1980). Human inference: Strategies
In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: and shortcomings of social judgment. Englewood Cliffs,
Theory, research, and experience. VoL 1. Theories of NJ: Prentice-Hall.
emotion (pp. 189-217). New York: Academic Press. Nomikos, M. S., Opton, E. M., Jr., Averill, J. R., &
Lazarus, R. S., & Launier, R. (1978). Stress-related trans- Lazarus, R. S. ( 1968). Surprise versus suspense in the
actions between person and environment. In L. A. production of stress reaction. Journal of Personality and
Pervin (Ed.), Perspectives in interactional psychology Social Psychology, 8, 204-208.
(pp. 287-327). New York: Plenum. Ortony, A. (1987). Is guilt an emotion? Cognition and
Lazarus, R. S., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Knowledge and Emotion, 1, 283-298.
t
I

636 RESEARCH TOPICS

Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Foss, M. A. (1987). The of Stanley Schachter (pp. 118-150). Hillsdale, NJ:
referential structure of the affective lexicon. Cognitive Erlbaum.
Science, I I, 341-364. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on
Owens, J., Bower, G. H., & Black, J.B. (1979). The "soap disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23-41.
opera" effect in story recall. Memory and Cognition, 7, Ryff, C. D. (1987). The place of personality and social
185-191. structure research in social psychology. Journal of Per-
Palys, T. S., & Little, B. R. (1983). Perceived life satisfac- sonality and Social Psychology, 53, 1192-1202.
tion and the organization of personal project systems. Sarbin, T. H. (1985, June). Emotions as situated actions.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 1221- Paper presented at the Conference_ on the Role of
j
. I
1230. Emotions in Ideal Human Development, Heinz Wer-
' Pastore, N. (1952). The role of arbitrariness in the ner Institute of Developmental Psychology, Clark
l frustration- aggression hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal University, Worcester, MA.
and Social Psychology, 47, 728- 731. Scheff, T. J. (1979). Catharsis in healing, ritual and drama.
JI Pervin, L. A. (1983). The stasis and flow of behavior: Berkeley: University of California Press.
!
T award a theory of goals. In M. M. Page (Ed.), Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation Vol. 30, pp. 1-

l
optimism and physical well-being: The influenced of
53). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. generalized outcome expectancies on health. Journal of
Peterson, C., & Barrett, L. C. (1987). Explanatory style Personality, 55, 169-210.
and academic performance among university fresh- Scherer, K. R. (1982). Emotion as process: Function, ori-
t men. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, gin and regulation. Social Science Information, 21,
603-607. 555-570.
Peterson, C., Semmel, A., von Baeyer, C., Abramson, L. Scherer, K. R. (1984a). Emotion as a multicomponent
Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1982). process: A model with some cros5-cultural data. In P.
The Attributional Style Questionnaire. Cognitive Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psycholo-
Therapy and Research, 6, 287-300. gy: VoL 5. Emotions, relationships, and health (pp. 37-
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New 63). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
York: International Universities Press. Scherer, K. R. (1984b). On the nature and function of
Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity. Palo Alto, CA: emotion: A component process approach. In K. R.
Annual Reviews Monographs. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion
Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthe- (pp. 293-317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
sis. New York: Harper & Row. Scherer, K. R. (1986). Vocal affect expression: A review
Plutchik, R. (1984). Emotions: A general psychoevolu- and a model for future research. Psychological Bulletin,
tionary theory. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), 99, 143-165.
Approaches to emotion (pp. 197-219). Hillsdale, NJ: Scherer, K. R., & Ekman, P. (Eds.). (1984). Approaches to
Erlbaum. emotion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Plutchik, R., & Kellerman, H. (Eds.). (1980). Emotion: Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., & Summerfield, A. B.
Theory, research, and experience. Vol. 1. Theories of (Eds.). (1986). Experiencing emotion: A cross~cultural
emotion. New York: Academic Press. study. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Postman, L., Bruner, J. S., & McGinnies, E. (1948). Schneider, D. M. (1976). Notes toward a theory of culture.
Personal values as selective factors in perception. In K. Basso & H. Selby (Eds.), Meaning in anthropology
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 142- (pp. 197-220). Albuquerque: University of New
154. Mexico Press.
Repetti, R. L. (1987). Individual and common com- Schwartz, G. E., Fair, P. L., Salt, P., Mandel, M. R., &
ponents of the social environment at work and psy- Klerman, G. L. (1976). Facial muscle patterning to
chological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social affective imagery in depressed and nondepressed sub-
Psychology, 52, 710- 720. jects. Science, 192, 489-491.
Riskind, J. H. (1984). They stoop to conquer: Guiding and Seligman, M. E. P., & Schulman, P. (1986). Explanatory
self-regulatory ftinctions of physical posture after suc- style as a predictor of productivity and quitting among
cess and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- life insurance sales agents. Journal of Personality and
chology, 47, 479-493. Social Psychology, 50, 832-838.
Rosch, E. (1978). Principles of categorization. In E. Rosch Selye, H. (197 4). Stress without distress. Philadelphia: J. B.
& B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and categorization (pp. Lippincott.
2 7 - 71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Shaver, K. G. (1977). Principles of social psychology. Cam-
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotion: bridge, MA: Winthrop.
A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of Shaver, K. G. (1985). The attribution of blame: Causality,
personality and social psychology: Vol. 5. Emotions, rela- responsibility, and blameworthiness. New York: Sprin-
tionships, and health (pp. 11-36). Beverly Hills, CA: ger.
Sage. Shaver, P. (Ed.). (1984). Review of personality and social
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his short- psychology: Vol. 5. Emotions, relationships, !!nd health.
comings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psy- Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O'Connor, C.
chology (Vol. 10, pp. 173-220). New York: Academic (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a
Press. prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social
Ross, L. (1987). The problem of construal in social in- Psychology, 52, 1061-1086.
ference and social psychology. In N. E. Grunberg, R. Shweder, R. A., & LeVine, R. A. (Eds.). (1984). Culture
E. Nisbett, J. Rodin, & J. E. Singer (Eds.), A dis- theory: Essays on mind, self, and emotion. Cambridge,
tinctive approach to psychological research: The influence England: Cambridge University Press.
Chapter 23. Emotion and Adaptation 637

Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychol-
York: Appleton. ogy: Vol. 5. Emotions, relationships, and health (pp.
Smith, C. A. ( 1989). Dimensions of appraisal and physi- 219-238). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
ological response in emotion. Journal of Personality and Thompson, 0. A., & Campbell, R. G. (1977). Hunger in
Social Psychology, 56, 339- 353. humans induced by 2-deoxy-D-glucose: Glucoprivic
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of control of taste preference and food intake. Science,
cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality 198, 1065-1068.
and Social Psychology, 48, 813-838. Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol.
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1987). Patterns of I. The positive affects. New York: Springer.
appraisal and emotion related to taking an exam. Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness: Vol.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 475- 2. The negative affects. New York: Springer.
488. Tomkins, S. S. ( 1980). Affect as amplification: Some
Smith, C. A., Lazarus, R. S., & Novacek, J. (1990). modifications in theory. In R. Plutchik & H. Keller-
Appraisal components, relational themes, and emotion. man (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience.
Manuscript submitted for publication, Vanderbilt Vol. 1. Theories of emotion (pp. 141-164). New York:
University. Academic Press.
Smith, C. A., McHugo, G. J., & Lanzetta, J. T. (1986). Vallone, R., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1985). The
The facial muscle patterning of posed and imagery- hostile media phenomenon: Biased perception and
induced expressions of emotion by expressive and perceptions of media bias in coverage of the Beirut
nonexpressive posers. Motivation and Emotion, 10, massacre. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
133-157. 49, 577-585.
Smith, C. A., Novacek J., Lazarus, R. S., & Pope, L. K. Vogel, W., Raymond, S., & Lazarus, R. S. (1959). Intrin-
( 1990). Antecedents of emotion: Situations, dispositions, sic motivation and psychological stress. Journal of
attributions, and appraisals. Manuscript in preparation, Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 225-233.
Vanderbilt University. Wallington, S. A. (1973). Consequences of transgression:
Solomon, Z., Mikulincer, M., & Hobfoll, S. E. (1987). Self-punishment and depression. Journal of Personality
Objective versus subjective measurement of stress and and Social Psychology, 28, 1- 7.
social support: Combat-related reactions. Journal of Watson, J. B. (1919). Psychology from rhe standpoint of a
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 577-583. behaviorist. Philadelphia: J. B. Lippincott.
Sroufe, A. L. (1979). Socioemotional development. In J. Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achieve-
Osofsky (Ed.), The handbook of infant development (pp. ment motivation and emotion. Psychological Review,
462-516) New York: Wiley. 92, 548-573.
Stein, N., & Levine, L. (1987). Thinking about feelings: Weiner, B., Amirkhan, ],, Folkes, V. S., & Verene, J. A.
The development and organization of emotional ( 1987). An attributional analysis of excuse giving:
knowledge. In R. E. Snow & M. Farr (Eds.), Aptitude, Studies of a naive theory of emotion. Journal of Per-
learning, and instruction: Vol. 3. Cognition, conation, sonality and Social Psychology, 52, 316-324.
and affect (pp. 165-198). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Weiner, B., Graham, S., & Chandler, C. (1982). Pity,
Strentz, T., &Auerbach, S. M. (1988). Adjustment to the anger, and guilt: An attributional analysis. Personality
stress of simulated captivity: Effects of emotion- and Social Psychology Bulletin, 8, 226-232.
focused versus problem-focused preparation on hos- Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental de-
tages differing in locus of control. Journal of Personality velopment (rev. ed.) Chicago: Follett.
and Social Psychology, 55, 652-660. Wicklund, R. A., & Brehm, J. W. (1976). Perspectives on
Thoits, P. A. (1984). Coping, social support, and psycho- cognitive dissonance. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
logical outcomes: The central role of emotion. In

l
l

I
I
t
i

You might also like