You are on page 1of 15

J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. Vol. 40, No. 3, pp.

465–477, 1999
Cambridge University Press
' 1999 Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry
Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved
0021–9630\99 $15.00j0.00

Avoidant\Ambivalent Attachment Style as a Mediator between Abusive


Childhood Experiences and Adult Relationship Difficulties
Gerard McCarthy and Alan Taylor
Medical Research Council’s Child Psychiatry Unit, Institute of Psychiatry, London, U.K.

The role of attachment style, self-esteem, and relationship attributions as possible mediators
between abusive childhood experiences and difficulties in establishing supportive love
relationships in adulthood were investigated in a sample of women known to be at risk of
experiencing relationship problems. Measures of child abuse, the quality of love relation-
ships, and the three potential mediators were made concurrently in adulthood. Participants
who had experienced child abuse were found to be six times more likely to be experiencing
difficulties in the domain of adult love relationships than those who had not. Self-esteem and
relationship attributions were not found to be related to child abuse. When both child abuse
and avoidant\ambivalent attachment style were considered together avoidant\ambivalent
attachment style, but not child abuse, was found to be related to relationship difficulties.
These findings indicate that avoidant\ambivalent attachment style, but not self-esteem and
relationship attributions, is a mediating factor in the route from child abuse to adult
relationship abilities.

Keywords : Child abuse, adult love relationships, mediators, avoidant\ambivalent attach-


ment style.

Abbreviations : A\C : avoidant-resistant ; APFA : Adult Personality Functioning Assess-


ment ; BDI : Beck Depression Inventory ; CSA : child sexual abuse ; ICC : interclass
correlation ; RAM : Relationship Attribution Measure ; RSD : Rosenberg self-derogation
scale ; SESS : Self-Evaluation and Social Support Instrument.

Jouriles, Piffner, & O’Leary, 1988 ; Patterson, 1982) and


Introduction to the development of mental health problems in adult
life (Brown & Harris, 1978 ; Quinton & Rutter, 1988 ;
A number of studies have shown that abusive child-
Sampson & Laub, 1993).
hood experiences are related to difficulties in establishing
As the evidence for a link between abusive childhood
supportive cohabiting relationships in adulthood. Studies
experiences and adult relationship difficulties grows so
into the long-term outcome of childhood sexual abuse
the need to understand how this effect occurs becomes
have found increased rates of interpersonal difficulties
more urgent. A major issue for all concerned with the
and sexual problems (Beitchman, Zucker, & Hood, 1992 ;
well-being of children and adolescents is how to intervene
Finkelhor, 1983 ; Finkelhor, Hotaling, Lewis, & Smith,
to prevent them going on to experience difficulties in
1990 ; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison,
adulthood. However, little is known about the psycho-
1994). Children who have experienced physical abuse or
logical processes underlying this association nor about
harsh and\or neglectful parenting have also been found
the role cognitive-affective mechanisms may play in
to experience difficulties in the domain of intimate adult
mediating the effects of abusive early experiences. A
relationships (Andrews & Brown, 1988 ; Birtchnell, 1993 ;
better understanding of the precise mechanisms through
Brown & Moran, 1994 ; Malinosky-Rummell & Hansen,
which negative childhood experiences exert their effect on
1993 ; Quinton, Pickles, Maughan, & Rutter, 1993). These
later psychosocial functioning is likely to have impli-
findings are important because research has established
cations for methods of intervention to prevent the
that difficulties in close relationships, such as marital
development of later problems.
discord and lack of support, are related both to the
At present, attempts to delineate the role of specific
development of emotional and behavioural problems in
processes in mediating developmental continuities be-
the next generation (Emery, 1982 ; Farrington, 1995 ;
tween abusive childhood experiences and adulthood are
hampered by the nature of the evidence available. Much
of this evidence comes from either short-term longi-
tudinal studies in childhood or from cross-sectional
Requests for reprints to : Dr Gerard McCarthy, Child and retrospective studies undertaken in adulthood (Maughan
Adolescent Service, Weston Clinic, 4a Beaconsfield Rd, & McCarthy, 1997). Researchers investigating the link
Weston-super-Mare, BS23 1YE, U.K. between child abuse and adult depression have recently

465
466 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

begun to undertake direct tests of the role of specific internal working models may play in mediating continu-
mediating factors. This work has shown that stable ities in relationship functioning between childhood and
negative characteristics of the self may mediate the adulthood.
association between child abuse and adult depression. In Investigations of attachment in maltreated children
particular two mediating factors have been identified. have shown high levels of insecure (avoidant and am-
These are bodily shame (Andrews, 1995) and charactero- bivalent) attachments (Crittenden, 1988 ; Egeland &
logical self-blame (Andrews & Brewin, 1990 ; Gold, 1986). Sroufe, 1981 ; Schneider-Rosen, Braunwald, Carlson, &
More recently Andrews (1997) has shown that bodily Cicchetti, 1985). In an inner-city disadvantaged sample,
shame also acts as a mediator between child abuse and Egeland and Sroufe (1981) found a specific association
bulimia in a community sample of young women. between child abuse and the development of avoidant
The present study attempts to build on this small body attachment although subsequent investigations have not
of research to identify specific psychological factors that replicated this finding. More recent research has shown
mediate links between child abuse and adverse psycho- high levels of atypical attachment patterns that do not fit
logical functioning in adulthood. However, it differs in smoothly into Ainsworth et al.’s (1978) traditional A-B-C
two principal ways from the studies by Andrews and her classification scheme (Main & Soloman, 1986). Critten-
colleagues. First, rather than focusing on a particular den (1988) found that most maltreated children in her
psychiatric outcome such as depression or bulimia, we sample could be classified as having avoidant-ambivalent
aimed to investigate psychological processes that mediate (A\C) patterns of attachment and Carlson, Cicchetti,
the link between child abuse and the quality of adult love Barnett, and Braunwald (1989) found that over 80 % of
relationships. As noted above, adult relationship abilities maltreated infants had disorganized\disoriented attach-
are hypothesised to be one of the pathways by which ments (Main & Soloman, 1986). Lynch and Cicchetti
adverse early experiences lead to increased risk of (1991) have demonstrated that distortions in maltreated
psychopathology in adult life. Second, rather than testing children’s mental representations of attachment may
the role of a single mediating factor such as self-esteem, persist into the preadolescent years. They found that
this study aimed to investigate the role of a number of 30 % of maltreated children between the ages of 7 and 13
potential mediators. In particular, attachment style, years reported having confused patterns of relatedness to
relationship attributions, and self-esteem were investi- their mothers.
gated as possible mediating factors between abusive Recent work on adult love relationships has shown
experiences in childhood and later difficulties in the that adult attachment style is associated with the quality
domain of adult love relationships in a sample of adult of these relationships (Hazan & Shaver, 1987 ; Shaver &
women. Hazan, 1993). Studies have demonstrated that attach-
ment style is related to levels of satisfaction, commitment,
Attachment Style and love and trust in the relationship (Collins & Reed,
1990 ; Davis, Kirkpatrick, Levy, & O’Hearn, 1994 ;
According to Bowlby’s theory of attachment (Bowlby, Mikulincer & Erev, 1991 ; Simpson, 1990) and with
1969, 1973, 1980) children develop cognitive\affective observed levels of self-disclosure and emotional support
representations or internal working models of their (Mikulincer & Nachshon, 1991 ; Simpson, Rholes, &
experiences in their attachment relationships. These are Nelligan, 1992). Secure attachment style is associated
hypothesised to provide a model about how close with a desire for intimate relationships, and within
relationships typically proceed and they are thought to relationships secure adults seek a balance of closeness
influence the quality of later love relationships (Bowlby, and autonomy and are comfortable with feelings of
1979, 1988 ; Crowell & Treboux, 1995). Indeed, a central dependency. Anxious-ambivalent attachment style is
tenet of attachment theory is ‘‘ that parent–child relation- characterised by a desire for close relationships, but
ships are prototypes of later love relationships ’’ (Crowell fear of rejection may lead ambivalent adults to seek
& Treboux, 1995, p. 296). Until recently, internal working extreme forms of intimacy and lower levels of autonomy.
models have mainly been investigated in the context of Avoidant attachment style is associated with a need
infants’ relationships with their principal caregivers, to maintain distance and avoidant adults are thought
where they are inferred on the basis of infant’s behaviour to feel uncomfortable with feelings of intimacy and
in separation-reunion procedures (Ainsworth, Blehar, dependency (Shaver & Hazan, 1993). Bartholomew
Waters, & Wall, 1978). New methods have now been (1990 ; Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991) has further
developed to assess older children’s internal working differentiated the avoidant style into a fearful avoidant
models of attachment (Main, Kaplan, & Cassidy, 1985) pattern and a dismissing avoidant pattern. Although
and adult’s representations of their childhood experiences both types share a negative view of others, dismissing
with parents (Main, 1991). At the same time, social and avoidants have a positive view of self, whereas fearful
clinical psychologists have begun to investigate the role of avoidants also have a negative view of self. Fearful
working models of attachment or attachment styles avoidant attachment style is known to be related to
in adult romantic relationships (Collins & Read, 1990 ; a number of high-risk environments such as having
Hazan & Shaver, 1987). However, although there are problem drinking parents (Brennan, Shaver, & Tobey,
growing literatures on both the relationship between 1991) and being the victim of incestuous abuse
maltreatment and the development of insecure attach- (Alexander, 1993). A number of studies have also
ment relationships in childhood and the relationship demonstrated that retrospective accounts of childhood
between attachment styles and the quality of love relationships with parents are related in theoretically
relationships in adulthood, little is known about the role meaningful ways with adult attachment styles (Shaver &
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 467

Hazan, 1993). However, at present empirical investi- retrospective reports of repeated physical and sexual
gations into the role attachment representations may abuse in childhood were associated with a tendency for
play in mediating developmental continuities between women in violent marriages to blame their own characters
childhood adversity and the quality of adult love for the marital violence. Following on from these studies
relationships have yet to be undertaken. we aimed to investigate the role causal and responsibility
attributions may play in mediating links between child
Relationship Attributions abuse and difficulties in adult love relationships.

There are two main reasons for thinking that attribu- Self-esteem
tional processes may play a role in mediating continuities
between childhood experiences and later relationship Many theorists have argued that difficulties in making
difficulties. First, research by Dodge and his colleagues close adult relationships derive from negative thoughts
has demonstrated that children who have been physically and feelings about the self (Erikson, 1963 ; Kohut, 1977 ;
abused and harshly parented tend to acquire deviant Sullivan, 1953). The idea that low self-esteem may
patterns of processing social information and that these mediate developmental continuities between adverse
predict the development of later aggressive behaviour childhood experiences and difficulties in adulthood comes
(Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990 ; Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & from a number of areas.
Valente, 1995 ; Weiss, Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1992). In First, research shows that negative childhood experi-
particular, physically abused children acquired a bias to ences are related to impairments in various aspects of
over-attribute hostile intent to others’ behaviour. Conse- children’s sense of self. Maltreated toddlers have been
quently, children who have experienced physical abuse in shown to have more negative responses to visual self-
childhood may be at increased risk of developing mal- recognition experiments, and to experience more diffi-
adaptive attributional processes that, if carried into later culties in dealing with issues to do with autonomy and
close relationships, may lead to difficulties in this domain. exploration, than comparison children (Egeland &
For example, children who have experienced harsh Sroufe, 1981 ; Schneider-Rosen & Cicchetti, 1984). Older
parenting and deliberate acts of physical or emotional maltreated children describe themselves as being less
cruelty may be at increased risk of interpreting future competent and accepted than comparison children and
negative partner behaviour in close relationships as being appear to have feelings of generalised low self-worth
intentionally motivated, and therefore deserving of blame (Vondra, Barnett, & Cicchetti, 1989). Also, in a series of
and retaliation. studies, Sroufe and his colleagues have shown that the
Second, a number of studies have demonstrated that quality of care children receive in their early relationships
the attributions that partners make for relationship events with parents is related to a number of measures of
are related to marital satisfaction (Baucom, 1987 ; Brad- children’s subsequent sense of self (Sroufe, 1986, 1989).
bury & Fincham, 1990). Distressed partners have been Children who received more sensitive care as infants tend
found to make causal and responsibility attributions that to have higher self-esteem, to have greater ego strength,
accentuate the impact of negative events. Causal attri- to be more ego resilient, to be more independent and
butions concern the explanations a partner makes for the resourceful, and to have more elaborate and complex
occurrence of a relationship event and responsibility fantasy play than children who received less sensitive care
attributions deal with the accountability or answerability (Arend, Gove, & Sroufe, 1979 ; Egeland & Sroufe, 1981 ;
for the event (Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). Researchers Matas, Arend, & Sroufe, 1978 ; D. Rosenberg, 1984 ;
have found that the attributions partners make for Sroufe, 1983 ; Waters, Wippman, & Sroufe, 1979).
relationship events are related to behaviour in adult love Second, whereas these studies show how negative early
relationships (Bradbury, Beach, Fincham, & Nelson, experiences can adversely affect the development of self-
1996 ; Bradbury & Fincham, 1992). Husbands who are esteem in childhood, a series of studies by Brown and his
physically violent to their wives have also been shown to colleagues suggest that these processes may be involved in
be more likely than nonviolent husbands to attribute mediating continuities between lack of affectionate care
negative intentions, selfish motivation, and blame to the in childhood, negativity in current close relationships,
wife (Holtzworth-Munroe, 1992 ; Holtzworth-Munroe & and vulnerability to depression (Andrews & Brown, 1988,
Anglin, 1991 ; Holtzworth-Monroe & Hutchinson, 1993). 1993 ; Brown, Bifulco, & Andrews, 1990 ; Brown, Bifulco,
Whereas current maladaptive attributional processes Veiel, & Andrews, 1990 ; Brown & Moran, 1994). Also of
may be a product of being in an unhappy current importance here is evidence showing that the protective
relationship, it is also possible that maladaptive attri- effects of successful coping and positive experiences may
butional processes play a causal role in bringing about exert their effect on the developmental process by
problems in intimate relationships (Bradbury & Fincham, bringing about an increase in self-confidence and self-
1992). Indeed, maladaptive attributional processes may esteem (Rutter, 1989).
pre-date the present relationship altogether and may have
their origins in an earlier developmental period. Recently, The Nature of Mediating Variables
researchers have begun to investigate associations be-
tween child abuse and maladaptive attributional pro- Given the lack of research in this area the main aim of
cesses in adulthood. Gold (1986) found evidence of this study was to carry out a preliminary investigation
greater dispositional blame for hypothetical bad events in into the role of these three potential mediating factors in
people who reported having been sexually victimised in the link between abusive childhood experiences and later
childhood. More recently, Andrews (1992) found that problems in making supportive love relationships. An
468 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

particular effects of child sexual abuse (CSA) from a


number of other negative childhood experiences (Mullen
et al., 1993). They found that while a number of other
factors also related to psychosocial difficulties in adult-
hood, CSA continued to have a direct negative impact
when these factors were controlled for. Results from the
Christchurch Health and Development Study have con-
firmed this finding (Fergusson, Horwood, & Lynskey,
1996). In order to address these issues a high-risk group
Figure 1. Mediator model. of subjects was used, all of whom had experienced poor
parenting in childhood. This enabled us to investigate
specific processes that may contribute to the impact of
child abuse on adult relationship difficulties.
important limitation of the present study was that The main aim of the study was therefore to investigate
measures of the three potential mediators were made in whether each of the potential mediators met the con-
adulthood rather than in childhood, concurrently with ditions outlined for judging whether a variable can be
measures of functioning in the domain of adult love said to function as a mediator. In particular the study
relationships. Ideally, prospective longitudinal studies aimed to test whether child abuse was significantly related
are required to provide a more powerful test of potential to the three mediating variables (path A), whether the
mediating variables. However, until suitable prospec- mediating variables were significantly related to the
tively studied samples are available, much can be learned quality of adult love relationships (path B), and whether,
from retrospective accounts of childhood experiences. when paths A and B were controlled, a previously
Prospective research in this area is also costly and time significant relationship between child abuse and relation-
consuming. In light of this it seemed reasonable to use the ship problems became nonsignificant. Such a situation
present study to explore as far as possible potential would provide evidence that a variable functions as a
mediators that might profitably be incorporated in future mediator between child abuse and adult relationship
prospective work. problems.
In order to test the value of our hypothesised medi-
ators, we used the framework outlined by Baron and Method
Kenny (1986) to guide our analyses. According to Baron
and Kenny a variable can be thought of as functioning as Participants
a mediator if it meets the following conditions (see Fig. Forty women known to have experienced poor parenting in
1) : (1) if variations in levels of the independent variable, childhood took part in the study. The sample was mainly drawn
in this case child abuse, significantly account for vari- from a number of previous longitudinal follow-up studies of
ations in the presumed mediator (Path A) ; (2) if variations children living in inner-city London (Champion, Goodall, &
in the proposed mediator significantly account for vari- Rutter, 1995 ; Maughan & Hagell, 1996 ; Quinton et al., 1993).
ations in the dependent variable, in this case relationship During these studies participants had been interviewed about
their childhood experiences and this information was used to
quality (Path B) ; (3) when paths (A) and (B) are
establish a sample of women all of whom had experienced poor
controlled, a previously significant relation between the parent–child relationships. A total of 44 women were identified
independent variable (child abuse) and the dependent who met the criterion for having experienced poor parenting in
variable (relationship quality) is no longer significant. childhood, described in more detail below, but we were not able
From a theoretical perspective, when path C is reduced to to trace the current whereabouts of 5 of them. The remainder
zero this provides strong evidence for a single dominant (N l 39) were contacted by letter to explain the purpose of the
mediator, whereas a significant but less complete re- new study and to ask whether they would agree to participate
duction in the residual path C demonstrates that a given and 5 (13 %) women refused to take part. The 34 women who
variable plays an important role in mediating an effect. agreed to take part were interviewed in their homes. We felt it
Research has also shown that abusive childhood was necessary to try to supplement the sample in order to
increase the number of cases available for statistical analyses. In
experiences are often associated with a range of other
order to do this a further sample of women were recruited from
negative childhood experiences such as lack of parental an inner-city general practice in South London. A total of 18
warmth and involvement and poor family functioning women from the general practice were interviewed and 6 were
(Bifulco, Brown, & Adler, 1991 ; Fergusson, Lynskey, & found to meet the criteria for inclusion in the study outlined
Horwood, 1996 ; Mullen et al., 1993). This raises the below.
possibility that the apparent correlation between child The average age of the women was 35n7 and ages ranged from
abuse and adverse psychosocial functioning in adult- 25 to 42 years. Twenty-nine of the women were currently in a
hood may account for the background factors that pre- cohabitation of at least 6 months duration ; 11 were not
dispose to child abuse and not the abuse itself (Mullen currently cohabiting. Twenty-one of the women were currently
et al., 1993 ; Paradise, Rose, Sleeper, & Nathanson, 1994). married and 10 were divorced or separated.
Indeed, there are a number of reports that raise the
possibility that poor family functioning may account for Measures
much of the variance in the outcomes of child sexual Childhood experiences. Adults were interviewed about their
abuse victims (Fromouth, 1986 ; Harter, Alexander, & childhood experiences using a nonscheduled standardised in-
Neimeyer, 1998 ; Wyatt & Mickey, 1987). Recently, terview (Brown & Rutter, 1966). The interview included detailed
Mullen and his colleagues attempted to disentangle the assessments of the quality of parent–child relationships. All of
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 469

the women reported having experienced a poor relationship shows good test–retest reliability (r l n85). There is evidence
with one of their parents. This was defined as having experienced suggesting that this scale reflects two independent aspects of
any two of the following three indicators in childhood or self-esteem, tapped by the positively and negatively worded
adolescence : (1) little or no warmth, (2) harsh or lax discipline, items (Kaplan & Pokorny, 1969 ; Kohn & Schooler,
(3) little or no communication or involvement in activities. 1983 ; Zeller & Carmaines, 1980) and that only the negative
The women were also questioned in detail about abusive measure is linked to childhood adversity and to negative
experiences in childhood. Abuse in childhood was defined as psychosocial outcomes (Andrews & Brown, 1993 ; Kaplan
that occurring before the age of 16 years. & Pokorny, 1969). Therefore only the score based on the
negative items (range 5 to 20) was used in the following
(1) Sexual abuse was defined as that involving direct physical analyses. Following Kaplan and Pokorny (1969) and Andrews
contact of the sexual parts. Willing sexual contact with and Brown (1993) we refer to the negative measure as the
nonrelated peers in the teenage years was excluded. These Rosenberg self-derogation (RSD) scale.
criteria are similar to those used in other studies that have Attributions. The procedure used in this study was a modi-
examined the relationship between childhood sexual fication of the Relationship Attribution Measure (RAM)
abuse and adult psychosocial functioning (Andrews, developed by Fincham and Bradbury (1992). This measure has
1995 ; Andrews & Brown, 1988 ; Bifulco, Brown, & high internal consistency and test–retest reliability. In the RAM
Harris, 1994). subjects are asked to imagine six hypothetical negative partner
(2) Physical abuse was defined as repeated acts of violence by behaviours and after each one they are asked to rate their
a parent towards the child and included acts such as being agreement with six statements on a 6-point scale. Subjects make
punched, burnt, kicked, hit with an instrument, or being the ratings after imagining that the behaviour has just occurred
tied up. in their relationship. Each scale point is labelled ranging from
(3) Emotional abuse referred to situations where the disagree strongly to agree strongly. The first three statements
parent(s) expressed marked negative feelings toward the are used to assess three different types of causal attributions.
child. This included expressions of hostility or scape- These are the extent to which the cause rested with the partner
goating of the child. The hostile behaviour was required (locus), is likely to change (stability), and affects other areas of
to be persistent over time and personally focused on the the relationship (globality). The second three statements are
child. used to assess partner intent, motivation, and blame.
In the procedure used here subjects were asked to think about
Attachment style. Attachment style was assessed using the
the most important love relationships they had been involved
Hazan and Shaver (1987) adult attachment questionnaire. This
in. Then each scenario was read out by the interviewer and the
is a standard procedure for measuring adult attachment style.
subject was asked to imagine themselves in the hypothetical
In the first part subjects were given the three Hazan and Shaver
situation. After this the subject was asked to rate their
descriptions of attachment styles and asked to rate on a 7-point
agreement with the six attribution statements from the RAM.
scale (1 l disagree strongly and 7 l agree strongly), how much
The stories were used to depict the following hypothetical
they agreed or disagreed with each style as a description of the
negative partner behaviours : jealousy, negative mood, lack of
way they generally are in love relationships. For example, the
interest, lack of support in carrying out household chores, an
secure prototype reads as follows : ‘‘ I find it relatively easy to get
unexpected let down, and abandonment. Many of these themes
close to others and am comfortable depending on them. I don’t
had been used in previous research in the field (Fincham &
often worry about being abandoned or about someone getting
Bradbury, 1992 ; Holtzworth-Munroe & Hutchinson, 1993).
too close to me ’’. The avoidant prototype reads : ‘‘ I am some-
Hypothetical behaviours were used because they enable stan-
what uncomfortable being close to others ; I find it difficult to
dard stimuli to be presented to all participating subjects and
trust them completely, difficult to allow myself to depend on
because they could be rated by subjects who were not currently
them. I am nervous when anyone gets too close, and often, love
in a love relationship. Following the RAM, negative partner
partners want me to be more intimate than I feel comfortable
events were used because previous research has shown that
being ’’. The ambivalent prototype reads ; ‘‘ I find that others are
attributions for negative events are related more consistently
reluctant to get as close as I would like. I often worry that my
and more strongly to marital satisfaction than are attributions
partner doesn’t really love me or want to stay with me. I want
for positive events (Baucom, Epstein, Sayers, & Goldman-Sher,
to get very close to my partner, and this some times scares
1989 ; Fincham, Beach, & Nelson, 1987).
people away ’’. In the second part subjects were asked to choose
Examples of the situations include the following :
the attachment style that best described the way they typically
feel in romantic relationships. In our analyses we have focused (1) A woman and her partner have two young children and
on the attachment scales rather than the discrete attachment she notices that he has not completed the chores and tasks
categories, to capitalise on the advantages of dimensional that he said he would do around the house.
analysis (Collins & Read, 1990). First, a limitation of discrete (2) A woman wants to tell her partner something that has
measures is that inevitably some members represent the been on her mind for a while and is important to her.
category ‘‘ better ’’ than others. Second, dimensional scales can Finally she tells him and as she does she notices that he is
help to retain information lost through a forced classification not paying attention to her and he is watching the
procedure, especially where individuals report high scores on television.
two or more of the attachment scales. Third, the use of scale
measures is also useful in terms of statistical power when sample Composite attribution indices were formed for responses across
sizes are small, as in our study (Fergusson & Horwood, 1995). the 18 causal attribution responses (3 dimensionsi6 stimulus
In particular we have focused on the avoidant and ambivalent events) and the 18 responsibility attribution responses (3
scales, as these were most likely to be involved in mediating dimensionsi6 stimulus events). Higher scores on each subscale
developmental continuities between child abuse and adult reflected attributions that accentuated the impact of the negative
relationship abilities. partner behaviour (e.g. see it as more stable, intentional, and
Self-esteem. This was assessed using the Rosenberg self- blameworthy). These composite attribution indices were found
esteem scale (M. Rosenberg, 1965). This is a 10-item Gutman to be reliable. The alpha coefficient for the causal attribution
scale tapping global positive and negative attitudes to the self. It composite was (α l n86) and for the responsibility attribution
has been used with a wide variety of subject populations and composite it was (α l n93).
470 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

Interview measures of love relationships. The quality of


subjects’ love relationships was assessed using the appropriate Results
section from the Adult Personality Functioning Assessment
(APFA), (Hill, Harrington, Fudge, Rutter, & Pickles, 1989). Child Abuse and Adult Relationships : Descriptive
The APFA assesses functioning in a range of domains (work, Statistics
love relationships, friendships, everyday coping, nonspecific
The incidence of child abuse was high in our sample.
social contacts, and negotiations with officials and others).
Unlike trait-based measures of personality functioning, the
Forty-one per cent (16\39) of women with complete data
APFA depends on detailed descriptions of behaviour and reported having experienced some form of child abuse :
ratings are made by the interviewer from relatively extended six reported having been sexually abused, seven reported
periods (of 5 years at the least). In this study ratings were made having been physically abused, and three reported emo-
for the previous 5 years. Questioning in each domain covers a tional abuse. Three women reported having experienced
range of issues. In relation to love relationships, questioning more than one form of child abuse. As expected, a high
covers relationship histories during the rating period, and the level of negative functioning in adult love relationships
extent of shared activities, support, confiding, arguments, and were found in this sample. Forty-nine per cent (19\39)
violence in each relevant relationship. The interview also were rated as having negative functioning in the domain
provides evidence of any deviance (defined as criminality, of adult love relationships over a 5-year period (rating of
drinking, problems of drug abuse) in participants’ partners’ 4, 5, or 6 on the APFA) whereas 51 % (20\39) were rated
functioning. Each domain is rated on a 6-point scale, with a
as having positive functioning in this domain (rating of 1,
rating of 1 indicating exceptionally positive functioning, 2
indicating good functioning, 3 reflecting relatively minor or 2, or 3 on the APFA).
transient problems, and ratings of 4, 5, and 6 indicating Association between child abuse and poor adult relation-
increasing degrees of difficulty. For example, in love relation- ships. Child abuse and negative functioning in adult love
ships if a person has been married or has cohabited for several relationships were strongly associated. Among those who
years, and there have not been any major difficulties, a rating of had experienced child abuse 75 % (12\16) were rated as
1 or 2 is made. When the subject has had persistent discord having experienced difficulties in adult love relationships
leading to repeated breakdown of relationships, or when all compared with only 30 % (7\23) of those who had not
relationships have lacked confiding and support, or there was experienced child abuse (Fisher’s Exact Test, p l n01).
an absence of sustained committed relationships, a rating of 5 Effects of current mood. We investigated the possible
or 6 is made. A rating of 4, 5, or 6 was taken as indicating that effect of current mood on the association between child
a person had experienced difficulties in the domain of adult love abuse and negative functioning in adult love relationships
relationships over the last 5 years and a rating of 1, 2, or 3 was
taken as indicating positive functioning in this domain. The
and found that the BDI measure was not significantly
APFA shows good inter-rater reliability with interclass cor- related to child abuse (Table 1). However, the level of
relations (ICCs) ranging from n65 to n88 in individual domains current depressive symptoms was considerably higher in
and a mean ICC of n77 (Hill et al., 1989). The ICC for the love the group with poor adult relationships compared to the
relationships domain was n81. group with good relationships, this difference just failing
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI ) (Beck, 1984). An additional to reach significance. As depressed mood was not
aim of the study was to investigate the impact of current mood associated with child abuse it cannot act as a mediator of
on the pattern of associations predicted above. For example, the association between child abuse and poor adult
current mood states may influence reports of the past or they relationships. Therefore, the BDI measure has not been
may lead to distortions or biases in the reporting of more recent included in subsequent analyses.
events or experiences (Lewinsohn & Rosenbaum, 1987). Con-
sequently, associations between child abuse, relationship
difficulties, and the potential mediators could in part be
accounted for by current mood state. In order to investigate
this issue the BDI was used. This is a widely used 21-item Possible Mediators : Descriptive Statistics
questionnaire designed to assess the severity of current de-
pressive symptoms. Theoretically we were interested in three possible
mediators of the association between child abuse and
difficulties in the domain of adult love relationships : self-
esteem, attachment style, and attributional style. De-
Statistical Methods scriptive statistics for measures of these characteristics
The majority of analyses were carried out using SPSS (SPSS are as follows : (1) Self-esteem : RSD scale mean 13n3 (SD
Inc., 1994). Due to the small sample size of our study the 3n6) ; (2) Relationship attribution scales : responsibility
association between categorical variables was tested using scale mean 64n1 (SD 17n7), causal scale mean 67n8 (SD
Fisher’s Exact test. Differences between groups on the con- 12n9) ; (3) Attachment scales : avoidant scale mean 3n4
tinuous measures were compared using Mann-Whitney U tests (SD 2n2), ambivalent scale mean 3n3 (SD 2n2). Although
using exact p values as the scales were non-normal. Correlations we have not used the secure attachment scale and the
between scales were investigated using Spearman’s rank coef- forced-choice attachment classification we include them
ficient. For the final mediation models exact logistic regression
here for completeness. As expected, in our sample there
using the package LogXact (LogXact, 1996 ; Mehta & Patel,
1995) was used. This enabled us to avoid problems with tests was a high rate of insecure attachment styles, with 41 %
based on large sample approximations that are used in standard of women rating themselves as having an avoidant
maximum likelihood logistic regression (Mehta & Patel, 1995). attachment style (16\39) and 15 % (6\39) rating them-
All analyses were carried out on 39 cases except for the analyses selves as anxious-ambivalent. The remaining 44 % rated
that included the BDI and the self-esteem scale, each of which themselves as having a secure attachment style (17\39).
had one case with missing data. This distribution of subjects across the three attachment
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 471

Table 1
Association between Measures
Experienced child abuse Poor adult relationships

Yes No Yes No
Test Test
Variable Mean (SD) Mean (SD) statistica p Mean (SD) Mean (SD) statistica p
Beck Depression Inventory 9n5 (9n2) 8n8 (6n8) 170n0 n953 11n6 (8n9) 6n8 (5n7) 116n5 n063
Self-esteem 12n5 (3n2) 13n8 (3n8) 128n0 n191 12n1 (3n2) 14n4 (3n6) 101n0 n021
Attribution scales
Responsibility 71n5 (16n6) 59n0 (17n0) 122n5 n079 73n9 (13n9) 54n9 (16n1) 68n5 n001
Causal 72n8 (11n1) 64n3 (13n2) 121n0 n074 74n2 (9n7) 61n8 (12n8) 61n0 n001
Avoidant attachment scale 4n4 (2n2) 2n7 (2n0) 109n5 n032 4n7 (2n1) 2n2 (1n6) 66n0 n001
Ambivalent attachment scale 4n2 (2n4) 2n7 (1n8) 116n5 n053 4n4 (2n1) 2n3 (1n7) 80n0 n002
Avoidant\ambivalent scale 8n6 (3n9) 5n4 (3n2) 98n5 n014 9n1 (3n3) 4n4 (2n6) 53n5 n001
Secure attachment scale 3n3 (2n1) 4n5 (2n1) 115n5 n050 3n1 (1n8) 4n9 (2n1) 90n0 n004
a All tests based on Mann–Whitney U.

categories was different from those found in other studies. was above significance levels. Child abuse was signifi-
In their 1987 study Hazan and Shaver, for example, cantly related to attachment style with the difference
found that 55 % were secure, 25 % avoidant, and 20 % between groups being largest for the avoidance scale,
ambivalent, whereas Feeney and Noller (1990), in their whereas the difference between groups for the ambiv-
study of Australian students, found 55 % were secure, alence scale was of slightly smaller magnitude and just
30 % avoidant, and 15 % ambivalent. In particular, lower over the nominal significance level (see Table 1). As the
rates of secure attachment style and higher rates of above analyses indicated that both types of insecure
avoidant attachment style were found here. The mean of attachment were related to child abuse and were highly
the secure attachment scale was 4n0 (SD l 2n1). correlated, we created a further scale by summing the
The pattern of associations between the three proposed individual ambivalence and avoidance scales. This re-
mediating constructs (attachment style, relationship attri- sulted in an overall avoidant\ambivalent scale, with high
butions, and self-esteem) were also investigated, and values indicating an attachment style characterised by
significant correlations were found between the measures. high level of both ambivalence and avoidance. We
The relationship attribution measures were correlated considered it preferable to use this measure rather than
with the two attachment scales, particularly the avoidant the secure attachment measure because it indexes negative
scale, which had a correlation of n43 (p l n006) with the attachment process explicitly and in further analyses we
causal scale and n42 (p l n008) with the responsibility will focus primarily on this attachment measure. Based
scale (correlations for the ambivalent scale were : .37, on this measure, those who had experienced child abuse
p l n019 and n35, p l n029, respectively). Scores on the had a much higher score on the avoidant\ambivalent
RSD were negatively correlated with both of the attach- scale than those who had not experienced child abuse
ment scales and the attributional scales. The individual (see Table 1).
correlations were as follows : avoidance scale lkn40, Attachment style and poor adult love relationships. In
p l n013 ; ambivalence scale lkn56, p n001 ; causal order to function as a mediator between poor adult
scale l kn39, p l n015 ; and responsibility scale lkn33, relationships and the experience of child abuse the
p l n042. The avoidant and ambivalent attachment attachment measure also needed to be associated with
scales were positively correlated (correlation l n51, relationship difficulties. When we investigated this as-
p l n001), as were the attributional style scales (corre- sociation we found a strong link with avoidant\
lation l n58, p  n001). ambivalent attachment scale and difficulties in the do-
main of adult love relationships. The mean of this scale
Possible Mediators : Preliminary Analyses among those with good functioning in this domain was
4n4 (SD 2n6) compare to a mean of 9n1 (SD 3n3) for those
As a first stage in our analyses we looked at the relation with poor functioning.
between our potential mediators and the experience of Other mediators and poor adult love relationships.
child abuse to determine the magnitude and significance Although attributions were not significantly related to
of their association and therefore the possibility of child abuse, they were strongly related to poor function-
mediation. Results of these analyses are given in Table 1. ing in the domain of adult love relationships (see Table 1).
The measures of self-esteem and attributional style were For example, subjects with poor functioning in the
not significantly related to the experience of child abuse in domain of adult relationships had more negative scores
this sample and therefore could not act as mediators to on the responsibility scale. This indicated that they were
problems in adult functioning. For the RSD, the mean more likely to attribute negative intent and blame than
scores by experience of child abuse were slightly lower in those who did not have poor adult relationships. The
the group who had experienced child abuse. The attri- RSD scale was also related to poor relationships in
butional style measures gave more differentiation be- adulthood, with those having experienced poor relation-
tween the two groups, but the difference between groups ships having significantly lower RSD scores (see Table 1).
472 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

Table 2
Logistic Regression Models of Poor Adult Relationships
95 % CI

OR Lower Upper Scorea pb


Model 1
Child abuse 6n48 1n36 38n27 7n31 n006
Model 2
Child abuse 3n70 0n51 32n90 2n43 n142
Avoidant\ambivalent scale 1n56 1n18 2n24 11n17 n001
a Exact conditional scores test (df l 1).
b Mid-p value.

Attachment style as a mediator between child abuse and recent experiences (Lewinsohn & Rosenbaum, 1987).
adult relationship difficulties. To investigate the potential Ratings of current mood on the BDI were not sig-
mediating effect of attachment style on the relation nificantly related to reports of having experienced child
between child abuse and poor adult relationships we used abuse nor to adult relationship difficulties. Also of interest
exact logistic regression models ; the results are presented was the finding that the other two potential mediating
in Table 2. Model 1, with only the effect for child abuse variables, self-esteem and relationship attributions, did
fitted, indicates the strength of the relation between child not appear to play a significant role in mediating the
abuse and poor adult relationships. Those who had effects of abusive childhood experiences on later re-
experienced child abuse were over six times more likely to lationship abilities. We have been able to establish that
have difficulties in the domain of intimate adult relation- child abuse appears to have a direct negative impact on
ships than those who had not. The large bounds to the functioning in later adult love relationships in a sample
confidence interval for this effect are due to the small where all the women had experienced poor parenting
sample size of the study, which results in a large degree in childhood, and that this link is partly mediated by
of uncertainty about the magnitude of the effect, attachment processes. Child abuse is often associated
particularly in its upper bound. In order to test the with other measures of poor family functioning and it has
possible mediating effect of attachment style on child been suggested that the latter may account for much of
abuse the avoidant\ambivalent scale was added to the the variance in the outcomes of child abuse victims
model (Model 2). This scale was strongly related to poor (Fromouth, 1986 ; Harter et al., 1988 ; Wyatt & Mickey,
adult relationships—the more insecure individuals being 1987). One of the particular strengths of this study’s
more likely to have poor functioning in the domain of design was that we were able to explore these specific links
adult love relationships. The effect of child abuse in in a sample where all the women had experienced adverse
this model was markedly diminished, becoming non- childhood experiences.
significant and with the odds ratio falling by 43 % to 3n7, Of particular interest was the finding that an avoidant\
indicating that an avoidant\ambivalent attachment style ambivalent style appeared to play an important role in
is a strong mediator in the route from child abuse to mediating the effects of child abuse on adult relationship
poor adult relationships. difficulties. This suggests that child abuse may increase
the risk of developing an attachment style that is
Discussion characterised by high levels of avoidance and ambiv-
alence. That is, abusive experiences in childhood may
Results from this study provide support for the idea increase the risk of developing multiple and contradictory
that the link between abusive childhood experiences and strategies for dealing with attachment-related issues in
difficulties in making supportive cohabiting relationships intimate relationships and this may give rise to recurrent
in adulthood are mediated in part by an avoidant\ oscillations between a desire for extreme forms of
ambivalent attachment style. We have been able to intimacy and a desire to maintain distance. For example,
demonstrate that the avoidant\ambivalent attachment when moving closer to romantic partners, women who
scale meets the three conditions outlined by Baron and have been abused in childhood may feel anxious about
Kenny (1986) for judging whether a variable can be said the prospect of developing an intimate relationship and
to function as a mediator. First, abusive childhood this may lead them to distance themselves from the
experiences were found to be significantly related to the partner. However, when they move away, they may feel
avoidant\ambivalent scale and second, the avoidant\ uncomfortable about being alone and this may in turn
ambivalent scale was significantly related to difficulties in fuel the desire again for more extreme forms of intimacy.
the domain of intimate adult relationships. Finally, a The finding that an avoidant\ambivalent attachment
previously significant relationship between child abuse style may play a role in mediating the effects of child
and difficulties in adult love relationships became non- abuse on adult relationship difficulties is in line with
significant when the above two pathways were controlled. findings from research on attachment in childhood and
Importantly, this pattern of findings could not be adulthood, where atypical attachment categories have
accounted for by current depressive mood. Previous been found in a range of high-risk groups (Belsky &
research has suggested that current mood states may be Cassidy, 1994). Main and Soloman (1986) identified a
associated with distortions or biases in the reporting of fourth pattern known as disorganised\disoriented in
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 473

children and Crittenden (1988) called these atypical to overcome the perceived limitations of questionnaire
patterns avoidant-resistant (A\C) as they are charac- measures of global self-esteem, Brown and his colleagues
terised in part by a co-mingling of avoidant and am- have developed an interview known as the Self-Evalu-
bivalent behaviour. Rates of these attachment classi- ation and Social Support Instrument (SESS), where
fications are known to increase as the severity of social ratings are made on the basis of details about the quality
risk factors increases (Carlson et al., 1989 ; Cicchetti & of the subjects’ life (Brown, Andrews, Bifulco, & Veiel,
Toth, 1995 ; Lyons-Ruth, Connell, Grunebaum, & 1990). They have also found that the SESS is more
Botein, 1990), and researchers have also recently es- effective in predicting depression than Rosenberg’s self-
tablished that disorganised attachment classification is esteem questionnaire and they suggest that this is because
associated with the development of hostile and aggressive the SESS taps specific areas of self-dissatisfaction in real-
behaviour in children both at home and in the classroom life situations and is less vulnerable to mood-state effects
(Lyons-Ruth, Alpern, & Repacholi, 1993). Latty-Mann than the more global questionnaire measure (Andrews &
(1989, 1991), in a study of adults who had grown up in Brown, 1993). In order to address these issues, inter-
families with an alcoholic parent, identified a subgroup of viewer-based measures of the self such as the SESS could
persons who rated themselves as experiencing high levels be incorporated into future studies.
of avoidance and ambivalence in their love relationships. The findings in relation to the attributional measures
Latty-Mann suggested that this attachment style could be also deserve further consideration. Patterns of causal and
called ‘‘ Ambivalent ’’ because they appeared both to responsibility attributions were significantly related to
want closeness but also to fear it and its consequences. investigator-based ratings of functioning in the domain
Latty-Mann and Davis (1996) have recently provided of adult love relationships and the causal and responsi-
evidence suggesting that this new attachment style is bility scales were also found to be moderately related to
closely related to Bartholomew’s Fearful-Avoidant at- child abuse. In particular, women with negative func-
tachment style (Bartholomew, 1990). Importantly, Fear- tioning in their love relationships were found to have
ful attachment style is also known to be related to a more maladaptive causal and responsibility attributions
number of high-risk environments such as having had than women with good functioning in this domain. These
problem drinking parents (Brennan et al., 1991) and findings add to the growing body of literature on the links
being the victim of incestuous abuse (Alexander, 1993). between attributional processes and the quality of in-
Interestingly, Brennan et al. suggested, following Latty- timate adult relationships (Baucom, 1987 ; Bradbury &
Mann and Davis (1988), that the fearful avoidant Fincham, 1990). Although our findings suggest that
category in Bartholomew’s scheme may be analogous to attachment processes are more powerful mediators than
the disorganised or A\C category in infancy (Crittenden, relationship attributions in the link between child abuse
1988 ; Main & Soloman, 1986). and adult relationship abilities, this pattern of findings
The fact that the residual pathway between child abuse may in part be related to a number of methodological
and relationship difficulties was not reduced to zero in difficulties associated with the measurement of attribu-
our final analyses suggests that an avoidant\ambivalent tional processes in close relationships. First, a number of
attachment style is one of a number of mediating factors researchers have highlighted the problems associated
rather than the single dominant mediator. More research with trying to untangle the association between attri-
is required to identify other mediating factors. These may bution processes and current relationship satisfaction
include other cognitive-affective variables such as dys- (Berscheid, 1994 ; Bradbury & Fincham, 1990). Second,
functional patterns of emotional regulation (Cicchetti, the use of hypothetical situations to assess attributional
Ganiban, & Barnett, 1991 ; Thompson, 1990) and bodily processes in close relationships has also been criticised
shame (Andrews, 1995, 1997 ; Gilbert, 1989 ; Gilbert, (Berscheid, 1994). In order to begin to address these
Pehl, & Allen, 1994), as well as genetic and temperamental issues future studies may need to incorporate other
factors (Rutter et al., 1997). techniques that have been developed to assess attributions
The finding that the Rosenberg self-derogation scale in close relationships. These include coding attributions
did not appear to be a significant mediator was perhaps from videotaped marital interactions (Holtzworth-
surprising given that negative self-esteem is hypothesised Munroe & Jacobsen, 1988) and thought sampling during
to play an important role in mediating the link between current interactions using an intercom (Berman, 1988).
early adversity and vulnerability to depression in adult- Results from this study also revealed significant associ-
hood (Brown & Moran, 1994 ; Harris, Brown, & Bifulco, ations between measures of relationship attributions and
1990). However, the findings from this study suggest that attachment style. This suggests that internal working
although negative self characteristics may mediate the models of attachment are related to the way in which
link between child abuse and adult depression, attach- adults interpret and attribute meaning to the behaviour
ment style is a more important mediator in the link of their partners in close relationships (Bowlby, 1973).
between child abuse and adult relationship abilities. Although the cross-sectional nature of the current study
Interestingly, recent work suggests that insecure attach- was not able to address the direction of effects between
ment leads to depressive symptoms in adulthood in- these two sets of constructs, the finding that attachment
directly through its impact on low self-esteem and style, but not attributional processes, were related to
dysfunctional attitudes (Roberts, Gotlib, & Kassel, 1996). childhood experiences suggests that attachment processes
Alternatively, the finding that self-esteem was not a may be playing a role in influencing the organisation of
significant mediator in the link between child abuse and attributional processes. However, it seems likely that
adult relationship abilities may in part reflect issues in the once established, internal working models of attachment
measurement of self-esteem. For example, in an attempt and attributional processes will influence one another in a
474 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

reciprocal manner, with both sets of cognitive processes 1987). Adolescents who have experienced abusive child-
acting to maintain the organisation of the other. hood experiences may be a group who would particularly
Interpretation of the present findings needs to be benefit from such interventions. During adolescence
qualified by several factors. First, it is possible that the young people are beginning to establish romantic rela-
procedures used here to assess childhood experiences may tionships and they have also acquired the necessary
not be an accurate reflection of the quality of these early cognitive skills to enable them think about the causes and
experiences. Adult accounts of experiences from child- consequences of their own and others’ behaviour.
hood may be affected by memory bias. However, a recent
review concluded that as long as retrospective reports are
restricted to factual accounts of significant episodes Acknowledgements—We would like to thank the women who
occurring after the period of infantile amnesia they are generously agreed to take part in this study, Dr Barbara
likely to be reasonably valid (Brewin, Andrew, & Gotlib, Maughan for her helpful comments on earlier drafts of this
1993). These authors suggested that problems associated paper, and Denise Shields for her help in preparing the
with recall bias can be minimised by using investigator- manuscript.
based interviews. They also found that there was no
convincing evidence that retrospective reports of child- References
hood experiences are compromised by current psychiatric
Ainsworth, M., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978).
status. Patterns of attachment : A psychological study of the Strange
Second, the cross-sectional design of this study makes Situation. Hilldale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
it difficult to interpret the direction of causality operating Alexander, P. (1993). The differential effects of abuse charac-
between attachment style and adult relationship diffi- teristics and attachment in the prediction of long-term effects
culties. Although attachment style may play a role in of sexual abuse. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 8, 346–362.
determining abilities in close relationships it is also Andrews, B. (1992). Attribution process in victims of marital
possible that people who have experienced negative close violence : Who do women blame and why ? In J. H. Harvey,
relationships might consequently develop a negative T. L. Orbuch, & A. L. Weber (Eds.), Attributions, accounts
attachment style. In order to address these issues longi- and close relationships (pp. 176–193). New York : Springer-
tudinal prospective studies need to be undertaken where Verlag.
Andrews, B. (1995). Bodily shame as a mediator between
measures of attachment representations are taken prior abusive experiences and depression. Journal of Abnormal
to the assessment of adult relationship abilities. Psychology, 104, 277–285.
Third, some circumspection is required in evaluating Andrews, B. (1997). Bodily shame in relation to abuse in
this exploratory study given the sample size. Although we childhood and bulimia : A preliminary investigation. British
have found significant group differences using standard Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 41–50.
levels of significance we have also found smaller group Andrews, B., & Brewin, C. R. (1990). Attributions for marital
differences, which might potentially be important even violence : A study of antecedents and consequences. Journal
though they are nonsignificant. This is a consequence of of Marriage and the Family, 52, 757–767.
the limited power of the study. For example, the Andrews, B., & Brown, G. W. (1988). Marital violence in the
difference in BDI levels between women with relationship community : A biographical approach. British Journal of
Psychiatry, 153, 305–312.
difficulties and women with good relationships just failed Andrews, B., & Brown, G. W. (1993). Self-esteem and vul-
to reach nominal significance levels even though the nerability to depression : The concurrent validity of interview
group differences were quite large. It is therefore im- and questionnaire measures. Journal of Abnormal Psycho-
portant to replicate the findings of this research in a larger logy, 102, 565–572.
sample to establish their generality. Arend, R., Gove, F., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Continuity of
If replicated, the finding that attachment processes individual adaptation from infancy to kindergarten : A
may play a role in mediating the link between abusive predictive study of ego-resiliency and curiosity in pre-
childhood experiences and adult relationship difficulties schoolers. Child Development, 50, 950–959.
raises the possibility of designing interventions aimed at Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator mediator
breaking intergenerational cycles of disadvantage. It variable in social-psychological research : Conceptual, stra-
tegic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
suggests that interventions aimed at helping women who and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
have been abused to become aware of the origins and Bartholomew, K. (1990). Avoidance of intimacy : An attach-
potential consequences of their working models of ment perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relation-
attachment may be useful in helping them develop ships, 7, 147–178.
more supportive adult love relationships. Insights from Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles
attachment theory suggest that therapeutic inter- among young adults : A test of a four-category model. Journal
ventions aimed at helping abused individuals ‘‘ work of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 226–244.
through ’’ unresolved childhood experiences could reduce Baucom, D. H. (1987). Attributions in distressed relations :
the likelihood of problems in later intimate relationships. How can we explain them ? In S. Duck & D. Perlman (Eds.),
Alternatively, educational programmes aimed at teaching Heterosexual relations, marriage, and divorce (pp. 177–206).
London : Sage.
at-risk groups about the impact of attachment processes
Baucom, D. H., Epstein, N., Sayers, S., & Goldman-Sher, T.
on intimate relationships and family life may be sufficient (1989). The role of cognitions in marital relationships :
to change the organisation of internal working models of Definitional methodological, and conceptual issues. Journal
attachment. More research is required to investigate the of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 31–38.
conditions under which representational models of at- Beck, A. (1984). The Beck Depression Inventory. In M.
tachment can develop and become updated (Hamilton, Williams (Ed.), The psychological treatment of depression : A
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 475

guide to the theory and practice of cognitive behavioral therapy. Carlson, V., Cicchetti, D., Barnett, D., & Braunwald, K. (1989).
New York : Free Press. Disorganized\disoriented attachment relationships in mal-
Belsky, J., & Cassidy, J. (1994). Attachment theory and treated infants. Developmental Psychology, 25, 525–531.
evidence. In M. Rutter & D. F. Hay (Eds.), Developmental Champion, L., Goodall, G., & Rutter, M. (1995). Behaviour
principles and clinical issues in psychology and psychiatry. problems in childhood and stressors in early adult life : I. A
Oxford : Blackwell. 20-year follow-up of London school children. Psychological
Beitchman, J. H., Zucker, K., & Hood, J. E. (1992). A review of Medicine, 25, 231–246.
the long-term consequences of child sexual abuse. Child Cicchetti, D., Ganiban, J., & Barnett, D. (1991). Contributions
Abuse and Neglect, 16, 101–118. from the study of high risk populations to understanding the
Berman, W. (1988). The role of attachment in the post-divorce development of emotion regulation. In K. Dodge & J. Garber
experience. Journal of Personality, Society and Psychology, (Eds.), The development of emotion regulation (pp. 15–48).
54, 496–504. New York : Cambridge University Press.
Berscheid, E. (1994). Interpersonal relationships. Annual Review Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. L. (1995). Developmental psycho-
of Psychology, 45, 79–129. pathology and disorders of affect. In D. Cicchetti & S. L.
Bifulco, A., Brown, G. W., & Adler, Z. (1991). Early sexual Toth (Eds.) Developmental psychopathology, Vol. 2 : Risk,
abuse and clinical depression in adult life. British Journal of disorder, adaption. New York : Wiley.
Psychiatry, 159, 115–122. Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working
Bifulco, A., Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1994). Childhood models, and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of
Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA) : A retrospective Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 644–663.
interview measure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psy- Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1994). Cognitive representations
chiatry, 35, 1419–1435. of attachment : The structure and function of working
Birtchnell, J. (1993). Does recollection of exposure to poor models. In K. Bartholomew & D. Perlman (Eds.), Advances
maternal care in childhood affect later ability to relate ?, in personal relationships, Vol. 5 : Attachment processes in
British Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 335–344. adulthood (pp. 53–90). London : Jessica Kingsley.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss, Vol 1 : Attachment. Crittenden, P. M. (1988). Relationships at risk. In J. Belsky &
London : Penguin. T. Nezworski (Eds.), Clinical implications of attachment.
Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss, Vol 2 : Separation. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
London : Penguin. Crowell, J. A., & Treboux, D. (1995). A review of adult
Bowlby, J. (1979). The making and breaking of affectional bonds. attachment measures : Implications for theory and research.
London : Tavistock Publications. Social Development, 4, 3.
Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss, Vol 3 : Loss, sadness and Davis, K., Kirkpatrick, L., Levy, M., & O’Hearn, R. (1994).
depression. New York : Basic Books.
Stalking the elusive love style : Attachment styles, love styles
Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base : Parent–child attachment and
and relationship developments. In R. Erber & R. Gilmour
healthy human development. New York : Basic Books.
(Eds.), Theoretical frameworks for personal relationships.
Bradbury, T. N., Beach, S. R. H., Fincham, F. D., & Nelson,
Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
G. M. (1996). Attributions and behavior in functional and
Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1990). Mechanisms
dysfunctional marriages. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
in the cycle of violence. Science, 250, 1678–1683.
Psychology, 64, 569–576.
Dodge, K. A., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., & Valente, E. (1995).
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1990). Attributions in
marriage : Review and critique. Psychological Bulletin, 107, Social-information-processing patterns mediate the effect of
3–33. early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of
Bradbury, T. N., & Fincham, F. D. (1992). Attributions and Abnormal Psychology, 104, 632–643.
behavior in marital interaction. Journal of Personality and Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1981). Developmental sequelae
Social Psychology, 63, 613–628. of maltreatment in infancy. In R. Rizley & D. Cicchetti
Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P., & Tobey, A. (1991). Attachment (Eds.), Developmental perspectives in child maltreatment. San
styles, gender and parental problem drinking. Journal of Francisco, CA : Jossey-Bass.
Social and Personal Relationships, 8, 451–466. Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparental conflict and the children of
Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psycho- discord and divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 310–330.
pathology and early experiences : A reappraisal of retro- Erikson, E. (1963). Childhood and society. New York : Norton.
spective reports. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 82–98. Farrington, D. F. (1995). The Twelfth Jack Tizard Memorial
Brown, G. W., Andrews, B., Bifulco, A., & Veiel, H. O. F. Lecture. The development of offending and antisocial be-
(1990). Self-esteem and depression. I. Measurement issues haviour from childhood : Key findings from the Cambridge
and prediction of onset. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Study in delinquent development. Journal of Child Psy-
Epidemiology, 25, 200–209. chology and Psychiatry, 36, 929–964.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., & Andrews, B. (1990). Self-esteem Feeney, J., & Noller, P. (1990). Attachment style as a predictor
and depression. III. Aetiological issues. Social Psychiatry and of adult romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 25, 235–243. Social Psychology, 8, 281–291.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., Veiel, H. O. F., & Andrews, B. Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1995). Predictive validity
(1990). Self-esteem and depression. II. Social correlates of of categorically and dimensionally scored measures of dis-
self-esteem. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, ruptive behaviour disorders. American Academy of Child and
25, 225–234. Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 477–487.
Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lynskey, M. T. (1996).
depression. London : Tavistock. Childhood sexual abuse and psychiatric disorder in young
Brown, G. W., & Moran, P. (1994). Clinical and psychosocial adulthood : II. Psychiatric outcomes of childhood sexual
origins of chronic depressive episodes I : A community survey. abuse. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
British Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 447–456. Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1365–1374.
Brown, G. W., & Rutter, M. L. (1966). The measurement of Fergusson, D. M., Lynskey, M. T., & Horwood, L. J. (1996).
family activities and relationships : A methodological study. Childhood sexual abuse and psychiatric disorder in young
Human Relations, 19, 241–263. adulthood : I. Prevalence of sexual abuse and factors associ-
476 G. MCARTHY and A. TAYLOR

ated with sexual abuse. Journal of the American Academy of Kohut, H. (1977). The restoration of the self. New York :
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 1355–1364. International Universities Press.
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R., & Nelson, G. (1987). Attribu- Latty-Mann, H. (1989). The influence of family of origin
tional processes in distressed and nondistressed couples : 3. dynamics on the etiology of adult attachment styles and love
Causal and responsibility attributions for spouse behavior. styles : With special focus on adult children of alcoholics.
Cognitive Therapy and Research, 11, 71–86. Unpublished Master’s thesis, University of South Carolina,
Fincham, F. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1992). Assessing attribu- Columbia, SC.
tions in marriage : The relationship attribution measure. Latty-Mann, H. (1991). An intergenerational approach to
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 457–468. studying the influence of family of origin dynamics on the
Finkelhor, D. (1983). Common features of family abuse. In D. etiology of adult attachment styles : With special focus on adult
Finkelhor, R. Gelles, G. Hotaling, & M. Straus (Eds.), The children of alcoholics. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
dark side of families : Current family violence research. University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC.
London : Sage. Latty-Mann, H., & Davis, K. E. (1988). Adult children of
Finkelor, D., Hotaling, G., Lewis, I. A., & Smith, C. (1990). alcoholics : Family dynamics and patterns of romantic relating.
Sexual abuse in a national survey of adult men and women : Presented at the International Conference on Personal
Prevalence, characteristics, and risk factors. Child Abuse and Relationships, Vancouver, Canada.
Neglect, 14, 19–28. Latty-Mann, H., & Davis, K. E. (1996). Attachment theory and
Fromouth, M. E. (1986). The relationship of childhood sexual partner choice : Preference and actuality. Journal of Social
abuse with later psychological and sexual adjustment in a and Personal Relationships, 13, 5–23.
sample of college women. Child Abuse and Neglect, 10, 5–15. Lewinsohn, P. M., & Rosenbaum, M. (1987). Recall of parental
Gilbert, P. (1989). Human nature and suffering. London : behaviour by acute depressives, remitted depressives, and
Lawrence Erlbaum. nondepressives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Gilbert, P., Pehl, J., & Allan, S. (1994). The phenomenology of 52, 611–619.
shame and guilt : An empirical investigation. British Journal LogXact. (1996). LogXact for Windows : User manual. Cam-
of Medical Psychology, 67, 23–36. bridge, MA : Cytel Software Corporation.
Gold, E. R. (1986). Long-term effects of sexual victimization in Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1991). Patterns of relatedness in
childhood : An attributional approach. Journal of Consulting maltreated and nonmaltreated children : Among multiple
and Clinical Psychology, 54, 471–475. representational models. Development and Psychopathology,
Hamilton, V. (1987). Some problems in the clinical application 3, 207–226.
of attachment theory. Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 3, Lyons-Ruth, K., Alpern, L., & Repacholi, B. (1993). Dis-
67–83. organized infant attachment classification and maternal
Harris, T., Brown, G. W., & Bifulco, A. (1990). Loss of a parent
psychosocial problems as predictors of hostile-aggressive
in childhood and adult psychiatric disorder : A tentative
behaviour in the classroom. Child Development, 64, 572–585.
overall model. Developmental Psychopathology, 2, 311–328.
Lyons-Ruth, K., Connell, D. B., Grunebaum, H., & Botein, S.
Harter, S., Alexander, P. C., & Neimeyer, R. A. (1988). Long-
(1990). Infants at social risk : Maternal depression and family
term effects of incestuous child abuse in college women :
support services as mediators of infant development and
Social adjustment, social cognition and family characteristics.
security of attachment. Child Development, 61, 85–98.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56, 5–8.
Main, M. (1991). Metacognitive knowledge, metacognitive
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized
as an attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social monitoring and singular (coherent) versus multiple (inco-
Psychology, 52, 511–524. herent) models of attachment : Findings and directions for
Hill, J., Harrington, R., Fudge, H., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. future research. In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P.
(1989). Adult Personality Functioning Assessment (APFA) : Marris (Eds.), Attachment across the life cycle (pp. 127–159)
An investigator-based standardized interview. British Journal London : Routledge.
of Psychiatry, 155, 24–35. Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy,
Holtzworth-Munroe, A. (1992). Social skill deficits in maritally childhood and adulthood : A move to the level of representa-
violent men : Interpreting the data using a social information tion. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.) Growing points of
processing model. Clinical Psychology Review, 12, 605–617. attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Anglin, K. (1991). The competency Research in Child Development, 50, 66–104.
of responses given by maritally violent versus nonviolent men Main, M., & Soloman, J. (1986). Discovery of an insecure-
to problematic marital situations. Violence and Victims, 6, disorganized\disorientated attachment pattern : Procedures,
257–269. findings and implications for the classification of behaviour.
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Hutchinson, G. (1993). Attributing In T. B. Brazelton & M. Yogman (Eds.), Affective devel-
negative intent to wife behavior : The attributions of maritally opment in infancy. Norwood, NJ : Ablex.
violent versus nonviolent men. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Malinosky-Rummell, R., & Hansen, D. J. (1993). Long-term
chology, 102, 206–211. consequences of childhood physical abuse. Psychological
Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1988). Toward a Bulletin, 114, 68–79.
methodology for coding spontaneous causal attributions : Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of
Preliminary results with married couples. Journal of Social adaptation in the second year. The relationship between
and Clinical Psychology, 7, 101–112. quality of attachment and later competent functioning. Child
Jouriles, E. N., Piffner, L. J., & O’Leary, S. G. (1988). Marital Development, 49, 547–556.
conflict, parenting, and toddler conduct problems. Journal of Maughan, B., & Hagell, A. (1996). Poor readers in adulthood :
Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 197–206. Psychosocial functioning. Development and Psychopathology,
Kaplan, H. B., & Pokorny, A. D. (1969). Self-derogation and 8, 457–476.
psycho-social adjustment. Journal of Nervous and Mental Maughan, B, & McCarthy, G. (1997). Childhood adversities
Disease, 149, 421–434. and psychosocial disorders. British Medical Bulletin, 53,
Kohn, M. L., & Schooler, C. (1983). Work and personality : An 156–169.
inquiry into the impact of social stratification. Norwood, NJ : Mehta, C. R., & Patel, N. R. (1995). Exact logistic regression :
Ablex. Theory and examples. Statistics in Medicine, 14, 2143–2160.
AVOIDANT\AMBIVALENT ATTACHMENT STYLE 477

Mikulincer, M., & Erev, I. (1991). Attachment style and the Schneider-Rosen, K., & Cicchetti, D. (1984). The relationship
structure of romantic love. British Journal of Social Psy- between affect and cognition in maltreated infants : Quality of
chology, 30, 273–291. attachment and the development of visual self-recognition.
Mikulincer, M., & Nachshon, O. (1991). Attachment styles and Child Development, 55, 648–658.
patterns of self-disclosure. Journal of Personality and Social Shaver, P. R., & Hazan, C. (1993). Adult romantic attachment :
Psychology, 61, 321–331. Theory and evidence. Advances in Personal Relationships, 4,
Mullen, P. E., Martin, J. L., Anderson, J. C., Romans, S. E., & 29–70.
Herbison, G. P. (1994). The effect of child sexual abuse on Simpson, J. A. (1990). Influence of attachment styles on
social, interpersonal and sexual function in adult life. British romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social
Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 35–47. Psychology, 59, 971–980.
Nurmi, J. E., & Pulliainen, H. (1991). The changing parent– Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Nelligan, J. S. (1992). Support-
child relationship, self-esteem, and intelligence as determi- seeking and support-giving within couple members in an
nants of orientation to the future during early adolescence. anxiety-provoking situation : The role of attachment styles.
Journal of Adolescence, 14, 35–51. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 434–446.
Paradise, J. E., Rose, L., Sleeper, L. A., & Nathanson, M. SPSS Inc. (1994). SPSS for Windows V. 6.1. Chicago, IL :
(1994). Behaviour, family functioning, school performance Author.
and predictors of persistent disturbance in sexually abused Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant–caregiver attachment and adap-
children. Pediatrics, 93, 452–459. tation in the preschool : The roots of competence and
Patterson, G. R. (1982). A social learning approach, Vol. 3. maladaptation. In M. Pearlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Sym-
Coercive family process. Eugene, OR : Castalia Publishing posium in Child Psychology, Vol. 16. Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence
Co. Erlbaum.
Quinton, D., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., & Rutter, M. (1993). Sroufe, L. A. (1986). Appraisal : Bowlby’s contribution to
Partners, peers and pathways : Assortative pairing and psychoanalytic theory and developmental psychology ; at-
continuities in conduct disorder. Development and Psycho- tachment : separation : loss. Journal of Child Psychology and
pathology, 5, 763–783. Psychiatry, 27, 841–849.
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parental breakdown : The Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Pathways to adaptation and maladap-
making and breaking of intergenerational links. Aldershot, tion : Psychopathology as developmental deviation. In D.
U.K. : Gower. Cicchetti (Ed.) Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psy-
Roberts, J. E., Gotlib, I. H., & Kassel, J. D. (1996). Adult chopathology, Vol. 1 : The emergence of a discipline (pp.
attachment security and symptoms of depression : The 13–40). Hillsdale, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum.
mediating roles of dysfunctional attitudes and low self- Sullivan, H. S. (1953). The interpersonal theory of psychiatry.
esteem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychiatry, 70, New York : Norton.
310–320. Thompson, R. (1990). Emotions and self-regulation. Nebraska
Rosenburg, D. (1984). The quality and content of preschool Symposium on Motivation, 36, 367–467.
fantasy play : Correlates in concurrent social\personality Urban, J., Carlson, E., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (1991).
function and early mother-child attachment relationships. Patterns of individual adaptation across childhood. Develop-
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Minnesota. ment and Psychopathology, 4, 445–460.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Vondra, J., Barnett, D., & Cicchetti, D. (1989). Perceived and
Princeton, NH : Princeton University Press. actual competence among maltreated and comparison school
Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. children. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 237–255.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 23–51. Waters, E., Wippman, J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment,
Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., positive affect, and competence in the peer group : Two
Maughan, B., Ormel, J., Meyer, J., & Eaves, L. (1997). studies in construct validation. Child Development, 50,
Integrating nature and nurture : Implications for person– 821–829.
environment correlations and interactions for developmental Weiss, B., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S. (1992).
psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 335– Some consequences of early harsh discipline : Child ag-
364. gression and a maladaptive social information processing
Sampson, R., & Laub, J. (1993). Crime in the making : Pathways style. Child Development, 63, 1321–1335.
and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA : Harvard Wyatt, G. E., & Mickey, M. R. (1987). Ameliorating the effects
University Press. of child sexual abuse : An exploratory study of support by
Schneider-Rosen, K., Braunwald, K. G., Carlson, V., & Cic- parents and others. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2,
chetti, D. (1985). Current perspectives in attachment theory : 403–414.
Illustration from the study of maltreated infants. In I. Zeller, R. A., & Carmaines, E. G. (1980). Measurement in the
Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points in attachment social sciences : The link between theory and data. Cambridge :
theory and research (pp. 194–210). Monographs of the Society Cambridge University Press.
for Research and Development in Child Development, 50 (1–2,
Serial No. 209). Manuscript accepted 30 June 1998
This document is a scanned copy of a printed document. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy.
Users should refer to the original published version of the material.

You might also like