You are on page 1of 18

Chemistry Education

Research and Practice


View Article Online
PAPER View Journal

Beyond the beaker: students’ use of a scaffold to


connect observations with the particle level in the
Cite this: DOI: 10.1039/d0rp00206b
organic chemistry laboratory
Liz Keiner and Nicole Graulich *
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Understanding ongoing chemical processes in the laboratory requires constant shifting between
different representational levels—the macroscopic, submicroscopic, and symbolic levels—and analysis of
the various mechanistic features of each of these levels. Thus, the ability to explain observations of
chemical phenomena with regard to their submicroscopic levels in the laboratory is a key requirement.
Research shows that students have difficulty in discerning and comprehending the meaning and
visualization of the submicroscopic level. Traditional laboratory instruction often fails to help students
discern the relationship between their observations and the corresponding chemical processes.
Consequently, there is a high demand for new teaching strategies which address these issues.
Therefore, we developed and implemented a scaffold for the organic laboratory and tested it in a
research study using qualitative methods. The scaffold encourages students to purposefully separate and
connect the macroscopic and submicroscopic representational levels. The implementation of the
scaffold was accompanied by semi-structured pre- and post-interviews with students (N = 22) and an
Received 10th July 2020, analysis of students’ work with the scaffold in the laboratory. We analysed students’ sense-making
Accepted 2nd October 2020 approach while reflecting on organic syntheses before and after working with the scaffold, and
DOI: 10.1039/d0rp00206b characterized changes in their approach. The findings emphasize the need to develop further resources
to support students’ understanding of the submicroscopic level. Implications of these findings for
rsc.li/cerp research and teaching to foster meaningful learning in the organic laboratory are discussed.

Introduction (Reid and Shah, 2007; Collison et al., 2012). While in the lab, the
processes beyond the beaker may not be discussed thoroughly
Learning and teaching in the laboratory is a core component enough to help students develop robust understanding of
of chemistry education and has the potential to enrich the processes at the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels and
formation of science concepts (Hofstein and Lunetta, 1982; to translate between them (Gabel and Bunce, 1984; Johnstone,
Johnstone, 1991; Bretz, 2019; Molvinger et al., 2020). Laboratory 2000; Gkitzia et al., 2019; Sumfleth and Nakoinz, 2019).
teaching aims at meeting numerous learning objectives required Students’ difficulties in understanding and representing the
to successfully grasp practical syntheses, connect theory presented nature of matter at the individual representational levels,
in lectures with laboratory observations, and understand the and especially at the submicroscopic level, is a well-known
processes ‘‘beyond the beaker’’ (Bretz, 2019; Walker et al., 2019; challenge in chemistry education (Johnstone, 1982; Ben-Zvi
Czysz et al., 2020). et al., 1986; Griffiths and Preston, 1992; Kozma and Russell,
Most laboratory curricula still employ ‘‘cookbook’’ procedures 1997; Gilbert and Treagust, 2009a, 2009b; Cheng and Gilbert,
that require students to follow the recipe, stepwise, in order to 2017; Faulconer et al., 2018).
synthesize the desired product. Consequently, students acquire Besides the challenge of transitioning between represen-
limited and superficial knowledge about procedures and tational levels, students also struggle to fully account for
chemical processes (Hofstein and Lunetta, 1982; Domin, 1999; underlying mechanisms when analysing laboratory work-up
Galloway and Bretz, 2015) and lose the connection between the procedures in organic chemistry. While they are able to
theory taught in lectures and the practical work of the laboratory identify the respective entities, they miss the properties and
activities that produce a given phenomenon (Keiner and
Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Institute of Chemistry Education,
Graulich, 2020). Although research emphasizes the need to
Heinrich-Buff-Ring 17, 35392 Giessen, Germany. explicitly communicate each level and to connect the macro-
E-mail: Nicole.Graulich@didaktik.chemie.uni-giessen.de scopic and submicroscopic levels in a meaningful way, the

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

potential for connecting macroscopic observations and Learning (POGIL) activities in the expository laboratory. The
analysing the submicroscopic level often goes untapped in effectiveness of POGIL in general, and particularly in organic
traditional labs. chemistry (Schroeder and Greenbowe, 2008; Vishnumolakala
Increased awareness of this untapped potential has led to et al., 2017), was evidenced by improved student performance
the implementation of various learning approaches to support and a corresponding decrease in the number of students
students’ laboratory work in chemistry education. Researchers withdrawing from the course (Farrell et al., 1999; Spencer,
have developed additional instructional media to improve 1999; Stegall et al., 2016).
the teaching in organic laboratories. Pölloth et al. (2020), for A recent study emphasized the metacognitive aspect of using
example, created a library of online videos to prepare students the representational levels. Thomas (2017) described a classroom
for the upcoming laboratory techniques and syntheses in the intervention in which the teacher used the term ‘triangulation’ as
laboratory. They could show that the videos have a positive an expression to stimulate metacognitive reflection in students to
impact on students’ self-concept of ability and that students consider the importance and use of the three representational
show an increase of knowledge in a know-how test on labora- levels for learning chemistry. In this study, students improved
tory techniques (Pölloth et al., 2020). Furthermore, there has their understanding due to this metacognitive reflection, however,
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

been an increase in the use of digital badges (Hennah and students’ views of the importance and the cognitive processes
Seery, 2017) and student-generated videos in chemistry labora- associated with it varied across individuals.
tories (Schmidt-McCormack et al., 2017; Seery et al., 2017; The above approaches were successful in helping students
Gallardo-Williams et al., 2020). In the organic teaching labora- understand and explain the underlying process of their
tory, for example, digital media applications, such as virtual lab procedures. However, the ability to reflect on and make
reality, become more and more important (Ferrell et al., 2019). connections between the macroscopic and the submicroscopic
Virtual reality allows immersive interactions with a dynamic representational levels in a laboratory setting has not yet been
molecular world and engage students in exploring molecular explored as an in-lab means to support students’ understanding.
structures, motions, and particle interactions. Such virtual Based on the ongoing discussion of students’ difficulties
reality applications could enable students to understand and in the organic laboratory, we designed and implemented a
visualize the invisible chemical processes beyond the beaker. scaffold that aims at encouraging students to consciously
Other research groups focused on specific practical laboratory separate and connect the macroscopic and submicroscopic
techniques and developed laboratory instructions for these representational levels during their practical lab work.
techniques. For example, meaningful learning strategies for
liquid–liquid extractions were developed and tested
(Assadieskandar et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2020). Other approaches Theoretical framework
include restructuring the curriculum to foster students’ cognitive
and practical skills (Seery et al., 2018; Lipton, 2020) and mixed Explaining the interactions ‘‘beyond the beaker’’
approaches with tutorials and practical work (Molvinger et al., Chemists need to think ‘‘beyond the beaker’’ in order to
2020). Supasorn et al. (2008) tested the impact of a pre-laboratory understand the system that, for instance, extracts, purifies,
organic extraction simulation on comprehension and attitudes of and transports molecules in the flask (see Fig. 1). To explain
undergraduate chemistry students. This instructional approach the interactions beyond the beaker, chemists use models at the
aimed at helping students to visualize extraction concepts at the particle level. With regard to making inferences, one of the
molecular level, and afterwards to connect these concepts with most powerful and productive ideas in chemistry education was
the respective macroscopic observations. The pre-lab activity and the introduction of the ‘‘chemistry triplet’’, also called the
the animations were limited to the extraction steps and did not ‘‘Johnstone triangle’’ (Johnstone, 1982).
presented further synthesis steps. Thanks to Johnstone’s triangle, it is common practice to
Other classroom interventions or initiatives, such as the rationalize chemical phenomena at three levels of representa-
Science Writing Heuristic (SWH) and Process-Oriented Guided tion—the macroscopic (observable) level, the submicroscopic
Inquiry Learning (POGIL), focused on a well guided and struc- (particle) level, and the symbolic level (Gabel et al., 1992;
tured prompting to support students in making sense of their Johnstone, 1993; Gabel, 1999; Gilbert and Treagust, 2009a, 2009b).
laboratory work. The Science Writing Heuristic (SWH) (Schroeder The macroscopic level describes tangible and visible obser-
and Greenbowe, 2008; Stephenson and Sadler-McKnight, 2016), vations of a phenomenon and is the most accessible level while
a laboratory report format is based on a learning cycle whereby conducting experiments in the laboratory. This includes obser-
students explore concepts to look for trends or patterns, rather vations, such as colour changes or the formation of new
than to verify an expected outcome (Lawson, 1989; Keys et al., products, e.g. gases (Treagust et al., 2003).
1999; Lawson, 2001). Students who used the SWH format per- The submicroscopic level describes the invisible processes
formed significantly better on an American Chemical Society of a phenomenon, the particle level (see Fig. 1), in terms of the
(ACS) standardized exam (Hand et al., 2004), as well as on movement, organization, or collision of particles, electrons,
in-class lecture exams and quizzes (Burke et al., 2006; Greenbowe molecules, or atoms.
et al., 2007). Another widespread approach in chemistry education To communicate and visualize the observed processes,
is the implementation of Process-Oriented Guided Inquiry chemists commonly use the symbolic level, which includes

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

to progress towards causal mechanistic reasoning (Crandell et al.,


2019; Crandell et al., 2020).
Typically, entities, properties, and activities are conceived as
parts of the submicroscopic level, but each of these parts and
its features are visible macroscopically. In a recent study, we
investigated to what extent students transit between individual
representational levels (macroscopic and submicroscopic)
and characterized the activated mechanistic features at each
of these levels while students were engaged in analysing
laboratory work-up procedures in an organic chemistry lab
(Keiner and Graulich, 2020).
Fig. 2 shows an overview of the features of the respective
representational levels, based on our analysis in a previous
study (Keiner and Graulich, 2020). The findings of this study
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

revealed that students’ language is dominated by macroscopic


terms and entities and tends not to consider submicroscopic
activities. Only a few students could explain the given pheno-
menon at the submicroscopic level. It became apparent that
Fig. 1 Thinking ‘‘beyond the beaker’’. An illustration of an observation students do not seem to know the level at which they are
(separation of two phases): one possible visualization at the particle level explaining a phenomenon or how to purposefully connect the
and an associated description of the sense-making process. representational levels to make sense of the phenomenon.
Our previous findings (2020) emphasized the need to create
awareness in students that the macroscopic and submicro-
pictorial, algebraic, physical, and computational forms (e.g.
scopic levels both contribute to understanding the phenomenon;
chemical equations, graphs, reaction mechanisms, analogies,
that students need opportunities to connect both levels; and that
and model kits) (Kozma and Russell, 1997; Russell et al., 1997;
students should use their thought processes to evaluate the
Treagust et al., 2003). The symbolic level can be seen as a
objectives of a synthesis step during laboratory work.
mediator between the macroscopic and submicroscopic level.
Often it cannot be clearly separated from the other representa- Scaffolding reasoning
tional levels because both the macroscopic and submicroscopic
Scaffolding is an instructional technique that, ideally, enables a
levels can be expressed symbolically (Taber, 2002).
novice to complete a task or problem that he or she could not
have accomplished unassisted (Wood et al., 1976; Belland et al.,
Reasoning about chemical phenomena 2011). Scaffolding students’ reasoning can be a powerful tool to
Analysing a given chemical phenomenon requires going one help students activate and connect prior knowledge pieces.
level below the visible surface to identify the entities and their A scaffold could as well encourage students to reflect on how
interactions at play in the process (Hempel and Oppenheim, they approached a task in a specific context (Davis, 2000).
1948; Salmon, 1984; Machamer et al., 2000; Talanquer, 2010; Scaffolding can thus combine structuring the cognitive process
Braaten and Windschitl, 2011; Rottman and Keil, 2011; Yeo and of solving a problem with metacognitive and procedural
Gilbert, 2014; Krist et al., 2018). In recent years, there has been prompting (Reiser, 2004). Scaffolding enables learners to inter-
increased interest in characterizing and fostering students’ nalise the guidance through distributed practice and, even-
ability to generate mechanistic explanations for phenomena tually, self-regulate their cognitive actions. Common features of
in science education (Grotzer, 2003; Russ et al., 2008a; Russ a scaffold are: focusing learner’s attention towards a goal,
et al., 2008b) and in chemistry education (Becker et al., 2016; simplifying the task in a stepwise manner, modelling and
Caspari et al., 2017; Caspari et al., 2018; Talanquer, 2018). demonstrating, prompting for ongoing diagnosis and assess-
A mechanistic explanation is described in the philosophy of ment, or even prompting the eventual transfer of responsibility
science as a detailed description of the underlying process (Reiser, 2004; Puntambekar and Hubscher, 2005; Seel, 2011).
responsible for the phenomenon. Mechanistic explanations Such prompts or questions could provide students with cues
first identify the relevant entities involved in the process, their what knowledge to activate, connect, and include in their
properties, the activities in which they are engaged, and their explanation (Kang et al., 2014; Kararo et al., 2019). Prompting
organization. The activities and the organization of core enti- students, for example, could direct student attention to facilitate
ties are responsible for the properties and behaviours of the awareness of potential knowledge gaps, help them organise their
system as a whole (Machamer et al., 2000). Usually, accounting thoughts, and recognise a need to evaluate the validity of their
for the underlying interactions of a mechanism refers to the solutions (Ge and Land, 2003).
invisible particulate or molecular level (Sevian and Talanquer, Learning or teaching with a scaffold is commonly a tem-
2014; Becker et al., 2016). Thus, transitioning from the macro- porary support that is withdrawn when the learner progres-
scopic to the submicroscopic level is a core competency needed sively internalizes the scaffold. The cognitive load that a

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice


Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Fig. 2 Description of the mechanistic features in general and at the macroscopic and submicroscopic representational levels.

complex task, such as conducting an organic synthesis, may underlying mental models while being engaged to make a
impose on the learner should be reduced by the scaffold continuous effort to comprehend and explain, i.e. to make
(Sweller et al., 1998; Kirschner, 2002). Slowly students learn to sense of a scientific phenomenon. Thus, in this study we
independently solve the problem, building up the necessary use the term ‘‘sense-making approach’’ to capture students’
reasoning skills and heuristics. approaches to describe and explain chemical phenomena and
the underlying molecular processes.
To answer these research questions, we performed a quali-
Goals and research questions tative interview study with chemistry student teachers enrolled
Based on our findings in a previous study (Keiner and in an organic laboratory course.
Graulich, 2020), we developed a scaffold that purposefully
prompts students to (a) carefully consider their macroscopic Methods
observations and submicroscopic explanations separately, and
(b) connect both levels and judge if they reached their synthesis Context and participants
goal. In this study we attempt to elicit, qualitatively, whether The research study was conducted at a German university in
the extent to which students’ make use of the scaffold changes July and August 2019. Students were recruited on a voluntary
the way they explain organic synthesis on both levels; and basis from the practical course ‘‘Organic Chemistry Laboratory’’
whether they make use of multiple mechanistic features when via in-class announcements. The course is part of the teacher
explaining organic syntheses. Our study is guided by the training programme for student teachers and normally takes
following research questions: place during the summer break between the fifth and sixth
1. To what extent does students’ activation of mechanistic semesters of their studies. The organic chemistry laboratory
features (at the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels) change course covers a period of four weeks during which students attend
after they have worked with the scaffold during two organic a lecture from 8:00 am to 10:00 am and work in the lab from 10:00
syntheses? am to 6:00 pm. Successful completion of the module ‘‘Organic
2. What kind of sense-making approaches (with regard to Chemistry I’’ (OC I) is the prerequisite for participating in the
the representational levels) do students use while reasoning practical ‘‘Organic Chemistry Laboratory’’ course. The OC I lecture
about an organic synthesis before and after working with the provides basic knowledge of organic chemistry and discussions
scaffold? on typical reaction mechanisms (e.g. electrophilic addition
3. To what extent is each student’s individual work with the reaction, nucleophilic substitution, radical polymerization,
scaffold related to a change in his or her sense-making and esterification).
approach? During the laboratory course, students synthesize six given
The term ‘‘sense-making’’ is widely used in the educational compounds through nucleophilic substitutions, electrophilic
literature (Fitzgerald and Palincsar, 2019) to express a learner’s additions, or elimination reactions and report the synthesis in
approach to make sense of phenomena. A sense-making process a traditional lab protocol that also requires information on
can be broadly understood as an expression of the learner’s the mechanism and the obtained yield. Students use typical

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

laboratory procedures—isolation and purification (e.g. washing 3. Particle level. In the third part, students were prompted
with sodium carbonate, fractional distillation)—for the work- to describe what they had concluded after their observations
up. These laboratory techniques are provided in advance in a (e.g. describe the underlying process of the mechanism of the
written online manual. The protocol used in this laboratory reaction). Afterwards, students were prompted to visualize the
course does not explicitly encourage students to connect the process before and after this step at the submicroscopic level.
different representational levels to each other. Students were free to choose whether to draw the particles or
A total of 22 undergraduate chemistry student teachers use symbolic representations of the molecules.
(15 female and 7 male) agreed to participate in the study. They 4. Conclusion and aim. In the fourth part, students were
were between 22 and 47 years old (the average age was 24 years) prompted to decide, based on their observations and sub-
and were invited via an announcement to students attending a microscopic explanations, if they had achieved the goal of the
class lecture or in the laboratory. All students who volunteered synthesis step. Students could not always assess whether they
for this study were provided with information about their rights had been successful or not because, as in the example with two
and the handling of the data; informed consent was obtained colourless solutions, the results were hard to differentiate. After
from all participants. Institutional Review Board approval is not each of the first three parts, students were asked to rate the
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

required at German universities, but the recruitment process perceived difficulty of filling out these parts of the scaffold.
followed ethical guidelines and it was clarified to the students Unlike the previous three parts, the last step required students
that they could opt out at any time. to rate how sure they were that they had achieved the aim of
All students provided written informed consent for (a) the this synthesis step.
use of transcripts of their interviews by the research team,
(b) the analysis of their data by the research team, and (c) the Data collection
use of their data, including their drawings and notes, for This study aims to understand the extent to which a scaffold
publication. In this study, participants were assigned pseu- administered in an organic laboratory course can stimulate
donyms; no identifying information was recorded or scanned students to activate mechanistic features and influence their
that would allow the re-identification of participants’ data. approach to making sense of synthesis steps. To compare
The interviews were conducted in German, and students’ students’ approaches before and after instruction, the first
interview excerpts and written notes were translated from author conducted pre- and post-semi-structured interviews.
German to English. During these semi-structured interviews, students were
prompted to comment on their experiences in the lab and to
Research instrument explain, step-by-step, how they carried out one of the six organic
The research instrument consisted of a scaffold (see Fig. 3) syntheses. Additional questions based on their responses, were
designed to support students’ reflection on macroscopic obser- used to engage students in reflecting on the macroscopic or the
vations, the respective submicroscopic level, and the goals of submicroscopic level (see Fig. 4).
each individual step of the synthesis (e.g. isolation or purifica- In our previous work, it became apparent that students
tion of a given compound). Generally, students concentrate on could take shortcuts in explaining a given phenomenon. Thus,
the experiment as a whole and pay little attention to the to ensure that we elicited the capabilities, we (a) prompted
individual steps (e.g. the purification steps). Since individual students to consider the submicroscopic level when they began
steps are important, we gave students explicit instructions to to describe the macroscopic level of the synthesis and
focus on these individual steps, collect their observations (b) prompted students to consider the macroscopic level when
during the synthesis, and reflect on the ongoing chemical they began to describe the submicroscopic level. In the pre-
processes of each synthesis step at the particle level. As a first interview, which took place after the second of six completed
part, students were asked to note the name of the synthesis and syntheses, students were asked to choose one synthesis and
describe the synthesis step they were working on currently. describe it step-by-step.
Second, students were asked to fill in four parts for each The decision to give the students the choice to choose which
synthesis step: synthesis and, consequently, which mechanism they would like
1. Goal. In the first part, students were instructed to describe to discuss in the interview was made consciously. Giving
the goal of this synthesis step and explain why they were students the opportunity to choose one of their two syntheses
working on this step. was intended to relieve their nervousness during the interview
2. Observation. After defining the goal of the synthesis step, situation and allowed them to choose a familiar content area.
students were prompted to describe and visualize their obser- The syntheses and related mechanisms are very similar. Almost
vations, before, during, and after this step. It should be noted every synthesis has certain preparatory steps, a main reaction,
that some observations did not always differ substantially. For and ongoing purification steps. Due to these similarities of the
example, if students mixed two colourless solutions for five respective syntheses, we considered it appropriate to allow the
hours and the solution remained colourless, they were not able students choose which synthesis they want to explain in the
to observe any visible changes. Furthermore, students were interview. To answer our research questions, we were more
instructed to draw a sketch of their observations before and interested in students’ sense-making approaches when they
after the synthesis step, if possible. explained a synthesis of their choice, than to test them all on

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice


Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Fig. 3 The scaffold used in the organic laboratory course for each synthesis step (translated from German).

the same synthesis. We assume that this process helps them The interview typically started with the sentence, ‘‘Please tell
feel at ease with the task, so that the students feel more me in detail what you have done in your organic synthesis and why
comfortable and are willing to share their thoughts. you have done the individual steps’’. After the interview, students

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

consult the first author or have a look at the provided sample


worksheet.
The second interview, after working with the scaffold, had
the same structure and prompts as the first one. In addition, it
included students’ evaluation and perceptions of the scaffold.
During the second interview students were not allowed to use
their scaffold worksheets but could choose again one of the two
Fig. 4 Study design. syntheses for the interview. Giving students the opportunity
to choose one of their two syntheses again, was intended to
relieve their nervousness and allow them to choose a familiar
were introduced to the scaffold and told how to use it during content area.
the next two syntheses (see Fig. 4). The collected data included transcripts of the two semi-
When filling out the worksheet following the interview, structured pre-and post-interviews with all students, and scans
students were told there were multiple possible answers, and of their completed scaffold worksheets and field notes follow-
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

that we were mainly interested in their imagination and ing the interviews. The interviews lasted on average 30 minutes.
conceptions of levels, particularly the particle level. To show
them how to use the scaffold in the laboratory, we demon- Data analysis
strated an exemplary solution to an organic synthesis that was To acquire deeper understanding of how students interacted
not part of the course. After introducing the scaffold, the with the scaffold, we conducted a qualitative data analysis. All
students used the scaffold for the next two syntheses. interviews were transcribed verbatim and analysed using the
During the lab activity, the first author played the role of a coding software MAXQDA. To evaluate whether the scaffold led
mediator. She interacted with the students, checked if they to changes in students’ approach and to answer our research
were comfortable with the instructional approach, answered questions, we analysed the data using the following steps:
questions, addressed any uncertainties and arranged the Step I: identifying the activated features of a mechanism. We
appointment for the second interview when students had used the coding scheme from our previous study (Keiner and
completed their scaffold assignments. During the lab activity, Graulich, 2020), which consists of eight categories, to code the
a question most frequently asked by students was how detailed mechanistic features students would need to activate in their
they should describe the individual parts of the scaffold and explanations. Table 1 provides an overview of the codes and a
how they should depict the processes on the particle level. The student example for each coding category. In this part of the
first author advised the students to describe the chemical analysis, we coded when and how often students used entities,
phenomena as detailed as possible and to choose which properties, organization, and activities at the macroscopic and
visualization (e.g. Lewis structures or particle drawings) they submicroscopic representational levels during the pre- and
want to use to represent the synthesis step. Overall, students post-interviews. The features were coded individually. If some-
felt comfortable working with the scaffold and could always one mentioned a macroscopic entity (e.g. water), it was coded

Table 1 Coding rubric for analysis step I: mechanistic features at the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels

Code Student examples Code description


EntityM ‘‘I still have contaminations like water and sulphuric Entities are described by using the name of the substance or naming a
acid in my flask.’’ solution of a substance (e.g. reaction educts or products), without
reference to the respective molecule or particle.
PropertyM ‘‘Ethanol is liquid like sulphuric acid. Salicylic acid is Properties are described by reference to an observable physical or
solid.’’ chemical property (e.g. the colour of a substance, the observable pH
value, the aggregate state).
ActivityM ‘‘Sodium chloride is formed, and we still have another Activities are described by referring to an observable physical or
chlorine which splits off.’’ chemical change of properties, with no reference to molecules or
particles (e.g. an ongoing phase separation).
OrganizationM ‘‘I could observe two phases; in the aqueous phase there Organization of entities is described by referring to the temporal
is water and sodium chloride, maybe sodium acetate. . .’’ (e.g. stand overnight) and spatial organization (e.g. observation of two
separate phases) of macroscopic entities.

EntityS ‘‘. . .CaCl2 which means, I have Ca2+ and two Cl ions.’’ Entities are described by using the name of the particle or referring to
the molecules of a substance (e.g. functional groups, ions, water
molecules).
PropertyS ‘‘. . .the partial positive charged carbon atom in acetic Properties are described by reference to an invisible physical or
acid.’’ chemical property (e.g. deprotonated entity, aggregate state).
ActivityS ‘‘The acetate molecule attacks the nucleophile.’’ Activities are described by referring to an invisible physical or chemical
transformation which results in a change of properties, with reference
to molecules or particles.
OrganizationS ‘‘. . .in the aqueous phase we have Na+ and Cl .’’ Organization of entities is described by referring to the temporal and
spatial organization (e.g. more H3O+ ions) of submicroscopic entities.

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

with the code macroscopic entity (EM). Those features that This process resulted in various combinations, such as from
students activated frequently or rarely may depend on the type the macroscopic (M) to the submicroscopic (S) level without a
of organic synthesis in question. After coding each feature, we prompt (M–S), from the macroscopic to the submicroscopic
counted the frequencies of the total number of coded features level after a prompt (M–P–S) or staying at the macroscopic level
and then calculated different percentages. The given percentages (MM). The individual sections of meaning were then coded with
in the result chapter (e.g. 75% macroscopic; 25% submicroscopic these three occurring combinations (MS; MM; MPS) between the
in the pre-interview) therefore refer to the individual features, macroscopic and submicroscopic representational level. In the
which means that 75% of the overall coded features (like entities, next step of analysis, we determined the distribution of the respec-
properties, activities and organization) are labelled as macro- tive transitions for each student in the pre- and post-interviews.
scopic ones and 25% of the coded features are submicroscopic. Therefore, we counted how often students used which approach to
The percentages of the coded mechanistic features overall explain the synthesis steps. For instance, we counted the amount
students in the pre- and post-interview (e.g. 57% EM; 20% PM. . .) of sections of meaning in which a student explained the chemical
are related to the total amount of all macroscopic and submicro- phenomena at the macroscopic level (sense-making approach I),
scopic respectively. in which the student transitioned after being prompted to the
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

The changes with regard to the activated mechanistic submicroscopic level (sense-making approach II), and in which the
features from the pre- to the post-interview were explored for student switched to the submicroscopic level without a prompt
statistical significance using a paired-sample t-test. (sense-making approach III). We, thus, generated a bar chart for
Step II: characterizing students by their attempt to make each student, showing the respective percentages of each approach
sense of an organic synthesis. To answer our second research in the pre- and post-interview (cf. Fig. 12). The changes in students’
question, we categorized students by their approach (with sense-making approaches from the pre- to the post-interview were
regard to the individual sections of meaning) to making sense explored for statistical significance using a paired-sample t-test.
of their chosen organic synthesis at the macroscopic and Step III: evaluating students’ individual work with the
submicroscopic representational levels, before and after scaffold. To explore a potential correlation between students’
working with the scaffold. We assumed that deliberately mak- sense-making approach on the sections of meaning and their
ing use of the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels when individual work with the scaffold, we created a rubric that
explaining a chosen synthesis (as students had done in the lab), categorizes students’ qualitative description of the reaction
was an indicator of proficiency, whereas a description limited process in the scaffold worksheet. For this purpose, we differ-
to the macroscopic level could indicate students’ difficulty with entiated between fully described (2 points), partially described
apprehending multiple levels. (1 point), and insufficiently described (0 points).
Therefore, we divided the interview transcripts into smaller Furthermore, we divided a synthesis into three main parts
content-related ‘‘sections of meaning’’ (e.g. the description of a (see Fig. 5). These were the preparatory steps, the main reaction
phase separation, the evolution of gas, or the main reaction step). step, and the work-up procedures (e.g. purification steps). For
The sections of meaning could be differentiated by an interview each of these three parts, we rated students’ description of the
prompt or a content-related change by the student. Such a verbal synthesis steps with the rubric by focusing on the completeness
section of meaning focuses on one aspect of the observed syn- of the descriptions and on the correct separation of the
thesis (e.g. a phase separation or a distillation process) and can macroscopic and submicroscopic levels. This rating indicates
occur at one or both representational levels. The subdivision into whether students made the correct observations of the respec-
smaller sections of meaning was useful because the students in tive step at the macroscopic level and described and visualized
this study did not consistently use just one approach to describe it at the particle level.
their synthesis. Depending on the synthesis step, they may have Unreadable or incomprehensible (insufficient) descriptions
reasoned on one of the two levels or changed the representational were awarded 0 points; partial descriptions of the individual
level in different ways. The subdivision into smaller sections of synthesis step, with correct differentiations between the macro-
meaning provided the opportunity to better compare changes scopic and submicroscopic levels and with few aspects missing
from the pre- to the post-interview (e.g. the explanation of a phase or incorrect, were awarded 1 point; full and correct descriptions
separation in both interviews). Consequently, we first separated of the synthesis step were awarded 2 points.
or divided each section of meaning in students’ interview To assess how students worked with the scaffold overall,
transcripts. The first part of analysis step II did not include any we summed up their points on both syntheses and calculated a
coding of the transcripts, but only the division of the interviews total score. The total score may differ depending on the
into content-related sections of meaning. described organic synthesis because not every synthesis has a
To better characterize students’ sense-making approaches, ‘‘preparatory step’’. Some organic syntheses start directly
we were interested in the type of transition used to explain their with the ‘‘main reaction step’’. We therefore calculated the
synthesis steps in such a section of meaning. Therefore, we percentage of available points achieved by each student. Based
categorized, for example, whether a transition occurred from on the attained percentage, we divided the students into three
one representational level to another and whether this shift was ‘‘groups’’, to indicate how students worked, in general, with the
caused by the interviewer’s prompt or was made freely and scaffold. Zero to 33% represented group I (mainly insufficient
deliberately by the student. description); 34% to 66% represented group II (mainly partial

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper


Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Fig. 5 Illustrative example of the rubric for use of the scaffold. In this case, the student obtained ten out of twelve points and therefore achieved 83% in
total, indicating that the synthesis steps were fully described in the scaffold worksheet.

description); and 67% to 100% represented group III (mainly post-interview (macroscopic features 73%, submicroscopic
full description). Fig. 5 gives an overview of this analysis. features 27%). In the post-interview, students included more
The first author (PhD candidate with a master’s degree in reasoning about the underlying chemical processes, as indi-
chemistry education) coded the entire data set from all rounds cated by an increase of the feature activity. Fig. 6 shows an
of coding and analysis. During the data analysis, the authors overview of the percentage of activated features in the pre- and
regularly discussed the coding scheme. Furthermore, constant post-interview. It is apparent that in both pre- and post-
discussions with the whole research group were conducted. interviews, entities were activated most frequently (pre: EM:
57%; ES: 69%; post: EM: 61%; ES: 69%), followed by properties
(pre: PM: 20%; PS: 14%; post: PM: 17%; PS: 14%), organization
Results and discussions (pre: OM: 15%; OS: 8%; post: OM: 14%; OS: 10%), and activities
(pre: AM: 8%; AS: 9%; post: AM: 8%; AS: 16%).
We analysed students’ interview data and written work and With regard to the activation of individual features at the
characterized different sense-making approaches in students’ representational levels, we did not observe any major differ-
sections of meaning and the related changes with regard to the ences except for two submicroscopic features—the entities and
scaffold worksheet. Overall, students demonstrated increased activities (see the dotted lines in Fig. 6). The submicroscopic
understanding of organic synthesis in their post-scaffold inter- activity was activated, on average, twice as often in the post-
views. To facilitate the interpretation and discussion of the results interview (16%) as in the pre-interview (8%). This increase is
in accordance with our research questions, we divided the results significant (p o 0.05) and has a large effect size (Cohen’s d =
into three parts. The first part describes students’ activation of 0.812). With the exception of two students, all students showed
mechanistic features while explaining their organic synthesis in an increase in this mechanistic feature in the post-interview.
the pre and post-scaffold interview. The second part illustrates The statistical analysis of the other features as well as the
students’ sense-making approach in the sections of meaning. The confidence intervals are presented in the appendix (cf. Table 3).
third part illustrates students’ work with the scaffold and the Consequently, they activated considerably fewer submicro-
related changes in their sense-making approach. scopic entities ( 9%) in the post-interview. One can interpret
To what extent did students’ activation of mechanistic
features (at the macroscopic and submicroscopic levels) change
after they worked with the scaffold during a synthesis?
All features of a mechanism were activated at least once by each
student during their explanation of the synthesis, but with varying
frequencies. Because naming a property, organization, or activity is
generally accompanied by a reference to an entity, it is unsurprising
that entity was the mechanistic feature used most at both levels.
Our analysis revealed that students activate, on average,
Fig. 6 Percentage of mechanistic features activated in students’ sense-
more macroscopic features than submicroscopic features. making approaches in the pre- (left) and post- (right) interview. The blue
Close to equal percentages were obtained in the pre-interview coloured elements indicate macroscopic features and the red coloured
(macroscopic features 75%, submicroscopic features 25%) and the submicroscopic features.

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

this finding as an increase in process-oriented thinking in the working with the scaffold, we observed small qualitative differ-
post-interviews. This is a promising finding as we found in one ences in their explanations (see Fig. 7).
of our previous studies (Keiner and Graulich, 2020) that stu- What kind of sense-making approaches (with regard to the
dents, when prompted to explain organic work-up procedures, representational levels) do students use while reasoning about an
tend to focus more on static entities and less on process- organic synthesis before and after working with the scaffold?
oriented features like the activity. Moreira et al. (2019) found To answer this question, we examined how students made
that students tend to explain chemical phenomena without sense of organic synthesis steps with regard to their transition
reference to the activity. They concluded that students’ expla- from the macroscopic level to the submicroscopic level, and
nations incorporate activities of one or more entities of the analysed whether their sense-making changed after working
system only at a higher level of sophistication, indicating a with the scaffold. Depending on how students explained the
change from a static to a more dynamic perspective of the reaction process in the section of meaning, different transitions
phenomenon. In our previous study (Keiner and Graulich, 2020) were observable (see Fig. 8). The sections of meaning fell into
we confirmed the observation that students often struggle to one of three categories: students who transitioned from the
describe the activities that give rise to entities or properties that macroscopic to the submicroscopic level independently, those
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

are responsible for earlier or subsequent activities. The following who did so after a prompt by the interviewer, and those who
example illustrates students’ use of the feature activity. remained at the macroscopic level even after a prompt. It is
important to note that none of the students began their
explanation of a phenomenon at the submicroscopic level.
The sections of meaning categories are as follows:
Sense-making approach I. The first approach that character-
ized a section of meaning was a student explaining chemical
synthesis macroscopically using macroscopic features. Even
after a prompt to consider the particle level, the explanation
remained at the macroscopic level and did not include sub-
microscopic features.
Sense-making approach II. This sense-making approach
characterized those sections in which students analysed a
synthesis step at the macroscopic level then, after a prompt
from the interviewer, transitioned to the submicroscopic repre-
sentational level and activated the submicroscopic features.
Fig. 7 Illustrative quotes from Darly as he thought through a nucleophilic Sense-making approach III. The third sense-making approach
attack in the pre- and post-interviews. characterized sections of meaning in which students described the
chemical phenomenon at the macroscopic level and transitioned,
without being prompted, to the submicroscopic level.
Specifically, we compared student Darly’s explanation from In the following section, we illustrate the sense-making
the pre- and post-interview in which he worked out the nucleo- approach with student examples. Fig. 9 shows student Carol’s
philic attack (the main reaction step) in two different organic
syntheses. In the pre-interview, Darly listed many properties
(e.g. ‘‘partially negative; partially positively charged; positively
charged’’) without reference to the formation (i.e. the respective
activities). In the post-interview, he once again mentioned the
comparable properties of entities (e.g. ‘‘negatively charged;
positively polarized C-atom; oxygen is electronegative’’). However,
in contrast to the pre-interview, he included more descriptions
about the formation of the properties (‘‘attracts the electrons’’)
and the activity of the entity (‘‘attacks as a nucleophile’’).
To summarize our findings for the first research question,
we noted that students activated all mechanistic features, but
with different frequencies. They most frequently mentioned
entities, followed by properties, organization, and, least fre-
quently, activities. The high percentage of activated entities in
both cases can be attributed to the fact that naming a property,
organization, or activity is generally accompanied by a refer-
ence to an entity. Therefore, it is not surprising that the most
used mechanistic feature at both levels was the entity. Although Fig. 8 Three sense-making approaches which characterize students’
students activated, predominantly, the same features after sections of meaning in explaining synthesis steps.

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

Fig. 9 Carol’s sense-making approach (I) to the aggregate states and the
dissolving process in the pre-interview.
Fig. 11 Darly’s sense-making approach (III) as he describes the separation
of two phases in the pre-interview.
sense-making approach for the dissolving process of chloroc-
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

tane with sodium acetate and methyltrioctylamine. First, he


described the macroscopic appearance (aggregate states) of the sections of meaning that correspond to sense-making approach II,
substances (‘‘liquid’’; ‘‘colourless’’; ‘‘solid’’). Carol made no students tend to consider chemical phenomena at the macroscopic
reference to the particle level of the different aggregate states level and transition to the submicroscopic level after a prompt.
(e.g. how the particles are arranged in the different aggregate The third sense-making approach that emerged from students’
states—closely packed or separated from each other). sections of meaning is illustrated by the example of student Darly
Carol was first explicitly prompted to explain his observation (see Fig. 11). He described a phase separation and immediately
(the dissolution process) at the particle level. However, he started to make sense of his observation at the particle level,
described his observations only at the macroscopic level (‘‘it without an explicit prompt. He then transitioned from the macro-
was relatively lumpy’’; ‘‘it mixed better’’), and did not mention scopic level (‘‘I observe two phases’’) to the submicroscopic level,
the dissolving process at the particle level as a way to activate activated submicroscopic mechanistic features, and explained his
submicroscopic features, even after being prompted (‘‘when you observation of organic synthesis at the particle level. Overall,
consider the particle level’’) on the meaning of the different sense-making approach I mostly characterized students’ explana-
aggregate states at the particle level. tions of their chosen organic synthesis in the pre- and post-interview
The following example of student Gabriel (Fig. 10) illustrates (see Table 2), followed by sense-making approach II. Sense-making
sense-making approach II. His sense-making approach dealt approach III was only present in a few sections of meaning. However,
with the different aggregate states of the molecules involved in the occurrences of sense-making approach III increased slightly
his organic synthesis. First, Gabriel named the different aggre- between the pre-scaffold and post-scaffold interviews—1% -
gate states (‘‘solid‘‘; ‘‘liquid’’) without further explanations. 9%—after students had worked with the scaffold.
After an interview prompt to consider the aggregate states at To what extent is a student’s individual work with the
the particle level, Gabriel transitioned to the submicroscopic scaffold related to a change in the sense-making approach?
level, activated submicroscopic features (‘‘particles’’), and To better understand how much students’ sense-making
explained the different aggregate states with reference to the approach in the section of meaning had changed, we rated
organization of the particles at the submicroscopic level students’ description of the reaction process in the scaffold
(e.g. ‘‘particles are more distant from each other’’; ‘‘particles are worksheet (cf. Appendix 2, Fig. 16), with reference to the rubric
closer together’’). (see Fig. 5), and correlated their descriptions to any changes
Compared to sense-making approach I, Gabriel started to between the pre- and post-scaffold interviews. Fig. 12 shows an
explain his observations at the particle level after the prompt. In overview of the changes in each of the three sense-making
groups, in combination with the ratings of students’ description
in the scaffold worksheet.
We evaluated the increase of the sense-making approaches
from the pre- to post-interview statistically. The data show a
statistically significant increase (p o 0.01) and a large effect
size (Cohen’s d = 0.877) for sense-making approach III. The

Table 2 Percentage of students’ sense-making approaches in the pre-


and post-interview

Approach I Approach II Approach III


Fig. 10 Gabriel’s sense-making approach (Scalco et al., 2018) to the Pre-interview 63% 36% 1%
aggregate states in the pre-interview. Post-interview 60% 31% 9%

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

comparison of sense-making approach I and II as well as the We noted that students who fully described the synthesis
confidence intervals are presented in the appendix (cf. Table 4). steps in the scaffold worksheet (green box) usually showed an
The eight students in group I (green box) described the increase in sense-making approach III in the post-scaffold
synthesis steps in the scaffold more or less fully. The twelve interview; this result is highlighted with red boxes (see
students in group II (yellow box) described the synthesis steps Fig. 12). This means that these students tended to describe
in the scaffold only partially. Only the two students in group III their chosen synthesis in the post-scaffold interview at the
(red box) described the steps insufficiently. Fig. 12 also shows macroscopic and submicroscopic levels without being explicitly
the percentage of sense-making approaches I-III in the prompted. Furthermore, we noticed a small increase in the
pre-scaffold interviews and the respective percentages in the number of students who explained the submicroscopic level after
post-scaffold interviews (visualized by different coloured bars, a prompt, whereas in the pre-scaffold interview they often
Fig. 12). We observed a relationship between the completeness remained at the macroscopic level even after being prompted.
of the description in the scaffold worksheet and the sense- Students who partially described the synthesis steps in the scaffold
making approaches in the pre- and post-scaffold interviews. (yellow box) showed few changes in their sense-making approach.
During the pre-scaffold interviews, only one student was explain-
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

ing a phenomenon using sense-making approach III. By contrast,


during the post-scaffold interviews, at least three students used
sense-making approach III. Further, some students made use of
sense-making approach II, which indicates that they explained the
synthesis steps at the submicroscopic level after being prompted.
Only two students insufficiently described the synthesis
steps in the scaffold (red box), and we observed an increased
use of sense-making approach III by one of them.
Based on the use of the scaffold (Fig. 2), students who
described the reaction processes in detail tended to switch
more often to the submicroscopic level, without prompts. Some
of these students explained their syntheses more reflective and
could better reactivate their knowledge from the work with the
scaffold. Students who struggled to fully explain the reaction
processes tended to switch less often to the submicroscopic
level. This indicates that the scaffold may encourage students
to consider the submicroscopic level.
Maggie’s case illustrates these changes for group 1 in the
post-scaffold interview. Fig. 13 and 15 show excerpts from
Maggie’s interview transcripts, and Fig. 14 shows parts of her
scaffold worksheet.
Maggie fully described the synthesis steps in the scaffold
worksheet but only started to use sense-making approach III
in the post-scaffold interview (see Fig. 12). During her pre-
interview, Maggie reasoned about the separation of two phases
(synthesis—nucleophilic substitution of octyl acetate). She was
then prompted by the interviewer to explain the formation of

Fig. 12 Comparison of the percentage of sense-making approaches in


the pre- and post-scaffold interviews of each student. Students are
organized according to their descriptions in the scaffold—full (green), Fig. 13 Maggie’s sense-making approach (I) in the pre-scaffold interview
partial (yellow), and insufficient (red). where she reasoned over the formation of two phases.

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

Fig. 15 Maggie’s sense-making approach in the post-scaffold interview in


Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

which she reflected about the formation of two phases.

submicroscopic features and described a submicroscopic pro-


perty (‘‘OH group is polar’’) as well as the formation of that
Fig. 14 An excerpt from Maggie’s scaffold worksheet in which she property (‘‘because we have a delta plus on the hydrogen atom and
describes the formation of two phases. Sketches and her description are
a delta minus on the oxygen atom’’).
translated from German into English.
Furthermore, she described properties and activities of the
CC-bonds (‘‘usually non-polar’’; ‘‘cannot form hydrogen bonds’’) and
the two phases in more detail but her thought processes concluded by describing the non-existent interactions between the
remained at the macroscopic level. entities and the resulting formation of two phases. Thus, she was
In her explanation Maggie named macroscopic properties able explain the activities and properties that were missing in the
(‘‘hydrophobic and hydrophilic’’) and said that the two properties pre-scaffold interview. Maggie’s case is exemplary because it shows
are important for the separation of two phases ‘‘because of the the connection between the scaffold worksheet and her thinking
characteristics of miscibility’’. She did not explain what hydro- processes during the post-scaffold interview.
phobic or hydrophilic meant at the particle level. Furthermore, We observed various incidences of drawings or a description in
she did not explain which functional groups were responsible the scaffold sheet seeming to encourage students to make more
for the formation of two phases at the submicroscopic level references to the submicroscopic level in the post-scaffold interview.
or how the functional groups relate to the formation of two As the work-up procedures are not normally explained in the
phases. In her scaffold worksheet Maggie mentioned these laboratory report at the end of each synthesis (which Maggie’s case
missing properties and activities at the submicroscopic level demonstrates), we can assume that students’ shift towards the
(synthesis—nucleophilic substitution of tert-butyl chloride). submicroscopic level was related to students’ work with the scaffold.
She also described the macroscopic properties (‘‘colourless’’, With regard to the changes effected by students’ use of the
‘‘clear’’, ‘‘liquid’’, ‘‘observation of two phases after shaking’’). With scaffold, students who described their reaction processes in
reference to the submicroscopic level, she alluded to the process of detail switched more frequently to the submicroscopic level
the formation of two phases by mentioning that the ‘‘OH group is and of their own volition.
protonated’’ (PS) and therefore ‘‘splits off’’ (AS). Next she described However, based on the exploratory and qualitative nature of the
‘‘the attack’’ (AS) of the chloride on the ‘‘positively charged’’ (PS) study, further research is needed to determine if these positive
‘‘C-atom’’ (ES). Maggie then considered the polarities of the resulting observations are replicable and how best to support those students
entities and the formation of the two phases. She said that ‘‘butyl who filled out the scaffold worksheet insufficiently and thus did not
chloride is largely non-polar and therefore not soluble in polar profit as much from the scaffold as other students.
mixtures’’. Additionally, she visualized her description in a drawing
(see Fig. 14). She drew the partial positive and negative charges (see Conclusions and implications
Maggie’s sketch is on the right side of Fig. 14) and tagged the polar
and non-polar parts of the molecules. Successfully making sense of organic synthesis in the laboratory
In her sketches of the particle level she used both symbolic requires the ability to explain observations using multiple mecha-
representations (e.g. Lewis structures) and representations nistic features at the macroscopic and submicroscopic representa-
of particles (bubbles) to represent her thoughts. During the tional levels and to connect these levels in a meaningful way.
post-scaffold interview, Maggie again described the formation We consciously do not propose ‘‘one’’ correct approach or a
of two phases (synthesis—nucleophilic substitution of specific starting point to explain a chemical phenomenon in a
tert-butyl chloride). The following quotes are excerpts from meaningful way. It is, however, important to be aware of the
Maggie’s post-scaffold interview during which she activated coexistence and the role of the two representational levels to fully

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

account for a chemical phenomenon. The order of use, i.e. whether the transition between the macroscopic and submicroscopic
a submicroscopic explanation is inferred from a macroscopic representational levels:
phenomenon or vice versa is secondary and depends on the context (1) Sense-making approach I (i.e. macroscopic–prompt–
and the question asked. macroscopic); (2) sense-making approach II (i.e. macroscopic–
If a chemical phenomenon shows no obvious observations prompt–submicroscopic); (3) sense-making approach III
(e.g. a colourless solution reacts with a colourless solution), it is (i.e. macroscopic–submicroscopic, without a prompt).
difficult to make sense of the observation. However, guiding We observed that students who fully described their synthesis
students to attend to macroscopic changes, even if they are steps in the scaffold exhibited an improvement in their sense-
small or hardly visible is an essential skill in the inquiry making approach between the pre- and post-scaffold interviews:
process. Relating observable changes to the underlying mole- they transitioned more often, without prompts, from the macro-
cular processes is as important as being aware, that a reaction scopic to the submicroscopic representational level (sense-making
may take place even when there is nothing to observe. approach III). Although these observations are promising, it is
If students argue and explain only at one of the two apparent from the scaffold worksheets and interview transcripts
representational levels and, for example, remain completely that some students struggled to describe and visualize their
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

on the macroscopic level, they are missing the other half of the observations at the particle level, even when explicitly prompted
story. The same issue arises when students neglect macroscopic to think about the properties and activities of the entities while
observations and only consider the submicroscopic level in working with the scaffold. In this study, students often referred to
their explanations. Thus, students need to be encouraged to the final product in their scaffold worksheet but did not explain
consciously reflect on a chemical process macroscopically and how to get there (e.g. when they described a property, they did not
submicroscopically (Taber, 2013). They also have to be able to mention how it is formed).
connect both representational levels in a meaningful way If a mental model of how particles interact is missing in
(Anderson, 1978; Johnstone, 1991; Gabel et al., 1992; Treagust students’ reasoning processes, it is difficult for them to make a
et al., 2003; Gilbert and Treagust, 2009a, 2009b; Krist et al., connection between the macroscopic and submicroscopic
2018; Keiner and Graulich, 2020). These abilities are addressed levels. Such mental models do not develop naturally in students
by the structure of the scaffold. (Deratzou, 2006). Thus, it is important to provide students with
To reprise, the central goals of this study were as follows: the opportunity to constantly reflect on and connect these
(1) Analyse to what extent students are able to activate representational levels. The scaffold sought to elicit this ability
mechanistic features (at the macroscopic and submicroscopic in students and it seems that reflective work with the scaffold
levels) during pre- and post-scaffold interviews. had a positive effect on students’ ability to transition between
(2) Characterize how students make sense of synthesis steps. the macroscopic and submicroscopic representational levels.
(3) Determine if there is a relationship between students’ This aspect of encouraging students to first think about how
sense-making approaches and their use of the scaffold. they can depict entities and chemical phenomena at the
Our analysis revealed that students use all mechanistic individual representational levels and how to combine them
features at both representational levels to make sense of afterwards in a meaningful way has as well been illustrated by
organic synthesis. In the pre- and post-scaffold interviews, the study from Thomas (2017). The worksheet used in this
students were able to activate entities and properties at both study is to some extend similar to our scaffold and directed
representational levels but used fewer activities and less orga- students’ metacognition in an explicit way, i.e. to attend to the
nization. Analysing the use of these features in detail revealed separate nature of each of the representational levels and the
that the causal link between the features (particularly during relationships between them.
the pre-scaffold interview) was often missing and students’ Conscious reflection on the macroscopic and submicro-
explanations can be considered as static. This finding is in scopic representational levels does not happen automatically
accordance with previous research (Grove et al., 2012; Caspari in the laboratory. This requires active engagement by students
et al., 2018; Moreira et al., 2018; Keiner and Graulich, 2020) in in the process such as scaffolding. For example, students who
which students’ explanations tend to include initial starting fully described their syntheses in the scaffold showed the most
material and products but little description of the underlying positive changes in their approach.
reaction. During the post-scaffold interview, after students had The structure of the scaffold worksheet could be applied to all
completed two organic syntheses using the scaffold, we observed occurring chemical processes where it is important that students’
a small increase in the activation of process-related mechanistic observations are connected at the particle level and that students
features as students described activities more frequently. are able to transition between representational levels. For
Thus, we can assume that students’ work with the scaffold example, inorganic chemistry students could use the scaffold
encouraged them to reflect more deeply on the particle level of while analysing an unknown compound (analytical chemistry);
their synthesis steps. However, further research is needed to or biology students could use the scaffold while testing various
elucidate the effect of the scaffold using quantitative methods fertilizers in relation to plant growth and visual changes. This type
and a larger sample. of scaffolding can be smoothly integrated into existing teaching
Our analysis also revealed that three main sense-making practices to expand upon what students already do and to help
approaches characterize students’ explanations with regard to them connect their observations with the particle level.

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

Limitations Table 4 Statistical analysis of the sense-making approaches (SMA) from


the pre- to the post-interview. **p o 0.01
The conclusions drawn here should be considered with caution
t-Value Cohen’s d 95% CI
given some of the inherent limitations of this study. First, this
was a qualitative study with volunteering students from a class SMA I 0.58 0.11 [ 0.70; 0.48]
SMA II 0.93 0.26 [ 0.85; 0.34]
of student teachers in an organic chemistry laboratory course. SMA III 2.92** 0.88 [0.25; 1.49]
This student population may be driven by different motivations
and interests with regard to chemistry than students majoring
in chemistry. The implication drawn from this qualitative effect. Due to the limited number of students in this study, it
analysis are thus limited to this group. Second, primarily should be mentioned, that the confidence interval is very large and
qualitative methods were used to describe the quality of students’ statistical statements are only possible to a limited extent.
explanations in the pre- and post-scaffold interviews. The quanti- Furthermore, the sense-making approaches were explored
tative analysis should be interpreted with caution, as the sample for statistical significance using a paired-sample t-test. Table 4
is too small for a conclusive statistical statement. Due to the small shows the comparison of the sense-making approaches from
number of students in the cohort, the confidence interval is large. the pre- to the post-interview. The data show that students used
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Nevertheless, the large effect size for the increase of sense-making sense-making approach III significantly (p = 0.008) more often
approach III indicates that the work with the scaffold seems to than sense-making approaches I or II. This increase is coupled
influence students’ reasoning. In order to generalize the results with a large effect size (Cohen’s d = 0.88). Changes in sense-
and to investigate whether this effect is sustainable or holds true making approach I and II are not statistically significant and
for larger classes, further studies are necessary. show no effect size.
Third, students were asked to explain their synthesis in an
individual interview setting that was quite different from the
environment in the laboratory. Students appeared to feel comforta- Appendix 2. Students’ evaluation of
ble with the research team and the interview setting, which may have
prompted them to pay closer attention to the work with the scaffold
the scaffold
than they would have paid in a laboratory situation. Fig. 16 shows the percentages achieved by students in the scaffold
and the points achieved in the separate categories—preparatory
Conflicts of interest steps, main reaction step, and work-up procedures. As not every

There is no conflict of interest to declare.

Appendix 1. Statistical analysis


of the comparison of the pre- and
post-interview data
The increase of the activated mechanistic features on the
respective representational level was explored for statistical
significance using a paired-sample t-test. The results are presented
in Table 3. Students activated significantly more macroscopic
entities and submicroscopic activities. The increase of the submi-
croscopic activity is coupled with a large effect size (Cohen’s
d = 0.81), whereas the macroscopic entity (Cohen’s d = 0.37) and
the submicroscopic organization (Cohen’s d = 0.41) show a small

Table 3 Statistical analysis of the activated features (E = entity; P =


property; O = organization; A = activity; M = macroscopic; S = submicro-
scopic) from the pre- to the post-interview. *p o 0.05; ***p o 0.001

t-Value Cohen’s d 95% CI


EM 2.35* 0.37 [ 0.23; 0.97]
PM 0.66 0.14 [ 0.73; 0.45]
Fig. 16 Evaluation of students’ descriptions in the scaffold worksheet
OM 0.14 0.03 [ 0.56; 0.62]
AM 0.48 0.12 [ 0.47; 0.71] divided into the three main categories (preparatory steps, reaction steps,
ES 0.20 0.05 [ 0.54; 0.64] work-up procedure), their achieved points (2 = fully described; 1 = partially
PS 1.21 0.24 [ 0.35; 0.84] described; 0 = insufficiently described) in each of these categories, and the
OS 1.34 0.41 [ 0.19; 0.99] resulting overall percentage. The colours highlight the groups as follows:
AS 4.61*** 0.81 [0.19; 1.42] green = fully; yellow = partially; red = insufficiently.

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

synthesis has a preparatory step, some fields are left blank. The Chemistry: Starting It in General Chemistry, J. Chem. Educ.,
last column indicates the overall percentage of available points 96, 213–226.
attained. Crandell O. M., Lockhart M. A. and Cooper M. M., (2020),
Arrows on the Page Are Not a Good Gauge: Evidence for
the Importance of Causal Mechanistic Explanations about
Acknowledgements Nucleophilic Substitution in Organic Chemistry, J. Chem.
Educ., 97, 313–327.
This publication represents part of the first author’s doctoral Czysz K., Schroeder L. and Clark G. A., (2020), Making Acids
(Dr rer. nat.) thesis at the Faculty of Biology and Chemistry, and Bases MORE Basic: Supporting Students’ Conceptuali-
Justus-Liebig-University Giessen, Germany. We thank all students zation of Acid Base Chemistry through a Laboratory Exer-
who participated in the study. We also thank the Graulich cise That Connects Molecular-Level Representations to
research group for fruitful discussions about the research and Symbolic Representations and Experimentally Derived Evi-
Professor Richard Göttlich for his support. dence, J. Chem. Educ., 97, 484–489.
Davis E. A., (2000), Scaffolding students’ knowledge integration:
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

prompts for reflection in KIE, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 22, 819–837.


References Deratzou S., (2006), A qualitative inquiry into the effects
of visualization on high school chemistry students’ learning
Anderson G. W., (1978), The Playfair Collection and the teaching process of molecular structure, Doctoral dissertation, Drexel
of chemistry at the University of Edinburgh, BRILL, pp. University.
1713–1858. Domin D. S., (1999), A review of laboratory instruction styles,
Assadieskandar A., Rezende Miranda R. and Broyer R. M., J. Chem. Educ., 76, 543–547.
(2020), Visually Tracking Acid–Base Extractions Using Farrell J. J., Moog R. S. and Spencer J. N., (1999), A guided-
Colorful Compounds, J. Chem. Educ., 97, 1402–1405. inquiry general chemistry course, J. Chem. Educ., 76, 570.
Becker N., Noyes K. and Cooper M., (2016), Characterizing Faulconer E. K., Griffith J. C., Wood B. L., Acharyya S. and
Students’ Mechanistic Reasoning about London Dispersion Roberts D. L., (2018), A comparison of online and traditional
Forces, J. Chem. Educ., 93, 1713–1724. chemistry lecture and lab, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 19, 392–397.
Belland B. R., Glazewski K. D. and Richardson J. C., (2011), Ferrell J. B., Campbell J. P. and McCarthy D. R., (2019),
Problem-based learning and argumentation: testing a scaf- Chemical Exploration with Virtual Reality in Organic Teach-
folding framework to support middle school students’ creation ing Laboratories, J. Chem. Educ., 96, 1961–1966.
of evidence-based arguments, Instru. Sci., 39, 667–694. Fitzgerald M. S. and Palincsar A. S., (2019), Teaching practices
Ben-Zvi R., Eylon B. and Silberstein J., (1986), Is an Atom of that support student sensemaking across grades and dis-
Copper Malleable? J. Chem. Educ., 63, 64. ciplines: a conceptual review, Rev. Res. Educ., 43, 227–248.
Braaten M. and Windschitl M., (2011), Working toward a Gabel D., (1999), Improving teaching and learning through
stronger conceptualization of scientific explanation for chemistry education research: a look to the future, J. Chem.
science education, Sci. Educ., 95, 639–669. Educ., 76, 548–554.
Bretz S. L., (2019), Evidence for the Importance of Laboratory Gabel D. L. and Bunce D. M., (1984), Research on problem
Courses, J. Chem. Educ., 96, 193–195. solving: chemistry. Handbook of research on science teaching
Burke K., Greenbowe T. J. and Hand B. M., (2006), Implementing and learning, New York: Macmillan, vol. 11, pp. 301–326.
the science writing heuristic in the chemistry laboratory, Gabel D., Briner D. and Haines D., (1992), Modelling with
J. Chem. Educ., 83, 1032. magnets: a unified approach to chemistry problem solving,
Caspari I., Weinrich M. L., Graulich N. and Sevian H., (2017), Sci. Teach., 59, 58–63.
This mechanistic step is ‘‘productive’’: organic chemistry Gallardo-Williams M., Morsch L. A., Paye C. and Seery M. K.,
students’ backward-oriented reasoning, Chem. Educ. Res. (2020), Student-generated video in chemistry education,
Pract., 19, 42–59. Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 21, 488–495.
Caspari I., Graulich N. and Kranz D., (2018), Resolving the Galloway K. R. and Bretz S. L., (2015), Development of an
complexity of organic chemistry students’ reasoning through Assessment Tool To Measure Students’ Meaningful Learning
the lens of a mechanistic framework, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., in the Undergraduate Chemistry Laboratory, J. Chem. Educ.,
19, 1117–1141. 92, 1149–1158.
Cheng M. M. and Gilbert J. K., (2017), Modelling students’ Ge X. and Land S. M., (2003), Scaffolding students’ problem-solving
visualisation of chemical reaction, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 39, processes in an ill-structured task using question prompts and
1173–1193. peer interactions, Educ. Technol. Res. Dev., 51, 21–38.
Collison C. G., Cody J. and Stanford C., (2012), An SN1–SN2 Gilbert J. K. and Treagust D. F., (2009a), Multiple representations
Lesson in an Organic Chemistry Lab Using a Studio-Based in chemical education, Dordrecht: Springer, pp. 1–8.
Approach, J. Chem. Educ., 89, 750–754. Gilbert J. K. and Treagust D. F., (2009b), Multiple representations
Crandell O. M., Kouyoumdjian H., Underwood S. M. and in chemical education, ed. Gilbert J. K. and Treagust D. F.,
Cooper M. M., (2019), Reasoning about Reactions in Organic Dordrecht: Springer, vol. 4, pp. 333–350.

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020
View Article Online

Chemistry Education Research and Practice Paper

Gkitzia V., Salta K. and Tzougraki C., (2019), Students’ Compe- Kozma R. B. and Russell J., (1997), Multimedia and under-
tence in Translating Between Different Types of Chemical standing: expert and novice responses to different represen-
Representations, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 21, 307–330. tations of chemical phenomena, J. Res. Sci. Teach., 34,
Greenbowe T. J., Poock J. R., Burke K. and Hand B. M., (2007), 949–968.
Using the science writing heuristic in the general chemistry Krist C., Schwarz C. V. and Reiser B. J., (2018), Identifying
laboratory to improve students’ academic performance, Essential Epistemic Heuristics for Guiding Mechanistic
J. Chem. Educ., 84, 1371. Reasoning in Science Learning, J. Learn. Sci., 28, 160–205.
Griffiths A. K. and Preston K. R., (1992), Grade-12 students’ Lawson A. E., (1989), A Theory of Instruction: Using the Learn-
misconceptions relating to fundamental characteristics of ing Cycle To Teach Science Concepts and Thinking Skills.
atoms and molecules, J. Res. Sci. Teach., 29, 611–628. NARST Monograph, Number One, 1989.
Grotzer T. A., (2003), Learning to understand the forms of Lawson A. E., (2001), Using the learning cycle to teach biology
causality implicit in scientifically accepted explanations, concepts and reasoning patterns, J. Biol. Educ., 35, 165–169.
Stud. Sci. Educ., 39, 74. Lipton M. A., (2020), Reorganization of the Organic Chemistry
Grove N. P., Cooper M. M. and Cox E. L., (2012), Does mecha- Curriculum to Improve Student Outcomes, J. Chem. Educ.,
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

nistic thinking improve student success in organic chemis- 97, 960–964.


try? J. Chem. Educ., 89, 850–853. Machamer P., Darden L. and Craver C. F., (2000), Thinking
Hand B., Wallace C. W. and Yang E. M., (2004), Using a Science about mechanisms, Philos. Sci., 67, 1–25.
Writing Heuristic to enhance learning outcomes from Molvinger K., Ayral R.-M. and Filhol J.-S., (2020), Integrating
laboratory activities in seventh-grade science: quantitative Lecture and Laboratory Work for a Materials Chemistry
and qualitative aspects, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 26, 131–149. Course to Engage and Motivate Students through Highly
Hempel C. G. and Oppenheim P., (1948), Studies in the logic of Visual and Intriguing Syntheses, J. Chem. Educ., 97, 866–872.
explanation, Philos. Sci., 15, 135–175. Moreira P., Marzabal A. and Talanquer V., (2018), Using a
Hennah N. and Seery M. K., (2017), Using digital badges for mechanistic framework to characterise chemistry students’
developing high school chemistry laboratory skills, J. Chem. reasoning in written explanations, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.,
Educ., 94, 844–848. 20, 120–131.
Hofstein A. and Lunetta V. N., (1982), The Role of the Labora- Moreira P., Marzabal A. and Talanquer V., (2019), Investigating
tory in Science Teaching: Neglected Aspects of Research, the effect of teacher mediation on student expressed reason-
Rev. Educ. Res., 52, 201–217. ing, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 20(3), 606–617.
Johnstone A. H., (1982), Macro- and microchemistry, Sch. Sci. Pölloth B., Schwarzer S. and Zipse H., (2020), Student Individual-
Rev., 64, 377–379. ity Impacts Use and Benefits of an Online Video Library
Johnstone A. H., (1991), Why is science difficult to learn? Things for the Organic Chemistry Laboratory, J. Chem. Educ., 97,
are seldom what they seem., J. Comput. Assis. Learn., 7, 75–83. 328–337.
Johnstone A. H., (1993), The Development of Chemistry Teach- Puntambekar S. and Hubscher R., (2005), Tools for scaffolding
ing: a changing response to changing demand, J. Chem. students in a complex learning environment: What have we
Educ., 70, 701–705. gained and what have we missed? Educ. Psychol., 40, 1–12.
Johnstone A. H., (2000), Teaching of chemistry- logical oder Reid N. and Shah I., (2007), The role of laboratory work in
psychological? Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 1, 9–15. university chemistry, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 8, 172–185.
Kang H., Thompson J. and Windschitl M., (2014), Creating Reiser B. J., (2004), Scaffolding complex learning: the mechanisms
opportunities for students to show what they know: the role of structuring and problematizing student work, J. Learn. Sci.,
of scaffolding in assessment tasks, Sci. Educ., 98, 674–704. 13, 273–304.
Kararo A. T., Colvin R. A., Cooper M. M. and Underwood S. M., Rottman B. M. and Keil F. C., (2011), What matters in scientific
(2019), Predictions and constructing explanations: an inves- explanations: effects of elaboration and content, Int. J. Cog.
tigation into introductory chemistry students’ understand- Sci., 121, 324–337.
ing of structure–property relationships, Chem. Educ. Res. Russ R. S., Coffey J. E., Hammer D. and Hutchison P., (2008a),
Pract., 20, 316–328. Making Classroom Assessment More Accountable to
Keiner L. and Graulich N., (2020), Transitions between representa- Scientific Reasoning: A Case for Attending to Mechanistic
tional levels: characterization of organic chemistry students’ Thinking, Sci. Educ., 93, 875–891.
mechanistic features when reasoning about laboratory Russ R. S., Scherr R. E., Hammer D. and Mikeska J., (2008b),
work-up procedures, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 21, 469–482. Recognizing Mechanistic Reasoning in Student Scientific
Keys C. W., Hand B., Prain V. and Collins S., (1999), Using the Inquiry: A Framework for Discourse Analysis Developed
science writing heuristic as a tool for learning from laboratory From Philosophy of Science, Sci. Educ., 92, 499–525.
investigations in secondary science, J. Res. Sci. Teach., 36, Russell J. W., Kozma R. B., Jones T., Wykoff J., Marx N. and
1065–1084. Davis J., (1997), Use of simultaneous-synchronized macro-
Kirschner P. A., (2002), Cognitive load theory: implications of scopic, microscopic, and symbolic representations to
cognitive load theory on the design of learning, Learning and enhance the teaching and learning of chemical concepts,
Instruction, 12, 1–10. J. Chem. Educ., 74, 330–334.

This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020 Chem. Educ. Res. Pract.
View Article Online

Paper Chemistry Education Research and Practice

Salmon W. C., (1984), Scientific explanation and the causal comprehension and attitudes of undergraduate chemistry
structure of the world, Princeton: Princeton University Press. students, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 9, 169–181.
Scalco K. C., Talanquer V., Kiill K. B. and Cordeiro M. R., (2018), Sweller J., Van Merrienboer J. J. and Paas F. G., (1998), Cognitive
Making Sense of Phenomena from Sequential Images versus architecture and instructional design, Educ. Psychol. Rev., 10,
Illustrated Text, J. Chem. Educ., 95, 347–354. 251–296.
Schmidt-McCormack J. A., Muniz M. N., Keuter E. C., Shaw S. K. Taber K. S., (2002), Chemical misconceptions: prevention, diagnosis
and Cole R. S., (2017), Design and implementation of and cure, London: Royal Society of Chemistry.
instructional videos for upper-division undergraduate Taber K. S., (2013), Revisiting the chemistry triplet: drawing
laboratory courses, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 18, 749–762. upon the nature of chemical knowledge and the psychology
Schroeder J. D. and Greenbowe T. J., (2008), Implementing of learning to inform chemistry education, Chem. Educ. Res.
POGIL in the lecture and the Science Writing Heuristic in the Pract., 14, 156–168.
laboratory—student perceptions and performance in under- Talanquer V., (2010), Exploring Dominant Types of Explana-
graduate organic chemistry, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 9, 149–156. tions Built by General Chemistry Students, Int. J. Sci. Educ.,
Seel N. M., (2011), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, 32, 2393–2412.
Published on 02 October 2020. Downloaded on 10/20/2020 9:45:58 PM.

Springer Science & Business Media. Talanquer V., (2018), Education Research and Practice in Asia-
Seery M. K., Agustian H. Y., Doidge E. D., Kucharski M. M., Pacific and Beyond, Singapore: Springer, pp. 39–52.
O’Connor H. M. and Price A., (2017), Developing laboratory Thomas G. P., (2017), ‘Triangulation:’an expression for
skills by incorporating peer-review and digital badges, Chem. stimulating metacognitive reflection regarding the use of
Educ. Res. Pract., 18, 403–419. ‘triplet’representations for chemistry learning, Chem. Educ.
Seery M. K., Agustian H. Y. and Zhang X., (2018), A Framework Res. Pract., 18, 533–548.
for Learning in the Chemistry Laboratory, Isr. J. Chem., 59, Treagust D. F., Chittleborough G. and Mamiala T., (2003), The
546–553. role of submicroscopic and symbolic representations in
Sevian H. and Talanquer V., (2014), Rethinking chemistry: a chemical explanations, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 25, 1353–1368.
learning progression on chemical thinking, Chem. Educ. Res. Vishnumolakala V. R., Southam D. C., Treagust D. F., Mocerino
Pract., 15, 10–23. M. and Qureshi S., (2017), Students’ attitudes, self-efficacy
Spencer J. N., (1999), New directions in teaching chemistry: a and experiences in a modified process-oriented guided
philosophical and pedagogical basis, J. Chem. Educ., 76, 566. inquiry learning undergraduate chemistry classroom, Chem.
Stegall S. L., Grushow A., Whitnell R. and Hunnicutt S. S., Educ. Res. Pract., 18, 340–352.
(2016), Evaluating the effectiveness of POGIL-PCL workshops, Walker J. P., Van Duzor A. G. and Lower M. A., (2019), Facil-
Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 17, 407–416. itating Argumentation in the Laboratory: The Challenges of
Stephenson N. S. and Sadler-McKnight N. P., (2016), Developing Claim Change and Justification by Theory, J. Chem. Educ.,
critical thinking skills using the Science Writing Heuristic in 96, 435–444.
the chemistry laboratory, Chem. Educ. Res. Pract., 17, 72–79. Wood D., Bruner J. S. and Ross G., (1976), The role of tutoring
Sumfleth E. and Nakoinz S., (2019), Chemie verstehen– in problem solving, J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry, 17, 89–100.
beobachtbare makroskopische Phänomene auf submikros- Wu N., Kubo T., Hall A. O., Zurcher D. M., Phadke S., Wallace
kopischer Ebene modellbasiert interpretieren [Understand R. L. and McNeil A. J., (2020), Adapting Meaningful Learning
chemistry - interpret observable macroscopic phenomena Strategies to Teach Liquid Liquid Extractions, J. Chem.
at the submicroscopic level based on models], Z. Didakt. Educ., 97, 80–86.
Naturwiss., 25, 231–243. Yeo J. and Gilbert J. K., (2014), Constructing a scientific
Supasorn S., Suits J. P., Jones L. L. and Vibuljan S., (2008), explanation—A narrative account, Int. J. Sci. Educ., 36,
Impact of a pre-laboratory organic-extraction simulation on 1902–1935.

Chem. Educ. Res. Pract. This journal is © The Royal Society of Chemistry 2020

You might also like