You are on page 1of 11

Asia-Pacific Edu Res

DOI 10.1007/s40299-014-0195-0

REGULAR ARTICLE

L2 Motivational Self System, Attitudes, and Affect as Predictors


of L2 WTC: An Imagined Community Perspective
Jian-E Peng

Ó De La Salle University 2014

Abstract This study explored the interrelationships Introduction


between the three components of second language (L2)
motivational self system (i.e., ideal L2 self, ought-to L2 The importance of talking in order to learn (MacIntyre et al.
self, and L2 learning experience), international posture, L2 2003) has been widely accepted in the field of second lan-
anxiety, and willingness to communicate (WTC) using guage acquisition (SLA). However, it is not unusual to find
structural equation modeling. Questionnaire data were learners unwilling to talk or communicate using the second
collected from 1,013 university students in China. The language (L2) despite their recognition of the need to do so.
results showed that ideal L2 self was predicted by learning The construct of willingness to communicate in an L2 (L2
experience, ought-to L2 self, and international posture. WTC) has been accorded increasing research attention in
Ideal L2 self and ought-to L2 self exerted, respectively, a recent years. L2 WTC refers to ‘‘a readiness to enter into
negative and positive effect on L2 anxiety. L2 WTC was discourse at a particular time with a specific person or per-
found to encompass two distinct factors: WTC inside and sons, using a L2’’ (MacIntyre et al. 1998, p. 547). It is theo-
outside the classroom. The former was predicted by L2 rized to energize L2 communication actions and thereby
anxiety, learning experience, and international posture, facilitate language learning. Chinese students have often been
whereas the latter had international posture as its only observed to be incompetent in or even shun from English
direct predictor. This model marks the first step toward communication, which cannot satisfy the pressing demand for
drawing on L2 motivational self system to account for competent English users raised by China’s rapid growth
foreign language learners’ communication intention from against the backdrop of globalization. It is of utter importance
the perspective of an imagined global community. to research students’ motivational aspirations and in partic-
ular L2 WTC in this context for purposes of promoting their
Keywords L2 motivational self system  L2 communication and communicative proficiency.
International posture  L2 anxiety  Many early studies have shown that L2 WTC is intri-
Willingness to communicate in an L2  cately related with motivation to learn the L2 and attitudes
An imagined global community known as integrativeness (Gardner 1985) in Canadian
bilingual ethnic contexts (Clément et al. 2003; MacIntyre
and Charos 1996). Integrativeness reflects an integrative
orientation toward learning the L2 with a desire to interact
or even identify with the L2 group. In contexts where
English is learned as a foreign language (EFL), Yashima
(2002) proposed the concept of ‘‘international posture’’ to
capture learners’ attitudes toward what English symbolizes,
J.-E. Peng (&) or what is termed as an imagined community (Norton
Department of Foreign Languages and Literature, College of
2001). This appears to be a promising perspective to
Liberal Arts, Shantou University, 243 Da Xue Road, Shantou
515063, Guangdong Province, China account for EFL learners’ language learning and commu-
e-mail: pengjiane@stu.edu.cn nication in the era of globalization.

123
J.-E. Peng

The theorizing of imagined communities has been par- which opens up more learning opportunities that are
ticularly pertinent to researching English learning and otherwise unavailable in the classroom context. It is both a
communication in EFL contexts. Kanno and Norton (2003) factor contributing to L2 learning and a non-linguistic
defined imagined communities as ‘‘groups of people, not outcome that educators would desire to achieve among
immediately tangible and accessible, with whom we con- language learners (MacIntyre 2007).
nect through the power of the imagination’’ (p. 241). Reaching the state of WTC is a complex process nur-
Imagination, according to Wenger (1998), is an important tured by many factors, most of which were capsulated in
distinct mode of belonging to a community with which one MacIntyre et al.’s (1998) heuristic L2 WTC model. These
creates images of the world and the self by transcending factors are classified as having enduring influences (e.g.,
time and space. English is increasingly viewed to be personality) and situational influences (e.g., state anxiety
detached from any specific Anglophone community but and perceived competence) on L2 WTC, which have been
instead represents a worldwide global community. Hence, subsequently supported in a large body of studies (Clément
English language learning seems to implicate a process of et al. 2003; MacIntyre and Charos 1996; MacIntyre et al.
learners’ negotiation of their identities as members of such 2003; MacIntyre and Legatto 2011). In-depth qualitative
a global community conceived in their minds, or an inquiries have also revealed strong evidence that in edu-
imagined global community (Ryan 2006). cational settings, L2 WTC dynamically fluctuates in
The recently proposed theory of L2 motivational self response to the interaction of individual and contextual
system (Dörnyei 2005, 2009) also draws on the notion of factors such as topics, interlocutors, task types, and com-
imagination. This theory posits that learners’ vision of their munication contexts (Cao 2011; Kang 2005).
future selves as L2 users engaging in interaction with Motivation and integrativeness, among many anteced-
members of a given community provides motivational ents of L2 WTC, are remarkable variables influencing
impetus for L2 learning. This community may be real or learners’ decision to cross their ‘‘Rubicon,’’ a metaphor for
exist in learners’ imagination. This theory has demon- committing oneself to L2 communication (MacIntyre
strated its validity in accounting for motivated learning 2007). Early L2 WTC studies succinctly showed the rela-
behavior in EFL contexts (Csizér and Kormos 2009; Papi tionship between L2 WTC and integrativeness and moti-
and Teimouri 2012; Taguchi et al. 2009). However, its vation from Gardner’s (1985) socio-educational model
influence on L2 WTC, another type of motivational pro- (MacIntyre and Charos 1996; MacIntyre et al. 2003).
pensity, remains largely underinvestigated. Similar results were replicated in Peng’s (2007) study with
From the perspective of imagined communities, inter- 174 Chinese EFL college students, except that motivation
national posture and English-related future selves reflect and integrativeness only accounted for a small proportion
learners’ sense of legitimate English speakers in a global of the variance of L2 WTC. This result attests to MacIntyre
community, which may propel their WTC in English. This et al.’s (1998) proposition that L2 WTC is susceptible to
study aims to examine to what extent EFL learners’ WTC many individual, contextual, and sociocultural factors.
in English is predicted by their L2 motivational self sys- Yashima (2002) proposed international posture to
tem, international posture, and language anxiety, the last explore EFL learners’ attitudes toward a global community
one being an indispensable antecedent of L2 WTC. This connected by English, given the fact that the pervasiveness
study represents an innovative attempt to explore the pre- of English is blurring boundaries between specific English-
diction of the latest L2 motivation theory coupled with speaking communities. International posture is defined as
attitudinal and affective factors on WTC. It provides ‘‘interest in foreign or international affairs, willingness to
implications for constructing EFL learners’ English-related go overseas to stay or work, readiness to interact with
imagined selves or identities to compensate for their lack of intercultural partners, and, one hopes, openness or a non-
real-life L2 contact. Since EFL learners have greatly out- ethnocentric attitude toward different cultures’’ (ibid.,
numbered native English speakers (Crystal 2003), this p. 57). It seeks to explain EFL learners’ motivation from
study can inspire an understanding of EFL learners’ their intended identification with the global community, or
learning and communication as involving a reconstruction more precisely, an imagined community residing in
of identities implicated in the process of globalization. learners’ minds. As Lamb (2004) contended, contemporary
aspirations to English learning, such as understanding pop
Relationship Between Motivation, International songs and studying or traveling abroad, reflect one’s
Posture, and L2 WTC international posture.
International posture has been found to be a valid atti-
Willingness to communicate in an L2 was conceptualized tudinal construct closely related to L2 WTC and motivation
as the last step before overt communication behavior and in a number of studies (Ghonsooly et al. 2012; Yashima
perceived to drive learners to engage in L2 communication, 2002, 2009). Yashima’s (2002) study with 297 Japanese

123
Self, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of WTC

students confirmed the direct effect of international posture students in Csizér and Kormos’ (2009) study in the Hun-
on L2 WTC and indirect effect of motivation on WTC via gary context.
L2 proficiency and L2 communication confidence. Although stemming from seemingly different theories,
Ghonsooly et al.’s (2012) investigation among 158 Iranian international posture and ideal L2 self both capitalize on
university undergraduates replicated the direct influence of learners’ power of imagination. International posture cap-
international posture on WTC. tures learners’ attitudes toward the international commu-
L2 motivation is also gaining theoretical momentum nity represented by English. This community apparently
with the introduction of Dörnyei’s (2005) L2 motivational does not have clear boundaries or defining properties but
self system. This theory draws on the mainstream psy- instead seems to develop from accumulated experience in
chology concept of possible selves (Higgins 1987; Markus learning and using English. Similarly, learners’ visions or
and Nurius 1986) as the source of L2 learning motivation. imagery of themselves as competent English users in an
Possible selves are future-oriented self-state representa- imagined community is the central essence of the ideal L2
tions that include hoped-for selves, feared selves, and self (Ryan 2006). Exposure to media and education may
expected selves, respectively, referring to future selves that prompt learners to develop a ‘‘cosmopolitan identity’’
one desires to become, fears to become, and is likely to (Kormos and Csizér 2008, p. 330) and visualize ideal
become (Markus and Nurius 1986). Individuals’ inclination English selves, which motivates their English learning. In
to reduce the discrepancies between the actual selves and Kormos and Csizér’s (2008) study, results of regression
future selves functions to regulate their actions (Higgins analyses showed that international posture was the best
1987). In the area of SLA, motivation to learn the L2 may predictor of ideal L2 self.
arise from learners’ constant evaluation of the current
selves against the future possible selves. Relationship Between L2 Anxiety and L2 WTC
The L2 motivational self system comprises three
dimensions: the ideal L2 self, ought-to L2 self, and L2 Counteracting the facilitating impetus brought by motiva-
learning experience (Dörnyei 2005, 2009). The ideal L2 tion, L2 anxiety is a significant variable impairing L2
self refers to the L2-specific desired image of one’s future learning and communication. L2 anxiety refers to the
self. If one’s coveted future self is a competent L2 speaker, ‘‘worry and negative emotional reaction aroused when
this vision can motivate learning behavior so that one can learning or using a second language’’ (MacIntyre 1999,
reduce the gap between the actual and ideal self. The p. 27). Anxious learners tend to display avoidance behav-
ought-to L2 self concerns L2-specific attributes that one ior, such as missing class, postponing assignments, or
believes one ought to possess. This self representation is avoiding conveying complex messages using the L2
mostly associated with obligations or duties. L2 learning (Horwitz et al. 1986). In L2 oral communication situations,
experience is related to motives that develop from one’s anxiety may consume cognitive resources and reduce one’s
immediate learning environment and experience. ability to retrieve already limited L2 linguistic forms.
The internal structure of the L2 motivational self system Cumulative findings have been reported that L2 anxiety
and its role in L2 learning has been explored in a growing inhibited learners from volunteering answers in oral
body of research. Csizér and Kormos (2009) reported that classroom activities (Ely 1986), negatively correlated to
in the Hungarian EFL context, L2 learning experience self-ratings of L2 proficiency (MacIntyre et al. 1997), and
significantly influenced ideal L2 self, and the influence of negatively predicted oral achievement in the L2 (Woodrow
ought-to L2 self on ideal L2 self was significant for the 2006).
sample of secondary students but non-significant for the In L2 WTC research, L2 anxiety has been examined
university students. Regarding their motivational force, either as an individual variable in its own right or as a
ideal L2 self and L2 learning experience have been found constituent of L2 self-confidence. L2 anxiety as an indi-
to directly impact on intended learning efforts or motivated vidual variable was found to correlate significantly with
learning behavior (Csizér and Kormos 2009; Papi 2010; motivation and self-perceived L2 competence (MacIntyre
Papi and Teimouri 2012; Taguchi et al. 2009). The role of et al. 2003) or negatively affect self-perceived competence
ought-to L2 self, on the other hand, seems to be less con- and L2 WTC (MacIntyre and Charos 1996). As a compo-
clusive. In Taguchi et al.’s (2009) study, the ought-to L2 nent factor, L2 anxiety mitigated by self-perceived com-
self significantly influenced intended learning efforts petence has been conceptualized to define L2 self-
among students from Japan, Iran, and China (cf. Papi confidence (c.f. Clément 1986). Self-confidence in turn
2010). This significant relationship was identified among directly influences L2 WTC (Clément et al. 2003;
the university students but not the secondary school Ghonsooly et al. 2012; Yashima 2002).

123
J.-E. Peng

The Study 4 L2 WTC. This scale was adopted from Yashima (2009,
based on Ryan 2009). The results of the CFA indicated
This study explored the interrelationships between the L2 that two distinct factors appear to underlie this scale:
motivational self system, international posture, L2 anxiety, L2 WTC inside the classroom and L2 WTC outside the
and L2 WTC using structural equation modeling (SEM). Its classroom (Peng 2013). Therefore, three items were
results were further interpreted from the perspective of an retained to capture WTC inside the classroom
imagined global community. (a = 0.76) and four to measure WTC outside the
classroom (a = 0.82). Their correlation coefficient was
Instrument .07.

The questionnaire used in this study was anonymous and


Hypothesized Model
comprised a demographic section and scales measuring L2
motivational self system, international posture, and anxi-
Based on the previous research, a hypothetical model, as
ety. The scales were adopted from the existing literature
illustrated in Fig. 1, was proposed to describe the interre-
and used a six-point Likert response scale.1The question-
lationships of three types of variables: motivational (the
naire was translated into Chinese by the author. Confir-
three components of the L2 motivational self system),
matory factor analysis (CFA) was performed for each scale
affective (international posture and L2 anxiety), and com-
to establish measurement validity (to be presented later).
municational (L2 WTC).
The description of the items and their internal consistency
As shown in Fig. 1, L2 WTC inside and outside the
reliability assessed using Cronbach’s alpha (a) are pre-
classroom, instead of being specified to constitute a higher
sented below.
order construct, were delineated to be two endogenous
1 Ideal L2 self, ought-to L2 self, and L2 learning variables due to their very low correlation coefficient.
experience. Ideal L2 self was operationalized by six However, they were hypothesized to share the same pre-
items used in Ryan (2009) (a = 0.76). These items dictors because there have been no previous findings sug-
elicit the participants’ perceived L2-specific ideal gesting the otherwise. In the following description of the
selves. Ought-to L2 self was measured by six items research assumptions, ‘‘L2 WTC’’ was used to cover L2
adopted from Papi (2010) (a = 0.75). This scale WTC both inside and outside the classroom.
measures the participants’ sense of duties or obligations Ought-to L2 self and L2 learning experience are the
in their pursuit of L2-specific attributes. Another six exogenous variables that jointly influence ideal L2 self,
items from Papi (2010) were used to measure L2 which is based on Csizér and Kormos’ (2009) model. With
learning experience (a = 0.83). reference to the models tested in Csizér and Kormos (2009)
2 International posture. This scale, which was adapted and Kormos et al. (2011), L2 learning experience was
from Yashima (2002, 2009), has three components: hypothesized to influence international posture, which in
intergroup approach-avoidance tendency (IAAT), inter- turn predicts ideal L2 self. In addition, ideal L2 self and
est in international vocation/activities (IIVA), and ought-to L2 self were hypothesized to influence L2 anxiety,
interest in international news (IIN). Based on the in accordance with Papi’s (2010) study.
preceding CFA results, IAAT subscale contains five
items measuring the participants’ tendency to approach
or avoid non-Chinese people (a = 0.70). The IIVA
subscale comprises four items assessing the extent to L2 learning
experience International
which the participants were interested in living abroad posture
or engaging in an international career (a = 0.70). IIN
was measured by three items (a = 0.76) that elicit the L2 WTC inside
participants’ interest in news or issues happening the classroom
around the world. Ideal L2 self
3 L2 anxiety. Six items from Ryan (2009) (a = 0.78)
were used. These items elicit how much one feels L2 WTC outside
anxious when conversing with fluent or native speakers L2 anxiety
the classroom
of English, or speaking English in class.
Ought-to L2 self

1
The questionnaire is available upon request. Fig. 1 The hypothesized model

123
Self, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of WTC

L2 WTC was treated as the criterion measure that is thus used because it tends to provide estimates robust to
subject to the direct impact of L2 learning experience, non-normal distribution (Brown 2006).
international posture, and L2 anxiety. Learners’ experience
of classroom environment has been found to directly Testing the Measurement Models
influence L2 WTC (Peng and Woodrow 2010). Since L2
learning experience captured in this study is conceptually The data were analyzed at two stages: testing the mea-
related to classroom experiences, it was expected to surement models for the six latent variables using CFAs
influence WTC. Additionally, paths respectively leading and testing the full structural model using SEM. CFA takes
from international posture and L2 anxiety to L2 WTC were a confirmatory approach to testing hypothesis about the
hypothesized according to the previous findings (Clément relationships between observed variables (e.g., scale items)
et al. 2003; Yashima 2002). This study, however, did not and latent variables, which is a necessary step before
speculate direct influence of ideal L2 self and ought-to L2 integrating them into a full structural model (Hair et al.
self on L2 WTC, because motivational variables have been 2006).
widely observed to influence L2 WTC indirectly via their The reliability of the instrumentation was assessed by
effects on other more proximal factors such as anxiety or examining the internal consistency reliability (Cronbach’s
confidence (MacIntyre et al. 1998; Yashima 2002). a), which was reported previously. The adequacy of the
measurement models and the ensuing full structural model
Participants and Procedures was evaluated using the Chi square (v2) statistic, normed v2
(v2/df), and other goodness-of-fit measures. The values of
The participants of this study were 1,073 students recruited normed v2 ranging from 2 to 5 are deemed acceptable
from six universities in China. Upon deleting cases with (Kline 2005). Several fit indexes were considered: the Root
missing values and outliers, 1,013 valid cases were retained Mean-Square Error Approximation (RMSEA), Standard-
for data analysis (male n = 671; female n = 342). The ized Root Mean-square Residual (SRMR), Goodness-of-Fit
average age of the participants was 19.40 years, with nine Index (GFI), the Normed Fit Index (NFI), and the Com-
unreported. They were all freshmen and sophomores, for parative Fit Index (CFI). Generally, the RMSEA and
whom English was a compulsory subject in the first 2 years SRMR should be lower than 0.08, and the GFI, NFI, and
of their studies. They majored in one of the following CFI should have values greater than 0.90 for a model to
disciplines: architecture; clinical medicine; business, eco- display acceptable goodness of fit (Byrne 1998). Items
nomics, and finance; engineering; law; mathematics; and were dropped from the measurement models based on the
history. By the time this study was carried out, the par- joint guidelines: a) the t statistics for their corresponding
ticipants had been learning English for about 6–9 years. parameter estimate was below ±1.96 and their R2 value
The targeted universities were approached and permis- was below 0.30 or greater than 1.00; b) there was theo-
sion to conduct the research was obtained. The teachers in retical justification (Byrne 1998).
these universities were contacted and informed of the The CFAs results showed that the measurement models
research purposes and procedures. The prospective partic- for ideal L2 self, ought-to L2 self, L2 learning experience,
ipants were told that their participation was optional and no and L2 anxiety were respectively unidimensional with
identifiable personal information would be revealed in any reasonable goodness of fit. The three-factor measurement
case. The Chinese version of the questionnaire was model for international posture was confirmed with four
administered to the participants in their regular class time. items progressively removed based on the above-men-
The questionnaire took approximately 15 min to complete. tioned guidelines. The L2 WTC scale, however, was found
to have two dimensions: L2 WTC inside the classroom
(WTCIN) and L2 WTC outside the classroom (WTCOUT)
Results (see Peng 2013). Table 1 shows the fit indexes for the
measurement model. Following Yashima (2002), item
Preliminary Analysis scores for IAAT, IIVA, and IIN were averaged to form
three composite variables, which were specified as three
The hypothesized model was tested using LISREL 8.70. indicators for international posture in the full structural
The assumption of univariate normality was met since the model.
values of skewness and kurtosis of each item fell within the
range of -2 and ?2 (Kunnan 1998). The data deviate from Testing the Full Structural Model
ideal multivariate normality, since the Mardia’s coeffi-
cients for the measurement models ranged between 3.08 The initial analysis indicated that the hypothetical model
and 21.54. Robust maximum likelihood estimation was provided an acceptable fit to the data. However, the paths

123
J.-E. Peng

leading respectively from L2 learning experience and L2 Marcoulides (2006) cautioned that unless indirect effects
anxiety to L2 WTC outside the classroom were non-sig- are considered, the relationships between variables in a
nificant. For the sake of model parsimony, they were pro- structural model cannot be fully explained. Table 3 pre-
gressively removed. The mean and standard deviation (SD) sents the direct, indirect, total effects between the vari-
of these variables and their correlation coefficients are ables, which are in the form of standardized regression
presented in Table 2. coefficients.
The final model reasonably fits the data (i.e., As seen in Table 3, ideal L2 self was strongly predicted
v2 = 1,945.59; df = 516; v2/df = 3.77; RMSEA = 0.05; by L2 learning experience (.64) and international posture
SRMR = 0.06; GFI = 0.89; NFI = 0.92; CFI = 0.94). (.42), implying that positive past experience and attitudes
The variances of the endogenous variables explained by toward the world could inspire learners to envision selves
this model are 63 % for ideal L2 self, 34 % for interna- being proficient in English. The influence of ought-to L2
tional posture, 17 % for L2 anxiety, and 28 and 3 % for L2 self on ideal L2 self was comparatively lower (.25), indi-
WTC inside and outside the classroom. The model is dis- cating less influence of external factors on ideal L2 self.
played in Fig. 2. International posture was strongly influenced by L2
To further explore these relationships, the direct, indi- learning experience.
rect, and total effects were examined. A direct effect refers The strongest predictor of L2 anxiety was ought-to L2
to the direct influence of one variable on another, which is self (0.35), followed by ideal L2 self (-0.27). It is note-
often shown beside the single-headed arrow connecting worthy that when the direct effect of ought-L2 self on L2
these two variables. Indirect effects are calculated as the anxiety (0.41) and its indirect effect via ideal L2 self
product of direct effects (Kline 2005). The direct and (0.25*-0.27 = -0.07) were summated, its total effect on
indirect effects of one variable on another constitute the L2 anxiety was reduced to 0.35.
total effect. For instance, as shown in Fig. 2, ought-to L2 The strongest direct predictor of L2 WTC inside the
self had a direct effect on L2 anxiety (0.41) and an indirect classroom was L2 anxiety (-0.30), whereas L2 learning
effect on L2 anxiety (0.068, i.e., 0.25*-0.27 = -0.068) experience exerted the largest total effect on this predicted
via its direct effect on ideal L2 self (0.25) and ideal L2 variable (0.42). These two variables exhibited much greater
self’s direct effect on L2 anxiety(-0.27), which results in a effect on in-class WTC than did international posture
total effect of 0.342 (i.e., 0.41 - 0.068 = 0.342). (0.18), ought-to L2 self (-0.11), and ideal L2 self (0.08),
Although direct effects are of primary interest, Raykov and which indicates that experience in classroom and L2

Table 1 Fit indexes for the measurement models of the six latent variables
v2 df v2/df RMSEA SRMR GFI NFI CFI

Ideal L2 self 19.99 8 2.50 0.04 0.02 0.99 0.99 0.99


Ought-to L2 self 18.03 6 3.01 0.05 0.02 0.99 0.99 0.99
L2 learning experience 35.73 8 4.47 0.06 0.03 0.99 0.99 0.99
International posture 270.14 50 5.40 0.07 0.05 0.95 0.95 0.96
L2 anxiety 18.23 7 2.60 0.04 0.02 0.99 0.99 0.99
L2 WTC 58.00 11 2.65 0.07 0.05 0.98 0.98 0.98

Table 2 Correlation coefficients between the latent variables


Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Ideal L2 self 4.48 0.81 1.00


2. Ought-to L2 self 3.47 0.84 0.28** 1.00
3. L2 learning experience 3.94 0.83 0.62** 0.20** 1.00
**
4. International posture 4.03 0.69 0.44 0.07* 0.40** 1.00
* **
5. L2 anxiety 3.63 0.86 -0.07 0.25 -0.10** -0.30** 1.00
6. L2 WTC inside the classroom 4.50 0.93 0.28** -0.02 0.33** 0.29** -0.34** 1.00
7. L2 WTC outside the classroom 3.73 1.14 0.10** 0.02 0.11** 0.14** -0.11** 0.10** 1.00
* **
p \ 0.05, p \ 0.01

123
Self, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of WTC

0.49 0.40 0.91


0.82 like atmosphere
intergroup approach/ interest in international interest in
avoidance tendency vocation/activities international news
0.36 interesting 0.43
0.80 0.72 0.78 0.31
0.61 more lessons
0.63 International
L2 learning 0.58
0.28 enjoy learning 0.85 posture
experience 0.14
0.55 group discussion 0.75
0.70 time passes faster 0.29 0.50 in class
0.68 talk freely
look forward to WTCIN 0.87 0.24
0.54 in class
classes 0.39 0.42 0.80
0.17 talk in front of
the class 0.36
0.63 future career
acquaintance 0.33
things to do in 0.61
0.57 0.82 before you
future
0.66 in a small group 0.68
imagine as WTCOUT 0.56 of strangers
0.79 0.46
someone able Ideal L2 self 0.89
friend before 0.21
0.77 imagine with 0.48 you
international friends 0.66
0.67 -0.30
in a small 0.57
0.54 use effectively group of friends
in future 0.68 -0.27
in future important feel nervous 0.61
0.54 0.25
to use 0.63
my English 0.69
0.74 gain approval 0.56 is strange
with a native 0.42
0.51 L2 anxiety 0.76
speaker
0.86 close friends think
0.63
0.38 0.41 speak in class 0.61
people I respect 0.43
0.49
think 0.71 Ought-to L2 self
people surrounding 0.69 0.64 volunteer answer 0.82
0.52
expect
0.56 stranger asked
let other people 0.59
0.68 me
down 0.59
0.65 people will respect
me

Fig. 2 Full structural equation model

Table 3 Standardized direct, indirect, and total effects for the structural model
Predicted variable Predictor variable Direct effect Indirect effect Total effect

Ideal L2 self L2 learning experience 0.39 0.25 0.64a


International posture 0.42 0.42
Ought-to L2 self 0.25 0.25
International posture L2 learning experience 0.58 0.58
L2 anxiety Ought-to L2 self 0.41 -0.07 0.35
Ideal L2 self -0.27 -0.27
L2 learning experience -0.17 -0.17
International posture -0.11 -0.11
WTC inside the classroom L2 learning experience 0.29 0.14 0.42
L2 anxiety -0.30 -0.30
International posture 0.14 0.03 0.18
Ought-to L2 self -0.11 -0.11
Ideal L2 self 0.08 0.08
WTC outside the classroom International posture 0.17 0.17
L2 learning experience 0.10 0.10
a
Figures in the table may not add up to the totals due to rounding

anxiety played important roles in learners’ WTC in class. Discussion


L2 WTC outside the classroom was directly influenced by
international posture (0.17) and indirectly by L2 learning This study found that L2 learning experience and ought-to
experience (0.10). L2 self directly influenced ideal L2 self, with the former

123
J.-E. Peng

exerting a stronger effect, which replicates Csizér and The identified two factors (i.e., WTC inside and outside
Kormos’ (2009) findings with Hungarian university stu- the classroom) underlying the L2 WTC scale contributed
dents. This result could be explained with reference to the evidence to the theoretical aspects of the L2 WTC con-
intrinsic features of ideal L2 self. Ideal L2 self concerns struct. Previous research has delineated these two factors,
one’s internalized aspiration to become a competent L2 and found that in-class WTC was higher than out-of-class
user, which presumably can be induced or enhanced by WTC among students learning French in Canada (MacIn-
successful or enjoyable personal experience. In contrast, tyre et al. 2001) and those learning English in mainland
because ought-to L2 self reflects externally imposed obli- China and Hong Kong (Yu and Lin 2004). This result
gation, it may not be internalized to the idealized images of suggests that L2 WTC is unlikely to be a unidimensional
the current participating university students who were construct and its complexity should be given due consid-
growing in maturity and independence. eration in any attempt to measure it.
The model reveals that international posture was The model identifies different antecedents of L2 WTC
strongly predicted by L2 learning experience, and signifi- inside and outside the classroom, indicating that L2 WTC
cantly impacted on ideal L2 self. The results are consistent may operate in different ways across situations. The in-
with Csizér and Kormos (2009) and added evidence to the class WTC was predicted directly and/or indirectly by all
proposed close relationship between L2 motivational self other non-WTC variables. The strong effect of L2 learning
system and international posture (Kormos et al. 2011; Ryan experience on in-class WTC corroborates previous findings
2006; Yashima 2009). The findings suggest that, although about the influence of experience on motivated learning
English learning in this EFL setting is highly examination- behavior in general (Csizér and Kormos 2009; Papi 2010)
oriented, international posture nonetheless could harness and on classroom WTC in particular (Peng and Woodrow
desired English-related self-guides among the participants. 2010). The negative effect of L2 anxiety on in-class L2
When students willingly relate themselves to the world or WTC was expected and conforms to the previous studies
the global community, they probably can construe a self (Horwitz et al. 1986; Peng and Woodrow 2010). It is
image of competent English user in that community. undeniable that speaking English in class is challenging
Mental imagery may empower them to transcend the due to, for instance, fear of negative evaluation (Horwitz
constraints of EFL contexts, for instance, a lack of et al. 1986), risks of being laughed at (Peng 2007), and lack
authentic contact with English-speaking communities. of perceived L2 competence (MacIntyre et al. 1997).
Ideal L2 self and ought-to L2 self, respectively, had L2 WTC outside the classroom, however, was only
negative and positive influence on L2 anxiety, which directly predicted by international posture and indirectly by
correspond to Papi’s (2010) finding. Dörnyei (2009) reit- L2 learning experience. This suggests that when stepping
erated that ideal self has a promotion focus whereas ought- outside the classroom and not being obliged to speak
to self has a prevention focus. Learners with strong ideal English as were in class, the participants who were aware
L2 selves are likely to align their aspirations to L2-related of the pervasiveness of English and readily identify with
attributes expected in L2 use situations, which would the global community were more likely to sustain their
possibly reduce their anxious arousal. However, the very WTC. This finding supports Lamb’s (2004) proposition
fear of failure and external pressure embodied in ought-to that international posture has the potential to account for
L2 selves could turn out heightening anxiety in L2 com- EFL learners’ motivation. It also mirrors the results
munication situations. Intriguingly, as the model indicates, regarding international posture’s explanatory power on L2
while ought-to L2 self positively correlated with L2 anx- WTC in the Japanese EFL context (Yashima 2002).
iety, its indirect effect on anxiety via ideal L2 self was The non-significant direct effect of L2 learning experi-
negative, which coincidently corresponds to Papi’s (2010) ence on WTC outside the classroom, however, is in con-
findings. These findings suggest that externally imposed trast with the previous finding that experience influenced
self-construal could increase learners’ linguistic anxiety. motivated learning behavior (Csizér and Kormos 2009;
In contrast, the internalized future self of being competent Papi and Teimouri 2012) or intended learning efforts (Papi
in English could reduce one’s L2 anxiety, probably 2010). A possible explanation is that for the participants
because the process of ideal self construction involves whose primary concern was passing exams, learning efforts
somewhat constant self-suggestion like ‘‘I will make it.’’ were expended more on accumulating test-related knowl-
Although further evidence is needed to confirm this find- edge (e.g., vocabulary or grammar) than on practicing oral
ing, this study brought light to the complex and dynamic skills, not to mention speaking English outside the class.
influence of the future self-guides on L2 anxiety. These This finding implies that in this context English learning
findings also justify the importance of considering both and communication may be distinct enterprises that are not
direct and indirect effects between variables in a structural equally prioritized, and hence learners’ WTC may not
model. parallel motivated learning behavior or efforts in essence or

123
Self, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of WTC

magnitude. Another reason may be that the scale items learners’ learning behavior may not be explicitly discern-
measuring L2 learning experience are related to in-class ible. As Ryan (2006) stated, ‘‘it is incredible to argue that
learning. Future research may investigate whether WTC individuals feel like ‘global citizens’ all the time’’ (p. 33).
outside the classroom will be more strongly predicted by Therefore, it requires significant others such as language
learning experience if out-of-class learning is incorporated educators and parents to shepherd learners toward a vision
into this scale. of ideal L2 selves and an awareness of their participation in
The largely unexplained variance of L2 WTC outside the global community.
the classroom is worth consideration. This indicates that This study has certain limitations that need to be taken
there may be more variables influencing out-of-class WTC into account. First, this study adopted a model generation
that have not been included in the present model. For strategy (Jöreskog 1993) that allows model modifications.
instance, sociocultural factors such as face concern and Its findings require support from further research. In
social support (MacIntyre et al. 2001) would also influence addition, although the findings have been interpreted with
learners’ WTC. Another possibility is that the communi- reference to the notions of imagination and imagined
cation situations described in this scale were of little per- communities that underpin the theories of L2 motivational
sonal relevance or contextually unexpected (e.g., ‘‘when self system and international posture, this quantitative
you find your acquaintance standing before you in a line’’). study was unable to provide rich and contextual descrip-
Understandably, monolingual interlocutors would converse tions in these regards. Future research may adopt qualita-
in their mother tongue unless otherwise required by spe- tive methods to probe learners’ perspectives in depth. In
cific contexts (e.g., course requirements in a language particular, EFL learners’ WTC may be explored from the
class). On the contrary, if the scale items present more novel perspective of English as an international language
‘‘natural situations’’ where English is the necessary med- (EIL) (Alsagoff et al. 2012). From this perspective, if
ium (e.g., ‘‘when sitting next to a foreigner on a train’’), learners accept that the localized Chinese English is a
out-of-class WTC may be more accurately measured (Peng legitimate variety of the EIL and that they have the own-
2013). ership of the EIL, their perceptions of the imagined com-
The present findings have inspired further reflection on munity and their WTC may be redefined. The EIL
how to interpret EFL learners’ learning and communication perspective may shed new light on future L2 WTC
in a globalized era. The notions of future self-guides and research.
international posture appear to apply to the current context. Despite its limitations, this study may suggest some
It seems plausible that the widespread use of English aided implications for teachers of EFL learners. Considering the
by information technology facilitates learners’ imagination significant effect of international posture on ideal L2 self
of membership of an imagined global community. Ener- and L2 WTC, teachers need to enrich students’ global
gized by imagination, learners would be able to envision vision. They can provide students with world news reports
English-related possible selves (Dörnyei 2009), or the on newspaper or TV and guide them to relate what are
selves situated in an English-used community, however, happening around the world to their lives. Explicit
spatially and temporally remote from the real world instructions on unleashing one’s imagination would be
(Yashima 2009), which accelerate their motivation to learn desirable to stimulate students’ vision of future selves. For
and communicate in English. instance, they may ask students to picture scenarios
On the other hand, the model suggests that the total involving using English in the future in their ideal work-
effects of the self-constructs on WTC were not substan- place, or to share information about and their attitudes
tially strong, implying that the formation or motivational toward their idols who happen to speak English profi-
capacity of English-related selves among EFL learners may ciently. Multimedia and multimodal materials such as
not be taken for granted. As Dörnyei (2009) emphasized, English films and television series can also be incorporated
the future self-guides should be elaborate and vivid. An into class to visually incite students’ imagery of the global
imagined future self is not formed out of nothing, but world to which they are readily connected by English.
should be experiential, representing ‘‘‘self state’ that peo- Ultimately, teachers’ such endeavors may help to situate
ple experience as reality’’ (ibid, p. 16). The reality of students in an imagined global community and promote
English learning and teaching in the current context their WTC in English.
remains largely exam-oriented. Success in examinations is
a key to winning competitions for tertiary education
opportunities or job openings. The intensified need for Conclusion
success in English examinations may undermine learners’
tendency or energy to identify with the global world. While The results of this study have demonstrated that the L2
globalization is changing the world, its influence on EFL motivational self system is applicable to this research

123
J.-E. Peng

context. L2 learning experience and international posture Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and second language
were the two strong predictors of ideal L2 self, which learning: The role of attitude and motivation. London: Edward
Arnold.
justifies the needs to endow EFL learners with positive Ghonsooly, B., Khajavy, G. H., & Asadpour, S. F. (2012). Willing-
language learning experience and a world perspective that ness to communicate in English among Iranian non-English
favors the value of English. The effects of ideal L2 self and major university students. Journal of Language and Social
ought-to L2 self on L2 anxiety indicated that a vision of Psychology, 31(2), 197–211.
Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R.
English-related ideal self could reduce anxiety in English L. (2006). Multivariate data analysis (6th ed.). Upper Saddle
use. L2 WTC inside the classroom was predicted by a River: Prentice Hall.
number of variables examined in this study, whereas there Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and
seemed to be more factors that would account for L2 WTC affect. Psychological Review, 94(3), 319–340.
Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. B., & Cope, J. (1986). Foreign language
outside the classroom. The results were further interpreted classroom anxiety. The Modern Language Journal, 70(2),
from the perspective of an imagined global community. 125–132.
This study is of far-reaching pertinence for understanding Jöreskog, K. G. (1993). Testing structural equation models. In K.
EFL learners’ English learning as involving a reconstruc- A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation
models (pp. 294–316). Newbury Park: Sage.
tion of identities implicated in the process of globalization. Kang, S.-J. (2005). Dynamic emergence of situational willingness to
communicate in a second language. System, 33(2), 277–292.
Acknowledgments I would like to thank the anonymous reviewers Kanno, Y., & Norton, B. (2003). Imagined communities and
for their insightful comments on the earlier versions of this paper. The educational possibilities: Introduction. Journal of Language,
study this article reports on was supported by the Ministry of Edu- Identity, and Education, 2(4), 241–249.
cation of China (MOE) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation
(Project No. 13YJA740041) modeling (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.
Kormos, J., & Csizér, K. (2008). Age-related differences in the
motivation of learning English as a foreign language: Attitudes,
References selves, and motivated learning behavior. Language Learning,
58(2), 327–355.
Kormos, J., Kiddle, T., & Csizér, K. (2011). Systems of goals,
Alsagoff, L., McKay, S. L., Hu, G., & Renandya, W. A. (Eds.). attitudes, and self-related beliefs in second-language-learning
(2012). Principles and practices for teaching English as an motivation. Applied Linguistics, 32(5), 495–516.
international language. New York: Routledge. Kunnan, A. J. (1998). An introduction to structural equation
Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied modelling for language assessment research. Language Testing,
research. New York: Guilford Press. 15(3), 295–332.
Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with LISREL, Lamb, M. (2004). Integrative motivation in a globalizing world.
PRELIS, and SIMPLIS: Basic concepts, applications, and System, 32(1), 3–19.
programming. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. MacIntyre, P. D. (1999). Language anxiety: A review of the research
Cao, Y. (2011). Investigating situational willingness to communicate for language teachers. In D. Young (Ed.), Affect in foreign
within second language classrooms from an ecological perspec- language and second language learning (pp. 24–46). Boston:
tive. System, 39(4), 468–479. MacGraw-Hill College.
Clément, R. (1986). Second language proficiency and acculturation: MacIntyre, P. D. (2007). Willingness to communicate in the second
An investigation of the effects of language status and individual language: Understanding the decision to speak as a volitional
characteristics. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, process. The Modern Language Journal, 91(4), 564–576.
5(4), 271–290. MacIntyre, P. D., Baker, S. C., Clément, R., & Conrod, S. (2001).
Clément, R., Baker, S. C., & MacIntyre, P. D. (2003). Willingness to Willingness to communicate, social support, and language-
communicate in a second language: The effects of context, learning orientations of immersion students. Studies in Second
norms, and vitality. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, Language Acquisition, 23, 369–388.
22(2), 190–209. MacIntyre, P. D., Baker, S. C., Clément, R., & Donovan, L. A.
Crystal, D. (2003). English as a Global Language (2nd ed.). (2003). Talking in order to learn: Willingness to communicate
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. and intensive language programs. Canadian Modern Language
Csizér, K., & Kormos, J. (2009). Learning experiences, selves and Review, 59(4), 589–607.
motivated learning behaviour: A comparative analysis of struc- MacIntyre, P. D., & Charos, C. (1996). Personality, attitudes, and
tural models for Hungarian secondary and university learners of affect as predictors of second language communication. Journal
English. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, of Language and Social Psychology, 15(1), 3–26.
language identity and the L2 self (pp. 98–119). Bristol: MacIntyre, P. D., Dörnyei, Z., Clément, R., & Noels, K. A. (1998).
Multilingual Matters. Conceptualizing willingness to communicate in a L2: A
Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The psychology of the language learner: situational model of L2 confidence and affiliation. The Modern
Individual differences in second language acquisition. Mahwah: Language Journal, 82(4), 545–562.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. MacIntyre, P. D., & Legatto, J. J. (2011). A dynamic system approach
Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The L2 motivational self system. In Z. Dörnyei & to willingness to communicate: Developing an idiodynamic
E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self method to capture rapidly changing affect. Applied Linguistics,
(pp. 9–42). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 32(2), 149–171.
Ely, C. M. (1986). An analysis of discomfort, risk taking, sociability, MacIntyre, P. D., Noels, K. A., & Clément, R. (1997). Biases in self-
and motivation in the L2 classroom. Language Learning, 36(1), ratings of second language proficiency: The role of language
1–25. anxiety. Language Learning, 47(2), 265–287.

123
Self, Attitudes and Affect as Predictors of WTC

Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Ryan, S. (2009). Self and identity in L2 motivation in Japan: The
Psychologist, 41(9), 954–969. ideal L2 self and Japanese learners of English. In Z. Dörnyei &
Norton, B. (2001). Non-participation, imagined communities and the E. Ushioda (Eds.), Motivation, language identity and the L2 self
language classroom. In M. P. Breen (Ed.), Learner contributions (pp. 120–143). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
to language learning: New directions in research (pp. 159–171). Taguchi, T., Magid, M., & Papi, M. (2009). The L2 motivational self
Harlow: Longman. system among Japanese, Chinese and Iranian learners of English:
Papi, M. (2010). The L2 motivational self system, L2 anxiety, and A comparative study. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.),
motivated behavior: A structural equation modeling approach. Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 66–97).
System, 38(3), 467–479. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
Papi, M., & Teimouri, Y. (2012). Dynamics of selves and motivation: Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning and
A cross-sectional study in the EFL context of Iran. International identity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Journal of Applied Linguistics, 22(3), 288–309. Woodrow, L. J. (2006). Anxiety and speaking English as a second
Peng, J. (2007). Willingness to communicate in an L2 and integrative language. RELC Journal, 37(3), 308–328.
motivation among college students in an intensive English Yashima, T. (2002). Willingness to communicate in a second
language program in China. University of Sydney Papers in language: The Japanese EFL context. The Modern Language
TESOL, 2, 33–59. Journal, 86(1), 54–66.
Peng, J. (2013). The challenge of measuring willingness to commu- Yashima, T. (2009). International posture and the ideal L2 self in the
nicate in EFL contexts. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, Japanese EFL context. In Z. Dörnyei & E. Ushioda (Eds.),
22(3), 281–291. Motivation, language identity and the L2 self (pp. 144–192).
Peng, J., & Woodrow, L. J. (2010). Willingness to communicate in Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
English: A model in Chinese EFL classroom context. Language Yu, W., & Lin, M. (2004). Shuigang gaoxiao xuesheng yingyu jiaoliu
Learning, 60(4), 834–876. yiyuan duibi yanjiu [Willingness to communicate: A compara-
Raykov, T., & Marcoulides, G. A. (2006). A first course in structural tive study of Guangdong and Hong Kong Chinese learners of
equation modeling (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum English in higher education]. Foreign Language Teaching and
Associates. Research, 36, 219–223.
Ryan, S. (2006). Language learning motivation within the context of
globalisation: An L2 self within an imagined global community.
Critical Inquiry in Language Studies, 3(1), 23–45.

123

You might also like