You are on page 1of 12

THOMAS LEDERMANN University of Connecticut

GUY BODENMANN University of Zurich, Switzerland*

MYRIAM RUDAZ University of Connecticut**

THOMAS N. BRADBURY University of California, Los Angeles***

Stress, Communication, and Marital Quality


in Couples

The association between daily stress outside and The quality of intimate relationships is likely to
inside of the relationship and marital functioning be influenced by both context and intradyadic
in the form of communication in conflict factors. The marital stress model by Boden-
situations and marital quality was examined. We mann (2000; see also Bodenmann, Ledermann,
hypothesized that relationship stress mediates & Bradbury, 2007), for example, posits that
the association between external stress and stress affects relationship outcomes directly and
marital functioning at the individual level, and indirectly through the quality of marital commu-
that the association between relationship stress nication, the spouses’ psychological and physi-
and marital quality is partially mediated by ological well-being, and the time spouses spend
communication at the dyadic level. Using the together. Likewise, Karney and Bradbury (1995)
Actor-Partner Interdependence and Common highlighted the role of spouses’ capabilities to
Fate Model, the results of 345 couples supported adapt to stressful circumstances (e.g., ability to
our hypotheses and revealed that a person’s support each other), which mediates the asso-
relationship stress is more strongly related ciation between stress and marital quality. In
with one’s own external stress than with the accordance with these theoretical approaches,
partner’s external stress. The findings indicate the model tested by Matthews, Conger, and
that both low relationship stress and a high Wickrama (1996) suggests that work-family
level of positive communication are important conflict increases psychological distress, which,
in relationships. in turn, affects marital quality indirectly through
marital interaction. All three models share the
assumption that the relation between stress and
Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, marital outcomes is mediated by intradyadic
Storrs, CT 06269-1020 (thomas.ledermann@uconn.edu) variables. The purpose of the present study is
*Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, 8050 to examine the interplay between self-perceived
Zurich, Switzerland. stress, marital communication, and marital qual-
**Department of Psychology, University of Connecticut, ity in heterosexual couples.
Storrs, CT 06269-1020.
***Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Empirical Background
Angeles, CA 90095-1563.
Key Words: APIM, CFM, communication, marital quality, A number of studies have shown that differ-
mediation, stress. ent forms of stress, such as work stress or
Family Relations 59 (April 2010): 195 – 206 195
DOI:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2010.00595.x
196 Family Relations

economic strains, have a negative effect on satisfaction, explaining 8% (Miller & Kannae,
marital quality and satisfaction (Bodenmann, 1999) to 77% (Banmen & Vogel, 1985) of the
2000; Howe, Levy, & Caplan, 2004; Karney & variance (see also Sprecher, Metts, Burleson,
Bradbury, 1995; Leidy, Parke, Cladis, Coltrane, Hatfield, & Thompson, 1995). In studies by
& Duffy, 2009; Neff & Karney, 2004, 2007). Gottman and colleagues (Carrère, Buehlman,
Some works have suggested that the association Gottman, Coan, & Ruckstuhl, 2000; Gottman &
between stress and marital quality is mediated Levenson, 1992), Rogge and Bradbury (1999)
by more variables. Specifically, there is evidence and Rogge, Bradbury, Hahlweg, Engl, and
that relationship communication partially medi- Thurmaier (2006), marital communication has
ates the effect of marital problems on marital been identified as a salient predictor of marital
quality (Ledermann & Macho, 2009). Further, outcomes in newlywed couples.
Conger et al. (1990) found that the relationship
between economic problems and marital quality
Goal and Hypotheses
is explained through both warm/supportive and
hostile behaviors that act as parallel mediators. The present study aimed to contribute to a bet-
Very little research, however, has incorporated ter understanding of intradyadic associations
the conceptualization proposed by Bodenmann between external daily stress and daily relation-
(2000; see also Randall & Bodenmann, 2009) ship stress and marital functioning, in the form of
and Karney and colleagues (Bradbury & Karney, marital communication in conflict situations and
2004; Karney & Bradbury; Karney, Story, & marital quality in intimate relationships. Build-
Bradbury, 2005; Neff & Karney, 2004), who ing on the theoretical models of Bodenmann
emphasized the distinction between external (2000) and Karney and Bradbury (1995) and the
stress and relationship stress. External stress empirical literature on stress, communication,
refers to tension that originates outside of and marital quality, two mediating hypotheses
a relationship—such as social and economic were tested.
strains, work stress, conflicts with neighbors,
or problems with authorities—whereas relation- Hypothesis 1. On the basis of the model
ship stress refers to tension that arises in the suggesting that daily stress has a direct effect
relationship, in the form of divergent attitudes on marital outcomes (Bodenmann, 2000) and
and needs or disturbing habits of one partner. previous findings indicating that relationship
Recent studies conducted by Bodenmann et al. stress mediates the association between external
(2000, 2007; Bodenmann, Ledermann, Blattner, stress and aspects of marital satisfaction
& Galluzo, 2006) revealed that marital satisfac- (Bodenmann et al., 2007), we assumed that
tion is more affected by stress occurring inside the association between external daily stress
the relationship than by stress originating outside and marital functioning is mediated by daily
of the relationship. Furthermore, studies incor- relationship stress at the level of the dyad
porating both daily stress and critical life events members. Specifically, we hypothesized that
have indicated that in community samples, mar- one’s own external stress is positively related
ital satisfaction is more negatively related to with one’s own relationship stress, which, in
microstress than to macro events (Bodenmann turn, is negatively associated with one’s own,
et al.; Williams, 1995; cf. Karney et al.). as well as the partner’s, marital communication
Communication behavior in interpersonal sit- or marital quality, respectively. Moreover, we
uations is another domain likely to be sensitive supposed that the direct associations between
to stress (Crouter, Perry-Jenkins, Huston, & external stress and relationship stress and
Crawford, 1989; Cutrona et al., 2003). Studies between relationship stress and the marital
conducted by Repetti and colleagues (Repetti, functioning variables are stronger than the direct
1989; Repetti & Wood, 1997) and Schulz, associations between external stress and the
Cowan, Cowan, and Brennan (2004) indicated marital functioning variables after controlling
that work stress can spill over into relationships the mediator relationship stress. This assumption
by increasing social withdrawal and hostility is supported by the results of previous studies
(see also Bolger, DeLongis, Kessler, & Wething- (Bodenmann et al., 2006) showing that external
ton, 1989). stress is more distal to marital functioning than
Several studies have shown that marital com- relationship stress. To test this first hypothesis,
munication is associated with marital quality and we set up two mediation models that differed in
Stress and Communication 197

the outcome variable but not in the independent reported lower marital quality than unmarried
variable and the mediator. Both models included couples (M = 40.2, SD = 9.0 for married and
external stress as independent variable and M = 42.9, SD = 9.5 for unmarried women;
relationship stress as mediator. In the first model, t (341) = 2.19, p < .05; M = 38.9, SD = 9.7
we implemented marital quality as outcome for married and M = 42.3, SD = 9.3 for unmar-
variable, and in the second model, marital ried men; t (341) = 1.97, p < .05).
communication in conflict situations. In both
mediation models, we tested for the presence of
specific dyadic patterns between the variables as Procedure and Measures
discussed by Kenny and Cook (1999). Participants were recruited by means of adver-
tisements in newspapers. Couples who showed
Hypothesis 2. Building on the theoretical mod- interest in participating and who had been
els by Karney and Bradbury (1995) and Boden- together for at least 1 year were sent a pack-
mann (2000) and the model tested by Matthews age of questionnaires including separate and
et al. (1996) in which marital interaction as medi- distinct materials for each partner, along with
ator and marital quality as outcome variable were instructions to complete the questionnaires inde-
modeled at the dyadic (relationship) level, we pendently and return the forms to the laboratory
hypothesized that marital communication in con- within 2 weeks. There were no financial incen-
flict situations mediates the association between tives for participants. In addition to demographic
relationship stress and marital quality at the information, we collected data on self-perceived
level of the dyads. We proposed that relationship stress, marital communication in conflict situa-
stress is directly and indirectly associated with tions, and marital quality by administering the
marital quality through marital communication. following measures.
That is, the association between relationship
stress and marital quality is only partially medi- Daily stress scales. A shortened and adapted
ated. This assumption is based on Bodenmann’s version of the Hassles Scale developed by
stress model and previous findings indicating Kanner, Coyne, Schaefer, and Lazarus (1981)
that marital quality is similar related to both was used to measure self-perceived stress in the
stress and marital communication (Banmen & form of external daily stress and relationship
Vogel, 1985). daily stress (Bodenmann et al., 2006). The
external daily stress scale consisted of 29
METHOD items—such as daily work stress, troublesome
neighbors (Cronbach’s α = .85 for women and
Participants .87 for men). The daily relationship stress
A total of 690 individuals residing in the scale contained eight items—such as demands
German-speaking part of Switzerland partici- of task sharing in household, different goals,
pated in this study, representing a convenience and annoying habits of the partner (Cronbach’s
sample of 345 heterosexual couples. The mean α = .76 for women and .73 for men). All items
age of women was 40.4 years (SD = 8.3) were rated with reference to the previous month
and that of men was 42.5 years (SD = 8.8). on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all stressful, 5 =
The mean relationship duration was 13.8 years very stressful).
(SD = 8.7), and 80% of the couples were mar-
ried and 73% had children. Most of the partic- Marital communication questionnaire (MCQ;
ipants reported a terminal college or university Bodenmann, 2000). This questionnaire assesses
degree (46% of women, 53% of men). Compar- different positive and negative marital commu-
ing married and unmarried participants, married nication behaviors in conflict situations—such
participants were older (women: t (337) = 4.34, as defensiveness, contempt, belligerence, domi-
p < .001; men: t (341) = 4.31, p < .001) and neering, and care. It is based on the communica-
reported a longer relationship duration (women: tion categories proposed by the Specific Affect
t (144) = 10.67, p < .001; men: t (123) = 9.28, (SPAFF) coding system developed by Gottman
p < .001). With respect to measured vari- (1994), and contains 19 items administered on
ables, married and unmarried participants did a 6-point scale (1 = never, 6 = very often). The
not differ significantly on any of these vari- items can be combined into a total score with
ables, with the exception that married couples high scores indicating high quality of marital
198 Family Relations

communication (Cronbach’s α = .85 for women also Kenny, 1996; Kenny & La Voi, 1985) was
and .83 for men). designed to model mediation effects at the level
of the dyads. The application of this model is
Partnership questionnaire (Partnerschaftsfra- especially appropriate if the variables measured
gebogen; Hahlweg, 1996). This marital ques- in both partners can be conceived of as common
tionnaire is composed of three subscales: (a) dyadic constructs and if the actor and partner
quarreling (e.g., ‘‘When we quarrel, he show- effects are substantial and similar in size, when
ers me with insults’’; Cronbach’s α = .96 for modeling the observed variables in an APIM. In
women and .96 for men); (b) tenderness (e.g., couples, relationship stress, marital communi-
‘‘Before going to sleep, we kiss and cuddle each cation, and marital quality can be conceived of
other’’; Cronbach’s α = .88 for women and .91 as variables that represent common dyadic con-
for men); and (c) togetherness or communica- structs (see Ledermann & Macho). In the CFM,
tion (e.g., ‘‘Usually, we talk together in the the variables measured in both dyad partners
evening for at least half an hour’’; Cronbach’s serve as pairwise indicators of the latent dyadic
α = .83 for women and .82 for men). The rating variables (constructs). The CF mediation model
scale ranges from never (0) to very often (3). allows a compact presentation and efficient eval-
Each subscale consists of 10 items, whose linear uation of mediating effects in dyadic data while
combination represents a global index of mari- accounting for measurement errors.
tal quality (Cronbach’s α = .91 for women and Using structural equation modeling (SEM),
.93 for men). In this study, we did not use the we tested the two mediating hypotheses in three
quarreling subscale and excluded all items from steps (see Ledermann & Macho, 2009): First,
the togetherness or communication subscale that we selected a reasonable, good fitting medi-
refer to communication in order to avoid overlap ation model by starting with a model that
with the MCQ on both the item and the con- assumes partial mediation (i.e., model with
ceptual level. The four remaining items of the direct effects between the independent and out-
togetherness or communication subscale were come variables). To evaluate the model fit, we
‘‘We make plans for the future together,’’ ‘‘We used the chi-square statistic, the comparative fit
plan together on how to spend the weekend,’’ index (CFI), and the root mean square error of
‘‘We are attentive to each other’s wishes and act approximation (RMSEA) with CFI > .95 and
accordingly when occasions arise,’’ and ‘‘My RMSEA < .05 indicating close fit (Browne &
partner shows me that he/she loves me.’’ These Cudeck, 1993). Secondly, we tested whether
four items were combined into a single mean the estimated structural coefficients were sta-
score that was then combined with the equally tistically different from zero using the model
weighted tenderness subscale in order to obtain selected. Finally, we tested the indirect (medi-
a single index, with high scores indicating high ating) effects for significance. In this work, we
marital quality (Cronbach’s α = .89 for women used z-statistics and Sobel’s (1982) formula to
and .91 for men). estimate the standard error of the indirect effect:

ab
Data Analyses z=  . (1)
We used an extended version of the Actor- a 2 sb2 + b2 sa2
Partner Interdependence Model (APIM) and the
Common Fate Model (CFM) to test the medi- The term ab denotes the estimated indirect
ating hypotheses. The API mediation model effect between X (independent variable) and Y
(Kenny, 1996; Kenny & Cook, 1999; Leder- (dependent variable) through M (mediator), a
mann & Bodenmann, 2006) was introduced to and b represent the estimated coefficients of the
assess mediation in dyadic data at the level of path X → M and M → Y , respectively, and
the dyad members (individuals) by estimating sa2 and sb2 are the estimated variances of a and
actor and partner effects. In this type of model, b, respectively. According to this procedure,
the influence of an individual’s independent vari- the assumption of mediation is verified if the
able on their dependent variable is called an actor selected model is consistent with the data, if
effect, the influence on the partner’s dependent the direct effects constituting an indirect effect
variable is called a partner effect. The CF medi- are substantial, and if the indirect effect itself
ation model (Ledermann & Macho, 2009; see is significant. In addition, we employed the
Stress and Communication 199

equation provided by MacKinnon (2000) to test correlations between measured variables were
whether the indirect effect in the CF mediation generally as expected.
model differs from the direct effect X → Y (i.e.,
ab − c): Mediation at the Level of the Partners
ab − c The two API mediation models to test the first
z=  (2) hypothesis are presented in Figure 1. The first
a sb + b2 sa2 − 2asbc + sc2
2 2
model incorporates external stress as indepen-
dent variables, relationship stress as mediators,
where c denotes the estimated coefficient of and marital communication in conflict situations
the path X → Y , sc2 represents the estimated as outcome variables (Figure 1, Model 1). The
variance of c, and sbc is the covariance between second model differs from the first in that the out-
the a and b. come variables were replaced by marital quality
(Figure 1, Model 2).
RESULTS Selection of a model. Estimating the API
Preliminary Analyses mediation models with direct effects between
the independent and outcome variables, which
Means and standard deviations are given in were just identified (i.e., df = 0), all four direct
Table 1. There were no significant differences effects were not significant in both models
between women and men with respect to marital (p-values ranged from .092 to .645 in the model
quality and communication in conflict situations. with marital communication and from .069 to
Women, however, reported higher relationship .604 in the model with marital quality). This
and external stress than men. Table 1 also shows is consistent with the assumption of complete
the bivariate correlations among the measured mediation and therefore we excluded these four
variables for women (above the diagonal) and insignificant direct effects. When we tested
men (below the diagonal) and between women these complete mediation models, both models
and men (on the diagonal). The correlations showed a good fit, providing further evidence for
between women and men were large for marital complete mediation (χ 2 (4) = 2.86, p = .581;
quality and medium for marital communica- CFI = 1.000; RMSEA < .001 for the model
tion in conflict situations. The finding that the with marital communication; χ 2 (4) = 3.69,
within-dyad correlation was relatively low for p = .450; CFI = 1.000; RMSEA < .001 for the
external stress compared to the correlation for model with marital quality).
relationship stress may be taken as evidence
that external stress does not represent a common Testing the direct effects. In both mediation
dyadic construct. As expected, there was a con- models, all actor effects (horizontal arrows)
siderable association between external stress and and partner effects (diagonal arrows) proved
relationship stress. Substantial associations were significant with the exception of the two
also found between relationship stress and the partner effects between external stress and
two marital functioning variables. In sum, the relationship stress (see Figure 1). The actor

Table 1. Means, Standard Deviations and Correlations (Women Above, Men Below, and Between Women and Men Along
the Diagonal) for Study Variables

Women Men Correlations


M SD M SD t d 1 2 3 4

Marital quality 1.93 0.54 1.89 0.54 1.35 0.07 .62∗∗∗ .30∗∗∗ −.33∗∗∗ −.18∗∗
Marital communication 4.61 0.43 4.57 0.42 1.69 0.09 .37∗∗∗ .46∗∗∗ −.35∗∗∗ −.25∗∗∗
Relationship stress 1.91 0.53 1.71 0.47 6.79∗∗∗ 0.37 −.38∗∗∗ −.31∗∗∗ .41∗∗∗ .59∗∗∗
External stress 1.78 0.39 1.68 0.36 4.03∗∗∗ 0.22 −.13∗∗∗ −.16∗∗ .56∗∗∗ .26∗∗∗
Note: Scores can range from 0 to 3 for marital quality, from 1 to 6 for marital communication, from 1 to 5 for relationship
and external daily stress. d = Cohen’s d. N = 345.
∗∗
p < .01. ∗∗∗ p < .001.
FIGURE 1. AIP MEDIATION MODELS.
200
Model 1: APIM with marital communication as outcome

34% 14%
External stress .58*** Relationship stress −.29*** Marital communi-
women (Xw) women (Mw) cation women (Yw) rw2

.03 rw1 -.14**


.26*** .42*** .39***
.07 rm1
-.16**
32% 12%
External stress .54*** Relationship stress -.25*** Marital communi-
men (Xm) men (Mm) cation men (Ym) rm2

Model 2: APIM with marital quality as outcome


34% 15%
External stress .58*** Relationship stress -.24*** Marital quality
women (Xw) women (Mw) women (Yw) rw2

.03 rw1 -.22***


.26*** .42*** .56***
.07 rm1
-.14**
32% 16%
External stress .54*** Relationship stress -.32*** Marital quality
men (Xm) men (Mm) men (Ym) rm2

Note: The mediation models with standardized parameters testing the association between external daily stress and marital functioning via daily relationship stress. w = women,
m = men. Percentages denote explained variances. **p < .01. ***p < .001.
Family Relations
Stress and Communication 201

effects between external stress and relationship 2


and men (χDiff (1) = 1.77, p = 1.84 for actor
stress were positive, which means that the effects; χDiff (1) = 1.32, p = .251 for partner
2
higher the external stress, the higher the effects). These findings suggest that marital com-
relationship stress. As expected, actor effects munication in conflict situations and women’s
were significantly greater in size than the marital quality seem to be similarly affected by
corresponding nonsignificant partner effects on both one’s own perceived relationship stress and
women’s and men’s relationship stress (chi- the partner’s relationship stress; this explained
2
square difference test: χDiff (1) = 49.47, p < up to 28% of the variance in the marital func-
.001 for the actor and partner effects on women’s tioning variables.
2
relationship stress; χDiff (1) = 44.94, p < .001
for the actor and partner effects on men’s Testing the indirect effects. In each API
relationship stress). This result indicates that mediation model, there were eight indirect
daily relationship stress was affected mainly by effects. The indirect effects that involve one
one’s own external daily stress rather than by of the nonsignificant partner effects between
the partner’s external daily stress and provides external stress and relationship stress were not
support for what Kenny and Cook (1999) called tested for significance because it makes less
an actor-only pattern. When we compared the sense to say mediation exists if not both direct
size of women’s and men’s actor effects, there effects are substantial that constitute an indirect
2
was no significant gender difference (χDiff (1) = effect (see Ledermann & Macho, 2009). The
0.584, p = .445). The external daily stress estimates of the remaining four indirect effects
variables accounted for 34% of the variance in per model are listed in Table 2. In accordance
women’s relationship stress and 32% in men’s with our hypothesis, all four indirect effects were
relationship stress. significant in both models, indicating that one’s
Consistent with our assumption, all actor and own relationship stress mediates the association
partner effects between relationship stress and between one’s own external stress and one’s
marital functioning were negative in both the own and the partner’s marital communication
models, indicating that marital functioning was in conflict situations and marital quality. That
negatively associated with relationship stress. is, mediation seems to occur not only within
Specifically, in the model with marital com- a person (e.g., women’s external stress →
munication in conflict situations, partner effects women’s relationship stress → women’s marital
on women’s and men’s marital communication
were similar in magnitude to the corresponding
Table 2. Mediating Effects for the Actor-Partner
2
actor effects (χDiff (1) = 1.67, p = .196 for the
Interdependence Models (APIMs) with External Daily
actor and partner effects on women’s communi-
Stress as Independent Variables, Daily Relationship Stress
cation behavior and χDiff2
(1) = 1.47, p = .225
as Mediators, and Marital Functioning as Outcome
for the actor and partner effects on men’s com-
Variables
munication behavior). These results support a
couple pattern for both women’s and men’s mar- Effect Estimate SE z p
ital communication (see Kenny & Cook, 1999).
When we tested for gender differences, there APIM with marital communication as outcome
was no substantial difference between women’s ESw → RSw → MCw −0.18 0.04 −4.91 <.001
and men’s actor effect and between their partner ESw → RSw → MCm −0.10 0.03 −2.80 .005
2
effects (χDiff (1) = 0.042, p = .837 and χDiff2 ESm → RSm → MCw −0.09 0.04 −2.57 .010
(1) = 0.010, p = .921). Likewise, in the model ESm → RSm → MCm −0.16 0.04 −4.16 <.001
with marital quality as outcome variables, there APIM with marital quality as outcome
was no significant difference between the actor ESw → RSw → MQw −0.19 0.05 −4.11 <.001
and the partner effects on women’s marital qual- ESw → RSw → MQm −0.11 0.04 −2.57 .010
2
ity (χDiff (1) = 0.09, p = .924), which again ESm → RSm → MQw −0.18 0.05 −3.75 <.001
suggests a couple pattern. In contrast, the actor ESm → RSm → MQm −0.26 0.05 −5.22 <.001
effect on men’s marital quality was stronger Note: ES = external stress; RS = relationship stress;
than the partner effect on men’s marital quality MQ = marital quality; MC = marital communication; w =
2
(χDiff (1) = 4.938, p = .026). When we tested women; m = men; SE = Standard error. Equation 1 was
for gender differences, again actor and partner used to compute z-scores; the denominator of Equation 1
effects did not differ in size between women is the SE.
202 Family Relations

quality) but also through the partner effects we set all factor loadings of the three latent vari-
between relationship stress and marital function ables relationship stress, marital communication,
(e.g., women’s external stress → women’s and marital quality to 1.
relationship stress → men’s marital quality).
These findings support the first hypothesis that Selection of a model. We started with the CFM
in both women and men the association between indicating partial mediation, which provided a
one’s own external stress and marital functioning good fit (χ 2 (3) = 3.145; p = .718; CF = 1.000;
is mediated by one’s own relationship stress and RMSEA < .001). Thus, this model indicating
that the association between one’s own external partial mediation was used for the subsequent
stress and the partner’s marital functioning is tests.
mediated by one’s own relationship stress. Testing the direct effects. In the CF mediation
model, all direct effects between the three latent
Mediation at the Level of the Dyads variables were significant (see Figure 2). This
result supports the assumption that the higher
The finding that actor and partner effects the daily relationship stress, the lower both
between relationship stress and the marital func- the marital communication in conflict situations
tioning variables were significant and quite and marital quality. The explained variance
similar in magnitude warrants the implementa- of marital communication through relationship
tion of these dyadic variables in a CF mediation stress was 37%, whereas relationship stress and
model and the test of the second hypothesis marital communication accounted together for
that the association between relationship stress 51% of the variance in marital quality.
and marital quality is partially mediated by
marital communication in conflict situations at Testing the indirect effect. For testing the
the dyadic level (see Figure 2). To test the model, indirect effect, Equation 1 was used, revealing

FIGURE 2. CF MEDIATION MODEL.

-.10 -.15

-.19* -.04 -.10 -.01

e3 e4

44% 48%
MC MC
e1 e2 women men e5 e6
(Mw) (Mm)

36% 47% .66 .70 63% 61%


RS RS 37% rM RQ RQ
women men Marital women men
(Xw) (Xm) Communication (Yw) (Ym)
(M)
.60 .69
-.61*** .51*** .79 .78
51% rY
Relationship -.27* Marital
stress quality
(X) (Y)

Note: The Common Fate Mediator Model with standardized coefficients testing the association between daily relationship
stress and marital quality via marital communication. RS = daily relationship stress; MC = marital communication;
MQ = marital quality; w = women; m = men. Percentages denote explained variances. *p < .05. ***p < .001.
Stress and Communication 203

a significant effect, ab = −0.39, SE = 0.11, presence of the actor-only pattern between exter-
z = −3.45, p = .001. Because the mediation is nal stress and relationship stress. Our results
partial, the relative proportion of the mediation provide further evidence that both men’s and
effect on the total effect was computed, yielding women’s marital communication in conflict sit-
that 51% of the association between relationship uations and women’s marital quality seem to
stress and marital quality is mediated by be affected to the same degree by one’s own
marital communication in conflict situations. To relationship stress and the partner’s relationship
compare the indirect effect with the direct effect stress; this finding supports the couple-oriented
X → Y , Equation 2 was employed. This test pattern, although men’s marital quality seems to
verifies that the indirect effect was as strong be more strongly affected by their own relation-
as the direct effect X → Y (ab − c = −0.01, ship stress than that of the partner.
SE = 0.25, z = −0.05, p < .961). In sum, the The finding that actor and partner effects
findings support the second hypothesis that the between relationship stress, marital communi-
association between daily relationship stress cation in conflict situations, and marital quality
and marital quality is partially mediated at were substantial and quite similar in size war-
the dyadic level by marital communication in rants the implementation of these variables in
conflict situations. a CFM. The results of the CF mediation model
In the final step, we extended the CF support our second hypothesis that the associa-
mediation model by including external daily tion between daily relationship stress and marital
stress as independent variables that influence quality is partially mediated at the dyadic level
relationship stress at the dyadic level. This by marital communication in conflict situations,
expanded model, however, was not consistent which means that marital quality seems to be
with the data (χ 2 (13) = 160.7, p < .001; CFI = affected by daily relationship stress directly as
.812; RMSEA = .182). well as indirectly through marital communica-
tion. This indicates that both relationship stress
DISCUSSION and marital communication in conflict situa-
tions have an effect on the quality of intimate
The aim of this study was to investigate the asso-
relationships.
ciation among variables focusing on daily stress,
In summary, our results provide evidence
marital communication in conflict situations, and
marital quality in consideration of stress that for the mediational mechanism between stress
is external to the relationship and relationship and marital functioning and support findings of
stress, at the individual and dyadic levels. Build- previous research showing that daily stress plays
ing on the stress model proposed by Bodenmann a central role for the understanding of martial
(2000) and Karney and Bradbury (1995) and discord (Karney & Bradbury, 1995; Morokoff
the hypotheses tested by Matthews et al. (1996), & Gillilland, 1993; Neff & Karney, 2004). The
three dyadic mediation models were tested. The findings of this study reveal that daily stress
results of the API mediation models support originating inside the relationship appears to
our first hypothesis that the association between be a highly salient characteristic of intimate
external daily stress and marital functioning is relationships for three reasons. First, daily
mediated by relationship stress at the level of relationship stress seems to mediate the effect
the dyad members. Specifically, in both women of daily external stress on marital functioning.
and men, one’s own relationship stress medi- Secondly, daily relationship stress tends to
ated the association between one’s own external affect both one’s own and the partner’s marital
stress and one’s own as well as the partner’s communication and marital quality. Finally,
marital quality and marital communication in evidence suggests that daily relationship stress
conflict situations. In line with results reported influences marital quality not only indirectly
by Bodenmann et al. (2007), there is evidence through marital communication but also directly.
that one’s own external stress spills over into The evaluation and understanding of media-
intimate relationships by exacerbating one’s own tion processes in psychology are important as
relationship stress, rather than the relationship they can reveal information about the signifi-
stress of the partner (see also Bolger et al., 1989; cance of direct and indirect associations among
Repetti, 1989; Repetti & Wood, 1997; Schulz multiple variables and provide clues about where
et al., 2004). Indeed, evidence was found for the it is appropriate to intervene. The mediation
204 Family Relations

results reported above allow the following con- 1990; MacCallum, Wegener, Uchiono, & Fab-
clusions. To improve or further enhance marital rigar, 1993). Secondly, the results are based on
functioning, people should try to reduce and a convenience sample of well-educated Swiss
cope—individually or dyadically—with both couples, which limits the generalizability of
high levels of external stress that tends to spill the findings. Thirdly, because of the use of
over into the relationship and high levels of rela- self-report measures, personality variables and
tionship stress. To reduce the level of external socially desirable response behavior may have
stress, employers are required to provide safe biased the reported estimates. This problem can
working conditions and fair wages. In addition, be alleviated by using observational methods.
governmental and other social service programs Finally, stability over time in the level of mar-
should pay special attention to the needs of ital outcomes and variation of stress could not
low-income couples and help them to overcome be taken into account in this study. Thus, no
external strains, as they often experience more claims can be made about how an enduring ver-
stress and face greater problems in building and sus a temporally acute high stress level might
maintaining a healthy intimate relationship than influence marital functioning.
better off couples. Finding effective ways to deal In conclusion, this study provided support
with stress occurring inside the relationship is for the actor-only pattern between external
important to stave off deterioration of marital stress and relationship stress and the couple-
functioning on both the individual and dyadic oriented pattern between relationship stress and
levels. Couple programs that teach coping skills, marital communication. It also demonstrated
such as the couple coping enhancement training that relationship stress acts in concert with
(Bodenmann & Shantinath, 2004; Ledermann, marital communication to affect marital quality,
Bodenmann, & Cina, 2007) or the mindfulness- and suggests that improvements in marital
based relationship enhancement, have demon- communication and reduction of the perceived
strated promising results in improving aspects of relationship stress in both partners can prevent
marital functioning (Carson, Carson, Gil, & Bau- deterioration of marital harmony.
com, 2004). Couples who experience high rela-
tionship distress may consult a couple therapist
or counselor to improve coping strategies such as REFERENCES
active coping, support seeking, distraction, and Banmen, J., & Vogel, N. A. (1985). The relationship
disengagement (Carver, Scheier, & Weintraub, between marital quality and interpersonal sexual
1989; Skinner, Edge, Altman, & Sherwood, communication. Family Therapy, 12, 45 – 58.
2003). In light of the fact that good communica- Bodenmann, G. (2000). Stress und Coping bei
tion is essential for a healthy intimate relation- Paaren [Stress and coping in couples]. Göttingen:
ship, couples should be aware of how important Hogrefe.
communication skills are for a long-lasting rela- Bodenmann, G., Ledermann, T., Blattner, D., &
tionship and may improve their marital skills and Galluzzo, C. (2006). Association between every-
day stress, critical life events, and sexual problems.
enrich their relationship by participating in a cou-
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 194,
ple training such as the Premarital Relationship 494 – 501.
Enhancement Program (Hahlweg & Markman, Bodenmann, G., Ledermann, T., & Bradbury, T. N.
1988; Hahlweg, Markman, Thurmaier, Engl, & (2007). Stress, sex, and satisfaction in marriage.
Eckert, 1998) or the PREPARE/ENRICH Pro- Personal Relationships, 14, 551 – 569.
gram (Olson & Olson-Sigg, 1999). Bodenmann, G., & Shantinath, S. D. (2004). The
The findings of this study need to be treated Couples Coping Enhancement Training (CCET):
with some caution. First, it is not possible to dis- A new approach to prevention of marital distress
cern the direction of the associations between the based upon stress and coping. Family Relations,
variables because (a) the cross-sectional data do 53, 477 – 484.
not allow the determination of causality and Bolger, N., DeLongis, A., Kessler, R. C., & Wething-
ton, E. (1989). The contagion of stress across
(b) statistically equivalent models (i.e., alter- multiple roles. Journal of Marriage and the Fam-
native models that fit the data equally well) ily, 51, 175 – 183.
exist for the models tested in this study, as, for Bradbury, T. N., & Karney, B. R. (2004). Under-
instance, the API mediation model with exter- standing and altering the longitudinal course of
nal stress as mediator and internal stress as marriage. Journal of Marriage and Family, 66,
independent variable (see Lee & Hershberger, 862 – 879.
Stress and Communication 205

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine,
ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen 4, 1 – 39.
& J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation Karney, B. R., & Bradbury, T. N. (1995). The longi-
models (pp. 136 – 162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. tudinal course of marital quality and stability: A
Carrère, S., Buehlman, K. T., Gottman, J. M., Coan, review of theory, method, and research. Psycho-
J. A., & Ruckstuhl, L. (2000). Predicting marital logical Bulletin, 118, 3 – 34.
stability and divorce in newlywed couples. Journal Karney, B. R., Story, L. B., & Bradbury, T. N. (2005).
of Family Psychology, 14, 42 – 58. Marriages in context: Interactions between
Carson, J. W., Carson, K. M., Gil, K. M., & Baucom, chronic and acute stress among newlyweds.
D. H. (2004). Mindfulness-based relationship In T. A. Revenson, K. Kayser, & G. Bodenmann
enhancement. Behavior Therapy, 35, 471 – 494. (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on couples’ coping
Carver, C. S., Scheier, M. F., & Weintraub, J. K. with stress (pp. 13 – 32). Washington, DC: APA.
(1989). Assessing coping strategies: A theoreti- Kenny, D. A. (1996). Models of non-independence in
cally based approach. Journal of Personality and dyadic research. Journal of Social and Personal
Social Psychology, 56, 267 – 283. Relationships, 13, 279 – 294.
Conger, R. D., Elder, G. H., Jr., Lorenz, F. O., Con- Kenny, D. A., & Cook, W. (1999). Partner effects in
ger, K. J., Simons, R. L., Whitbeck, L. B., Huck, S. relationship research: Conceptual issues, analytic
& Melby, J. N. (1990). Linking economic hard- difficulties, and illustrations. Personal Relation-
ship to marital quality and instability. Journal of ship, 6, 433 – 448.
Marriage and the Family, 52, 643 – 656. Kenny, D. A., & La Voie, L. (1985). Separating indi-
Crouter, A. C., Perry-Jerkins, M., Huston, T. L., & vidual and group effects. Journal of Personality
Crawford, D. W. (1989). The influence of work- and Social Psychology, 48, 339 – 348.
induced psychological states on behavior at Ledermann, T., & Bodenmann, G. (2006). Moderator-
home. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 10, und Mediatoreffekte bei dyadischen Daten: Zwei
273 – 292. Erweiterungen des Akteur-Partner-Interdependenz-
Cutrona, C. E., Russell, D. W., Abraham, W. T., Modells [Moderator and mediator effects in dyadic
Gardner, K. A., Melby, J. N., Bryant, C., & Con-
research: Two extensions of the Actor-Partner
ger, R. D. (2003). Neighborhood context and
Interdependence Model]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsy-
financial strain as predictors of marital interaction
chologie, 37, 27 – 40.
and marital quality in African American couples.
Ledermann, T., Bodenmann, G., & Cina, A. (2007).
Personal Relationships, 10, 389 – 409.
The efficacy of the Couples Coping Enhancement
Gottman, J. M. (1994). What predicts divorce?
Training (CCET) in improving relationship qual-
The relationship between marital processes and
marital outcomes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ity. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26,
Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital 940 – 959.
processes predictive of later dissolution: Behavior, Ledermann, T., & Macho, S. (2009). Mediation in
physiology, and health. Journal of Personality and dyadic data at the level of the dyads: A Structural
Social Psychology, 63, 221 – 233. Equation Modeling approach. Journal of Family
Hahlweg, K. (1996). Fragebogen zur Partnerschafts- Psychology, 23, 661 – 670.
diagnostik (FPD) [Partnership questionnaire]. Lee, D., & Hershberger, S. (1990). A simple rule for
Göttingen: Hogrefe. generating equivalent models in covariance struc-
Hahlweg, K., & Markman, H. J. (1988). Effectiveness ture modeling. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
of behavioral marital therapy: Empirical status of 25, 313 – 334.
behavioral techniques in preventing and alleviating Leidy, M. S., Parke, R. D., Cladis, M., Coltrane, S.,
marital distress. Journal of Consulting and Clinical & Duffy, S. (2009). Positive marital quality,
Psychology, 56, 440 – 447. acculturative stress, and child outcomes among
Hahlweg, K., Markman, H. J., Thurmaier, F., Engl, J., Mexican Americans. Journal of Marriage and
& Eckert, V. (1998). Prevention of marital distress: Family, 71, 833 – 847.
Results of a German prospective longitudi- MacCallum, R. C., Wegener, D. T., Uchiono, B. N.,
nal study. Journal of Family Psychology, 12, & Fabrigar, L. R. (1993). The problem of equiva-
543 – 556. lent models in applications of covariance structure
Howe, G., Levy, M., & Caplan, R. (2004). Job loss analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 185 – 199.
and depressive symptoms in couples: Common MacKinnon, D. P. (2000). Contrasts in multiple medi-
stressors, stress transmission, or relationship ator models. In J. Rose, L. Chassin, C. C. Presson,
disruption? Journal of Family Psychology, 18, & S. J. Sherman (Eds.), Multivariate applications
639 – 650. in substance use research: New methods for new
Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, questions (pp. 141 – 160). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
R. S. (1981). Comparison of two modes of stress Matthews, L. S., Conger, R. D., & Wickrama,
measurements: Daily hassles and uplifts versus K. A. S. (1996). Work-family conflict and marital
206 Family Relations

quality: Mediating processes. Social Psychology Rogge, R. D., & Bradbury, T. N. (1999). Till violence
Quarterly, 59, 62 – 79. does us part: The differing roles of communication
Miller, N. B., & Kannae, L. A. (1999). Predicting and aggression in predicting adverse marital
marital quality in Ghana. Journal of Comparative outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Family Studies, 30, 599 – 615. Psychology, 67, 340 – 351.
Morokoff, P. J., & Gillilland, R. (1993). Stress, sexual Rogge, R. D., Bradbury, T. N., Hahlweg, K., Engl, J.,
functioning, and marital satisfaction. Journal of & Thurmaier, F. (2006). Predicting marital dis-
Sex Research, 30, 43 – 53. tress and dissolution: Refining the two-factor
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2004). How does hypothesis. Journal of Family Psychology, 20,
context affect intimate relationships? Linking 156 – 159.
external stress and cognitive processes within Schulz, M. S., Cowan, P. A., Cowan, C. P., & Bren-
marriage. Personality and Social Psychology nan, R. T. (2004). Coming home upset: Gender,
Bulletin, 30, 134 – 148. marital satisfaction, and the daily spillover of
Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2007). Stress crossover workday experience into couples interactions.
in newlywed marriage: A longitudinal and dyadic Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 250 – 263.
perspective. Journal of Marriage and Family, 69, Skinner, E. A., Edge, K., Altman, J., & Sherwood, H.
594 – 607. (2003). Searching for the structure of coping:
Olson, D. H., & Olson-Sigg, A. K. (1999). PRE- A review and critique of category systems for
PARE/ENRICH Program: Version 2000. In classifying ways of coping. Psychological Bulletin,
R. Berger & M. Hannah (Eds.), Handbook 129, 216 – 269.
of preventative approach in couple therapy Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals
(pp. 196 – 216). New York: Brunner/Mazel. for indirect effects in structural equation models.
Randall, A. K., & Bodenmann, G. (2009). The role of In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological methodology
stress on close relationships and marital satisfac- (pp. 290 – 312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
tion. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 105 – 115. Sprecher, S., Metts, S., Burleson, B., Hatfield, E., &
Repetti, R. L. (1989). Effects of daily workload on Thompson, A. (1995). Domains of expressive
subsequent behavior during marital interaction: interaction in intimate relationships: Associations
The roles of social withdrawal and spouse support. with satisfaction and commitment. Family Rela-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, tions: Journal of Applied Family and Child Studies,
651 – 659. 44, 203 – 210.
Repetti, R. L., & Wood, J. (1997). Effects of daily Williams, L. M. (1995). Association of stressful life
stress at work on mother’s interactions with presch- events and marital quality. Psychological Reports,
oolers. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 90 – 108. 76, 1115 – 1122.

You might also like