You are on page 1of 14

Product/Consumption-Based Affective Responses and Postpurchase Processes

Author(s): Robert A. Westbrook


Source: Journal of Marketing Research, Vol. 24, No. 3 (Aug., 1987), pp. 258-270
Published by: American Marketing Association
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3151636 .
Accessed: 17/11/2013 15:52

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.
JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of
content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms
of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

American Marketing Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to
Journal of Marketing Research.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
ROBERTA. WESTBROOK*

The authorexaminesconsumeraffective responsesto product/consumptionex-


periencesand their relationshipto selected aspects of postpurchaseprocesses. In
separatefield studiesof automobileownersand CATVsubscribers,subjectsreported
the natureand frequencyof emotionalexperiencesin connectionwith productown-
ershipand usage. Analysisconfirmshypothesesabout the existenceof independent
dimensionsof positiveand negative affect. Both dimensionsof affective response
are found directlyrelated to the favorabilityof consumersatisfactionjudgnments,
extent of seller-directedcomplaintbehavior, and extent of word-of-mouthtrans-
mission.

Product/Consumption-B Affective Responses


and PostpurchaseProcesses

Traditional models of consumer behavior in the mar- glected (for noteworthy exceptions, see Holbrook et al.
keting literature implicitly assume that consumer deci- 1984; Westbrook 1980). This neglect is surprising for
sion making is principally the result of cognitive pro- several reasons. First, the postpurchaseperiod comprises
cesses involving the semantic meaning of product product ownership and usage, both of which might be
attributes(e.g., Bettman 1979; Howard and Sheth 1969). expected to provide opportunities for varied affective re-
Affective processes, broadly described as those involv- sponse of considerable personal significance to con-
ing subjective feelings, generally are relegated to a sec- sumers (Holbrook and Hirschman 1982). Second, affec-
ondary role (Holbrook and Hirschman 1982; Peterson, tive variablesmight be expected to contributesubstantially
Hoyer, and Wilson 1986). However, recent advances in to the explanation and prediction of postpurchase be-
social cognition, cognitive psychology, and social psy- havior, which in addition to product usage includes sat-
chology suggest that affective processes may constitute isfactionappraisal,seller-directedcomplaintactions, word-
not only a powerful source of human motivation, but of-mouth transmission, disposition behavior, and repur-
also a major influence on information processing and chase planning. Typically, accounts of these processes
choice (Hoffman 1986; Isen 1984; Zajonc 1980). As a have been limited to cognitive/semantic belief variables
result, marketers are increasingly interested in under- (e.g., Beardenand Teel 1983; Oliver 1980; Richins 1983).
standing the nature of affect and its contribution to con- The inclusion of product/consumption affective re-
sumer decision making and response to marketing vari- sponses therefore might be expected to extend the ex-
ables (Aaker, Stayman, and Hagerty 1985; Batra and Ray planatory ability of current theoretical postpurchase
1986; Donovan and Rossiter 1982; Gardner 1985; Wein- models.
berg and Gottwald 1982). Accordingly, the role of product/consumption-based
Despite a growing number of conceptual and empiri- affective responses is examined in three central forms of
cal studies of consumer affect, most attention to date has postpurchase behavior: satisfaction appraisal, seller-di-
been on its role in consumer prepurchaseprocesses. Af- rected complaint actions, and word-of-mouth transmis-
fect in postpurchase processes has been relatively ne- sions. Satisfaction appraisal usually is regarded as the
central mediator of postpurchase behavior, linking pre-
choice product beliefs to postchoice cognitive structure,
*Robert A. Westbrook is Professor of Marketing, College of Busi- consumer communications, and repurchase behavior
ness and Public Administration, The University of Arizona.
The author gratefully acknowledges the many constructive criti-
(Beardenand Teel 1983; LaBarberaand Mazursky 1983;
cisms provided by three anonymous JMR reviewers.
Oliver 1980). Complaint behavior is of interest as an
important form of market feedback to organizations
258

Journal of Marketing Research


Vol. XXIV (August 1987), 258-70

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE 259

(Fornell and Westbrook 1984). Finally, postpurchase the contemporary emotionsliterature(Izard1977).


word-of-mouthtransmissionsare relevantas a signifi- Of the varietyof affectiveresponsesin Table 1, which
cantnon-marketer-dominatedpurchaseinfluence(Arndt ones might be particularlyrelevantto consumerpost-
1967;Dichter1966). purchaseprocesses?Some insight is gained by consid-
ering the theoreticalmechanismspostulatedto explain
THEORY the elicitationof differenttypes of affect (for a review,
see Hoffman1986). One means throughwhich differ-
entialaffectelicitationis believedto occuris the process
Affective Response
of cognitive appraisal(Arnold 1960; Lazarus,Kanner,
Thoughaffect has been defined variouslyin the lit- andFolkman1980;Weiner,Russell, andLerman1979).
erature,the conceptgenerallyis understoodto comprise In essence, affects are held to arise as a functionof the
a class of mentalphenomenauniquelycharacterizedby individual'sevaluationof the meaning, causes, conse-
a consciouslyexperienced,subjectivefeeling state,com- quences, and/or personal implicationsof a particular
monly accompanyingemotionsand moods. Many vari- stimulus.
etiesof affectcan be discerned,as evidencedby the large Considerthe variousunpleasant,negativelyvalenced
numberof wordsin the Englishlanguagethatdenotedif- fundamental affectsin Table 1 andtheirunderlyingcog-
ferent feeling states (Davitz 1969). However, several nitiveappraisals.Anger, disgust, and contempteach re-
taxonomieshavebeen proposedto classify the varietyof flect an underlyingattributionof causal agency to the
subjectivefeelings into a small set of fundamental,or elicitingstimulus,guilt andshamedeflectthe attribution
primaryaffects (Izard 1977; Plutchik 1980; Tomkins for causalagencyto the individualhim- or herself, and
1980).Thesetaxonomiesarein substantialagreementon fearandsadnessinvolve an assignmentof causalagency
the basic categoriesof affective experience(for a com- neitherto the stimulusnor the individualbut insteadto
parison,see Ekman,Friesen, and Ellsworth1982). Ta- the situation(Izard1977; Scherer1982; Smithand Ells-
ble 1 lists the fundamentalaffects identifiedby one em- worth1985). In the contextof product/consumption ex-
piricallybasedtaxonomythathas been well receivedin periences,onlythoseaffectsinvolvingattribution of causal
agencyto the productor its seller might be expectedto
have a systematicinfluence on postpurchasebehavior
Table 1 relativeto the product.
IZARD'S
(1977) TAXONOMY
OF AFFECTIVE
EXPERIENCE As an illustration,consideran unfavorableconsump-
tionexperience,suchas a majorenginefailuresoon after
Funda- expirationof an automobile'swarranty.If causalagency
mental Natureof is assignedto the automobileand/or its manufacturer-
affect subjectiveexperience Valence that is, if the consumerbelieves the problemis due to
Interest Engaged,attentive,caught-up,curious, Positive defectivedesign or manufacture-affectsof anger, dis-
fascinated;when intense, a feeling of gust, and contemptare likely to be aroused, possibly
excitementand animation
Joy Sense of confidenceand significance; Positive motivatingdemandsfor free repairor replacement.If
feeling loved and lovable;a good re-
insteadcause is assignedto oneself-that is, if the con-
lationshipto the objectof joy sumerbelieves he or she failed to maintainthe engine
Anger Hostility,desireto attackthe sourceof Negative properlyor heed its operatinginstructions-affects of
anger,physicalpower, impulsiveness guilt and shameare likely to be aroused,perhapsstim-
Disgust Feelingsof revulsion;impulsesto es- Negative
cape from or removethe objectof ulatingfurthercognitionabout how the problemcould
disgustfromproximityto oneself have been averted.Finally, if cause is attributedto sit-
Contempt Superiorityto otherpeople, groups,or Negative uationalfactorssuch as an earlierunavoidableaccident
things;hostility(mild);prejudice; thatdamagedthe engine and led to the presentfailure,
coldnessand distance affects of fear and sadness would tend to be aroused,
Distress Sadness;discouragement,downhearted- Negative
ness; lonelinessand isolation;feeling possibly stimulatingwithdrawalbehavior such as dis-
miserable;sense of loss posingof the vehicle.
Fear Apprehensionto terror,dependingon Negative In relationto the positive affects in Table 1, previous
intensity;sense of imminentdanger; researchindicatesthat pleasantemotionalexperienceis
feeling unsafe;slowed thought;ten- thatis, linkeddirectlyto
sion invariablyoutcome-dependent,
Shame Suddenlyheightenedself-consciousness, Negative an elicitingstimuluswith no furtherattributionalsearch
self-awareness;feelings of incompe- (Weiner,Russell, andLerman1979). Hencejoy and in-
tence, indignity,defeat;in mild form, terest, once elicited in a consumptionexperience, are
shyness necessarilyinvolvedin behaviorrelevantto the stimulus.
Guilt Gnawingfeelings of being in the wrong, Negative
"notright"with othersor the self Finally,the affectof surprisealso is likely to be involved
Surprise Fleetingsense of interruption of ongoing Neutral in stimulus-relevant behaviorbecause of its theoretical
thought;brief uncertainty;amazement role as an amplifierof eitherpositive or negativeemo-
and startle tionalexperience(Izard1977; Tomkins 1980).

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
260 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,
Dimensions of Product- and Consumption-Based pected) to neutral (obtained outcomes exactly meet those
Affective Response expected) to negative (obtained outcomes fall short of
those expected). In the second stage, expectancy dis-
Though the phenomenology of product ownership and confirmationbeliefs and initial expectation beliefs, as re-
consumption involves discernible states of joy, surprise, called from memory, are combined additively to produce
anger, and so on, the relationship of affect to postpur- the satisfaction evaluation.
chase behaviorcan be conceived parsimoniouslyin terms As a global evaluative judgment about product usage/
of the dimensions of meaning underlying these differ-
entiated affects. Considerable research has addressed the consumption, however, satisfaction judgments logically
should be determined at least in part by the occurrence
dimensionality of affective experience (for reviews, see of product/consumption-related affective responses in
Daly, Lancee, and Polivy 1983; Smith and Ellsworth addition to the effects of cognitive/semantic belief vari-
1985). In essence, dimensional analyses treat discrete ables. Though subjective experiences of product/con-
types of affect as salient, familiar regions of a multidi- sumption affect may be relatively transient during the
mensional psychological space of affective response.
The major structural dimension of affective experi- postpurchase period, they also can be highly salient in
consciousness, depending on intensity (Izard 1977;
ence often is found to be the ubiquitous bipolar contin- Tomkins 1980), which facilitates their retrieval from
uum of pleasantness-unpleasantness(e.g., Russell 1983).
memory (Robinson 1976, 1980; Waldfogel 1948). Thus
However, research on personal reports of individual af-
fective experiences has indicated two largely indepen- past affective responses may be available to exert effects
on the evaluative processes yielding satisfaction judg-
dent unipolar dimensions corresponding to positive and ments. Abelson et al. (1982) provide empirical support,
negative affect (Abelson et al. 1982; Bradburn 1969). in the context of political candidate preferences, for the
Because the bidimensional conceptualization allows for
ambivalence or the joint occurrence of pleasant and un- proposition that evaluative judgments may be influenced
by both affective responses and cognitive beliefs. More-
pleasant states, as well as indifference or the occurrence over, the two dimensions of positive and negative af-
of neither pleasant nor unpleasant states, it appears more fective response were related independently to prefer-
suitable than the unidimensional view for investigating ence formation,the formerdirectlyand the latterinversely.
product ownership and consumption experiences. Isen Hence,
(1984) likewise has argued for a bidimensional concep-
tualization of affect in everyday situations that might in- H2: The frequencyof positive product/consumption af-
volve only mild, "everyday" affect ("feelings"). Thus it fective responseis relateddirectlyto judgmentsof
is hypothesized that: satisfactionwith the product.
H3: The frequencyof negativeproduct/consumption af-
H1: Consumeraffective response relevant to postpur- fective responseis relatedinverselyto judgmentsof
chase processes can be describedby separateuni- satisfactionwith the product.
polardimensionsof positive and negativeaffect. H4: Neitherexpectationnor disconfirmationbeliefs me-
diatethe relationshipbetweenpositive and negative
In terms of the fundamental classes of affect in Table 1, product/consumption-based affective response and
the positive dimension is expected to comprise product/ judgmentsof satisfaction.
consumption experiences involving joy and interest. The
Complaint Behavior
negative dimension is expected to encompass experi-
ences of anger, disgust, and contempt. Both positive and Postpurchase complaining behavior comprises con-
negative affect dimensions may contain the affect of sur- sumer-initiated communications to marketers, their
prise, for reasons noted before. channel members, or public agencies to obtain remedy
or restitution for purchase- or usage-related problems in
Satisfaction particularmarkettransactions. Research has shown com-
plaint behavior to be chiefly the result of judged dissat-
As described by Hunt (1977, p. 459), "Satisfaction is isfaction with the product and/or its consumption (e.g.,
not the pleasurableness of the [consumption] experience Bearden and Teel 1983; Richins 1983), that is, to be
..it is the evaluation renderedthat the experience was relatedinversely to satisfactionappraisal.Day (1984) has
at least as good as it was supposed to be." Thus it can proposed, however, that the actual source of complaint
be distinguished from cognitive beliefs about product/ motivation is not the judgment of (dis)satisfaction per
consumption outcomes and from affective responses to se, but rather the antecedent negative emotional state
the outcomes. Satisfaction appraisal is believed to occur produced by the appraisal of unfavorable product/con-
as a two-stage process (Oliver 1980). In the first stage, sumption outcomes. Positive affective responses, being
post-usage beliefs about product attributes or outcomes independentof their negative counterparts,do not reduce
actually realized are compared with prepurchase expec- or offset any subsequent negative affects during con-
tations, yielding a new belief about the extent of expec- sumption; thus complaint behavior theoretically is un-
tancy disconfirmation. Expectancy disconfirmation can related to positive product/consumption-based affective
range from positive (obtained outcomes exceed those ex- responses.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE 261

The foregoing notions suggest that (dis)satisfaction determinants andelementsin suchcommunications(All-


mediatesthe influenceof negative affect on complaint port and Postman 1947).
behavior.However,negativeaffect also seems likely to The foregoingconceptualizationsuggests that salient
influencecomplaintactionsdirectlybecauseoveralldis- affectiveresponsesof eithervalencemay stimulatecon-
satisfactionwith a productis not a requisitefor voicing sumerWOM transmissions.Though empiricalstudies
complaints;even satisfied consumerscomplainto note offer conflictingevidence on the directionof the rela-
minorconcernsor problems(JacobyandJaccard1981). tionshipbetweenthe favorabilityof postpurchaseeval-
Thusproductoutcomescan promptnegativeaffect, trig- uationsand the extent of WOM transmission(Engel,
geringcomplaining,thoughthe net effect of such neg- Kergerreis,andBlackwell 1969;Holmesand Lett 1977;
ative affect on satisfactionmay be outweighedby other TARP1981), nonehas controlledfor the effects of prod-
occurrencesof positive affect, high levels of expecta- uct/consumption-based affectiveresponses,which may
tions, or positive disconfirmationbeliefs. have confoundedobserved results. Because affective
Findingsin the consumercomplaintliteratureare con- variablesare believed to occur temporallypriorto sat-
sistent with the foregoingtheoreticalmodel. External, isfactiondetermination,their effects on WOM may be
product-based attributionsof causalagency for unfavor- considereddirectand unmediated.Hence,
able productoutcomesare relateddirectlyto increased H8: The frequenciesof both positive and negativeprod-
complaintactivity (Krishnanand Valle 1979; Richins uct/consumption-based affectsarerelateddirectlyto
1983) and such attributionsalso increasethe likelihood the extent of postpurchaseword-of-mouthtransmis-
of feelings of angerin consumers(Folkes 1984). Three sion.
hypothesesare indicated.
Because product/consumption-basedaffective re-
H5: The extentof postpurchaseconsumercomplaintbe- sponsesareheldto influencebothsatisfactionandWOM
havior is a function of the frequencyof negative
affect. transmission,at least some of the latterrelationshipmay
product/consumption-based be due to their common emotionalantecedents.How-
H6: The extentof postpurchaseconsumercomplaintbe-
havioris unrelatedto the frequencyof positiveprod- ever, whetheror not the relationshipis solely the result
uct/consumption-based affect. of affectiveresponses-that is, completelyspurious-is
H7: The relationship between negative product/con- unknown.The situationis similarfor the relationshipbe-
sumptionaffectandcomplaintbehavioris not wholly tweencomplaintbehaviorand WOM.
mediatedby satisfactionjudgments.
METHOD
Word-of-MouthTransmission
In a postpurchasecontext, consumerword-of-mouth Sample and Procedure
(WOM)transmissionsconsist of informalcommunica- Becauseof limitedknowledgeof affective responses
tions directedat other consumersaboutthe ownership, in actualconsumptioncontexts, data were gatheredin
usage, or characteristics
of particulargoods and services field studies of consumersreportingon their personal
and/or their sellers. The determinantsof WOM trans- productownership/usageexperiencesand postpurchase
mission seldom have been studied,but theory suggests behavior.Such naturalisticsettingsare increasinglyfa-
these communicationsare the result of consumer in- voredfor examiningthe influenceof affectivephenom-
volvementin the product/usesituation(Dichter 1966). ena in "everyday"environments(Abelson et al. 1982;
Threedifferentstates of involvementhave been identi- Smithand Kluegel 1982).
fied: (1) productinvolvement,in which the user desires Two productcategorieswere selected for study be-
to talk about the purchaseand the gratificationsit af- cause of their potentialto evoke varioustypes of both
fords, (2) self-involvement,in which the user seeks to positive and negative affect in the course of product/
gainattention,recognition,or statusin tellingothersabout service usage: automobilesand cable pay television
the purchase,and (3) other-involvement, wherethe user (CATV)subscriptionservices.Suchvarianceis essential
seeks to help other consumersby sharinghis or her if emotionalresponsesare to be relatedeffectively to
knowledgeor experiences. postpurchasebehavior through correlationalanalysis
Each of these forms of involvementhas a substantial methodsnecessitatedby field studies. Automobilesare
affectivebasis. For example, productinvolvementmay associatedwith large variationsin consumerinvolve-
".. . produce a tension . . . not eased by use of the ment (Bloch 1981) and CATV services afford a wide
productalone but [which]mustbe channeledby way of rangeof psychologicalgratifications(McGuire1974).
talk, recommendation and enthusiasm . . . to provide Separatefield studiesfor each productcategorywere
relief" (Dichter1966, p. 148). Both self- and other-in- based on independentsurvey samplesrepresentativeof
volvementcomprise comparablysignificantemotional theirrespectiveconsumerpopulations.For CATV ser-
needs and/or experienceson the part of the potential vices, a self-weightingareaprobabilitysampleof house-
WOMsender.The literatureon public opinionrumors, holds in a U.S. metropolitanarea was used, yielding a
which are conceptuallysimilarto productWOM trans- total of 154 adultmale and female heads of household.
missions, has amply noted the centralityof emotional Respondentswere interviewedat home and in personby

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
262 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,
trainedprofessionalsurvey interviewers.For automo- relatedwith social desirabilityresponseset (Crowneand
biles, 200 vehicle ownersin the same metropolitanarea Marlowe1964) in the auto owner pilot study. Finally,
were obtainedby an area-basedjudgmentalsampling to confirmthatthe DES-IIsubscaleswere assessing af-
procedure.Respondentscompletedpersonallydelivered, fective responsesto specific product/consumptionout-
self-administered questionnairesthat were retrievedin comes as opposed to generalizedproductcategory af-
person within two daysof delivery.Bothsamplesmatched fect, in two pilot studies respondentswere asked to
closely their respectivepopulationson selected demo- indicateto what,if anything,theiraffectreportsrelated.2
graphicand productusage characteristics. At least65%of the respondentsin eachproductcategory
nameda specific attribute/outcome,or class thereof, in
Measures relationto each affect type. Moreover,the modalrefer-
ents differedsubstantiallyby DES-II subscale. For au-
Affect. Product/consumption-based affective re-
sponseswereassessedby usingthe DES-II(Izard1977), tomobiles,reportsof joy typicallywere associatedwith
a self-administered inventoryof discreteemotionalex- productperformancecharacteristicsand appearance;in-
terestwith upkeepand maintenance; anger,disgust,and/
periences(e.g., happy,scornful,amazed)to which sub- or contemptwith productmalfunctionand repairs;and
jects providerelativefrequencyreportsin relationto des-
ignatedexperiences,stimuli,or time periods.The DES- surprisewith specificunusualperformance outcomes.For
II consistsof 10 subscalescorrespondingto the funda- CATV,reportsof joy typicallywereassociatedwith spe-
mentaltypes of affect describedin Table 1. In effect, cific types of programming; interestwith particularpro-
the DES-II is an aided recall task employingaffective gramsandthe initiationof service;anger,disgust, and/
wordprompts;studiesof the retrievalof autobiograph- or contemptwith serviceinterruptions, installationprob-
ical memoryconfirmthe accessibilityof past emotional lems, andbillingerrors;and surprisewith programming
experiencesby means of such cues (Robinson 1976, variety.However,these resultsdo not rule out the pos-
1980).Substantial validationevidencefor the DES-IIhas sibilitythat the affective responseswere due to gener-
beenreported:(1) each of the DES-IIsubscalesprovides alizedcategory-basedaffect, becausethe namingof spe-
cific product/consumption outcomes might reflect
internallyreliable estimates of affect frequency, and
moreovereach converges with independentself-report cognitiveconsistencybiasesonce respondentshad named
indicatorsof emotionalstate (Izard1977), (2) the sub- a particularaffect.
scales demonstratediscriminantvalidity in distinguish- Descriptivestatistics,reliabilityestimates,and inter-
correlationsfor the six DES-II subscalesfrom the final
ing variedemotionalincidents(Bartlettand Izard 1972; auto and CATV field studies are reportedin Table 2.
McHugo, Ianzetta, and Smith 1982), and (3) self-re-
portsbased on DES-II scale descriptorsagree with ex- Thoughthe affect measuresagain have acceptablein-
ternalobservationsof subjects'emotionalfacial expres- ternalconsistencyreliabilityfor analyticalpurposes,the
sions (Ekman,Friesen, and Ancoli 1980). positive affects are substantiallyintercorrelated,as are
BothstudiesincludedDES-IIsubscalemeasuresof the thosecorrespondingto the negativeaffects. These find-
six types of affect relevantto postpurchaseprocesses ings suggestsome cautionaboutthe discriminantvalid-
(anger,disgust, contempt,interest,joy, and surprise). ity of the individualDES-IIsubscalesin relationto each
Beforethey were used in the final field studies, several otherin the product/consumption context.
checksweremadeon theirsuitabilityfor use in the prod- Postpurchase constructs. Measurement procedures for
uct consumptioncontext. After an exploratorystudyon postpurchaseconstructsof satisfaction,complainingbe-
a small conveniencesampleof consumersto assess the havior,and word-of-mouth,as well as for expectations
and disconfirmationbeliefs, are given in Table 3. Be-
feasibilityof retrospectiverecall of product-relatedaf- cause both criterionand predictormeasureswere ad-
fects, three pilot studies were conductedamong auto-
mobile owners (N = 66), new car shoppers (N = 162),
andnew CATVsubscribers(N = 98). The DES-IImea-
sures were found consistentlyreliable, exceeding .75 shoppers(N = 162) were significant(p < .05) for five of the six
DES-IIsubscales.No a priori predictionswere made for differences
(Cronbach'salpha)in all instances. betweenautomobileand CATV consumption.Two-tailedt-tests of
The pilot studies also indicatedthat the DES-II sub- mean affect ratingswere significant(p < .05) for four of the six
scales varied significantlyby productcategory (auto- subscales.Thesefindingswereregardedas an indicationof the ability
mobiles vs. CATV) and experientialfocus (shopping of the DES-IImeasuresto discriminateaffectivefrequencybetween
vs. consumption),necessarypreconditionsfor measure differentconsumercontexts.Thoughhelpfulfor the purposesof the
study,these data obviouslydo not providea comprehensiveassess-
validity.'Moreover,none of the six subscaleswas cor- mentof the discriminantvalidityof the individualDES-II subscales
as indicatorsof differentaffectiveresponsesin product/consumption
contexts.
2Thesedataalso provideempiricalsupportfor the analysisof affect
'Becauseproductusagegenerallyis considereda consummatory ac- categoriesrelevantto postpurchaseprocesses.Auto ownerstypically
tivity in whichconsumergoals or desiresare fulfilled, in contrastto namedaccidentaldamageor theftin relationto fearandsadness,and
shoppingwhichis an instrumental activityundertakento bringabout lack of routinemaintenancein relationto guilt and shame. CATV
suchconsummation,morefrequentreportsof bothpositiveand neg- subscribersnamedspecificdramaticprogramsin relationto affective
ative affect were predictedfor productusage experiencesthan for reportsof fear and sadness, and excessive televisionviewing in re-
shopping.One-tailedt-testsbetweenautoowners(N = 66) and auto lationto guilt and shame.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED 263

Table 2
STATISTICSFOR AFFECTMEASURESAND REGRESSIONMODELVARIABLE$
DESCRIPTIVE

Automobiles CATV
DES-II (N = 200) (N = 151) DES-IIsubscale correlations
measures M S M S 1 2 3 4 5 6
1. Joy 3.43 .91 2.78 .83 (.79, .69) .66 -.08 .10 .04 .60
2. Interest 3.25 .91 2.85 .88 .76 (.78, .74) -.02 .07 .09 .58
3. Anger 1.48 .77 2.00 .91 -.43 -.22 (.89, .80) .62 .66 .22
4. Disgust 1.45 .70 2.14 .86 -.28 -.27 .66 (.89, .76) .57 .30
5. Contempt 1.33 .69 1.56 .78 -.25 -.15 .67 .82 (.76, .86) .30
6. Surprise 2.30 1.07 2.28 .84 .38 .42 -.07 .14 .15 (.76, .77)

Regression
Regeson
model Regressionmodelvariablecorrelations
variables' M S M S 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. POS AFF 3.51 .86 3.12 .88 .02 .01 .40 .36 -.04 .29
2. NEG AFF 2.02 .91 2.34 .88 -.37 -.05 -.24 -.32 .32 .32
3. EXP BEN 8.09 1.62 .32 -.21
4. EXP PRB 4.25 2.70 -.23 .09 -.18
5. EXP TOT 11.14 2.59 -.23 .02 -.01 .02
6. DISCONF 4.59 1.13 5.51 1.84 .47 -.30 .22 -.18 .53 -.28 -.11
7. SAT 7.18 1.33 5.04 1.16 .54 .45 .46 -.32 .61 -.47 -.20
8. COMPLN 1.85 2.47 .31
9. WOM 6.60 4.43
Note:Entriesin parenthesesareCronbachalphaestimatesof DES-IIreliabilityfor automobilesandCATV, respectively.Valuesbelow diagonal
areproduct-moment correlationsfor automobiles;those above arefor CATV. Valuesgreaterthan .12 are statisticallyat the .05 level andbeyond
for autos;for CATVthe correspondingvalue is .14. Blanksindicatedatanot availablebecauseof study design.
'See Table 3 for constructs.

Table 3
MEASUREMENT
OF POSTPURCHASEVARIABLES

Construct Legend Operationalization Study Reliability


Satisfaction SAT Averageof circles and D-T satisfactionratingscales (Andrewsand Withey AUTO .80
1976) CATV .81
Complaining COMPLN Indexformedby averaging(a) numberof complaintincidentsand (b) CATV .84
behavior numberof topics voiced
Word-of-Mouth WOM Indexformedby averaging(a) reportedfrequencyof discussionswith CATV .86
transmission othersaboutCATV and local cable operator,(b) numberof persons
involved, and (c) numberof topics discussed
Expectation
beliefs'
Benefits EXP BENb Ratingof prechoicesubjectivelikelihoodof receivingproductbenefits, AUTO
advantages,and good points along 10-pointcontinuum
Problems EXP PRBb Same as EXP BEN except for focus on productproblems,disadvantages, AUTO
and bad points
Overall EXP TOT Averageof (a) ratingof prechoicelevel of overallexpectationsalong 10- CATV .62
pointcontinuum(10 = expecteda lot, 1 = expectedvery little); (b)
ratingof extentof prechoiceanticipationalong 5-pointcontinuum(5 =
lookingforwarda lot, 1 = not looking forwardat all)
Disconfirma- DISCONF Averageof ratingsof the extentto which (a) productbenefits, advantages, AUTO .84
tion beliefsa and good points, (b) productproblems,disadvantages,and bad points, CATV
and (c) the productoverallwere seen as better/worsethanexpected,
along 7-pointcontinuum;developedby Oliver (1980); only measure(c)
used for CATV
of these constructsin the two field studies.
'Differencesin productcontextrequiredslightlydifferentoperationalizations
bMeasuresnot combinedbecauseof weakly negativecorrelation(n = -.19, p < .05).

ministered during a single interview, a check was made isfaction measures. The multiple correlations between
on the seriousness of potential common methods vari- satisfaction and the two sets of affect measures do not
ance. In the CATV pilot study the affective measures differ significantly (F73,73= 1.072; p > .05), indicating
were assessed on two separate occasions, the first time that the inflation in relationships due to this source of
alone and the second time in conjunction with the sat- methods variance is minimal.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
264 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,

Finally, a test for order effects in the sequence of mea- 8.30, 7 d.f., p = .305; GFI = .981. Though in the case
sure administrationwas made among subsamples (N = of automobiles the chi squaretest suggests the two-factor
20) of respondents in both final field studies, who were model leaves unexplaineda statisticallysignificant amount
given the DES-II subscales prior to other measures in of the covariationamong the DES-II subscales, the model
lieu of the opposite ordering as used among the rest of GFI of .960 indicates that relatively little variation of
the respondents. No significant differences were found practical import remains to be explained. Therefore, the
between the two order conditions in affect, satisfaction, two-factor model can be considered a plausible repre-
complaining, or word-of-mouth measures (all F-statis- sentation of the interrelations among the DES-II affect
tics p > .10). measures in the product/consumption contexts studied.
Parameterestimates for the two-factor model are re-
RESULTS ported in Table 4, as well as estimates of construct re-
liability and shared indicator variance. All factor load-
DES-II Subscales ings are statistically significant and in the expected
Table 2 indicates the relative frequency with which direction in both product categories. The positive and
each type of postpurchase affect was reported for auto- negative affect factors are uncorrelated for CATV, but
mobiles and CATV. Joy and interest are more frequent inversely related for automobiles (p < .001). Thus H1
for automobiles than for CATV (t = 6.12 and 2.81, can be accepted for both automobiles and CATV, though
p < .01), whereas anger and disgust are more frequent the independence of the factors appears to vary by prod-
for CATV than for autos (t = 4.83 and 6.78, p < .01). uct category.
Despite these differences, the relationships between the
six classes of affect appear broadly similar within the Relationships to Postpurchase Behavior
two product categories. An exception is surprise, which Hypothesized relationships between affective response
correlatesmore highly with the affect measuresfor CATV dimensions and satisfaction appraisal, complaint behav-
than for automobiles. ior, and word-of-mouth activity were evaluated by hi-
erarchicalOLS regressions. Factor scores (computed by
Factor Structure of Affective Response the regression method) from the two-factor confirmatory
To examine the validity of the hypothesized dimen- models served as indicators of positive and negative af-
sional structure of product- and consumption-based af-
fective response, confirmatory factor analysis was per-
formed on the six DES-II subscales. Tests were made of Table 4
both one- and two-factor models corresponding to the DES-IISUBSCALE
FACTOR TWO-FACTOR
LOADINGS:
unidimensional and bidimensional conceptualizations of MODELSa
affective response. Because the covariance structures of
the affect measures varied significantly by product cat- Automobiles CATV
egory (X2 = 124.59, 21 d.f., p < .000), separate con- DES-II Positive Negative Positive Negative
firmatory analyses were conducted. subscale affect affect affct affect
aect
The single-factor model of affective response does not Joy .888 .000 .710 .000
have a suitable fit to the original covariance matrix in t 13.483b 10.758b
each product category (for automobiles, x2 = 315.71, 9 Intearest .761 .000 .700 .000
t 11.377b 10.040b
d.f., p < .000; for CATV, X2 = 182.74, 9 d.f., p < Antger .000 .622 .000 .774
.000). In both instances the Joreskog-Sorbom (1986) t 11.629b 11.003b
goodness-of-fit index indicates that substantial unex- Dis gust .000 .578 .000 .608
plained variation remains. The null hypothesis of a un- t 13.579b 9.243b
idimensional model of affective response structurethere- Contempt .000 .614 .000 .569
t 15.102b 10.137"
fore is rejected. Surprise .573 .313 .592 .260
Next, a simple two-factor model was tested in which t 6.614b 3.859" 9.213b 4.432b
joy and interestloaded on a positive affect factor whereas
Factor
anger, disgust, and contempt loaded on an independent correlation
(possibly correlated) negative affect factor. Surprise was -.379 .002
assumed to load on both factors because of its theoretical t 4.952b .016
properties. To identify the model, the variances of the
common factors were fixed to 1.0. This model achieved Construct
a large and significant improvement in goodness of fit reliability .784 .769 .725 .762
Shared indicator
over the one-factor model (X2iff = 295.61, 2 d.f., p < variance .555 .468 .467 .472
.000 for automobiles; X2ff = 174.40, 2 d.f., p < .001
'All parameters based on LISREL VI maximum likelihood esti-
for CATV). For automobiles, test statistics on the two- mation (Joreskog and Sorbom 1986); only standardized values are
factor model are X2 = 20.10, 7 d.f., p = .005; GFI = shown; zero values fixed.
.960. For CATV, the corresponding statistics are X2 = bp < .001.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED 265

fective response. Table 5 gives the estimated regression tion-based affect is linked to all three forms of postpur-
coefficients and Table 2 indicates the simple zero-order chase behavior as hypothesized: positive affect relates
correlations among the regression model variables. directly to satisfaction appraisal (H2) and WOM activity
Note first the effects of affective responses alone. Ta- (H8) but not to complaint behavior (H6), whereas nega-
ble 5 indicates that the multiple correlations in equations tive affect relates inversely to satisfaction (H3) and di-
la, lb, 4, and 10 are all statistically significant and at rectly to complaint behavior (H5) and WOM (H8).
least moderate in size. The magnitude of the multiple To what extent do positive and negative dimensions
R's is greatest for the prediction of satisfaction and least of affective response add to the explanatory ability of
for complaint behavior. Moreover, the multiple R's also cognitive satisfaction determinants? Comparing equa-
exceed the largest of the correlations between individual tions 2a, 2b, 3a, and 3b in Table 5, one sees that the
affect factor scores and each criterion. Both positive and affective variablesadd significantlyto the predictivepower
negative affect regression coefficients in satisfaction of the cognitive expectation and disconfirmation belief
equations la and lb and WOM equation 10 are statis- variables alone (F2,194 = 16.855, p < .001 for auto-
tically significant (p < .001). In complaint behavior mobiles; F2,46 = 8.922, p < .001 for CATV). The af-
equation 4, only the negative affect factor has a signif- fective variables alone (equations la and lb) explain al-
icant coefficient, as expected. Thus, product/consump- most as much variance in satisfaction judgments as do

Table 5
OLSREGRESSIONS
OF SATISFACTION, COMPLAINT BEHAVIOR, AND WORD-OF-MOUTHTRANSMISSION
(beta with
coefficients significancelevels in parentheses)

Criterion Predictorvariables' Multiple


Product variable POS AFF NEG AFF EXP BEN EXP PRB EXP TOT DISCONF SAT COMPLN R N Equation
AUTO Satisfaction .435 -.296 .609 200 la
(.000) (.000)
CATV Satisfaction .365 -.323 .483 151 lb
(.000) (.000)
AUTO Satisfaction .321 .169 .511 .718 200 2a
(.000) (.001) (.000)
CATV Satisfaction .147 .565 .550 151 2b
(.039) (.000)
AUTO Satisfaction .172 -.209 .252 -.146 .387 .767 200 3a
(.002) (.000) (.000) (.002) (.000)
CATV Satisfaction .193 -.214 .102 .428 .605 151 3b
(.009) (.002) (.145) (.000)
CATV Complaining -.047 .324 .327 151 4
(.542) (.000)
CATV Complaining .118 .117 -.454 .514 151 5
(.126) (.020) (.000)
CATV Complaining -.082 -.301 .293 151 6
(.311) (.001)
CATV Complaining -.017 -.053 -.440 .514 151 7
(.819) (.558) (.000)
CATV Complaining .056 .259 -.059 -.257 .393 151 8
(.509) (.002) (.463) (.005)
CATV Complaining .137 .168 -.015 -.075 -.423 .518 151 9
(.090) (.030) (.837) (.413) (.000)
CATV Word-of-mouth .284 .315 .427 151 10
(.000) (.000)
CATV Word-of-mouth -.006 -.108 .107b 151 11
(.947) (.201)
CATV Word-of-mouth .024 .007 -.203 .200b 151 12
(.773) (.947) (.058)
CATV Word-of-mouth .379 .230 -.262 .485 151 13
(.000) (.003) (.002)
CATV Word-of-mouth .402 .220 .009 -.092 -.225 .491 151 14
(.000) (.005) (.906) (.325) (.015)
CATV Word-of-mouth .358 .199 -.183 .173 .507 151 15
(.000) (.011) (.042) (.038)
CATV Word-of-mouth .379 .192 .012 -.080 -.153 .168 .512 151 16
(.000) (.016) (.878) (.392) (.115) (.046)
aSeeTable 3 for constructs.
bp > .05.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
266 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,

the cognitive/semantic belief variables(equations2a and littleto the explanationof WOM;neitherof the multiple
2b). R values is significant(p > .05). By the ANOCOVA
The full regressionmodelof equations3a and 3b also approach,neithersatisfactionalone (equation 13) nor
affordsa morerigoroustest of the affect-satisfaction hy- satisfactionin conjunctionwith its cognitive belief an-
pothesesH2, H3, and H4by holdingconstantthe effects tecedents(equation14) eliminatesthe significanteffects
of the cognitive satisfactionantecedents.Both positive of the affectiveresponsevariables.Likewise, the intro-
andnegativeaffectdimensionsmaintaintheirsignificant ductionof the complaintbehaviorvariable,whetherwith
positive and negative coefficients, respectively,of ap- satisfactionalone (equation15) or togetherwith its cog-
proximately equalmagnitude.Disconllinationbeliefsalso nitive antecedents(equation16), has no impacton the
continueto relatedirectlyto satisfaction(p < .001). Ex- significance of the affectivevariablesin explainingWOM.
pectationbeliefs are relatedsignificantlyto satisfaction Thusthe affect-WOMlinkagescan be considereddirect
in the case of automobiles(expectedbenefits directly, andunmediatedby the otherpostpurchasevariables.
expectedproblemsinversely).ForCATV, the single ex- Finally,the regressionmodelof equations14, 15, and
pectationbelief measurefails to attainconventionalsta- 16 indicatesthat aftercontrollingfor the effects of af-
tisticalsignificance,despitethe correctsign. This find- fective responses,satisfactionhas a modest inverse re-
ing may reflectthe marginalreliabilityof the measure, lationshipand complaintbehaviora modestpositive re-
as noted previously.Overall, these findings strengthen lationshipto extent of WOM transmissions.Hence the
supportfor H2 and H3 and affirmthat the influenceof relationshipsbetween satisfactionand WOM and be-
affectivevariableson satisfactionis unmediated(H4). tween complaintbehaviorand WOM do not appearto
As the hypothesizedrelationshipof negativeaffect to be the spuriousresultof theirjoint dependenceon post-
complaintbehavior(H5)is supported,the next issue is purchaseaffectiveresponses.
whetherthis linkageis director wholly mediatedvia sat-
isfactionjudgments.To test the possible mediatingrole DISCUSSION
of satisfaction,the analysisof covariance(ANOCOVA)
approachwas employed (Batraand Ray 1986; Insko, Themajorresultsof thisresearchon product-andcon-
Turnbull,and Yandell 1974). This approachcompares sumption-based affectin two separateproductcategories
directand mediatedcausal models by enteringthe hy- arethreefold.First,good andbad feelings representrel-
pothesizedmediatorinto the regressionequationas a ativelyindependentdimensionsof affective responseto
covariate.When satisfactionis introducedin this way, productsin use. Second, reportsof such feelings relate
the regressioncoefficientof negativeaffectdoes not drop directly to product satisfactionjudgments, extent of
to nonsignificance,indicatingthat satisfactiondoes not complaint behavior, and volume of word-of-mouth
completelymediatethe affect-complainingrelationship. transmissionin the postpurchaseperiod.Third,affective
Theseresultsare shownin Table 5, equation5. Hence, responsesappearto accountfor significantincremental
negativeaffectappearsto influencecomplainingactivity variancebeyondextanttheoreticalexplanationsof these
directly,independently of its indirecteffects throughsat- postpurchase phenomena.
isfaction,supportingH7.
As a furtherexplorationof the role of satisfactionin Dimensionality of Affective Response
mediatingcomplaintbehavior, equations6 and 7 ex- Especiallynoteworthyis the relativeindependenceof
aminethe relationship betweencognitiveexpectationand positiveandnegativeaffectiveresponsein postpurchase
disconfirmationbeliefs and complaintbehavior.Equa- processes.The two dimensionsof affect are completely
tion 6 reveals that disconfirmationbeliefs (but not ex- orthogonalin the case of CATVandonly modestlyneg-
pectationbeliefs) are negatively (p < .01) related to atively relatedfor automobiles.Evidentlypleasant af-
complaining.However, its regressioncoefficient drops fects in relationto product/consumption outcomes,such
to nonsignificancewhen satisfactionis enteredas a cov- asjoy, interest,andexcitement,do not necessarilyimply
ariatein equation7. Thussatisfactionfully mediatesthe the absenceof unpleasantaffectssuch as anger,disgust,
effectsof disconfirmation beliefs on complaintbehavior. andcontempt.The resultsconfirmthe findingsof Abel-
This mediationalrole is uniqueto satisfactionand is not son et al. (1982) for politicalcandidatesandquestionthe
sharedwith the affectivevariables.When the latterare assumptionof a single bipolar affective dimensionof
enteredinto the regressionof complainingon expecta- good-bad(e.g. Davitz 1969; Russell 1983). Dual uni-
tion and disconfirmation beliefs (equation8), disconfir- polaraffectivedimensionsareparticularlyappropriate in
mationretainsits significant(p < .01) effect. product/consumption contextsbecause they make pos-
Next, it is of interestto see whetherthe simple rela- sible a distinctionbetween ambivalenceand indiffer-
tionshipsbetweenproduct/consumption affectandWOM ence, which may not be infrequentreactionsin this do-
transmissionsnoted before occur directly, as hypothe- main.
sized in H8, or are mediatedthroughsatisfactionjudg- Thoughaffecttaxonomies(e.g., Izard1977)assertthat
mentsand/or its cognitiveantecedents.First, however, subjectivefeelings are organizedinto a finite set of dis-
note fromequations11 and 12 thatthe lattercontribute tinctiveaffect categories,the resultshere indicatethata

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED 267

parsimonioustwo-dimensionalrepresentationmay suf- Thoughthe findingsare consistentwith the hypothe-


fice for understandingpostpurchaseprocesses. Empiri- sized causalsequenceof consumptionoutcome-- affect
- satisfaction,it must be acknowledgedthat the true
cally, high covariationis observed among the interest
andjoy DES-II subscales, and similarlyamongthe an- temporalorderingof this linkagecannotbe established
ger, disgust,andcontemptsubscales,raisingsome ques- conclusivelyby meansof correlationalanalyses. Alter-
tion about the discriminantvalidity of the individual natively,positiveand negativeaffectiveresponsemight
measureswith respect to one anotherin product/con- resultfrom satisfactionappraisal.Weiner, Russell, and
sumptioncontexts. However,this patternof resultsalso Lerman(1979) have suggestedthe possibilityof inter-
could have occurredif severalaffects had been elicited play betweencognitivebeliefs and affective responses,
simultaneously,perhapsas a resultof a commonunder- each evokingthe otherin a dynamicsequentialprocess.
process(Izard1977).
lying cognitiveappraisal/attribution Certainaffectsmightoccurearlyin the causalchainand
Finally, it must be acknowledgedthat the two-factor triggerappraisalof satisfaction,which in turnelicits ad-
structureobservedhere and elsewhere (Abelson et al. ditional(anddifferent)affects, in turnmodifyinginitial
1982;Bradburn1969) mightsimplyreflectthe common satisfactionappraisal.Furtherstudy is neededto disen-
analyticalpracticeof aggregatingemotionalreportsacross tanglethese issues of causal sequence. At a minimum,
variedelicitingstimuliand/or circumstances.Furtherre- however, the affect -* satisfactionportion of the se-
searchon the dimensionalityof affective responsecon- quenceappearswell supportedby priorwork(Isen 1984;
trollingfor variedproduct/consumption outcomeswould Schwarzand Clore 1983).
be helpfulin evaluatingthis possibility. A plausiblerival interpretationof these findings is
Affect and Satisfaction Judgment suggestedby the effects of affective state on cognitive
retrievalprocesses(Isen 1984). In this view, reportsof
As hypothesized,both pleasantand unpleasantaffec- pastexperiencesduringpostpurchaseprocessesmightbe
tive responsesin the postpurchaseperiodare relatedto biasedsystematicallyin the directionof the affectivestate
satisfactionappraisal,approximatelyequally in magni- at the time of reporting.As a result, the relationships
tude and oppositelyin direction,in a strikinglysimilar betweenpostpurchaseaffect, cognitivebeliefs, and sat-
manneracrosstwo dissimilarproductcategories.More- isfactioncould be overstatedsubstantially.
over, theserelationshipsarenot mediatedby expectation Affect and Postpurchase Communications
and disconfirmationbeliefs. Hence, they suggest that
productsatisfactionjudgmentsare determinednot only Though the role of product/consumptionaffect in
by ex post cognitive/semanticcomparisonprocessesas postpurchase communications has been suspected,direct
typicallyassumed,but also by additionalprocesses in- empirical evidence heretoforehas been lacking. Both
volvingthe retrievaland integrationof relevantproduct- positive and negative dimensions of affect are relatedto
relatedaffectiveexperiences. the amountof word-of-mouthtransmitted.Moreover,
Thoughparalleling the findingsof Abelsonet al. (1982) consumersappearmore likely to voice their product/
on the role of affect in the evaluationof political can- consumptionexperiencesto otherconsumersto the ex-
didates,the presentstudy does not supportthe finding tentthatthose experiencesinvolve notableaffectiveele-
thatpositiveaffect is the dominantinfluence.However, ments-good or bad-independently of their overall
the focal stimuli,affectivemeasures,and criterionvari- judgmentsof satisfactionand/or seller-directedcom-
ables all differbetweenthe studies. Also noteworthyis plaintactions. Satisfactionactuallyshows a weak neg-
the greateroverlapin the presentfindings between af- ative relationshipto word-of-mouthonce the affective
fective reportsand cognitive/semanticbelief variables; influenceshave been partialledout, suggestingit is the
Abelson et al. (1982) found little redundancybetween affectthatstimulatesthe "webof word-of-mouth" rather
the two classes of measures.In this researchpositiveaf- thansatisfactionper se, therebyqualifyingconventional
fective responsesshow substantialcovariationwith dis- marketingwisdom.
confirmation beliefs, thoughthe latterpresumablyreflect Seller-directedcomplaintbehavior also appearsdi-
"pure" semantic judgmentsostensiblyfree of affect. This rectlyrelatedto negativepostpurchaseaffects involving
finding indicates either shared methods variance or a anger,disgust, and contempt.However,these variables
common causal antecedent,such as the cognitive ap- exert some effect directlyon complaintbehavior, and
praisalprocesspostulatedto accountfor differentialaf- productsatisfactiondoes not fully mediatethe relation-
fect elicitation.In either instance, perhapsgreatercre- ship.Thuseven satisfiedconsumerswill complainif often
denceshouldbe givento the affectivereports;theirgreater enoughangered,disgusted,or contemptuousin connec-
validityis suggestedby theirmore "primitive"andnaive tion with consumptionof a particularproduct/service.
nature(Zajonc 1980) whereas disconfirmationbeliefs The absenceof any relationshipbetweenpositive affect
appearto involve higherlevels of cognitiveprocessing. and complainingsuggests that affects of differentva-
Furtherstudy of the discriminantvalidity of disconfir- lences are not "nettedout" againsteach otherin a com-
mationbeliefs vis a vis affective responses clearly is pensatoryway in the productionof such behavior.
needed. Theforegoingrelationshipsareparticularlysignificant

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
268 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,
because they position product/consumption affective re- REFERENCES
sponses as a potentially pervasive influence throughout
the postpurchase period, perhaps rivaling satisfaction as Aaker,DavidA., DouglasM. Stayman,andMichaelR. Hag-
a central mediating construct. This role sharply contrasts erty(1985), "Warmthin Advertising:Measurement,Impact
with that of expectation and disconfirmation belief vari- andSequenceEffects,"Journalof ConsumerResearch, 12
ables, whose contribution in the postpurchase period ap- (March),365-81.
pears limited to determining satisfaction. Abelson,RobertP., DonaldR. Kinder,MarkD. Peters, and
SusanT. Fiske (1982), "Affectiveand SemanticCompo-
nentsin PoliticalPersonPerception,"Journalof Personality
Implications and Social Psychology,42 (April), 619-30.
The findings have implications for future research, Allport,Gordonand Leo Postman(1947), ThePsychologyof
Rumor.New York:Holt, Rinehartand Winston,Inc.
methodology, and management practice. Further study
of product/consumption affect is warranted,both to con- Andrews,FrankM. and StevenB. Withey(1976), Social In-
dicatorsof Well-Being.New York:PlenumPress.
firm the results of this exploratory inquiry and to ex-
Arndt,Johan(1967), "TheRole of ProductRelatedConver-
amine the role of such affect in other important post- sationsin the Diffusionof a New Product,"Journalof Mar-
purchase processes. Especially worthy of attention are ketingResearch,4 (August),291-5.
the cognitive appraisal processes underlying differential Arnold,MagdaB. (1960), Emotionsand Personality,Volume
affect elicitation. Also importantare issues of causal se- 1: PsychologicalAspects. New York:ColumbiaUniversity
quence, particularly the linkages between affective re- Press.
sponses and specific consumption outcomes and/or Bartlett,EdwardS. and CarrollE. Izard(1972), "A Dimen-
product attributes. sionalandDiscreteEmotionsInvestigationof the Subjective
For methodology, the results suggest that the DES-II Experienceof Emotion,"in Patternsof Emotions:A New
measurement approach can be fruitfully employed in Analysisof Anxietyand Depression, CarrollE. Izard, ed.
New York:AcademicPress, Inc., 129-73.
studying affect in additional research contexts. Adver- Batra, Rajeev and Michael L. Ray (1986), "Affective Re-
tising researchers in particular should find its compre- sponsesMediatingAcceptanceof Advertising,"Journalof
hensive taxonomic framework useful in more fully ex- ConsumerResearch, 13 (September),234-49.
ploring the range of emotional experiences evoked by Bearden,William 0. and Jesse E. Teel (1983), "Selected
advertising. Extant research has been limited to ad hoc Determinantsof Consumer Satisfaction and Complaint
measures of affective response not grounded explicitly Reports,"Journal of MarketingResearch, 20 (February),
in theory (e.g., Schlinger 1979), or has addressed only 21-8.
selected regions of the affective space, such as warmth Bettman,JamesR. (1979), An InformationProcessingTheory
(Aaker, Stayman, and Hagerty 1985) or surgency/ela- of ConsumerChoice. Reading,MA: Addison-WesleyPub-
tion/vigor/activation and social affection (Batra and Ray lishingCompany.
Bloch, Peter H. (1981), "An ExplorationInto the Scaling
1986). of Consumers'Involvementwith a Product Class," in
Finally, the results suggest several interesting impli- Advancesin ConsumerResearch,Vol. 8, KentB. Monroe,
cations for management consideration that should be ed. Ann Arbor,MI: Associationfor ConsumerResearch,
verified through future research. Most notably, the sub- 61-5.
stantial role of positive affect in product satisfaction Bradbur, NormanM. (1969), TheStructureof Psychological
judgmentsraises the possibility that customerloyalty could Well-Being.Chicago:Aldine PublishingCompany.
be enhanced by stimulating consumers' affective re- Crowne,D. and D. Marlowe(1964), The ApprovalMotive.
sponses in the postpurchase period. Tranformationalad- New York:JohnWiley & Sons, Inc.
vertising (Puto and Wells 1984), which seeks to trans- Daly, E. M., W. J. Lancee,andJ. Polivy (1983), "A Conical
form product ownership and consumption into a more Modelfor the Taxonomyof EmotionalExperience,"Jour-
nal of PersonalityandSocialPsychology,45 (August),443-
affectively significant experience, affords an interesting 57.
opportunityfor accomplishing this enhancement through Davitz,JoelR. (1969), TheLanguageof Emotions.New York:
market communications. Such efforts also might be ap- AcademicPress, Inc.
propriatefor eliciting the positive affect central to stim- Day, RalphL. (1984), "ModellingChoices Among Alterna-
ulating the "web of word-of-mouth." Though clearly a tive Responsesto Dissatisfaction,"in Advances in Con-
significant purchase influence, word-of-mouth by con- sumerResearch,Vol. 11, ThomasC. Kinnear,ed. Ann Ar-
sumers typically has been considered beyond the direct bor, MI: Associationfor ConsumerResearch,496-9.
control of marketers. Regardless of success in these en- Dichter,ErnestA. (1966), "HowWordof MouthAdvertising
deavors, however, marketers cannot assume that en- Works,"HarvardBusinessReview,44 (November-Decem-
ber), 147-57.
hancing positive affect in the postpurchase period will Donovan,RobertJ. and JohnR. Rossiter(1982), "StoreAt-
correspondinglyreduce consumer complaints; rather, the mosphere:An Environmental PsychologyApproach,"Jour-
independence of positive and negative affect dimensions nal of Retailing,58 (Spring),34-57.
suggests that effective channels of redress still will be Ekman,Paul, WallaceV. Friesen,and SusanAncoli (1980),
needed to ensure high levels of "ultimate"customer sat- "FacialSigns of EmotionalExperience,"Journal of Per-
isfaction. sonalityand Social Psychology,39 (December),1125-34.

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
RESPONSES
AFFECTIVE
PRODUCT/CONSUMPTION-BASED 269

, and PhoebeEllsworth(1982), "WhatEmo- nal of Marketing Research, 20 (November), 393-404.


tion Categoriesor DimensionsCan ObserversJudgeFrom Lazarus,RobertS., A. D. Kanner,and S. Folkman(1980),
Facial Behavior?" in Emotion in the Human Face, 2nd ed., "Emotions:A Cognitive PhenomenologicalAnalysis," in
P. Ekman,ed. Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress, Emotion: Theory, Research and Experience: Vol. 1, Themes
39-55. of Emotion,R. PlutchikandH. Kellerman,eds. New York:
Engel, JamesF., RobertJ. Kergerreis,and Roger D. Black- AcademicPress, Inc., 189-217.
well (1969), "Wordof MouthCommunicationby the In- McGuire,William J. (1974), "PsychologicalMotives and
novator," Journal of Marketing, 33 (July), 15-19. Communication Gratification,"in The Uses of Mass Com-
Folkes, Valerie S. (1984), "ConsumerReactionsto Product munications: Current Perspectives on Gratifications Re-
Failure:An AttributionalApproach,"Journalof Consumer search, J. F. Blumlerand E. J. Katz, eds. Beverly Hills,
Research, 10 (March),398-409. CA: Sage Publications,Inc., 106-7.
Forell, ClaesandRobertA. Westbrook(1984), "TheVicious McHugo,Gregory,Craig Smith, and John Lanzetta(1982),
Circleof ConsumerComplaints,"Journalof Marketing,48 "TheStructureof Self-Reportsas EmotionalResponsesto
(Summer),68-78. Film Segments," Motivation and Emotion, 6, 365-85.
Gardner,MerylP. (1985), "ConsumerMoodStates:A Critical Oliver,RichardL. (1980), "A CognitiveModel of the Ante-
Review," Journal of Consumer Research, 12 (December), cedentsandConsequencesof SatisfactionDecisions,"Jour-
281-300. nal of Marketing Research, 17 (November), 460-9.
Hoffman,MartinL. (1986), "Affect, Cognition, and Moti- Peterson,RobertA., WayneD. Hoyer, and WilliamR. Wil-
vation," in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foun- son, eds. (1986), The Role of Affect in Consumer Behavior:
dations of Social Behavior, R. M. Sorrentino and E. T. Hig- Emerging Theories and Applications. Lexington, MA: D.
gins, eds. New York:GuilfordPress, 244-80. C. Heathand Company.
Holbrook,MorrisB., RobertW. Chestnut,TerenceA. Oliva, Plutchik, Robert (1980), Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Syn-
andEric A. Greenleaf(1984), "Playas a ConsumptionEx- thesis. New York:Harper& Row Publishers,Inc.
perience:The Roles of Emotion,Performanceand Person- Puto, ChristopherP. and William D. Wells (1984), "Infor-
ality in the Enjoyment of Games," Journal of Consumer Re- mationaland Transformational Advertising:The Differing
search, 11 (September),728-39. Effects of Time," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol.
andElizabethC. Hirschman(1982), "TheExperiential 11, ThomasC. Kinnear,ed. Ann Arbor, MI: Association
Aspectsof Consumption: ConsumerFantasies,Feelingsand for ConsumerResearch,849-67.
Fun," Journal of Consumer Research, 9 (September), 132- Richins,MarshaL. (1983), "NegativeWordof Mouthby Dis-
40. satisfiedConsumers:A Pilot Study,"Journalof Marketing,
Holmes,JohnD. andJohnD. Lett(1977), "ProductSampling 47 (Winter),68-78.
and Word of Mouth," Journal of Advertising Research, 17 Robinson, John A. (1976), "Sampling Autobiographical
(October),35-9. Memory," Cognitive Psychology, 8 (October), 578-95.
Howard,JohnA. andJagdishN. Sheth(1969), TheTheoryof (1980), "AffectandRetrievalof PersonalMemories,"
BuyerBehavior.New York:JohnWiley & Sons, Inc. Motivation and Emotion, 4 (June), 149-74.
Hunt,,H. Keith (1977), "CS/D: Overview and FutureRe- Aspectsof the Human
Russell,JamesA. (1983), "Pancultural
search Directions," in Conceptualization and Measurement ConceptualOrganizationof Emotions,"Journalof Person-
of ConsumerSatisfaction and Dissatisfaction, H. Keith Hunt, ality and Social Psychology, 45 (June), 1281-8.
ed. Cambridge,MA: MarketingScience Institute,455-88. Scherer,KlausR. (1982), "Emotionas Process:Function,Or-
Insko,ChesterA., W. Turbull, and B. Yandell(1974), "Fa- igin and Regulation," Social Science Information, 21 (415),
cilitativeand InhibitingEffects of Distractionon Attitude 555-70.
Change."Sociometry,34, 508-28. Schlinger,Mary Jane (1979), "A Profile of Responses to
Isen, Alice M. (1984), "TowardUnderstandingthe Role of Commercials," Journal of Advertising Research, 19 (April),
Affect in Cognition," in Handbook of Social Cognition, R. 37-46.
Wyer and T. Srull, eds. Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaum Schwarz,Norbertand GeraldL. Clore (1983), "Mood,Mis-
Associates, 179-236. attribution,andJudgementsof Well-Being:Informativeand
Izard,CarrollE. (1977), HumanEmotion.New York:Plenum DirectiveFunctionsof Affective States," Journal of Per-
Press. sonality and Social Psychology, 45 (September), 513-23.
Jacoby, Jacob and James L. Jaccard(1981), "The Sources, Smith, CraigA. and Phoebe C. Ellsworth(1985), "Patterns
MeaningandValidityof ConsumerComplaintBehavior:A of CognitiveAppraisalin Emotion,"Journalof Personality
PsychologicalAnalysis,"Journalof Retailing,57 (Fall), 4- and Social Psychology, 48 (April), 813-38.
24. Smith, Elliott R. and James R. Kluegel (1982), "Cognitive
J6reskog,KarlG. andDag Sorbom(1986), LISRELVI:Anal- and Social Bases of EmotionalExperience:Outcome, At-
ysis of Linear Structural Relationships by the Method of tribution, and Affect," Journal of Personality and Social
MaximumLikelihood,Mooresville,IN: ScientificSoftware, Psychology,43 (June), 1129-41.
Inc. TARP/TechnicalAssistanceResearchPrograms(1981), Mea-
Krishnan,S. andValerieA. Valle (1979), "DissatisfactionAt- suring the Grapevine-Consumer Response and Word-of-
tributionsandConsumerComplaintBehavior,"Advancesin Mouth,Washington,DC.
ConsumerResearch, Vol. 6, William L. Wilkie, ed. Ann Tomkins,Sylvan S. (1980), "Affectas Amplification:Some
Arbor,MI: Associationfor ConsumerResearch,445-9. Modificationsin Theory," in Emotion: Theory, Research and
LaBarbera,PriscillaA. and DavidMazursky(1983), "A Lon- Experience,R. PlutchikandH. Kellerman,eds. New York:
gitudinalAssessmentof ConsumerSatisfaction/Dissatisfac- AcademicPress, Inc.
tion:The DynamicNatureof the CognitiveProcess,"Jour- Waldfogel, Samuel (1948), "The Frequencyand Affective

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
270 JOURNAL
OF MARKETING AUGUST1987
RESEARCH,

Characterof ChildhoodMemories,"PsychologicalMono- texts," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37


graphs: General and Applied, 62 (4), Whole No. 291, 1- (July), 1211-20.
39. Westbrook,RobertA. (1980), "Intrapersonal
Affective Influ-
Weinberg,Peterand WolfgangGottwald(1982), "Impulsive ences Upon ConsumerSatisfaction,"Journalof Consumer
ConsumerBuying as a Result of Emotions,"Journal of Research, 7 (June), 49-54.
Business Research, 10 (March), 43-57. Zajonc,RobertB. (1980), "FeelingandThinking:Preferences
Weiner,Bernard,DanRussell,andDavidLerman(1979), "The Need No Inferences," American Psychologist, 35 (Febru-
Cognition-EmotionProcess in Achievement-RelatedCon- ary), 151-75.

World
names
Becausemostwesternlanguagesderivefroma singlesource
(Indo-European), it is possibleto construct'worldnames"'..
productand companynameswhich are properlymeaningfulin
mostwesternnationsandJapan.(Roughly10%of urbanJapanese
is derivedfromEnglish.)
At NameLab,weVemade such worldnamesasAcura,
CognosandSentraby constructionallinguistics.
Theresultof a NameLabprojectis a reportpresentingand
analyzingtrademark-cleared nameswhichexpressyourmarketing
identitypreciselyand powerfully. Wequotecosts accuratelyin
advanceand completemostprojectswithinfourweeks.
Foran informationpacket,contactNameLabInc.,
711MarinaBlvd.,SanFrancisco,CA94123,415-563-1639.

NAMELAB"

This content downloaded from 148.61.13.133 on Sun, 17 Nov 2013 15:52:56 PM


All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

You might also like