You are on page 1of 20

Mood States and Consumer Behavior:

A Critical Review
MERYL PAULA GARDNER*

A conceptual framework is presented that depicts both the mediating role of mood
states and their potential importance in consumer behavior. Reviewing findings from
the psychological literature indicates that mood states have direct and indirect effects
on behavior, evaluation, and recall. The scope and limitations of these effects are
addressed, and the implications for consumer behavior in three areas-service en-
counters, point-of-purchase stimuli, and communications (context and content)-
are examined. Finally, the potential feasibility and viability of mood-related approaches
to marketing research and practice are discussed.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


I ndividuals often try to anticipate each others' moods
prior to interactions and to read each others' moods
Moore and Hutchinson 1983; Rossiter and Percy 1980;
Shimp 1981). Findings indicate that these feelings-ori-
during encounters. In these ways, mood information is ented factors may playa major role in consumer-atti-
acquired and used informally to facilitate social and tude formation and brand selection.
professional interactions. For example, knowledge of Since mood states are a particularly important set of
the boss's mood on a particular day may help an em- affective factors (Gardner and Vandersteel1984; West-
ployee anticipate the boss's reactions to a request for a brook 1980), they form a part of all marketing situations
raise. Analogously, knowledge of consumers' mood (Belk 1975; Lutz and Kakkar 1975) and may influence
states in marketing situations may provide marketers consumer behavior in many contexts, e.g., advertise-
with a more complete understanding of consumers and ment exposure and brand selection. Mood states may
their reactions to marketing strategies and tactics. This be quite transient and easily influenced by little things
mood-state knowledge may be particularly relevant for (lsen et al. 1982). Small changes in physical surround-
understanding consumer behavior as it is affected by ings may influence consumers' moods at the point of
service encounters, point-of-purchase stimuli, the con- purchase, and slight deviations in communications
tent of marketing communications, and the context in strategies may significantly affect moods upon exposure
which these communications appear. to advertising. In fact, although consumers' moods are
More generally, insights into consumer behavior may often affected by factors beyond a marketer's control,
be gleaned by examining consumers' thoughts and feel- moods can be greatly influenced by seemingly small
ings. Considerable research using a traditional infor- aspects of marketer behavior, e.g., a salesperson's smile
mation processing paradigm has enriched our under- or a long wait for a doctor's appointment.
standing of the cognitive mediators of consumer be- Because mood states are omnipresent and readily in-
havior. Significant insights into consumer behavior have fluenced by marketer action, they may have important
also come from research that has examined noncog- effects on consumer behavior. This article provides a
nitive (nonbelief) factors such as subjective familiarity preliminary exploration of this potentially important,
(Park, Gardner, and Thukral 1984), fun and fantasy but inadequately charted territory. Specifically, we pro-
(Hirschman and Holbrook 1982; Holbrook and pose to examine the effects of consumers' moods on
Hirschman 1982), motor and somatic representation behavior, evaluation, and recall in marketing contexts.
of affect (Zajonc and Markus 1982), emotion (Fennell Informal analysis has suggested three areas where mood
1981; Weinberg and Gottwald 1982), and attitude to- effects appear to be significant and where mood states
ward the advertisement (Gardner 1985; Lutz, Mac- can be influenced by marketing tactics: service encoun-
Kenzie, and Belch 1983; Mitchell and Olson 1981; ters, point-of-purchase stimuli, and communications
(context and content). The present article will focus on
"Meryl Paula Gardner is Assistant Professor of Marketing, New these domains, although they are not the only areas
York University, New York, NY 10003. The author wishes to thank where mood states may have important effects on con-
C. Samuel Craig, John Czepiel, C. W. Park, John Rossiter, and the sumer behavior.
reviewers of JCR for their helpful comments on earlier drafts of this First, I will define terms to help clarify the distinctions
paper.
between moods and other feeling states. Second, I will
281
© JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH. Vol. 12. December 1985
282 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

present a conceptual framework and discuss findings One is almost always aware of one's emotions and their
about the nature and limitations of the direct and in- effects, which may redirect attention to the source of
direct effects of mood on behavior, evaluation, and re- the emotion and interrupt ongoing beh.avior (Simon
call. Third, I will examine the potential implications of 1967). One mayor may not be aware of one's mood
these findings for consumer behavior with respect to and its effects, which may color attentional processes
service encounters, point-of-purchase stimuli, and and influence, but rarely interrupt ongoing behavior
communications. In each of these areas I will address (Clark and Isen 1982).
three questions: Different types of positive moods (e.g., cheeriness,
peacefulness, and sexual warmth) and negative moods
I. What inferences can be drawn for consumer behavior (e.g., anxiety, guilt, and depression) can be readily
from our knowledge of the effects of mood states on identified. Although categorizing moods as positive or
behavior, evaluation, and recall? negative may be an oversimplification (Belk 1984), ex-
2. Can marketers take advantage of the opportunities isting research does not provide much insight into the
suggested by the effects of mood states on consumer effects of specific moods. In fact, most studies have in-
behavior, or do logistical limitations prohibit imple- volved broad manipulations designed to induce positive
mentation of mood-oriented marketing approaches? and/or negative moods and have not attempted to affect
3. What is the current status and future potential of re- or assess specific moods. It is often difficult to infer the

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


search to assess the impact of mood on consumer be- induced mood or its strength; e.g., subjects told that
havior? they have failed a test of perceptual motor skills may
not respond emotionally or they may feel depressed,
Fourth, I will discuss the implications of mood effects frustrated, or anxious. In addition, many common ma-
on consumer behavior as they relate to the design of nipulations may fail to induce discrete moods, and nat-
marketing-research studies and the development of urally occurring feeling states may appear in clusters
marketing actions. Finally, I will make suggestions for (Polivy 1981). Only two studies have investigated the
future research. effects of more than one positive or negative mood
(Fried and Berkowitz 1979; Laird et al. 1982). To enable
Definition of Terms readers to form their own inferences about specific pos-
The word "mood" has a wide range of usages and itive and negative mood states induced in the relevant
meaning. One might use the term to describe a phe- studies, Table 1 provides information about the induc-
nomenological property of an individual's subjectively tion procedures used.
perceived affective state; e.g., someone may be in a Although it is difficult to compare findings for specific
cheerful mood or a hostile mood. One might also use positive and negative moods across studies since the
mood to describe a property of an inanimate object; induction procedures and dependent variables are con-
e.g., a point-of-purchase display may have a "sophis- founded, the effects of different negative mood states
ticated mood" or a "fun mood." For the purposes of seem to be more heterogeneous than the effects of pos-
this article, we will adopt the former, phenomenological, itive mood states (Isen 1984). For example, Baumann,
approach and view moods as feeling states that are sub- Cialdini, and Kenrick (1981) note that helping may be
jectively perceived by individuals. As such, moods are enhanced by some negative mood states (e.g., sadness)
a subcategory of feeling states. but not by others (e.g., frustration). Research is needed
The phrase "feeling state" will be used to refer to an to investigate the effects of specific positive and negative
affective state that is general and pervasive. Such states moods on behavior, evaluation, and recall.
"suffuse all one's experiences, even though directed at
none in particular" (Fiske 1981, p 231). These states CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK
can be contrasted with feelings directed toward specific
objects, e.g., the affective component of brand attitude. Common models of consumer behavior do not ex-
Mood will refer to feeling states that are transient; plicitly recognize the role of mood states. At best, the
such states are particularized to specific times and sit- term "antecedent state" is used to encompass all of the
uations (Peterson and Sauber 1983) and may be con- momentary financial, psychological, and physiological
trasted with those that are relatively stable and per- baggage with which a consumer arrives at a marketing
manent (Westbrook 1980). Examples of invariant feel- interaction (Belk 1975). In order to examine the role
ing states include personality dispositions such as of mood states in consumer behavior and suggest their
optimism/pessimism (Goldman-Eisler 1960; Tiger marketing implications, a framework is needed that in-
1979), and enduring global attitude structures such as terconnects strategic areas, marketing tactics, and con-
satisfaction (Andrews and Withey 1976). sumer-behavior processes. The framework adopted for
Moods may be distinguished from emotions, which, this article attempts to meet this need and appears in
in contrast, are usually more intense, attention-getting, the Figure. It is not all-inclusive, but facilitates the ex-
and tied to a specifiable behavior (Clark and Isen 1982). amination of:
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 283

I. Relationships between some key strategic marketing han 1973). Some positive moods appear to increase the
areas (indicated by rectangles in the Figure) and mood likelihood of performance of behaviors with expected
induction tactics (indicated by ovals) positive associations and to decrease the likelihood of
2. Effects of induced mood states on some important performance of behaviors that lead to negative out-
psychological processes underlying consumer behav- comes (perhaps by enhancing one's sense of personal
ior (indicated by triangles). power and self-perceived freedom to do as one wishes-
Forest et al. 1979; Isen and Simmonds 1978). Research
The depicted relationships are discussed in the next two is needed to predict a priori which activities are believed
sections. to have positive outcomes and which are thought to
have negative outcomes.
Strategic Areas and Mood Induction Tactics Studies reviewed in Table 1 suggest that the behav-
iorial effects of negative moods may be more complex
Although it may seem obvious that some marketing than the effects of positive moods. Two factors may
actions affect consumers' mood states, the relationship contribute to the diversity of effects of negative mood
between strategic areas and mood-related tactics re- states. First, there are some indications that negative
quires systematic investigation. Examining three stra- mood states are themselves more heterogeneous than
tegic areas-service encounters, point-of-purchase positive mood states (Isen 1984). Second, controlled
stimuli, and communications (context and content)-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


processes that terminate unpleasant negative mood
serves two purposes. First, it facilitates the discussion states may compete with automatic tendencies to engage
of tactics that are useful in more than one area. Second, in mood-congruent behavior (Clark and Isen 1982).
it facilitates the evaluation of the importance and fea- The link between mood states and behavior may be
sibility of mood-related approaches in each area. seen as both direct and indirect (see Figure). A direct
In the service sector, mood induction can be contig- linkage may involve associations in memory between
uous to an encounter, thus increasing the likelihood mood states and behaviors. In this context, a behavior
that its effects will have an impact on a transaction. may be viewed as a conditioned response. (For a review
Mood induction may be affected by aspects of the of theoretical issues in conditioning, see Bugelski 1982;
transaction procedure, interactions with service pro- for a discussion ofthe consumer-behavior implications
viders, and physical settings (see Figure). At the point of conditioning, see McSweeney and Bierley 1984; Peter
of purchase, a mood may be induced by aspects of the and Nord 1982). In some cases, automatic behavioral
retail environment and by interactions with sales per- responses may be largely inborn or instinctive. Zajonc
sonnel. Because these mood inducers are contiguous (1980) discusses the universality of affective responses
with much in-store information acquisition and deci- among animal species, citing the example of the fright-
sion making, they may have a substantial impact on ened rabbit running from a snake. Plutchik (1980) has
such processes. Communications effects on consumers' postulated that feelings mediate situation perceptions
mood states include those effects due to media context and instinctive responses. Although Plutchik (1980) and
and advertisement content (see Figure). Media-context Zajonc (1980) provide phyllogenetic and ontogenic
effects relate to material that precedes or surrounds the support for an innate link between emotions and be-
advertiser's message in a communications vehicle. Ad- havior, research is needed to investigate the possibility
vertisement content may affect consumers' mood states of such a link between mood states and behavior.
through the use of emotional music, graphics, or copy. It may be more likely that associations between mood
states and behaviors may be learned from repeated ex-
EFFECTS OF MOOD STATES ON perience, socialization, or acculturation. For example,
PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES Cialdini and Kenrick (1976) found that an experimen-
tally induced depressed condition increased helpfulness
Research in psychology indicates that mood states among older children, but not among younger ones.
exert an important influence on behavior, judgment, The data were taken as support for the notion that so-
and recall (see Figure). As a result of this, several con- cialization forms the basis for the effects of negative
clusions about the effects of mood states emerge. Many mood on altruistic behavior.
of these findings are based upon laboratory studies and One may also view the link between mood states and
require replication under more realistic conditions. behavior as indirect, in the sense that the behavioral
effects of mood states may be somewhat mediated by
Effects of Mood States on Behavior their effects on expectations, evaluations, and judg-
ments. This view suggests that positive moods may in-
Positive moods appear to enhance the likelihood that crease the likelihood that a behavior will be performed
a host of behaviors may be performed (see Table 1). It by increasing the accessibility of positive associations
appears that a positive mood makes one kinder both to to the behavior, thus leading to more positive evalua-
oneself and to others (Underwood, Moore, and Rosen- tions of the behavior (Clark and Isen 1982).
284 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1
STUDIES INVESTIGATING THE EFFECTS OF MOOD STATES ON PSYCHOLOGICAL PROCESSES: INDUCTIONS AND FINDINGS

Study Induction Finding

Behavioral Effects

Berkowitz and (a) Success Success subjects were more willing to work for a
Connor (1966) (b) Failure dependent peer than control subjects. Failure subjects
(c) No experience on a preliminary irrelevant task expressed stronger dislike for their peer the greater their
peer's dependency on them.

Mischel, Coates, and (a) Success Relative to children in condition (b), those in condition (a)
Raskoff (1968) (b) Failure were more noncontingently generous to themselves under
some conditions.
Isen (1970) Receiving feedback that is: Subjects in the success condition (a) were more generous
(a) Above the norm-success and helpful than those in the failure condition (b).
(b) Below the norm-failure
Aderman (1972) Reading Velten statements designed to induce: Subjects who read the elation statements (a)

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


(a) Elation outperformed those who read the depression statements
(b) Depression (b) on a helping task. In addition, subjects in condition (a)
were more likely than those in condition (b) to volunteer
for a future unpleasant experiment, perhaps because the
latter groups resented their induced depression. This
finding can be contrasted with those of other
investigations of the effects of positive mood on the
likelihood of performance of unpleasant tasks. Research
indicates that performance is less likely for subjects in
positive mood conditions than for those in control
conditions (Forest et al. 1979; Isen and Simmonds 1978).
Isen and Levin Study 1: In each study, subjects in condition (a) were more willing
(1972) (a) Receiving cookies while studying in a library to help others than those in condition (b).
(b) No manipulation
Study 2:
(a) Finding a dime planted in a phone booth
(b) No manipulation

Regan, Williams, and Camera would not work. Experimenter implies: Subjects in condition (a) were more likely than those in
Sparling (1972) (a) The subject broke the camera condition (b) to perform an unrelated helping task.
(b) The misfunction was not the subject's fault

Moore, Underwood, Self-generated thoughts: Children in condition (a) contributed the most and those in
and Rosenham (a) Happy condition (b) contributed the least to a charity in the
(1973) (b) Sad experimenter's absence.
(c) Neutral

Isen, Horn, and (a) Success Success was associated with increased charitability in
Rosenham (1973) (b) Failure children subjects. The effect of failure depended on the
(c) Control circumstances of the failure.

Underwood et al. Self-generated thoughts: Relative to children in condition (b), those in condition (a)
(1973) (a) Happy reward themselves more generously in the experimenters'
(b) Sad absence.
(c) Neutral

Seeman and (a) Success Relative to children in the failure condition, those in the
Schwarz (1974) (b) Failure success condition chose a large delayed reward rather
than a small immediate reward.

Blevins and Murphy (a) Finding a dime planted in a phone booth No relationship was observed between finding a dime and
(1974) (b) No manipulation helping.

Fry (1975) Self-generated thoughts: Children in condition (a) resisted temptation longer than
(a) Happy those in condition (c), who in turn resisted longer than
(b) Unhappy those in condition (b).
(c) Neutral

(ContInued next page)


MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 285

TABLE l-(Continued)

Study Induction Finding

Behavioral Effects (Continued)

Donnerstein, (a) Viewing slides showing flowers, animals, and Condition (a) did not appear to influence helping, but
Donnerstein, and sunsets condition (b) was associated with enhanced willingness to
Munger (1975) (b) Viewing slides showing old people and migrant help. The authors explain this finding in terms of expiation
workers of guilt.
(c) Writing pro/con arguments
Moore, Clyburn, and Self-generated thoughts: Relative to children in condition (c) those in condition (a)
Underwood (1976) (a) Happy were more likely to choose a large delayed reward than a
(b) Sad small immediate reward and those in condition (b) were
(c) Neutral more likely to choose a small immediate reward than a
large delayed reward.
Cialdini and Kenrick Self-generated thoughts: Older children, but not younger ones, were more
(1976) (a) Sad generous when in condition (a) than when in condition (b)

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


(b) Neutral
Isen and Simmonds (a) Finding a dime planted in a phone booth Relative to subjects in condition (b), those in condition (a)
(1978) (b) No manipulation were more willing to read statements allegedly designed
to induce good moods and less willing to read statements
designed to induce bad moods.
Weyant (1978) Feedback on test: Helping was enhanced in condition (a) and dependent
(a) Positive upon the costs and benefits associated with the helping
(b) Negative task in condition (b).
No Test taken:
(c) Control
Batson et al. (1979) (a) Finding a dime planted in a phone booth Subjects who found the dime were more likely to help
(b) No manipulation another person and to acquire information than those who
did not.
Forest et al. (1979) False meter feedback regarding feelings: Condition (a) was associated with enhanced helping on an
(a) Positive agreeable task, but not a disagreeable one.
(b) Negative
(c) Neutral
Cunningham (1979) No induction, but sunshine, temperature, humidity, Sunshine related to enhanced self-reports of mood,
wind velocity, and lunar phase assessed greater willingness to assist an interviewer, and larger tips
for restaurant waitresses.
Fried and Berkowitz Subjects heard music: Condition (a) was associated with peaceful feelings,
(1979) (a) Mendelssohn's "Songs Without Words" condition (b) with joyful feelings, and condition (c) with
(b) Duke Ellington's "One O'Clock Jump" irritated feelings on self-report measures. In addition,
(c) John Coltrane's "Meditations" subjects in condition (a) were most apt to be helpful
immediately afterwards, significantly more so than those
No music heard: in conditions (c) or (d).
(d) Control condition

Effects on Affective Reactions and Judgments

Mood states bias evaluations of novel stimuli in mood-congruent directions


Griffitt (1970) Effective temperature High effective temperature associated with negative mood
and negative evaluations of anonymous others.
Laird (1974) Instructions to: Subjects appeared to feel more happy in condition (a) and
(a) Smile more angry in condition (b). In addition, cartoons viewed in
(b) Frown condition (a) were rated funnier than those viewed in
without awareness of the nature of their condition (b).
expressions
Veitch and Griffitt Heard broadcasts conveying: Subjects in condition (a) reported greater positive affect
(1976) (a) Good news and evaluated anonymous others more favorably than
(b) Bad news those in condition (b).
286 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

TABLE 1-(Continued)

Study Induction Finding

Effects on Affective Reactions and Judgments (Continued)

Isen and Shalker (a) Finding a dime planted in a phone booth Relative to subjects in condition (d), those in condition (a)
(1982) (b) Receiving success test feedback rated slides more favorably and those in condition (c)
(c) Receiving failure test feedback rated slides less favorably. The ratings of subjects in
(d) No manipulation condition (b) did not differ from those of subjects in
condition (d).
Mood states bias evaluations of familiar stimuli in mood-congruent directions
Isen et al. (1978) (a) Receiving a free gift Subjects in condition (a) rated products they owned more
(b) No manipulation favorably than those in condition (b).
Carson and Adams Reading Velten statements designed to induce: Expected enjoyableness of activities was enhanced in
(1980) (a) Elation condition (a) and diminished in condition (c).
(b) Depression

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


(c) Neutral mood
Schwarz and Clore Study 1 In both studies, relative to subjects in condition (b), those
(1983) (a) Self-generated happy life events in condition (a) indicated more satisfaction with their lives.
(b) Self-generated sad live events
Study 2
(a) Sunny weather
(b) Rainy weather
Mood states bias judgments of the likelihood of mood-congruent events
Masters and Furman Self-generated: Relative to children in condition (b) those in condition (a)
(1976) (a) Happy thoughts had a greater expectancy for positive unrelated
(b) Neutral thoughts serendipitous outcomes.
Johnson and (a) Reading an account of a tragic event Condition (a) was associated with increased estimates of
Tversky (1983) (b) Control the frequency of many risks and unrelated, undesirable
events.

Effects on recall
Exposure mood enhances recall of mood-congruent items
Bower (1981) Post hypnotic suggestion-i.e., hypnotizing subjects Subjects read stories with happy and sad characters
and asking them to relive (a) happy or (b) sad Subjects in condition (b) attended more to sad material,
experiences from their own lives. identified with the sad character from the story, and
recalled more about that character.

Bower, Gilligan, and Post hypnotic suggestion-Le., hypnotizing subjects Subjects read stories with happy and sad characters
Monteiro Experiment and asking them to recall (a) happy or (b) sad associated with both happy and sad events. Subjects in
5 (1981) experiences from their own lives. condition (a) remembered happy events better than sad
events and those in condition (b) remembered sad events,
regardless of the character with which the events were
associated.
Retrieval mood enhances recall of mood-congruent items
Isen et al. (1978) (a) Receiving a small gift Condition (a) was associated with the ability to recall
(b) No manipulation positive material in memory about products subjects own.

Teasdale and Reading Velten statements designed to induce: Time to retrieve pleasant memories of life experiences
Fogarty (1979) (a) Elation relative to time to retrieve unpleasant memories was
(b) Depression significantly longer in condition (b) than in condition (a).

Natale and Hantas Post-hypnotic suggestion: Condition (a) was associated with decreased recall for
(1982) (a) Elation negative life events and increased recall for positive
(b) Depression events. Condition (b) was associated with decreased
recall of positive life experiences and weaker memory
strength for positive infonmation about oneself.
Laird et al. (1982) Instructions to manipulate facial expressions without
awareness of the nature of the expressions. Mood
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 287

TABLE 1-(Continued)

Study Induction Finding

Effects on recall (Continued)

response to manipulated expressions was first


assessed in a separate procedure. Subjects whose
moods were affected were designated the self-
produced cue group.
Study 1: Study 1-ln the self-produced cue group, recall for anger-
(a) Frowning provoking editorials was significantly better in condition (a)
(b) Smiling and for humorous selections in condition (b). In the non-
self cue group, expressions did not affect recall.
Study 2: Study 2-ln the self-produced cue group, recall was
(a) Angry expression better for sentences consistent with expression. In the
(b) Sad expression non-self cue group, recall was not affected.
(c) Fearful expression

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


Clark and Waddell Receiving feedback about test performance: Subjects were asked to respond to descriptions of 3
(1983) (a) Positive situations with whatever thoughts came to mind first.
(b) Negative Subjects in condition (a) had significantly more positive
(c) No feedback first affective reactions to 2 out of 3 situations. Subjects in
condition (b) had more negative first affective reactions to
all 3 situations, but these differences were not statistically
significant.
Srull (1983a) Self-generated thoughts: Subjects recalled more attribute information that was
(a) Happy experiences incongruent with their retrieval conditions than material
(b) Sad experiences from own life that was congruent, perhaps due to cue overload.
Match between exposure and retrieval mood enhances recall
Bower et al. (1978) Post-hypnotic suggestion-hypnotized and asked to Match between learning and retrieval conditions facilitated
recall thoughts: recall only where confusion and interference may have
(a) Positive otherwise occurred.
(b) Negative
Bartlett and Telling children stories with appropriate pictures and Same condition upon exposure and retrieval facilitated the
Santrock (1979) experimenter behavior: generation of cues needed to perform free recall task, but
(a) Happy did not affect recognition or cued recall.
(b) Sad

NOTE: Findings that indicate somewhat limitad or atypical effects 01 mood states are ~alicizad.

Effects of Mood States on Affective evaluations of the likelihood of mood congruent events
Reactions and Judgments (Johnson and Tversky 1983; Masters and Furman
1976).2
In general, mood states seem to bias evaluations and The link between mood states and affective responses
judgments in mood congruent directions. 1 Folk wisdom and judgments may be viewed as both direct and in-
and experimental evidence agree: a good mood may be direct (see Figure). A direct linkage may involve asso-
associated with looking at one's world through rose- ciations in memory between mood states and affective
colored glasses, while a bad mood may analogously reactions; in this context, an affective reaction may be
color evaluations (Clark and Isen 1982). And mood viewed as a conditioned response. Griffitt and Guay
states appear to bias several types ofjudgments in mood- have postulated (1969, pp. 1-2):
congruent directions (see Table 1). These directions in-
clude evaluations of novel stimuli (Griffitt 1970; Isen evaluation of any given stimulus object is a positive linear
function of the proportion of stimuli with positive re-
and Shalker 1982; Laird 1974; Veitch and Griffitt 1976), inforcement properties associated with it. Stimuli with
evaluations of familiar stimuli (Carson and Adams
1980; Isen et al. 1978; Schwarz and Clore 1983), and
2Positive mood may be associated with increased yielding to per-
suasive messages (Dabbs and Janis 1965; Dribben and Brabender
'This general conclusion may not hold in all cases; products strongly 1979; Galizio and Hendrick 1972; Janis, Kaye, and Kirschner 1965).
associated with reducing negative mood states may be evaluated more Additional research is needed to investigate the limitations of this
favorably by consumers when in such negative states. finding and the pyschological processes involved.
288 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

FIGURE

A CONCEPTUAL MODEL OF THE ROLE


OF MOOD STATES IN CONSUMER BEHAVIOR

Service Point-of-
Encounters Purchase Communications
Stimuli

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


I
,--_.....1 = strategic marketing areas Consumers
c::::> = mood inducing tactics Mood
o = consumers' mood states States
..c:>.. - psychological processes

positive and negative reinforcement properties are hy- mood congruent items more accessible in memory, and
pothesized to act as unconditioned stimuli which evoke thus, more likely to affect evaluations (Isen et al. 1978).
implicit affective responses. Any discriminable stimulus, The information-processing approach is compatible
including a person, associated with such unconditioned with a situational perspective that views attitudes as a
stimuli becomes a conditioned stimulus capable of elic- function of readily accessible information.
iting the implicit affective responses. The affective re-
sponses, in tum, mediate overt evaluative responses such
as verbal assessments, preferences, and approach-avoid- Effects of Mood States on Recall
ance behaviors.
One way to understand the effects of mood on recall
Note that stimuli with positive reinforcement properties involves the use of network models of memory (Collins
may be viewed as inducers of positive mood states. and Loftus 1975). In such models, constructs may be
Conditioning may involve a wide range of mood in- conceptualized as nodes, and relationships between
ducers-including music (e.g., Gorn 1982; Milliman constructs as links. From this perspective, mood may
1982)-and may sometimes involve extensive accul- be viewed as stored with, or linked to, a set of constructs
turation and socialization (Zajonc and Markus 1982). or experiences in memory. As indicated in Table 1, re-
One may also view the association between positive call may be affected by the consumer's mood at the
mood inducers and favorable evaluations as indirect, time of exposure or retrieval, or by a match between
and influenced by information processing. This ap- exposure and retrieval moods. (For an extensive review
proach posits that the effects of positive mood may be of the effects of mood on cognition, see Isen 1984.)
mediated by such cognitive activity as information re- Mood at the time of exposure may affect what infor-
trieval; i.e., mood may affect evaluations by making mation is recalled by facilitating the retrieval of mood-
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 289

congruent items. This effect may be due to greater en- lication of existing studies should be encouraged and
coding-elaboration of mood-congruent material at the careful attention paid to nonsignificant results. Findings
time of exposure. The facilitating effect seems to appear that indicate somewhat limited or atypical effects of
only when retrieval cues are needed (Bower 1981; mood states are in italics in Table 1.
Bower, Gilligan, and Monteiro 1981). In addition, it is difficult to assess the scope of mood
Retrieval mood may facilitate overall retrieval of effects because induction procedures are almost com-
mood-congruent material from memory (Clark and pletely confounded with the types of effects investigated.
Waddell 1983; Isen et al. 1978; Natale and Hantas 1982; The studies reviewed in Table 1 suggest that, in general,
Teasdale and Fogarty 1979) and may enhance recall of memory effects have been investigated with relatively
specific mood-congruent information, if sufficient cues strong or direct induction procedures, while behavioral
are available for the identification of such items (Laird effects have involved milder, less direct manipulations.
et al. 1982). Retrieval mood may not enhance recall for Additional research is needed to replicate the findings
specific mood-congruent material if cues to identify of prior studies using new and varied procedures.
such items are inadequate. In the absence of sufficient Mood states are not expected to significantly affect
cues, recall for mood-congruent items may suffer, and behavior, evaluation, or recall under all conditions.
recall of mood-incongruent items may be enhanced Mood-state effects may be diminished or enhanced by
(Srull 1983a). circumstances related to situational ambiguity, degree

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


Several researchers (e.g., Bartlett and Santrock 1979; of arousal, time between mood induction and assess-
Bower, Monteiro, and Gilligan 1978) have found in- ment of effects, situational factors that encourage pre-
dications that unaided recall is enhanced when mood cision, and the specific moods involved.
at the time of retrieval matches mood at the time of The effects of mood states may be greater for situa-
encoding and when the encoding mood can serve as a tions that are somewhat ambiguous than for clear-cut
retrieval cue. If such a cue is unnecessary because of situations. Mood states may have the greatest impact
the properties of the task or of the stimulus, recall does when differences are marginal and no alternative dom-
not appear to be enhanced by a match between encoding inates the choice set (Clark and Isen 1982). This view
and retrieval moods. Analogous findings have been re- is supported by Isen and Shalker's (1982) study that
ported for other variables that affect an individual's found that:
psychological or physical state such as alcohol, drugs, 1. Subjects in experimentally induced positive/negative
or underwater submersion. (For a review of state de- mood conditions rated slides more/less favorably than
pendent effects on recall, see Eich 1980; c.f., Isen 1984.) those in a control mood condition, but that the rank
order of slide types (pleasant, neutral, and unpleasant)
remained constant across mood conditions.
Scope of the Effects of Mood States
2. Mood conditions appeared to influence evaluations
of neutral slides more than assessments of pleasant
The preceding discussion examined the effects of or unpleasant slides.
mood states on behavior, evaluation, and recall and
noted the specific limitations of these effects. I now turn These findings suggest that mood effects do not domi-
to more general issues, including the prevalence of the nate the mood-incongruent aspects of stimuli and that
impact of mood states and the factors that may atten- mood effects have a greater impact when evaluations
uate mood-related effects. are ambiguous than when they are clear-cut.
It is difficult to assess the prevalence of mood-related In addition, a moderate level of arousal may facilitate
effects because published studies may be biased toward mood effects, perhaps because naturally occurring feel-
research that reports statistically significant findings. ing states are often associated with arousal. Bartlett,
(N.B., research that failed to produce significant results Burleson, and Santrock (1982) found that the state de-
may have been performed, but not published.) Some pendent effects of mood on recall were absent when
of the difficulties involved in empirical mood studies relaxation exercises preceded an experimental task.
may have encouraged those investigators who failed to Clark, Milburg, and Ross (1983) found that arousal in
produce significant results to continually modify their combination with positive mood enhanced the effect of
procedures until they obtained significant findings. For the positive mood on subsequent judgments.
example, given the difficulties involved in evaluating It should be noted that the effect of any given mood
experimental mood-induction procedures, failure to is not longlasting (Schellenberg and Blevins 1973). The
observe postulated effects may be viewed as a failure of effect of a positive mood induction on the performance
the induction manipulation, not ofthe hypothesis under of a helping task has been found to last approximately
scrutiny. Such studies may be rejected by journals as twenty minutes (Isen, Clark, and Schwartz 1976).
inadequately supported by manipulation checks, or In addition, mood effects may be minimized by fac-
discarded by experimenters as pretest results. Given tors that encourage objectivity or precision. Under such
enough attempts, statistically significant results may be circumstances, controlled processes may be used to re-
obtained-if only by chance. I do not mean to imply trieve all relevant information from memory, and the
that all observed results are due to chance, but the rep- effect of mood on accessibility may be unimportant.
290 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

For example, when assessment encourages subjects to TABLE 2


respond with stored evaluations, responses do not ap- SUMMARY OF MOOD-STATE EFFECTS FOR THREE
pear to be biased by mood (Srull 1983b, 1984; West- CONSUMER BEHAVIOR SITUATIONS
brook 1980). Factors that encourage the use of con-
trolled processes may involve experimental settings and Situation
the perceived consequences of responses.
Point-of- Communications
Research is needed to investigate the limitations of Effects of mood Service purchase
the effects associated with specific mood states. In gen- states encounters stimuli Context Content
eral, the effects of negative moods may be more limited
than those of positive moods. Support is provided by Positive moods may
Schwarz and Clore (1983). Findings indicate that: enhance the likeli-
hood of perfor-
1. Subjects in positive mood conditions indicated more mance of behaviors
satisfaction with their lives than those in negative with positive ex-
mood conditions pected outcomes
and decrease the
2. The negative impact of bad mood on assessments of likelihood of perfor-
life satisfaction disappeared when subjects were di- mance of behaviors
with negative ex-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


rected to attribute negative feelings to transient ex-
pected outcomes. X X X X
ternal circumstances
Mood may bias
3. The positive impact of good mood was not similarly evaluations and like-
affected. lihood assessments
in mood congruent
These findings suggest that people in some negative directions. X X X
mood states may try to resist negative thinking and do
Exposure mood
so whenever possible. may enhance recall
The preceding discussion suggests that the effects of of mood-congruent
mood states may be greatest in those consumer-behavior items. X X X X
situations where stimuli are ambiguous, consumers are Retrieval mood may
somewhat aroused, induction and action are temporally enhance recall of
contiguous, perceived benefits of being precise are low, mood-congruent
and moods are positive. Opportunities for consumer- items. X X
behavior situations which meet these criteria arise dur- Match between ex-
ing service encounters, at the point-of-purchase, and posure and retrieval
with respect to advertising, i.e., communications con- moods may en-
hance recall. X X
tent and context. The potential for mood effects to have
a significant impact in each of these areas is explored NOTE: X = presence 01 effect.
in the next three sections.

SERVICE ENCOUNTERS dyadic nature of many service encounters. Service pro-


viders' mood states may affect their job performance,
The literature reviewed for this article suggests that whereas consumers' moods may affect consumer be-
mood states may at least marginally influence behavior, havior during a service encounter, the evaluation of the
affective responses, and recall. In order to evaluate the encounter and its result, and the subsequent recall of
importance of these influences on consumer behavior, the service encounter.
marketing-related settings and potential mood inducers
under the control of a manager must be examined. Key
findings from the preceding section are summarized in Mood Effects in Service Encounters
Table 2, and areas in which each finding may be im-
portant for service encounters, point-of-purchase stim- Mood may affect behavior during service encounters;
uli, and communications are indicated. The present consumers in good moods may be more likely to be
section examines the possible consumer-behavior im- helpful and easy to please than consumers in neutral
plications of the effects of consumers' mood states dur- moods. For example, consumers in good niOods may
ing service encounters. be willing to postpone gratification, follow doctors' or-
In the service sector, both consumers' and service ders, or bag their own groceries. On the other hand,
providers' mood states may be important. The study of service recipients in good moods may be less likely to
service-provider or seller behavior has been neglected perform acts with expected negative consequences such
by marketing research (Lutz 1979), but this behavior is as painful rehabilitation exercises or undergo medical
particularly important to the service sector, given the tests that may indicate illness. But when the need for
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 291

the behavior is unambiguous, mood effects on perfor- indicate that design-related factors can have powerful
mance may be minimal or nonexistent. However, the effects on human behavior. For example, in order to
effects of negative mood on behavior in the service sec- reduce the number of suicides attempted from Black-
tor may depend on the nature of the negative mood friar Bridge in England, the black bridge was repainted
(e.g., guilt versus anger), the reasons consumers give for bright green (Hatwick 1950, p. 188). In addition, Wener
the mood, and the costs/benefits of the behavior to be (1984) has noted that confusing or disorienting service
performed. settings may induce feelings of frustration and anxiety
Mood may affect consumer evaluation of service en- in service providers and recipients. These negative ef-
counters or the encounters' results by biasing the eval- fects may be ameliorated by using simple linear layouts
uations in mood-congruent directions. If consumers with orientation aids and maximum visibility. Aids may
form global impressions of a service encounter, they be overt (such as signs and guides) or covert (such as
may later recall the impressions rather than the specific artwork or plants that serve as landmarks).
facts on which the impressions were based. Consumers Designing a mood-inducing service setting involves
in good/bad moods may evaluate novel and familiar a consideration of the interaction of the setting with
encounters more positively/negatively than consumers consumers' perceptions of other facets of the setting's
in neutral moods. sponsor. Kotler and Rath (1983) have de-emphasized
Exposure mood may affect a consumer's subsequent the role ofindividual components and have stressed the

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


recall of a service encounter or its results by enhancing importance of the overall design of a sponsor's settings,
the consumer's recall of mood-congruent items. For image, and products. Recent efforts in environmental
example, customers who are in a good mood when they psychology have developed research techniques to fa-
open bank accounts may be more likely to recall positive cilitate the investigation of environments as contextual
information about their accounts later. Mood may affect settings rather than as sets of components (Stokols
the recall/retrieval of past service encounters by en- 1982). Research is needed to explore both the effects of
hancing recall of mood-congruent items, which, in tum, various marketing settings on consumer behavior and
may be associated with biased assessments of the like- the mediating role of mood states. Studies are also
lihood of mood-congruent events. For example, patients needed that compare the impact of marketer-controlled
admitted to hospitals in bad moods may retrieve more factors to the impact of factors that marketers do not
negative information about past hospital experiences control.
than those in good moods and may be more likely to
presume the worst about an impending hospital stay. Procedures and Interactions. For monadic interac-
tions (e.g., customer/machine transactions), procedures
may be designed to move customers from an entry
Mood Induction in Service Encounters mood toward a desired mood. Frequently, these pro-
cedures may involve such positive mood inducers as
Service encounters offer marketers many opportu- clear instructions, user-friendly systems, and positive
nities for mood inductions that are temporally contig- feedback throughout a task.
uous to a transaction and so are of potential strategic For dyadic interactions (e.g., customer/service-pro-
importance. The service setting, the procedure, and the vider transactions), the moods of both parties may affect
interaction with the service provider present opportu- interactions. Service companies must develop interac-
nities for marketers to influence or respond to mood tion strategies and tactics appropriate to customers with
states. different moods. If consumers vary widely in mood, it
may sometimes be efficient to segment on the mood
Physical Surroundings. Aspects of ambient envi- variable and train service providers to specialize in
ronments have been found to correlate with assessments serving consumers in specific mood states.
of mood and postulated mood effects. For example, nice Because both monadic and dyadic interactions in-
weather has been found to correlate positively with self- volve the use of limited mechanical or human resources,
assessments of mood and with tips left in restaurants consumers must often wait to engage in a transaction.
(Cunningham 1979). Effective temperature has been Waiting is generally considered to be a disagreeable ex-
found to correlate negatively with several measures of perience, and it may be so distasteful to some consumers
mood and with evaluations of anonymous others (Grif- that they will hire others to wait in their place (Geist
fitt 1970). Kotler (1974) has postulated that the effects 1984). The negative moods induced by waiting for ser-
of ambient environments should be more important in vice may persist through contiguous service encounters.
situations where products are purchased or where the Suggested tactics for improving waiting consumers'
seller has design options. Such conditions are frequently mood states have been proposed by Maister (1984). In
met in the service sector. addition, consumers may look for ways to pull them-
The many aspects of an environment's physical sur- selves out of the bad moods associated with waiting for
roundings that are under marketer control encourage service. Marketers may find it advantageous to inves-
optimism about the potential for inducing moods that tigate and encourage such consumer-originated mech-
will serve specific marketing ends. There is evidence to anisms.
292 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

Service companies may also benefit by examining the the effects of mood on the evaluation of new and fa-
moods of their employees and understanding the role miliar brands. Also, the potential implications for un-
of these feelings during interactions with consumers. derstanding trial and repeat purchasing should be in-
Hochschild (1983) provides evidence to show that at vestigated.
least some service providers (stewardesses and bill col- Information acquisition at the point-of-purchase may
lectors) do a great deal of emotional work, e.g., mood have important effects on subsequent brand evaluation.
management. Companies may find it profitable to in- The consumer's mood at the time of initial exposure
stitute programs to help service providers handle their to a product may affect the valence of product features
own feelings and the interaction of their moods with readily accessible to subsequent recall. This effect may
the customer's mood. be particularly important in family buying; i.e., expo-
sure effects may influence the valence of stored infor-
Current Status and Future Potential mation brought home by information gatherers and
used to make purchase decisions.
Many service encounters involve at least minimal In-store mood may affect the retrieval of information
levels of ambiguity and arousal and they do not en- from memory related to brands, personal usage expe-
courage service recipients to be particularly objective rience, or advertisements. Compared to shoppers in
or precise. Mood induction can be contiguous to the neutral moods, shoppers in good moods may be more
service encounter, increasing the likelihood that its ef-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


likely to retrieve positive stored information.
fects will have an impact on transactions. However, the If the mood created by a brand's advertising matches
magnitude of the induced effects is an empirical ques- the mood induced at the point-of-purchase, message
tion that requires further research. recall may be enhanced as the result of state-dependent
Additionally, because service providers have personal memory effects. To induce such effects, manufacturers
contact with consumers, they can adjust their tactics to may select appropriate retail outlets or influence in-
suit consumers' mood states. Training procedures might store settings near their merchandise by using special
be used to help service providers interpret consumers' personnel, events, colors, and lighting.
moods, perhaps by interpreting facial cues. (For a dis-
cussion of one interpretation technique, see Weinberg
and Gottwald 1982.) Mood Induction at Point-of-Purchase
Consumers' mood states at the point-of-purchase
may be influenced by physical settings and interactions
POINT-OF -PURCHASE STIMULI with sales personnel. Belk's work on situational effects
in buyer behavior emphasized the importance of con-
Store atmospherics and interactions with salespeople sumer subjective reactions to environmental stimuli and
may affect the consumer's mood at the point-of-pur- called for a taxonomy of environmental variables (Belk
chase in a retail setting. In turn, mood states may in- 1974, 1975). Kotler (1974) has suggested that store at-
fluence purchase behavior, brand evaluation, and in- mospherics may be especially important when stores
formation acquisition. carry similar product lines and are equally convenient
(which is often the case in retailing today).
Mood Effects at Point-of-Purchase There are indications that environmental factors can
significantly influence evaluations. Maslow and Mintz
Positive mood states at the point-of-purchase may (1956) found that evaluations made in a "beautiful"
both increase shoppers' willingness to perform tasks room were significantly higher than those made in either
with positive expected outcomes, and decrease their an "average" room or an "ugly" room. Griffitt (1970)
willingness to perform behaviors with negative expected found that high ambient temperature was associated
outcomes. In order to anticipate the effects of positive with negative assessments of mood and with less fa-
moods on target activities, we must be able to categorize vorable ratings of anonymous others. If these findings
consumers' own assessments of whether their behavior can be generalized to product evaluations, they might
will lead to positive or negative outcomes and we must suggest that comfortable settings may enhance mer-
be able to understand the effects of consumers' mood chandise evaluation. Obermiller and Bitner (1984),
states on their assessments. For some activities, valences however, found that under some conditions favorable
of expected outcomes may vary widely across con- environments may be associated with unfavorable
sumers. For example, trying on clothing may be asso- product evaluations; i.e., the environments can distract
ciated with positive outcomes for some shoppers and consumers and detract from merchandise. Obermiller
with negative outcomes for others. Expected outcomes and Bitner found indications that atmosphere had a
may be related to media exposure (e.g., reading fashion marginally greater (p = 0.14) effect on evaluations when
magazines) or responsiveness to marketing-mix vari- subjects were instructed to browse in a simulated shop-
ables (e.g., being greatly influenced by advertising). ping environment than when they were instructed to
In-store mood may also affect the evaluation of fa- evaluate products. This suggests that in-store environ-
miliar and novel stimuli. Research is needed to compare ments may be even more important for understanding
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 293

shopping behavior than for investigating choice be- who attempt to use mood-related strategies-especially
havior. at the point-of-purchase-must maintain intense, in-
Donovan and Rossiter (1982) provide indications formal contact with their consumers.
that mood states induced by retail environments may In addition, research is needed to examine the effects
affect purchase intentions. In a study, measures of mood of mood states on such basic decisions as whether or
(arousal and pleasantness) and purchase intentions were not to shop, what to shop for, and whether to shop alone
assessed by students who were randomly assigned to or with others. Note that the behavioral effects of mood
visit 66 stores and fill out questionnaires in each. The states at the point-of-purchase may be mediated by
relationship of mood measures to assessments of be- whether the consumer anticipates that the shopping trip
havioral intentions in each environment were explored. will be a positive or negative experience. To fully un-
Findings indicated that (1) for stores rated as pleasant, derstand the effects of point-of-purchase moods, mar-
shopping intentions increased with increased levels of keters must gain insight into the role of shopping in the
arousal, and (2) for stores rated neutral or unpleasant, consumer's life.
intentions were unrelated to arousal. Note that biases
may exist due to students assessing their own in-store
moods and purchase intentions. In addition, whenever COMMUNICATIONS STIMULI:
possible consumers may subjectively self-select stores CONTEXT

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


that induce positive moods and avoid those which in-
duce negative moods. Thus, some settings may induce Service encounters and point-of-purchase stimuli
negative moods in some individuals, but stores which may induce moods that affect on-site consumer behav-
induce negative moods in all consumers may not be ior. Analogously, marketing communications, e.g., ad-
able to survive. This suggests that it is inappropriate to vertising, may affect at-home consumer responses by
label stores as pleasant or unpleasant without specifying inducing mood states from the context in which the
the group for which such subjective assessments apply. communication appears. The moods created by the
A conservative interpretation of the reported findings context in which ads appear may be quite strong-e.g.,
appears to be warranted; the links among atmospherics, the negative moods induced by "The Day After"-and
mood, and purchase intentions require further research. may affect the mood states induced by exposure to ad-
A second way in which in-store mood may be induced vertising messages (Gardner and Raj 1979). In tum,
involves interactions with sales personnel. On an ag- these mood states may affect behavior, evaluation, and
gregate level, the tone set by salespeople may induce recall with respect to advertised brands.
appropriate moods, e.g., an upbeat mood in a disco
boutique. On a more personal level, salespeople may Effects of Context-Induced Moods
develop relationships with specific customers and adjust
their selling tactics to an individual's moods. Research Positive moods induced by media contexts may affect
is needed to explore the relationship between mood- behavioral responses to advertising strategies. Insights
related sales techniques and success on the selling floor. into this thesis may be gleaned from two studies re-
ported by Cunningham, Steinberg, and Grev (1980).
Current Status and Future Potential The findings of Study 1 indicated that either a positive
mood induction (in this case, finding a dime in a phone
The effects of mood states on consumer behavior at booth) or a negative mood induction such as guilt (in
the point-of-purchase may be substantial and poten- this case, making subjects feel that they had broken the
tially important for marketing. Physical settings and experimenter's camera) increased helping (in this case,
interactions with store personnel may be powerful mood picking up dropped papers). The findings of Study 2
inducers, contiguous with much decision making. indicated that the positive mood manipulation in-
Findings from empirical studies of mood effects con- creased helping (in this case, donating money to a char-
ducted in shopping malls (e.g., Isen et al. 1978) indicate ity) only when a request stressing the desirability of
that the level of arousal associated with walking around helping was made, while the guilt manipulation pro-
a mall is great enough to make mood effects observable. duced increases only when a request stressing an obli-
Point-of-purchase environments are often ambiguous gation to help was made. The Cunningham et al. find-
and rarely encourage precise processing. However, re- ings for the positive mood condition are consistent with
search is needed to investigate the magnitude of mood findings (Forest et al. 1979; Isen and Simmonds 1978)
effects and the feasibility of evoking desired moods from that suggest that subjects in a good mood may readily
different consumers. generate positive associations toward donating money
In many cases, designing point-of-purchase displays when presented with a request that stresses the desir-
and retailing environments to induce particular moods ability of helping, but feel that they have enough per-
involves the selection of sets of mood-related symbols sonal power to resist the coercive appeal of a request
(see Kotler and Rath 1983). To select appropriate sym- stressing an obligation to help. Cunningham et al.
bols, marketers must be aware of the current trends and (1980) discuss a variety of mechanisms that are consis-
fads of their target market. This suggests that marketers tent with the performance of subjects in the guilty mood
294 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

condition and suggest the need for research to explore selected by members of a target audience may help ad-
the proposed mechanisms. Research is also needed to vertisers develop appropriate strategies and tactics.
extend these findings to moods of different types and For some products, contexts which induce negative
to the product marketing domain. moods may be more effective than those which induce
Media exposure may also induce mood states that positive moods. Although consumers may avoid con-
may, in tum, affect beliefs about products. Axelrod texts which are extremely unpleasant or threatening,
(1963) found that the mood states induced by viewing those contexts that induce mildly negative moods may
emotional films appeared to enhance subjects' assess- increase consumers' assessments that a negatively va-
ments that use of a product would lead to the induced lenced event is likely to occur. This, in tum, may in-
mood state. crease the consumer's self-perceived need for the prod-
Finally, moods induced by exposure to commercials ucts (e.g., insurance) associated with such an event, in
may enhance the learning of mood-congruent message spite of the limited effects associated with negative
arguments. Yuspeh (1979) found that the individual's moods.
recall of commercials was greatly affected by the pro- At times, marketers may have little control over the
grams in which they were viewed, but she did not in- story line or advertisement immediately preceding their
vestigate the specific mood states induced. Research is messages, so they may have little control over the moods
needed to explore qualitative media effects in general induced by media context. In such cases, media man-

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


and the mediating role of mood states in particular. agers cannot effectively implement mood-oriented ap-
proaches, but the effects of context-induced mood may
be important for copytesting. Since advertisements are
Mood Induction Via Communications frequently tested in a single context, the mood induced
Context by that context may bias the results of the test. Research
The material preceding an advertisement may in- is needed to assess the magnitude of such biases and to
determine the types of advertisements whose ratings
clude such cognitive mood inducers as positive or neg-
ative statements and such noncognitive mood inducers are most strongly affected.
as scary or happy music. Although a full discussion of Some insights are provided by Isen and Shalker's
the techniques involved in the induction of feelings in (1982) work with slides oflandscapes. As discussed ear-
media contexts is beyond the scope of this article, it lier, results indicate that the assessments of stimuli that
should be noted that laboratory studies involving simple were neutrally rated were more strongly influenced by
verbal messages have found that statements in radio or induced moods than the assessments of stimuli that
newspaper reports can influence mood under forced were rated favorably or unfavorably. Research is needed
exposure conditions. Veitch and Griffitt (1976) have to explore these effects for exposure to advertisements
manipulated the affective content of presented infor- under natural viewing conditions and to investigate the
mation and found that hearing good/bad news was as- possibility that the resulting attitude toward the adver-
sociated with measures of positive/negative moods. tisement is strong enough, and sufficiently associated
Johnson and Tversky (1983), using newspaper reports with a brand, to affect brand attitude. If the mood ->
of negative events, found that self-report mood scales attitude toward advertising -+ attitude toward brand
yielded significant differences. The effects of such in- chain is supported by future evidence, this suggests that
duction procedures should be investigated to gain in- there may be a general advantage to placing advertise-
sight into the importance of mood states relative to that ments in contexts which induce positive moods. Ad-
of other mediators of context effects (Hornstein et al. ditionally, the effect may be more critical for neutrally-
1975). rated advertisements than for those advertisements that
produce more extreme evaluations. The relationship
between neutral advertisement ratings and aspects of
Current Status and Future Potential copy execution-e.g., pictures versus words-is unclear.
Research is now underway that examines the effects of
Evidence for the mediating role of context-induced mood-inducing media contexts on the effectiveness of
mood on consumer response to advertising is extremely emotional and informational copy strategies. Finally,
limited. Research is needed to explore the range of media contexts may sometimes encourage very low
moods induced under normal exposure conditions. If arousal levels; research is needed to investigate the me-
consumers self-select programs, newspapers, and mag- diating role of arousal on mood effects associated with
azines that make them feel good, the range may be nar- media contexts.
row, and the impact of context-induced moods may be
small. COMMUNICATIONS STIMULI:
In some cases, target-market consumers may have
extremely selective media habits, leaving marketers little CONTENT
latitude in which to select vehicles with specific mood- Media contexts may provide background moods for
inducing properties. In such cases, understanding the advertising exposures, but for many product classes,
effects of the moods commonly induced by the media moods induced by brand advertisements playa critical
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 295

role in brand identification and evaluation. Such moods statements, and such noncognitive mood inducers as
may affect purchase behavior, brand evaluation, and scary or happy music. Some advertisers (e.g., Ralph
information acquisition. Lauren) buy blocks of consecutive pages in a periodical,
in part to accentuate the mood-inducing properties of
Effects of Content-Induced Mood States their ads. In addition, Moore (1982) has presented ev-
idence that subliminal stimuli may influence mood
Feelings induced by direct-marketing commercials
states without conscious awareness.
may affect consumer behavior with respect to advertised
The importance of advertising-induced feelings is
brands when mood induction is contiguous to required
underscored by efforts to assess noncognitive reactions
action-mail-in or phone-in orders. The positive feel-
to advertisements. Schlinger (1979) and Wells et al.
ings induced by commercials may enhance the likeli-
(1971) have developed measurement instrum~nts to as-
hood that consumers will engage in purchase activities
sess individuals' emotional reactions to advertisements.
associated with positive outcomes. If so, commercials
Batra and Ray (1984) have proposed a coding scheme
that induce positive moods may be effective for the di-
to classify consumer affective responses to advertise-
rect marketing of products associated with improving
ments.
one's home or one's life.
The effects of advertisement-induced negative moods
may be more complex. Extremely depressing commer- Current Status and Future Potential

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


cials may attract attention because oftheir relative rar- Research is needed to evaluate the effects of mood-
ity, or they may be ignored due to a perceptual defense related advertising strategies on consumer behavior.
(as documented by the fear-appeal literature, see Stern- Wells (1983) has suggested that mood should playa
thaI and Craig 1974). However, the negative moods in- more central role in the advertising for products asso-
duced by commercials may increase consumers' per- ciated with pleasurable usage experiences, i.e., approach
ceptions that a negative outcome is likely. If this is so, products. This guideline requires a priori knowledge of
then commercials that induce mildly negative moods consumers' feelings toward product use and is consistent
may be effective direct marketing tools for disaster-re- with previously cited findings indicating that positive
lated products, e.g., life insurance and burglar alarms, moods may be associated with the performance of be-
in spite of the somewhat limited impact of negative haviors that have positive expected outcomes.
moods. Research is needed to investigate these possi- Researchers in consumer behavior are currently ex-
bilities. ploring the role of affect-inducing commercials from
Advertisements that induce positive exposure moods many perspectives. Shimp (1981) has noted that moods
may facilitate the learning, integration, and acquisition induced by advertisement content are not contiguous
of favorable material and may enhance the evaluation to in-store decision making and that research is needed
of advertised brands. Such ads are also likely to be fa- to investigate possible reinduction at the point-of-pur-
vorably evaluated; at times, a positive relationship be- chase. Shimp has also postulated that an attitude toward
tween attitude toward the advertisement and attitude an advertisement has an emotional aspect which may
toward the brand may be observed (see Gardner 1985; be related to feeling states, and Allen and Madden
Mitchell and Olson 1981; Shimp and Yokum 1982). (1983) have empirically examined this compo~ent. ~n
Advertisements may be designed to elicit the mood addition researchers have investigated the relatIOnshIp
states associated with particular life situations or ex- of emoti~nal stimuli in advertising to advertiser goals
periences. When such experiences occur, the elicited (e.g., Mizerski and White 1985), low involvement ex-
moods may key the retrieval of advertisements asso- posure situations (e.g., Batra and Ray 1983; Ray and
ciated with these moods, because of state-dependent Batra 1983), the assessment of advertising effects (e.g.,
memory effects. For example, thinking about loved ones Leckenby and Stout 1985), physiological measures (e.g.,
in distant places may elicit warm, sentimental feelings. Kroeber-Riel 1979, 1984), and advertisement sequence
These feelings may, in turn, elicit the retrieval of sim- (e.g., Aaker, Stayman, and Hagerty 1985). Although
ilarly toned advertisements for Hallmark cards. How- much research is being done to gain insight into the
ever, such tactics are not expected to be equally effective effects of mood-inducing advertisements, their preva-
for all brands and product classes. A Hallmark cam- lence and importance encourages even greater efforts.
paign may be able to take advantage of state-dependent
memory effects, because the company has a favorable
image, and product use is associated with feeling states DISCUSSION
that, in turn, are linked to particular life experiences.
Implications for Marketing Research
Mood Induction Via Communications The preceding sections of this article have examined
Content areas where it may be wise to consider the effects of
Advertisements, like media contexts, may include mood on consumer behavior. Such mood effects may
such cognitive mood inducers as positive or negative also have important implications for developing mar-
296 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

keting research techniques to assess consumers' atti- 1. Mood states may have important effects on consumer
tudes and predict behavior. behavior with respect to services, but research is
In some contexts, the effects of respondents' moods needed to assess the magnitude of such effects. There
on their evaluations may be viewed as a biasing nuisance appear to be many opportunities for mood induction
factor. Peterson and Sauber (1983) present evidence for in the service sector, e.g., settings, procedures, and
interactions with service providers. Mood induction
such biases and provide a measurement instrument to can be contiguous to the service encounter, increasing
take them into account. If respondents' moods upon the likelihood that its effects will have an impact on
completion of a questionnaire are distributed in the transactions. In addition, because service providers
same way as their moods during relevant activities (e.g., have personal contact with consumers, they can adjust
shopping), the biases would not be critical for aggregate- their tactics to suit consumers' mood states.
level analysis. However, if moods are systematically re-
2. The effects of mood states at the point-of-purchase
lated to filling out questionnaires or going shopping, may be substantial and potentially important for
biases due to the effects of mood states may be prob- marketing action, but research is needed to investigate
lematical. If participation in a survey is viewed as an the strength of mood effects. Although physical set-
experience that has a favorable expected outcome, con- tings and interactions with store personnel may be
sumers in positive moods may be more inclined to powerful mood inducers contiguous with much de-
complete questionnaires than those in neutral or neg- cision making, research is needed that examines the

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


ative moods. This would suggest that mood states in effects of mood states on such basic decisions as
the respondent sample may be skewed in a positive di- whether or not to shop, what to shop for, and whether
rection. to shop alone or with others. In addition, the behav-
Mood in the population of shoppers may be bimod- ioral effects of mood states at a point-of-purchase may
be mediated by whether the consumer anticipates that
ally distributed. People in good moods may choose to the shopping trip will be a positive or negative expe-
perform or avoid consumption-related behavior, de- rience. To fully understand the effects of point-of-
pending on their assessments of the likelihood of the purchase moods, marketers must gain insight into the
activities being associated with positive or negative role of shopping in the consumer's life.
outcomes. Consumers in bad moods may also choose
to shop, perhaps to cheer themselves up. Langer (1983) 3. Although context-induced moods may significantly
affect consumer response to advertising, there is a
has suggested that difficult economic conditions may dearth of evidence supporting this claim. Research is
increase consumers' needs for products to serve as needed to explore the range and impact of moods in-
treats and emotional charges. duced under normal exposure conditions. Addition-
To gain insight into the magnitude of biases resulting ally, the following questions must be addressed: (1)
from the effects of mood states, the distribution of for which types of advertisements are the effects of
moods of individuals engaged in activities of interest context-induced mood states most significant, and (2)
must be examined. Activities related to participation when should contexts be sought that induce specific
in research should be investigated as well as shopping, positive or negative moods? Note that the role of con-
buying, and consuming activities. If the distribution of text-induced moods may be more important for copy-
testing procedures than it is for media selection if in-
moods during the completion of a questionnaire differs stitutional factors limit the control media managers
from the distribution during an activity of interest, have over the immediate context in which advertising
biases resulting from this difference may require closer is placed.
examination.
4. The role of mood states induced by advertising has
recently received some well-deserved attention, but
Implications for Marketing Action many important issues remain unresolved. It is clear
that advertisements can affect consumer moods, but
the impact of these effects may depend on the adver-
I have used the phrase "mood states" to refer to the tiser's purpose, product, and target audience. Al-
general, pervasive, affective states that are transient and though much research is being done to address these
particularized to specific times and situations. A con- issues, the prevalence and importance of mood-in-
ceptual framework has been presented to depict the ef- ducing advertising encourages even greater efforts.
fects of consumers' mood states and their potential rel-
evance to marketing strategy and tactics. Findings from
the psychological literature were reviewed, and they in- Directions for Future Research
dicated that mood states affect behavior, evaluation,
and recall. I have examined the implications of these The nature of the preceding discussion has necessarily
effects for consumer behavior in three areas: service en- been very tentative, but it should help guide thinking
counters, point-of-purchase stimuli, and communica- and should serve as a basis for research in this area. In
tions (context and content). In addition, I have assessed addition to the specific issues discussed above, more
the feasibility of mood induction in each area. Several general questions must be addressed:
conclusions and unanswered questions emerge for each 1. Does social responsibility suggest limitations for the
area: appropriate use of mood-induction strategies and
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 297

tactics? This issue is particularly problematical with o/Experimental Child Psychology, 34 (August-Decem-
respect to the induction of negative moods and the ber), 59-76.
use of mood inductions in advertising directed at Batra, Rajeev and Michael L. Ray (1983), "Advertising Sit-
children. uations: The Implications of Differential Involvement
and Accompanying Affect Responses," in In/ormation
2. Can marketer-induced moods significantly affect Processing Research In Advertising, ed. Richard J. Harris,
consumer behavior? Research is needed that compares Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 127-151.
the impact of marketer-induced moods to moods in- - - - and Michael L. Ray (1984), "Affective Responses
duced by other aspects of the consumer's life. Research Mediating Acceptance of Advertising," working paper,
is also needed to explore the interaction of marketer- Columbia University, New York, NY 10027.
controlled inductions and pre-existing mood states. Batson, C. Daniel, Jay Coke, Fred Chard, Debra Smith, and
Guidelines are needed to help marketers assess the Antonia Taliaferro (1979), "Generality of the 'Glow of
potential value of mood-oriented approaches under Goodwill': Effects of Mood on Helping and Information
alternative sets of circumstances. Acquisition," Social Psychology Quarterly, 42 (2), 176-
3. How can marketers design strategies and implement 179.
tactics to influence consumers' moods? Psychometric Baumann, Donald J., Robert B. Cialdini, and Douglas T.
measurement instruments that assess the effects of Kenrick, (1981), "Altruism as Hedonism: Helping and
mood induction procedures must be developed and Self-Gratification as Equivalent Responses," Journal of
evaluative criteria established. Also, the implications Personality and Social Psychology, 40 (6), 1039-1046.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


for interdepartmental relations must be considered. Belk, Russell (1974), "An Exploratory Assessment of Situa-
For example, lawyers must be consulted before in- tional Effects in Buyer Bheavior," Journal of Marketing
dividual-specific mood induction procedures that may Research, 11 (May), 156-163.
appear discriminatory are implemented. Personnel - - - (1975), "Situational Variables and Consumer Behav-
departments and labor unions must be involved in ior," Journal of Consumer Research, 2 (December), 157-
the development of mood-management training pro- 164.
grams for employees. - - - (1984), "Applications of Mood Inducement in Buyer
Behavior," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 11,
In spite of the need for further empirical work, there ed. Thomas Kinnear, Provo, UT: Association for Con-
are indications that consumer-mood states may influ- sumer Research, 544-547.
ence behavior, evaluation, and recall. Although many Berkowitz, Leonard and William Connor (1966), "Success,
unanswered questions must be addressed, the effects of Failure, and Social Responsibility," Journal of Person-
mood states on consumer behavior with respect to ser- ality and Social Psychology, 4 (6), 664-669.
Blevins, Gregory and Terrance Murphy (1974), "Feeling Good
vice encounters, point-of-purchase stimuli, and com- and Helping: Further Phonebooth Findings," Psycho-
munications seem to merit further exploration. logical Reports, Vol. 34 (February), p. 326.
Bower, Gordon (1981), "Mood and Memory," American
[Received April 1984. Revised June 1985] Psychologist, 36 (2), 129-148.
- - - , Stephen Gilligan, and Kenneth Monteiro (1981),
"Selectivity of Learning Caused by Affective States,"
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110 (De-
REFERENCES cember),451-473.
- - - , Kenneth Monteiro, and Stephen Gilligan (1978),
Aaker, David A., Douglas M. Stayman, and Michael R.. Hag- "Emotional Mood as a Context for Learning and Recall,"
erty (1986), "Warmth in Advertising: Measurement, Journal 0/ Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 17,
Impact, and Sequence Effects," Journal of Consumer 573-585.
Research, 12 (March), in press. Bugelski, B. R. (1982), "Learning and Imagery," Journal of
Aderman, David (1972), "Elation, Depression, and Helping Mental Imagery, 6 (2), 1-92.
Behavior," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Carson, Tracey and Henry Adams (1980), "Activity Valence
24 ( I), 9 1-101. as a Function of Mood Change," Journal of Abnormal
Allen, Chris and Thomas Madden (1983), "Examining the Psychology, 89 (3), 368-377.
Link Between Attitude Towards an Ad and Brand At- Cialdini, Robert and Douglas Kenrick (1976), "Altruism as
titude: A Classical Conditioning Approach," Working Hedonism: A Social Development Perspective on the
Paper No. 83-30, Management Research Center, School Relationship of Negative Mood State and Helping,"
of Business Administration, University of Massachusetts, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34 (5),
Amherst, MA 01003. 907-914.
Andrews, Frank and Stephen Withey (1976), Social Indicators Clark, Margaret and Alice Isen (1982), "Toward Under-
of Well Being, New York: Plenum Press. standing the Relationship Between Feeling States and
Axelrod, Joel (1963), "Induced Moods and Attitudes Towards Social Behavior," in Cognitive Social Psychology, eds.
Products," Journal ofAdvertising Research, 3 (June), 19- Albert Hastorf and Alice Isen, New York: Elsevier/North-
24. Holland, 73-108.
Bartlett, James and John Santrock (1979), "Affect-Dependent - - - , Sandra Milberg, and John Ross (1983), "Arousal Cues
Episodic Memory in Young Children," Child Develop- Arousal-Related Material in Memory: Implications for
ment, 50 (2), 513-518. Understanding Effects of Mood on Memory," Journal
- - - , Georgia Burleson, and John Santrock (1982), "Emo- o/Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22 (December)
tional Mood and Memory in Young Children," Journal 633-649.
298 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

- - - , and Barbara Waddell (1983), "Effects of Moods on in Consumer Research, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear,
Thoughts About Helping, Attraction and Information Provo, UT: Association for Consumer Research, 525-
Acquisition," Social Psychology Quarterly, 46 (1), 31- 529.
35. Geist, William (1984), "They're Hiring Others to Stand in
Collins, Allan M. and Elizabeth A. Loftus (1975), "A Spread- Line," New York Times, (June 6).
ing-Activation Theory of Semantic Processing," Psycho- Goldman-Eisler, Frieda (1960), "Breastfeeding and Character
logical Review, 82 (November), 407-428. Formation," in Personality in Nature, Society, and Cul-
Cunningham, Michael (1979), "Weather, Mood, and Helping ture, eds. C. Kluckholn and H. Murray, New York: Alfred
Behavior: Quasi Experiments With the Sunshine Sa- A. Knopf.
maritan," Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, Gorn, Gerald (1982), "The Effects of Music in Advertising
37 (11), 1947-1956. on Choice Behavior: A Classical Conditioning Ap-
- - - , Jeff Steinberg, and Rita Grev (1980), "Wanting to proach," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Winter), 94-101.
and Having to Help: Separate Motivations for Positive Griffitt, William (1970), "Environmental Effects on Inter-
Mood and Guilt-Induced Helping," Journal of Person- personal Affective Behavior: Ambient Temperature and
ality and Social Psychology, 38 (2), 181-192. Attraction," Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Dabbs, James M. and Irving L. Janis (1965), "Why Does Eat- ogy, 15 (3), 240-244.
ing While Reading Facilitate Opinion Change?," Journal - - - and Peter Guay (1969), "'Object' Evaluation and
of Experimental Social Psychology, 1 (May), 133-144. Conditioned Affect," Journal of Experimental Research
Donnerstein, Edward, Marcia Donnerstein, and Gerry Mun- in Personality, 4 (July), 1-8.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


ger (1975), "Helping Behavior as a Function of Pictorially Hatwick, Melvin (1950), How to Use Psychology for Better
Induced Moods," The Journal of Social Psychology, 97 Advertising, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall, p. 188.
(December), 221-225. Hirschman, Elizabeth and Morris Holbrook (1982), "Hedonic
Donovan, Robert and John Rossiter (1982), "Store Atmo- Consumption: Emerging Concepts, Methods and Prop-
sphere: An Environmental Psychology Approach," ositions," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 92-101.
Journal of Retailing, 58 (Spring), 34-57. Hochschild, Arlie (1983), The Managed Heart: The Com-
Dribben, Ellen and Virginia Brabender (1979), "The Effect mercialization of Human Feeling, Berkeley, CA: Uni-
of Mood Inducement Upon Audience Receptiveness," versity of California Press.
The Journal of Social Psychology, 107 (February), 135- Holbrook, Morris and Elizabeth Hirschman (1982), "The
136. Experiential Aspects of Consumption: Consumer Fan-
Eich, James (1980), The Cue-Dependent Nature of State-De- tasies, Feelings, and Fun," Journal of Consumer Re-
pendent Retrieval," Memory and Cognition, 8 (2), 157- search, 9 (September), 132-140.
173. Hornstein, Harvey, Elizabeth LaKind, Gladys Frankel, and
Fennell, Geraldine (1981), "Emotion: A Neglected Aspect of Stella Manne (1975), "Effects of Knowledge About Re-
Consumer Behavior," in Proceedings ofDivision 23 Pro- mote Social Events on Prosocial Behavior, Social Con-
gram, 89th Annual Convention of the American Psy- ception, and Mood," Journal of Personality and Social
chological Association, ed. Richard Lutz, Nashville, TN: Psychology, 32 (6), 1038-1046.
Owen Graduate School of Management, Vanderbilt Isen, Alice (1970), "Success, Failure, Attention, and Reaction
University. to Others: The Warm Glow of Success," Journal of Per-
Fiske, Susan (1981), "Social Cognition and Affect," in Cog- sonality and Social Psychology, 15 (4), 294-301.
nition, Social Behavior, and the Environment, ed. John - - - (1984), "Toward Understanding the Role of Affect in
Harvey, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 227-264. Cognition," in Handbook of Social Cognition, eds. Rob-
Forest, Duncan, Margaret Clark, Judson Mills, and Alice Isen ert Wyer, Jr. and Thomas Srull, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
(1979), "Helping as a Function of Feeling State and Na- Erlbaum, 179-236.
ture of the Helping Behavior," Motivation and Emotion, - - - , Margaret Clark, and Mark F. Schwartz (1976), "Du-
3 (2), 161-169. ration of the Effect of Good Mood on Helping: Footprints
Fried, Rona and Leonard Berkowitz (1979), "Music Hath on the Sands of Time," Journal of Personality and Social
Charms . . . And Can Influence Helpfulness," Journal Psychology, 34 (3), 385-393.
ofApplied Social Psychology, 9 (2), 199-208. - - - , Nancy Horn, and D. L. Rosenhan (1973), "Effects
Fry, P. S. (1975), "Affect and Resistance to Temptation," of Success and Failure on Children's Generosity," Jour-
Developmental Psychology, 11 (4),466-472. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27 (2), 239-
Galizio, Mark and Clyde Hendrick (1972), "Effect of Musical 247.
Accompaniment on Attitude: The Guitar as a Prop for - - - and Paula Levin (1972), "Effect of Feeling Good on
Persuasion," Journal ofApplied Social Psychology, 2 (4), Helping: Cookies and Kindness," Journal ofPersonality
350-359. and Social Psychology, 21 (3),384-388.
Gardner, Meryl P. (1985), "Does Attitude Toward the Ad - - - , Barbara Means, Robert Patrick, and Gary Nowicki
Affect Brand Attitude Under a Brand Evaluation 'Set'?," (1982), "Some Factors Influencing Decision-Making
Journal of Marketing Research, 22 (May), 192-198. Strategy and Risk Taking," in Cognition and Affect, eds.
- - - and S. P. Raj (1983), "Responses to Commercials in Margaret Clark and Susan Fiske, Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
Laboratory Versus Natural Settings: A Conceptual Erlbaum, 243-261.
Framework," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. - - - and Thomas Shalker (1982), "The Effect of Feeling
10, eds. Richard Bagozzi and Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, State on Evaluation of Positive, Neutral, and Negative
MI: Association for Consumer Research, 142-146. Stimuli: When You 'Accentuate the Positive,' Do You
- - - and Marion Vandersteel (1984), "The Consumer's 'Eliminate the Negative'?," Social Psychology Quarterly,
Mood: An Important Situational Variable," in Advances 45 (I), 58-63.
MOOD STATES AND CONSUMER BEHAVIOR 299

- - - , Thomas Shalker, Margaret Clark, and Lynn Karp Surroundings: I. Initial Effects of Three Esthetic Con-
(1978), "Affect, Accessibility of Material in Memory, and ditions Upon Perceived "Energy" and "Well-being" in
Behavior: A Cognitive Loop?," Journal of Personality Faces," The Journal of Psychology, 41 (January), 247-
and Social Psychology, 36 (January), 1-12. 254.
- - - and Stanley Simmonds (1978), "The Effect of Feeling Masters, John and Wyndol Furman (1976), "Effects of Af-
Good on a Helping Task That Is Incompatible with Good fective States on Noncontingent Outcome Expectancies
Mood," Social Psychology, 41 (4), 346-349. and Beliefs in Internal or External Control," Develop-
Janis, Irving, Donald Kaye, and Paul Kirschner (1965), "Fa- mental Psychology, (5), 481-482.
cilitating Effects of 'Eating-While-Reading' on Respon- Milliman, Ronald (1982), "Using Background Music to Affect
siveness to Persuasive Communications," Journal of the Behavior of Supermarket Shoppers," Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 1 (2), 181-186. Marketing, 46 (Summer), 86-91.
Johnson, Eric and Amos Tversky (1983), "Affect General- Mischel, Walter, Brian Coates, and Antonette Raskoff(1968),
ization, and the Perception of Risk," Journal ofPerson- "Effects of Success and Failure on Self-Gratification,"
ality and Social Psychology, 45 (1),20-31. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 10 (4),
Kotler, Philip (1974), "Atmospherics as a Marketing Tool," 381-390.
The Journal of Retailing, 49 (Winter), 48-64. Mitchell, Andrew and Jerry Olson (1981), "Are Product At-
- - - and G. Alexander Rath (1983), "Design: A Powerful tribute Beliefs the Only Mediator of Advertising Effects
But Neglected Marketing Tool," paper presented at the on Brand Attitude?," Journal ofMarketing Research, 18
Fourteenth Annual Conference of the Association for (August), 318-332.

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


Consumer Research, Chicago, IL. Mizerski, Richard W. and J. Dennis White (1985), "Under-
Kroeber-Rtel, Werner (1979), "Activation Research: Psycho- standing and Using Emotions in Advertising," working
biological Approaches in Consumer Research," Journal paper, Florida State University, Tallahassee, 33206.
of Consumer Research, 5 (March), 240-250. Moore, Bert, Andrea Clyburn, and Bill Underwood (1976),
- - - (1984), "Emotional Product Differentiation by Clas- "The Role of Affect in Delay of Gratification," Child
sical Conditioning," in Advances in Consumer Research, Development, 47, 273-276.
Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Provo, UT: Association - - - , Bill Underwood, and D. L. Rosenhan (1973), "Affect
for Consumer Research, 538-543. and Altruism," Developmental Psychology, 8 (1), 99-
Laird, James (1974), Self-Attribution of Emotion: The Effects 104.
of Expressive Behavior on the Quality of Emotional Ex- Moore, Danny and J. Wesley Hutchinson (1983), "The Effects
perience," Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, of Ad Affect on Advertising Effectiveness," in Advances
29 (4), 475-486. in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, eds. Richard Bagozzi
- - - John Wagener, Mark Halal, and Martha Szegda and Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Con-
(1982), "Remembering What You Feel: Effects of Emo- sumer Research, 526-531.
tion on Memory," Journal of Personality and Social Moore, Timothy (1982), "Subliminal Advertising: What You
Psychology, 42 (4), 646-657. See Is What You Get," Journal ofMarketing, 46 (Spring),
Langer, Judith (1983), "Treats and Luxuries: Marketing in 38-47.
Hard Times," Marketing Review, 38 (December/Janu- Natale, Michael and Michael Hantas (1982), "Effect of Tem-
ary),31-37. porary Mood States on Selective Memory About the
Leckenby, John D. and Patricia A. Stout (1985), "Conceptual Self," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42
and Methodological Issues in Persuasion Measurement," (5),927-934.
Winter Marketing Educators' Conference, American Obermiller, Carl and Mary Bitner (1984), "Store Atmosphere:
Marketing Association, Phoenix, AZ. Peripheral Cue for Product Evaluation," Proceedings of
Lutz, Richard J. (1979), "Opening Statement," in Conceptual Annual Conference, American Psychological Associa-
and Theoretical Developments in Marketing, eds. O. C. tion, Consumer Psychology Division.
Ferrell, S. W. Brown, and Charles W. Lamb, Chicago: Park, C. Whan, Meryl P. Gardner, and Vinod Thukral (1984),
American Marketing Association, 3-6. "Effects of Actual and Perceived Knowledge on Selected
- - - and Pradeep Kakkar (1975), "The Psychological Sit- Aspects of Consumer Information Processing," working
uation as a Determinant of Consumer Behavior," in Ad- paper, School of Business Administration, New York
vances in Consumer Research, Vol. 1, eds. Scott Ward University, New York, 10003.
and Peter Wright, 439-453. Peter, J. Paul and Walter R. Nord (1982), "A Clarification
- - - , Scott MacKenzie, and George Belch (1983), "Attitude and Extension of Operant Conditioning Principles in
Toward the Ad as a Mediator of Advertising Effective- Marketing," Journal of Marketing, 46 (Summer), 102-
ness: Determinants and Consequences," in Advances in 107.
Consumer Research, Vol. 10, eds. Richard Bagozzi and Peterson, Robert and Matthew Sauber (1983), "A Mood Scale
Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer for Survey Research," in 1983 AMA Educators' Pro-
Research, 532-539. ceedings, eds. Patrick Murphy et aI., Chicago, IL: Amer-
McSweeney, Frances K. and Calvin Bierley (1984), "Recent ican Marketing Association, 409-414.
Developments in Classical Conditioning," Journal of Plutchik, Robert (1980), Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary
Consumer Research, 11 (September),619-631. Synthesis, New York: Harper & Row.
Maister, David (1985), "The Psychology of Waiting Lines," Polivy, Janet (1981), "On the Induction of Emotion in the
in The Service Encounter, eds. John Czepiel, Michael Laboratory: Discrete Moods or Multiple Affective
Solomon, and Carol Suprenant, Lexington, MA: Lex- States?," Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
ington Books, 113-123. 41 (October), 803-817.
Maslow, A. H. and N. L. Mintz (1956), "Effects of Esthetic Ray, Michael and Rajeev Batra (1983), "Emotion and Per-
300 THE JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

suasion in Advertising: What We Do and Don't Know Sternthal, Brian and C. Samuel Craig (1974), "Fear Appeals:
About Affect," in Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. Revisited and Revised," Journal of Consumer Research,
10, eds. Richard Bagozzi and Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, 1 (December), 22-34.
MI: Association for Consumer Research, 543-548. Stokols, Daniel (1982), "Environmental Psychology: A Com-
Regan, Dennis, Margo Williams, and Sondra Sparling (1972), ing of Age," G. Stanley Hall Lecture Series, Vol. 2, 155-
"Voluntary Expiation of Guilt: A Field Experiment," 205.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24 (I), 42- Teasdale, John and Sarah Fogarty (1979), "Differential Effects
45. oflnduced Mood on Retrieval of Pleasant and Unpleas-
Rossiter, John and Larry Percy (1980), "Attitude Change ant Events From Episodic Memory," Journal of Abnor-
through Visual Imagery in Advertising," Journal ofAd- mal Psychology, 88,248-257.
vertising, 9 (Winter), 10-16. Tiger, Lionel (1979), Optimism, The Biology of Hope, New
Schellenberg, James and Gregory Blevins (1973), "Feeling York: Simon and Schuster.
Good and Helping: How Quickly Does the Smile of Underwood, Bill, Bert Moore, and D. L. Rosenhan (1973),
Dame Fortune Fade?," Psychological Reports, 33, 72- "Affect and Self-Gratification," Developmental Psychol-
74. ogy, 8 (2), 209-214.
Schlinger, Mary Jane (1979), "A Profile of Responses to Veitch, Russell and William Griffitt (1976), "Good News-
Commercials," Journal of Advertising Research, 19 Bad News: Affective and Interpersonal Effects," Journal
(April), 37-46. of Applied Social Psychology, 6 (I), 69-75.
Schwarz, Norbert and Gerald Clore (1983), "Mood, Misat- Weinberg, Peter and Wolfgang Gottwald (1982), "Impulsive

Downloaded from http://jcr.oxfordjournals.org/ by guest on June 22, 2016


tribution, and Judgments of Well-Being: Informative and Consumer Buying as a Result of Emotions," Journal of
Directive Functions of Affective States," Journal ofPer- Business Research, 10 (March), 43-57.
sonality and Social Psychology, 45 (3), 513-523. Wells, William (1986), "Three Useful Ideas," in Advances in
Seeman, Gloria and J. Conrad Schwarz (1974), "Affective Consumer Research, Vol. 13, ed. Richard Lutz, Provo,
State and Preference for Immediate Versus Delayed Re- UT: Association for Consumer Research, in press.
ward," Journal of Research in Personality, 7, 384-394. - - - , Clark Leavitt, and Maureen McConville (1971), "A
Shimp, Terence (1981), "Attitude Toward the Ad as a Me- Reaction Profile for TV Commercials," Journal of Ad-
diator of Consumer Brand Choice," Journal of Adver- vertising Research, II (6), 11-17.
tising, 10 (2),9-15. Wener, Richard (1985), "The Environmental Psychology of
- - - and J. Thomas Yokum (1982), "Advertising Inputs The Service Encounter," in The Service Encounter, eds.
and Psychophysical Judgments in Vending-Machine John Czepiel, Michael Solomon, and Carl Surprenant,
Retailing," Journal of Retailing, 58 (Spring), 95-113. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, 10 1-112.
Simon, Herbert (1967), "Motivational and Emotional Con- Westbrook, Robert (1980), "Intrapersonal Affective Influ-
trols of Cognition," Psychological Review, 74 (I), 29- ences on Consumer Satisfaction with Products," Journal
39. of Consumer Research, 7 (June), 49-54.
Srull, Thomas (1983a), "Affect and Memory: The Impact of Weyant, James (1978), "Effects of Mood States, Costs, and
Affective Reactions in Advertising on the Representation Benefits on Helping," Journal of Personality and Social
of Product Information in Memory," in Advances in Psychology, 36 (10), 1169-1176.
Consumer Research, Vol. 10, eds. Richard Bagozzi and Yuspeh, Sonia (1979), "The Medium Versus the Message,"
Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, MI: Association for Consumer in A Look Back, A Look Ahead, Proceedings of the 10th
Research, 520-525. National Attitude Research Conference, ed. George
- - - (1983b), "The Role of Prior Knowledge in the Ac- Hafer, Chicago: American Marketing Association, 109-
quisition, Retention, and Use of New Information," in 138.
Advances in Consumer Research, Vol. 10, eds. Richard Zajonc, Robert (1980), "Feeling and Thinking: Preferences
Bagozzi and Alice Tybout, Ann Arbor, MI: Association Need No Inferences," American Psychologist, 35 (Feb-
for Consumer Research, 572-576. ruary),151-175.
- - - (1984), "The Effects of Subjective Affective States on - - - and Hazel Markus (1982), "Affective and Cognitive
Memory and Judgment," in Advances in Consumer Re- Factors in Preferences," Journal of Consumer Research,
search, Vol. 11, ed. Thomas Kinnear, Provo, UT: As- 9 (September), 123-131.
sociation for Consumer Research, 530-533.

You might also like