Gender Differences in The Relational and Collective Bases For Trust

You might also like

You are on page 1of 13

05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 159

Group Processes &


G
Intergroup Relations P
2005 Vol 8(2) 159–171
I
R
Gender Differences in the
Relational and Collective
Bases for Trust
William W. Maddux
Northwestern University
Marilynn B. Brewer
The Ohio State University

A variety of research suggests that men and women differ in their interdependent orientation:
whereas women tend to be more relationally interdependent, men tend to be more collectively
interdependent (e.g. Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). The current study sought to investigate
differences in interdependence within the domain of trust. In particular, the authors predicted
that men would tend to trust individuals based on whether or not they shared group
memberships. On the other hand, women were predicted to trust those who shared direct or
indirect relationship connections. Results from an online trust-dilemma game supported these
predictions. Implications for our understanding of the impact of gender on social identity and
self-representation are discussed.

keywords gender, self-construal, social identity, trust

M E N and women differ. Such a statement Although it is clear that gender impacts a
would strike most people as intuitively obvious. variety of domains, recent research is beginning
That gender differences are believed to be rela- to uncover some of the specific psychological
tively ubiquitous is underscored by the fact that foundations of how men and women differ. One
the topic is well-picked fodder for popular primary area of interest is self-representation.
culture and the entertainment media; gender For example, in a review of the literature Cross
conflicts are often explored in sitcoms, reality and Madson (1997) proposed that a number of
TV shows, comedic and dramatic movies, and
stand-up comedy. However, results from
psychological research indicate that there may Author’s note
be some basis for the widespread belief that Address correspondence to William W.
men and women differ. Empirical evidence Maddux, Dispute Resolution Research Center,
suggests that gender differences exist across a Kellogg School of Management,
variety of psychological domains, including Northwestern University, 2001 Sheridan Rd.,
emotion, cognition, behavior, and language use Evanston, IL 60208, USA
(for a review, see Deaux, 1998). [email: w-maddux@kellogg.northwestern.edu]

Copyright © 2005 SAGE Publications


(London, Thousand Oaks, CA and New Delhi)
8:2; 159–171; DOI: 10.1177/1368430205051065
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 160

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

previously documented gender differences Gabriel and Gardner (1999) obtained a


could be explained in terms of differences in variety of support for the existence of gender
self-construals: while men may have more of an differences in relational versus collective inter-
independent construal of self, women may dependence. In one study involving a diary-
have more of an interdependent construal of reading paradigm, women showed better
self. Within this framework, an independent selective memory for relational items in the
self-construal accentuates self-related features diary, while men showed better memory for
and excludes the influence of others in the collective items. In another study, women were
self-schema, whereas an interdependent self- found to be more likely to put their own
construal represents inclusion of others in the personal desires aside for a friend, while men
self (Aron, Aron, & Smollan, 1992; Brewer & were more likely to sacrifice for a group
Gardner, 1996; Markus & Kitayama, 1991). (Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). In addition, similar
Cross and Madson (1997) reviewed a variety gender differences have been found to impact
of evidence in support of this hypothesis. For the subjective importance of different types
example, they noted that while women often of groups (Seeley, Gardner, Pennington, &
describe themselves more in terms of relation- Gabriel, 2003). For women, group importance
ships with others, men have a stronger is mainly determined by the degree to which
tendency to describe themselves in terms of the group fulfills relational needs, while men
separateness from others (e.g. Pratt, Prancer, place a greater importance on the collective
Hunsberger, & Manchester, 1990). In addition, identity that groups offer.1
women tend to rate themselves more highly on The existence of gender differences in
self-related dimensions concerning interdepen- interdependence is consistent with the idea
dence, while men rate themselves higher on that there are two distinct types of self-
independent dimensions (e.g. Zuckerman, representations that operate in social contexts.
1985). Finally, Cross and Madson cited evidence More specifically, Brewer and Gardner (1996)
that in terms of conversational norms, women postulated that there are two different levels of
prefer talking about relationships, while men ‘social selves’: a relational self and a collective
prefer discussing less personal topics such as self. In this framework the relational self
sports and politics (e.g. Aries & Johnson, 1983). concerns self-representations derived from
Additional research has extended the idea of one’s relationships with specific other people (‘I
gender differences in self-construals from the am a father’). In terms of gender research, the
individual level to the collective level. For idea of the relational self corresponds closely to
example, Baumeister and Sommer (1997) Cross and Madson’s conception of the interde-
proposed that a sense of belongingness is a pendent self-construal that predominates for
fundamental human need (cf. Baumeister & women. On the other hand, the collective self is
Leary, 1995), and thus having a sense of con- constructed from one’s identity as derived from
nectedness to others is essential psychologically memberships in groups (‘I am a Cubs fan’). This
for both men and women. However, Baumeis- idea of the collective self seems to correspond
ter and Sommer proposed that men and more closely to the collective-interdependent
women differ in the way in which they satisfy nature of men. Thus, in terms of Brewer and
this need. While men may indeed be more Gardner’s (1996) conception of social selves,
independent than women, at the interdepen- the relational self seems the more salient type of
dent level men may also place a greater import- social self for women, while the collective self is
ance on group memberships and large the more salient type of social self for men.
collectives. In other words, in terms of the way It is important to point out that the above
in which people feel a sense of interdepen- research has focused primarily on the self-
dence with others, women may be more rela- construal of women in Western cultures (i.e. the
tionally oriented, men may be more collectively United States, Canada, Australia, Western
oriented (Baumeister & Sommer, 1997). Europe). Although interdependent self-construals

160
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 161

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

have also been shown to be particularly salient these differences should also manifest them-
for individuals from East Asian cultures (Markus selves in situations where men and women must
& Kitayama, 1991), to date there is little research decide whether or not to trust other people,
on the similarities of the interdependent self- particularly strangers. Although the concept of
construals of Western and East Asian women; trust has always been an integral aspect of
however, there is evidence for some overlap, research in social psychology, there has been a
specifically concerning emotional relatedness recent resurgence of interest in trust as a
with significant others (Kashima et al., 1995). central psychological construct. Of particular
However, other areas of potential overlap interest to social psychologists is the role of
remain unexplored, and it remains to be seen trust in contexts and institutions where partici-
whether universal gender differences exist in pants must decide whether or not to cooperate
other psychological domains relevant to self-con- with others with whom they have no personal
struals. knowledge or history of interpersonal relation-
The way in which one construes the self is of ship. Such ‘depersonalized trust’ is essential for
importance primarily because of the tremen- the creation and maintenance of many forms of
dous influence that the self has on a host of economic exchange, organizations, and social
psychological processes. The self has been and political institutions.
shown to be a major organizing factor for the There are at least two possible bases for
type of thoughts we have about ourselves and trusting strangers. First of all, shared category
others, what information we pay attention to membership may become a basis for deperson-
and what information we ignore, how we alized trust (Brewer, 1981; Buchan, Croson, &
feel about and evaluate ourselves, with whom Dawes, 2002; Macy & Skvoretz, 1998; Yamagishi
we choose to compare ourselves, and how we & Kiyonari, 2000). As a consequence of shifting
choose to present ourselves to others (for a psychologically from the personal to the collec-
review, see Baumeister, 1998). Thus, whether tive level of identity, one may be less likely to
one’s dominant self-construal is independent or distinguish the interests of other ingroup
interdependent, or whether one’s salient social members from those of oneself, leading to
self tends to involve relationships or collective increasing trust toward fellow ingroup members.
memberships, these construals of self have the Hence, simply knowing that an otherwise
potential to have a great impact on how we deal unknown person is a member of a salient
with almost any type of situation in daily life. ingroup may be sufficient to engender trust as
Overall, then, there is growing evidence that a default assumption. A second route involves
gender differences impact the way in which sharing a network of interpersonal relations
people feel a sense of interdependence with with others. Individuals may trust others if they
others and define their ingroups. Compared to know (or believe) that they are directly or indi-
men, women place more emphasis on relation- rectly connected to each other through mutual
ships and interpersonal connections, while men friendships or acquaintances (Coleman, 1990).
are more likely to emphasize more depersonal- In other words, we may trust a stranger if we
ized group memberships and the importance believe that the person is potentially a member
of group identity. Importantly, these gender of a generalized exchange network of relation-
differences in interdependence have been ships (Yamagishi & Yamagishi, 1994).
shown to impact a variety of psychological vari- Given these distinctions in the basis for
ables, including self-representation, selective depersonalized trust, if men are more collec-
memory, and group attachment. tively interdependent than women, men should
be more likely to trust individuals who share
ingroup membership, regardless of personal
Consequences for depersonalized trust relationship connections. By contrast, if women
If there are indeed reliable gender differences are more relationally interdependent, then
in relational and collective interdependence, women should tend to trust individuals who are

161
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 162

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

likely to share a direct or indirect network of gender differences in an American sample.


relationships, and ingroup/outgroup distinc- Previous research has sometimes found it
tions should be less important. We tested these necessary to prime the interdependent self in
predictions in an online, real-time money allo- order to induce interdependent cognition for
cation game that provided an engrossing, American participants (Brewer & Gardner,
meaningful trust-related situation. 1996).
Thus, in the current research we first pre-
sented American men and women with one of
The present research two types of primes designed to activate inter-
The current study employed a modified form dependence with others; in other words, the
of the allocator/dictator game devised by primes served to activate the ‘social self ’.2
Kiyonari and Yamagishi (1999). In this game, Subsequently men and women then partici-
participants make online decisions about pated in an online, allocator/dictator game
whether to accept a ‘sure-thing’ payment of that measured how much they trusted a variety
US$3 from the experimenter, or an unknown of targets whose personal identity was unknown.
allocation from a stranger (who had ostensibly One target was an ingroup target, identified as
been given US$11 to distribute as he or she a student at the same university as the partici-
wished). Since actual monetary payments were pant. A second target, representing a cross-
involved and participants were told that their group relationship target, was identified as a
payment at the end of the experiment student at an outgroup university where the
depended on the outcomes of their decisions, participant had earlier indicated that he or she
this paradigm provides a compelling test of had an acquaintance. A third target, represent-
participants’ willingness to place faith in a ing a basic outgroup target, was identified as a
stranger based on minimal information about student at a university where the participant did
another person’s social group memberships or not indicate knowing anyone. In reality, all
potential relationship connections. targets were fictitious and were used as part of
This paradigm has previously proven to be a the cover story to create a realistic setting for
reliable method for investigating cross-cultural trust decisions. Participants performed one
differences in depersonalized trust (Yuki, trial with each target, the order of which was
Maddux, Brewer, & Takemura, 2005). For randomly determined.
example, Yuki and colleagues (2005) showed Based on the collective-interdependent
that although American participants trusted nature of men, we predicted that men would
ingroup members more than outgroup show the highest level of trust for the ingroup
members (regardless of the presence of cross- target, and significantly less trust for both
group relationship connections), Japanese outgroup targets. Based on the relationship-
participants showed no differences in trust interdependent nature of women, we predicted
toward ingroup members and outgroup that the presence of a cross-group relationship
members who had a potential cross-group would increase trust for an outgroup member
relationship connection. Since a variety of more for women than for men. We expected
research has demonstrated that Japanese are women to show high levels of trust for both the
more relationally oriented than Americans ingroup and cross-group relationship target,
(e.g. Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Yuki, 2003), and significantly less trust for the outgroup
the current study sought to explore similar target. Thus, our paradigm had a 2 (version of
gender effects in this paradigm. Although no interdependent prime) ⫻ 2 (participant
gender differences emerged in Yuki et al.’s gender) ⫻ 3 (type of target) mixed factorial
(2005) American sample, we hypothesized that design, with prime and gender as between-
priming both men and women with interdepen- subjects variables, and type of target as a within-
dence could potentially activate pre-existing subjects variable.

162
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 163

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

Method participants they were free to begin, at which


time she left the room.
Participants
Participants then completed one of the two
Participants were 147 students (79 males, 68
independent priming packets as part of an
females) at Ohio State University. All partici-
experiment on ‘verbal ability and memor y’,
pants were citizens of the United States. Partici-
which was in actuality the priming phase of the
pants were recruited from the introductor y
experiment. Following this priming phase,
psychology subject pool, and voluntarily signed
participants turned on their computers and
up in return for partial course credit and a
began the main trust experiment. The instruc-
monetary payment. The experimental require-
tions on the screen explained that the second
ments indicated that in order to be eligible for
study was an online decision-making game in
the experiment, participants had to have
which they would be interacting with other
acquaintances at other Big Ten universities (the
participants in a real-time, money allocation
conference of universities that includes Ohio
situation. It was explained that participants
State University). Four participants (two males
would be paid for their participation in the
and two females) gave at least one incorrect
experiment, as well as receiving partial course
response on one of the manipulation checks,
credit. Participants were initially asked to enter
and their data were excluded from further
the names and universities of all acquaintances
analysis. This left the data from 143 participants
they had at Big Ten universities. The computer
(77 males, 66 females) for formal analysis.
then paused for a few seconds, and as part of
the cover story, the instructions indicated that
Priming materials the computer was connecting to an online
Participants were randomly assigned to read one network. In reality, the computer did not
of two scenarios designed to prime interdepen- connect to any network, and was simply pro-
dence. One scenario involved a stor y about grammed to wait 20 seconds before continuing.
playing a round of miniature golf with a group The main instructions then began by reiter-
of friends, and a second scenario involved a story ating that the experiment was concerned with
about attending a baseball game. For both judgment and decision-making, and that it was
scenarios, participants were instructed to circle being conducted in conjunction with students
all the pronouns in the story, and then were from other universities. Participants were told
asked several questions about the content of the that some conditions involved making decisions
story (e.g. what was the story about?, what type with other Ohio State students, while other
of words did you circle?) as part of the cover conditions involved decisions with students
story that the experiment concerned ‘verbal from other universities, who were performing
ability and memory’. Both versions contained 21 the experiment at the same time, but in a
plural pronouns (us, we, our). These scenarios different location. The decision task involved
were designed to prime an interdependent dividing a fixed sum of money (US$11)
frame of mind (Brewer & Gardner, 1996). between two paired individuals. Participants
were told that one person would be randomly
Procedure assigned the role of the allocator (the person
Participants were shown into the lab and seated who decides how much to give to each person),
at a computer terminal. A female experimenter and the other would be assigned the role of the
explained that they would be participating in recipient. The allocator’s task was ostensibly to
two separate experiments. Participants were decide how much of the money he/she would
instructed to complete the first paper-and- keep for himself/herself, and how much to give
pencil experimental packet, after which they to the other participant. There were no rules
were to turn on their computers to complete for the monetary division, and the allocator was
the second experiment on judgment and supposedly allowed to divide up the money
decision-making. The experimenter then told however he/she chose. Participants were told

163
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 164

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

that their final payment was to be based on the $3, or taking whatever amount the allocator
amount of money they received (or kept) had given them. This yes/no decision was our
during this decision-making task. The instruc- behavioral measure of whether participants
tions indicated that each participant would only trusted the target individuals or not. Following
know one thing about the other person—the the decision trial, participants were asked three
university he or she was attending. follow-up questions. First, participants were
The computer then paused and the instruc- asked, ‘To what extent did you trust the alloca-
tions indicated that it was randomly being tor to make a decision that was favorable to
determined which person would play the role you?’ (1 did not trust at all, 5 completely trusted ).
of allocator, and which person would be the Next participants were asked, ‘How much
recipient. However, the computer always money do you think the allocator gave you?’
assigned participants to the role of the Participants were asked to enter an amount in
recipient, while the ficticious partner was always US dollars, from $0 to $11. Finally, as a manipu-
assigned the role of allocator. The instructions lation check to insure participants were paying
explained that the recipient had to choose attention to the instructions, participants were
between two options in the game: the exit asked to ‘please indicate what university the
option of a ‘sure thing’ (receiving US$3 from previous allocator was from’. The computer
the experimenter), or the option of taking then provided two options: Ohio State or
whatever money the allocator decided to give another university. Participants were asked to
him/her. The instructions also indicated that mark the correct response.
this choice had to be made before the other Following completion of the first trial, the
partner’s choice was disclosed. In reality, of instructions stated that there were to be two
course, the allocator never made a decision. We more decision trials. Participants were told that
were simply interested in whether participants their final payment would be based on the
chose to take this unknown allocation of decision made on one trial, and that this critical
money, or whether they took the sure thing. trial would be randomly selected at the end of
Thus, the decision to accept the allocator’s the experimental session. This was done in
allotment involved giving up $3, with the risk of order to motivate participants to maximize
winding up with less (or no) money allocation.3 their possible reward for each trial indepen-
The computer paused while the instructions dently of the others. The choice paradigm was
indicated that the participant’s partner was then repeated over two more trials. All partici-
now being chosen, then randomly selected pants performed one trial for each target type,
either an ingroup target, a potential relation- i.e. once with an ingroup target, once with a
ship target, or an outgroup target. If the potential relationship target, and once with an
computer selected the ingroup target, the outgroup target, with the order varied
instructions indicated that the allocator was a randomly across participants. Following each
student from Ohio State. If the potential decision trial, participants were again asked to
relationship target was chosen, the computer indicate their trust for the target, the amount of
indicated that the allocator was from another money allocated by the target, and the alloca-
university, but that our records indicated that tor’s university.
this was a university at which the participant After these questions, the computer indi-
had an acquaintance. If the outgroup target was cated that one trial would be randomly selected
chosen, the computer said that the allocator and that participants would receive payment
was from another university, and that our based on the outcome of that trial. However,
records indicated that the participant did not the computer always chose the second decision
know anyone at that university.4 trial as a basis for payment. Participants were
Once the allocator target was chosen and paid the $3 if they had chosen the sure-thing
identified, participants were instructed to make option on trial 2, and they were paid $4 if they
their choice, either the sure-thing payment of had chosen to forgo the sure thing and take the

164
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 165

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

partner’s allocation. Following payment, favor of the allocator’s decision for any of the
participants were probed for suspicions about three targets (ingroup target: men = 88%,
the cover story, debriefed about the true nature women = 90%; relationship target: men = 84%,
of the experiment, and thanked for taking part. women = 82%; outgroup target: men = 73%,
women = 77%, all ps > .22).
However, results on the trust ratings and
Results and discussion expectations of monetary awards did support
Online decisions predictions. A 2 (version of interdependent
Initial analyses were carried out on partici- prime) ⫻ 2 (gender) ⫻ 3 (target type), mixed-
pants’ decisions for each of the three different factorial analysis of variance (ANOVA) was
target allocators. These decisions were sub- conducted on trust ratings, with target as a
jected to several successive nonparametric within-subjects variable, and participant gender
analyses to compare targets.5 An initial and type of prime as between-subjects variables.
Cochran’s Q test indicated that participants’ The results indicated a main effect for target
decisions whether to accept the allocator’s type (F(2, 496) = 33.70, p < .001, ␩2 = .328), such
division of the money differed significantly that most trust was shown toward the ingroup
across the three target conditions, with partici- member, and least trust toward the outgroup
pants taking the ingroup target’s allocation member, consistent with the pattern of choice
89% of the time, the relationship target’s decisions. However, this main effect was quali-
allocation 83% of the time, and outgroup fied by a significant two-way interaction between
target’s allocation 75% of the time (Q(2) = target type and gender (F(2, 278) = 4.80, p =
20.15, p < .001). .009, ␩2 = .033).6
Individual McNemar tests on the compari- A series of pair-wise comparisons was carried
sons for specific targets showed that compared out to decompose the differences in trust
to the ingroup target, participants were less toward specific targets. Within-gender compari-
likely to accept the allocator’s decision and sons indicated that men trusted the ingroup
more likely to take the sure thing for the target significantly more than both the poten-
relationship target (␹2(143) = 4.53, p = .035), tial relationship target (F(1, 76) = 33.61,
and for the outgroup target (␹2(143) = 16.69, p < .001, ␩2 = .301), and the outgroup target
p < .001). In addition, participants were also (F(1, 76) = 44.45, p < .001, ␩2 = .353), while
significantly less likely to accept the allocator’s there was no significant difference in trust for
decision and more likely to take the sure thing the potential relationship target compared to
for the outgroup target compared to the the outgroup target (p > .46). Thus, men
relationship target (␹2(143) = 4.70, p = .030). trusted ingroup members more than outgroup
These results indicate that participants were members.
most willing to give up the sure-thing option However, for women, trust was greater
when the target was an ingroup member, less toward the ingroup target than the outgroup
likely with the potential relationship target, and target (F(1, 65) = 24.91, p < .001, ␩2 = .277), but
least likely when the target was an outgroup women also trusted the potential relationship
member. Thus, willingness to forgo a sure thing target more than the outgroup target (F(1, 65)
in favor of an allocation from an unknown = 8.97, p = .004, ␩2 = .121). There was no differ-
person was clearly sensitive to both types of ence in trust for the ingroup target and poten-
information; that is, whether the target was an tial relationship target for women (p > .25).
ingroup or outgroup member and whether the Thus, women trusted the ingroup target and
target was potentially part of an indirect the potential relationship target more than the
relationship network. However, contrar y to outgroup target.
expectations, this decision was not affected by Cross-gender comparisons indicated no
gender; men and women did not differ in their differences in trust toward the ingroup and
likelihood of forgoing the sure thing option in outgroup targets (ps > 21). However, women

165
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 166

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

were significantly more trusting of the potential ANOVA was conducted on these data, with
relationship target than were men (F(1, 141) = target as a within-subjects variable, and partici-
11.16, p = .001, ␩2 = .073). Thus, these results pant gender and type of prime as between-
support our predictions that the presence of a subjects factors. A significant main effect
cross-group relationship would increase trust emerged for target (F(2, 278) = 11.94, p < .001,
more for men than for women. While there ␩2 = .079), such that the most money was
were no gender differences in trust toward expected from the ingroup member, and the
ingroup and outgroup members, women least from the outgroup member. This main
trusted the potential relationship target signifi- effect was qualified by a significant two-way
cantly more than did men. In fact, for women, interaction between target type and gender
there was no significant difference in trust (F(2, 278) = 3.37, p = .036, ␩2 = .024), indicating
toward the ingroup member and the potential that the expected money differed depending
relationship target (see Figure 1). on participants’ gender. No differences
We also analyzed the amount of money emerged based on type of prime, so further
participants expected to receive from each of analyses were again collapsed across primes.
the three allocators. Results of these analyses As with the trust analyses, pair-wise compari-
mirrored those of allocator trust (see Figure 2). sons indicated that men expected more money
A 2 (version of interdependent prime) ⫻ 2 from the ingroup target than the potential
(gender) ⫻ 3 (target type), mixed-factorial relationship target (F(1, 75) = 11.45, p = .001,

Amount of Trust
3.2

2.8
Trust

Males
2.6
Females

2.4

2.2

2
Ingroup target Relationship Outgroup target
target
Allocator
Figure 1. Amount of trust per target as a function of relationship type and participant gender (1 don’t trust at
all, 5 trust completely).

166
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 167

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

␩2 = .132), and more money from the ingroup p = .078, ␩2 = .022). However, as predicted,
target than the outgroup target (F(1, 75) = women also expected significantly more money
23.50, p < .001, ␩2 = .239). There was no differ- from the potential relationship target than men
ence between the potential relationship target (F(1, 141) = 6.72, p = .011, ␩2 = .046). These
and the outgroup target (p > .26). For women, results were essentially identical to those on the
expected money did not differ between the trust rating measure.
ingroup target and the potential relationship To summarize, although the predicted differ-
target (p > .84). However, a difference in ences did not emerge on our behavioral
expected money did emerge between the measure, self-report measures did reveal the
ingroup target and the outgroup target (F(1, expected pattern of trust. Compared to men,
64) = 10.62, p = .002, ␩2 = .142), and there was women were significantly more likely to trust an
a marginal difference between the potential outgroup member who shared a potential cross-
relationship target and the outgroup target group relationship connection. In addition, for
(F(1, 64) = 3.96, p = .051, ␩2 = .058). women the level of trust toward an ingroup
Finally, cross-gender comparisons of the member and toward an outgroup member with
expectation ratings indicated no differences a cross-group relationship connection did not
between men and women in trust toward the differ; however, men trusted ingroup members
ingroup targets (p > .98), although women significantly more than outgroup members
expected marginally more money from the regardless of cross-group relationship connec-
outgroup target than men did (F(1, 141) = 3.15, tions. This same pattern also appeared on the

Expected Money
5

4.5
Money (US dollars)

4
Males
Females
3.5

2.5
Ingroup target Relationship Outgroup target
target
Allocator
Figure 2. Expected money per target as a function of relationship type and participant gender (US$0 to US$11).

167
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 168

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

measure of the amount of money participants encouraged to maximize their monetar y


expected from each target. Men expected reward for the online decisions. Thus, not only
significantly more money from ingroup was trust not made particularly salient for the
members than from outgroup members. decision trials, but any gender differences in
Women expected no differences in money trust may have been masked by other influ-
from an ingroup member and an outgroup ences, particularly differences in preferences
member with a potential cross-group relation- for risky decision-making.
ship, but significantly less money was expected For example, a variety of research suggests
when the outgroup member had no relation- that men are more likely, across a variety of
ship connections. These results indicate that domains, to make more risky decisions than
trust for men was largely dependent on sharing women (e.g. Sorentino, Hewitt, & Raso-Knott,
group memberships, while trust for women 1992; Weber, Blais, & Betz, 2003; for a review,
depended more on sharing relationship links. see Byrnes, Miller, & Schafer, 1999). Thus,
although women may have actually trusted the
relationship target more than men (as evi-
General discussion denced on the self-report measures), women’s
The main goal of the present research was to decreased tolerance for risky decisions may
investigate the impact of gender differences in have inflated their preference for choosing the
interdependence within the domain of trust. safer, sure thing option, eliminating any gender
The results from an online, allocator/dictator differences that may have existed. This expla-
trust game largely supported our predictions nation is supported by the fact that compared
based on the idea that women are more rela- to men, women actually expected to receive
tionally interdependent and men are more significantly more money from the potential
collectively interdependent. For men, trust relationship allocator, but nevertheless did not
(and expected money) was highest for ingroup decide to give up the sure thing more often
members compared to outgroup members. than men. Thus, it is quite likely that women
However, the presence of a potential cross- simply had a higher threshold for deciding to
group relationship increased trust (and give up the safe option.
expected money) more for women than for In addition, we would argue that the self-
men. This potential cross-group relationship report measures of trust and expected money
connection was sufficiently meaningful for are more reliable than the behavioral measure
women that no differences in trust emerged because they directly and explicitly assess
toward the ingroup target and the potential participants’ level of trust toward targets, as well
relationship target. The fact that even a potential as their expectations for others’ behaviors. The
relationship connection with an outgroup fact that these self-report measures replicated
member increased trust to the level of an earlier research using the same paradigm (Yuki
ingroup member is striking evidence that rela- et al., 2005) is additional evidence for their
tional interdependence is particularly salient reliability and validity. Finally, it is important
for women. Thus, cross-group relationships had to note that the allocator/dictator game was
a greater impact on trust for women, while the an effective way to create an engaging and
categorical distinction between ingroup and meaningful situation in which to assess trust
outgroup was more important for men. and expected monetary allocations toward a
Although the predicted pattern of trust did variety of targets—a paradigm likely to be more
not emerge on our behavioral measure—the diagnostic of trust compared to paradigms
decision to take or forgo a sure-thing option of where participants are merely asked to imagine
$3—we believe this is not overly problematic, how they would respond.
primarily because such a decision is a relatively Overall, the current results offer support for
indirect measure of trust. In the current different types of interdependence for men
paradigm participants were instructed and and women. The research also extends previous

168
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 169

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

findings into a new domain, that of trust. countries, Kashima et al. (1995) found a pan-
Decisions concerning whether or not to trust cultural gender difference in relational inter-
strangers are important in a variety of areas of dependence. Across all cultures, women scored
ever yday life, including the creation and higher than men on a scale assessing emotional
maintenance of many forms of economic relatedness of the self with significant others,
exchange, social relationships, and political providing support for the idea that gender
institutions. In social exchange situations, it is differences in interdependence may be rela-
important to know what types of information or tively ubiquitous, and not specific to Western
minimal social cues will lead one person to cultures.
expect to be able to trust another. To the It is also noteworthy that the current results
extent that there are cultural and gender differ- show an interesting parallel with results from
ences in the bases for depersonalized trust, our previous research concerning cross-cultural
coordination of trust may require an under- differences in depersonalized trust (Yuki et al.,
standing of those differences. 2005). As was true for women in the current
Our results also offer additional evidence for study, in our earlier experiments the presence
the existence of two distinct types of social of a potential cross-group relationship increased
selves, a relational self and a collective self trust more for Japanese than for Americans
(Brewer & Gardner, 1996). While the relational within the same allocator/dictator paradigm
self refers to the inclusion of relationships used in the present study. This earlier study
within one’s self-representation, the collective showed no gender differences within the
self concerns the inclusion of group identity Japanese sample, although there was a non-
and group memberships as part of the self. We significant trend (p = .18), with Japanese
assume that both men and women need and women showing more trust toward the rela-
value both relational connections with others tional target than Japanese men.
and group memberships, but that they differ in However, despite the parallel between
which level of social self is most salient or cultural differences and gender differences in
accessible. That trust for women seems to self-construal, it is not necessarily the case that
depend largely on direct and indirect relation- effects of culture and gender on trust stem from
ships is consistent with the idea that the rela- the same underlying processes. After all, the
tional self is the predominant type of social self meaning of interpersonal relationships is
for women. The finding that trust for men was culturally embedded, and it is possible that
mostly dependent on the categorical, ingroup/ Asian and Western women may differ quali-
outgroup distinction suggests that the collective tatively in the way in which they are interdepen-
self may be the more salient type of social self dent. Although emotional interdependence
for men. with significant others has been shown to be
It is likely that gender differences in the similar across cultures (Kashima et al., 1995), it
social self are influenced by socialization norms remains to be seen how much overlap there is in
and culturally specific role expectations as indi- interdependent self-construal in other relevant
viduals grow up (cf. Eagly, 1987). For example, domains, such as cognition, motivation, and
there is evidence that among children and ado- behavior.
lescents, female play-groups tend to emphasize Thus, although East Asians (both men and
interpersonal interactions, while male play- women) and American women have been
groups emphasize teams and large groups (e.g. shown to be sensitive to cross-group relation-
Benenson, Apostoleris, & Parnass, 1997). ships as a basis for trusting a stranger, this trust
Although most research has focused on social- may reflect ver y different expectations or
ization and self-construal of women in the West, assumptions in the two cases. Future research
differential socialization of males and females should continue to explore the nature of
may be very similar across cultures. In a large culture and gender differences in relational
study involving respondents from five different and collective interdependence, and the way in

169
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 170

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations 8(2)

which the foundations of interdependent References


orientation may be similar or different depend-
Aries, E. J., & Johnson, F. L. (1983). Close
ing on gender and culture. friendships in adulthood: Conversational
content between same-sex friends. Sex Roles, 9,
Notes 1183–1196.
Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D. (1992).
1. It should be noted that some research has Inclusion of other in the self scale and the
demonstrated main effects for the primary structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of
importance of relational interdependence over Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 596–612.
collective interdependence across genders Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. Gilbert,
(Gabriel & Gardner, 1999). In addition, other S. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social
results have shown that men are concerned with psychology (pp. 680–740). New York: Oxford
both relational and collective interdependence University Press.
(e.g. Seeley et al., 2003). However, we have Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need
maintained the current terminology emphasizing to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as
relative differences between genders since a fundamental human motivation. Psychological
findings consistently show an interaction between Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
gender and type of interdependence, with women Baumeister, R. F., & Sommer, K. L. (1997). What do
being more relationally and less collectively men want? Gender differences and two spheres of
oriented than men. belongingness: Comment on Cross and Madson.
2. Two types of primes were used in order to be able Psychological Bulletin, 122, 38–44.
to generalize our findings across primes Benenson, J. F., Apostoleris, N. H., & Parnass, J.
regardless of idiosyncratic content of each (1997). Age and sex differences in dyadic and
particular prime. Participants received only one group interaction. Developmental Psychology, 33,
of the two primes. 538–543.
3. It is important to note that the allocator Brewer, M. B. (1981). Ethnocentrism and its role in
supposedly only knew the university identity of interpersonal trust. In M. Brewer & B. Collins
the recipient; he/she was not supposed to be (Eds.), Scientific inquiry and the social sciences
aware of the sure-thing option (Kiyonari & (pp. 345–360). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Yamagishi, 1999). Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is
4. For both outgroup targets, the specific university this ‘we’? Levels of collective identity and
membership of the allocator was not mentioned self-representations. Journal of Personality and
in order to avoid the effects of specific Social Psychology, 71, 83–93.
stereotypes unique to each university. Buchan, N. R., Croson, R. T. A., & Dawes, R. M.
5. Results of the online decisions were the same (2002). Swift neighbors and persistent strangers:
regardless of the type of prime participants were A cross-cultural investigation of trust and
given, so reported analyses are collapsed across reciprocity in social exchange. American Journal
both types of primes. of Sociology, 108, 168–206.
6. There was also a marginally significant three-way Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999).
interaction involving target, gender, and version Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis.
of the prime. The gender ⫻ target effect was Psychological Bulletin, 125, 367–383.
somewhat stronger for participants who had Coleman, J. S. (1990). The foundation of social theory.
seen the golfing prime stor y than the baseball Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
game stor y. Nonetheless we have collapsed Cross, S. E., & Madson, L. (1997). Models of the self:
across the versions of the prime because the Self-construals and gender. Psychological Bulletin,
overall interaction pattern was the same for 122, 5–37.
both. Deaux, K. (1998). Gender. In D. Gilbert, S. Fiske, &
G. Lindsay (Eds.) The handbook of social psychology
(Vol. 1, 4th ed.), pp. 788–828. New York:
Acknowledgment
McGraw-Hill.
This research was supported by a grant from the Eagly, A. H. (1987). Sex differences in social behavior:
Russell Sage Foundation. A social-role interpretation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

170
05 Maddux (bc-s) 29/3/05 1:53 pm Page 171

Maddux & Brewer gender and trust

Gabriel, S., & Gardner, W. L. (1999). Are there ‘his’ Weber, E. U., Blais, A., & Betz, N. E. (2003). A
and ‘hers’ types of interdependence? The domain-specific risk-attitude scale: Measuring risk
implications of gender differences in collective perceptions and risk behaviors. Journal of
versus relational interdependence for affect, Behavioral Decision Making, 15, 263–290.
behavior, and cognition. Journal of Personality and Yamagishi, T., & Kiyonari, T. (2000). The group as
Social Psychology, 77, 642–655. the container of generalized reciprocity. Social
Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S., Psychology Quarterly, 63, 116–132.
Gelfand, M.J., & Yuki, M. (1995). Culture, Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and
gender, and self: A perspective from commitment in the United States and Japan.
individualism–collectivism research. Journal of Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129–166.
Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 925–937. Yuki, M. (2003). Intergroup comparison versus
Kiyonari, T., & Yamagishi, T. (1999). A comparative intragroup cooperation: A cross-cultural
study of trust and trustworthiness using the game examination of social identity theory in North
of enthronement. Japanese Journal of Social American and East Asian cultural contexts. Social
Psychology, 15, 100–109. Psychology Quarterly, 66, 166–183.
Macy, M. W., & Skvoretz, J. (1998). The evolution of Yuki, M., Maddux, W. W., Brewer, M. B., &
trust and cooperation between strangers: A Takemura, K. (2005). Cultural differences in
computational model. American Sociological Review, relationship- and group-based trust. Personality and
63, 638–60. Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 48–62.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and Zuckerman, D. M. (1985). Confidence and
the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and aspirations: Self-esteem and self-concepts as
motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253. predictors of students’ life goals. Journal of
Pratt, M. W., Prancer, M., Hunsberger, B., & Personality, 53, 543–560.
Manchester, J. (1990). Reasoning about the self
and relationships in maturity: An integrative Paper received 12 March 2004; revised version accepted 30
complexity analysis of individual differences. September 2004.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,
575–581.
Seeley, E. A., Gardner, W. L., Pennington, G., & Biographical notes
Gabriel, S. (2003). Circle of friends or members W I L L I A M W. M A D D U X
is a Visiting Assistant
of a group? Sex differences in relational and Professor of Management & Organizations, and a
collective attachment to groups. Group Processes Post-Doctoral Fellow at the Dispute Resolution
& Intergroup Relations, 6, 251–263. Research Center, Kellogg School of Management,
Sorrentino, R. M., Hewitt, E. C., & Raso-Knott, P. A. Northwestern University.
(1992). Risk-taking in games of chance and skill:
Informational and affective influences on choice MARILYNN B. BREWER is a Professor of Psychology,
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, and the Ohio State Eminent Scholar in Social
62, 522–533. Psychology at the Ohio State University.

171

You might also like