You are on page 1of 8

Preventing School Failure, 54(3), 145–151, 2010

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LCC


ISSN: 1045-988X print
DOI: 10.1080/10459880903493104

Schema-Based Instruction: Facilitating


Mathematical Word Problem Solving
for Students with Emotional and
Behavioral Disorders
Asha K. Jitendra, Michael P. George, Sheetal Sood, and Kelly Price

ABSTRACT: The authors describe how schema-based instruction Research Council’s Adding It Up (Kilpatrick, Swafford, &
(SBI), a conceptual teaching approach that integrates the National Findell, 2001) report suggested, “All young Americans
Council for Teachers of Mathematics (2000) Standards, improved must learn to think mathematically, and they must think
the mathematical problem-solving ability of 2 students with emo-
tional and behavioral disorders (EBD). The authors illustrate the mathematically to learn” (p. 1). For teachers working
application of SBI to support students as they make sense of, and with students with special needs, the challenge is to shift
successfully solve, word problems. Last, the authors report on the from a focus on procedural skills and rote procedures to
factors that may have contributed to the effectiveness of SBI for new ways of conceptualizing mathematics instruction to
students with EBD. broaden students’ experiences in developing proficiency in
mathematics. Central to this instruction is the act of fos-
KEYWORDS: emotional and behavioral disorders, mathemat- tering students’ conceptual understanding. One approach
ics, problem-solving ability, schema-based instruction, teaching that accentuates conceptual understanding and is known to
approach improve students’ problem-solving skills is schema-based
CHILDREN WITH EMOTIONAL AND BEHAVIORAL instruction (SBI; Jitendra et al., 2007).
DISORDERS (EBD) evidence severe behavioral and aca-
What is SBI?
demic problems that negatively affect their overall function-
ing at school (e.g., poor grades, retention, suspensions or SBI emphasizes the essential role of the mathematical
expulsions from school). Although research on these students’ structure of problems as critical to problem comprehen-
academic status has suggested that students perform signifi- sion and representation. Instruction highlights problem
cantly below grade level in all subject areas, students seem to structure through the use of schematic diagrams that help
experience the greatest deficits in mathematics and spelling the learner categorize various problem types and organize
(Reid, Gonzalez, Nordness, Trout, & Epstein, 2004). Further, information to determine the most appropriate solution
deficits in mathematics tend to persist and increase over time procedures. For example, using semantic cues (e.g., both
(Nelson, Benner, Lane, & Smith, 2004). To date, most instruc- red apples and green apples are apples) and schematic
tional interventions in mathematics for students with EBD diagrams, SBI involves modeling the problem situation
have targeted basic skills rather than higher level mathematics to highlight the relations (red apples and green apples are
skills such as problem solving (Hodge, Riccomini, Buford, & subsets and all apples are supersets) between objects in
Herbst, 2006).
Proficiency in mathematics, in particular, knowing how
Address correspondence to Asha K. Jitendra, University of
to reason and solve problems, is crucial to adequately func- Minnesota, Department of Educational Psychology, 245 Educa-
tion in the context of daily life situations such as on the job, tion Sciences Building, 56 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN
at home, and in the community. In addition, the National 55455, USA; jiten001@umn.edu (e-mail).

145
146 Preventing School Failure Vol. 54, No. 3

the problem text. The understanding of those relations is constructing a model to represent the situation in the text fol-
critical to setting up the mathematical model (e.g., n red lowed by solution planning on the basis of the model (Hegarty,
apples ⫹ m green apples ⫽ x apples) and selecting an Mayer, & Monk, 1995). SBI is one approach to address these
appropriate mathematical operation to solve the problem students’ difficulties. Rather than beginning with word prob-
(add to solve for the superset or subtract to solve for the lems, each problem schema or problem type for a additive
subset). Because the linking of the algorithmic procedure problem structure (e.g., change, group, compare) is presented
(e.g., adding or subtracting) to the conceptual idea (e.g., first as a story situation with no unknown information so that
the sum of the parts is equal to the whole) is much more students focus on identifying and representing the features of
important than knowing that procedure itself, procedural the story situation using schematic diagrams (see Figure 1).
rules (add when the whole or total is unknown; subtract Next, word problems with unknowns that require using either
when one of the parts is unknown) in SBI are not taught addition or subtraction operation to solve them are intro-
in isolation. Instead, they are linked to the underlying duced. The compare problem “Arthur’s brother, Andrew, is
concepts. 25 inches tall. He is 8 inches shorter than Arthur. How tall
The research by Jitendra and colleagues (e.g., Jitendra is Arthur?” is used to illustrate the application of the FOPS
et al., 2007) on SBI has resulted in a problem-solving pro- strategy. As students apply each step of the FOPS strategy
gram in which instruction is aligned with the National Coun- (see Figure 2), they learn to monitor their strategy use. The
cil of Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM, 2000). Standards by following are the word problem-solving steps.
emphasizing the mathematical processes of problem solv-
ing, communicating, connecting, reasoning, and represent- Step 1: Find the Problem Type
ing word problems (Jitendra, 2007). At the same time, the To find the problem type, SBI focuses students’ attention
SBI mathematics program incorporates research-supported on reading the problem and then paraphrasing it in their own
instructional practices (e.g., explicit instruction, strategic words by emphasizing what is known in the problem and
integration, judicious review) for struggling learners. Specifi- what is unknown to understand the problem context. Next,
cally, SBI uses a paradigm of teacher-mediated instruction teacher explanations and elaborations of the problem context
followed by paired partner learning and independent learning help students identify it as a compare the problem because
activities. It scaffolds student learning using visual diagrams it involves the comparison of two disjoint sets; that is, the
(see Figure 1) and checklists that are eventually faded as problem compares Andrew’s height to that of his brother,
students become independent learners. A self-monitoring Arthur, and the compare words, “shorter than,” cue the
heuristic or plan (FOPS; in which F represents Find the learner to the compare situation in the problem.
problem type, O represents Organize the information in
the problem using the diagram, P represents Plan to solve Step 2: Organize the Information in the Problem Using a
the problem, and S represents Solve the problem) is used to Compare Diagram
anchor student learning in preparation for the transition from To facilitate problem representation, students are prompt-
teacher-mediated instruction to independent learning (see ed to use the schematic diagram for organizing or represent-
Figure 2). The FOPS problem-solving heuristic serves to ing the information in the problem text. For the compare
guide students in self-regulating (e.g., “Why is this a change problem, it involves reading the comparison sentence (i.e.,
problem?”) their strategy use, justifying the derived solu- Andrew is 8 in shorter than Arthur) to identify the two sets
tions using the problem features as anchors for explanations compared in the problem, determining the identity of the
and elaborations, and checking the accuracy of not only the bigger (Arthur) and smaller (Andrew) sets, labeling them in
computation but also the representation. These procedures the diagram for the bigger and smaller sets, and writing the
ensure that students engage in thinking and reasoning rather difference amount (8 in) in the diagram. Next, students read
than applying rote procedures. Last, frequent measures of stu- the problem to find the quantities associated with the two sets
dent word problem solving performance that have been vali- and write them in the diagram (see Figure 1). The unknown
dated in previous research (Jitendra, Sczesniak, & Deatline- quantity in the diagram is represented using a question mark.
Buchman, 2005; Leh, Jitendra, Caskie, & Griffin, 2007) are Last, students summarize the information in the story using
used to monitor student progress and inform instruction. the completed diagram and check the accuracy of the rep-
resentation by reviewing the information related to each set
Teaching Problem Solving Using SBI (i.e., bigger, smaller, and difference). In this step, underlining
Although some students are able to translate and integrate of critical information (e.g., the comparison sentence) and
information in the problem into a coherent mental repre- circling of quantities associated with the objects and relations
sentation (Mayer, 1999; Mayer & Hegarty, 1996), students in the problem helps students with attention and memory
who struggle in mathematics have difficulty with problem problems to disregard irrelevant information and focus on the
comprehension and would benefit from explicit instruction in essential information needed to solve the problem.
Vol. 54, No. 3 Jitendra, George, Sood, & Price 147

Change: Bobby prepared a lot of meals for his long hike. He ate 42 meals as he hiked through
the mountains. There were 15 meals left at the end of his hike. How many meals did he prepare
for his hke?

42
meals

Change

Whole or
total not ?
meals 15
known meals

Beginning Ending

42 + 15 = ?
So ? = 57

Answer: He prepared 57 meals for his hike.

Group: Farmer Jake has 88 animals on his farm. He only has horses and goats. There are 49
horses on the farm. How many goats are on the farm?

Horses Goats Horses and


goats (all
animals)
Whole or
49 ? 88 total
known

Small groups Large group or


or parts whole (sum)

88 – 49 = ?
So ? = 39

Answer: There are 39 goats are on the farm.

FIGURE 1. Sample of change, group, and compare word problems and schematic diagrams.
Note. Change diagram adapted from Schemas in Problem Solving (p. 133), by S. P. Marshall, 1995. Copyright 1995 by Cambridge
University Press. Adapted with permission. Word problems from Solving Math Word Problems: Teaching Students with Learning
Disabilities Using Schema-Based Instruction (p. 31, 78, 118), by A. K. Jitendra, 2007, Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Copyright 2007 by
PRO-ED, Inc. Reprinted with permission.

Step 3: Plan to Solve the Problem into a number sentence (i.e., 25 ⫹ 8 ⫽ ?) even though the
This step emphasizes planning to solve for the unknown semantic equation derived directly from the compare dia-
quantity by first selecting the appropriate operation. For gram is ? ⫺ 25 ⫽ 8. The semantic equation lists the num-
compare problems, students learn that the bigger set is bers in “the order that follows the meaning of the problem”
the whole or total and the smaller set and difference are and may not necessarily isolate the unknown on one side
the two parts that make up the whole. On the basis of this of the equal sign (Van de Walle, 2004, p. 138). As such,
information, they transform the information in the diagram by writing an equivalent equation, 25 ⫹ 8 ⫽ ?, referred to
148 Preventing School Failure Vol. 54, No. 3

Compare: Arthur’s brother, Andrew, is 25 inches tall. He is 8 inches shorter than Arthur. How tall
is Arthur?

Arthur
Whole or Andrew
total not ? 25 8
known inches inches inches

Bigger Smaller Difference

25 + 8 = ?
So ? = 33

Answer: Arthur is 33 inches tall.

FIGURE 2. Compare problem checklist.

TABLE 1. General Description of the Two Case-Study Participants

Description Matt Debbie

Gender Male Female


Grade 5 4
Race Caucaçsian African American
Age (in years) 12.0 13.0
Free/Reduced Lunch Yes No
Diagnosis Behavioral disorder Emotional disturbance
Medication Yes (5 types) No
Cognitive functioning Borderline to average: Average to above average: verbal IQ 112;
verbal IQ 102; performance IQ 72 performance IQ 123
Achievementa
Math 85 109
Reading and oral language 97 NA
Writing 83 82
Math computation CBM 31 dcpm 28 dcpm
Reading CBM NA 114 wcpm

Note. CBM ⫽ curriculum-based measurement; dcpm ⫽ digits correct per minute; NA ⫽ not available.
a
Standard scores on Wechsler Individual Achievement Test II (Psychological Corporation, 2001).

as the computational form of the equation, the unknown is “Andrew is 33 inches tall”) seems right, because Andrew
isolated on one side of the equation, which, for students, is is taller than Arthur, who is 25 inches tall. Also, they
easier than the earlier semantic equation to solve. check the answer by subtracting 33 ⫺ 25 ⫽ ? (i.e., the dif-
ference) or 33 ⫺ 8 ⫽ ? (i.e., the smaller set amount).
Step 4: Solve the Problem
Last, students solve for the unknown in the math sen- Case Study on SBI Implementation with 2 Students with
tence using the operation identified in Step 3 and write the Emotional and Behavioral Disorders
complete answer (i.e., number and unit). They are prompt- Matt and Debbie were two students who were diagnosed
ed to check not only the reasonableness of their answer with EBD participated in this case study (for a summary of
but also the accuracy of their representation and computa- student characteristics, see Table 1). Debbie was diagnosed
tion. For example, students reason that their answer (i.e., with severe learning disability in addition to an emotional
Vol. 54, No. 3 Jitendra, George, Sood, & Price 149

100

90

80

70
Percent correct scores

60

50

40

30

20

10 Matt
0
WPS pretest WPS-F time 1 WPS-F time 2 WPS-F time 3 WPS-F time 4 WPS-F time 5 WPS-F time 6 WPS post-test

WPS test WPS-F probes

100

90

80
Percentage correct scores

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0 Debbie
WPS pretest WPS-F time 1 WPS-F time 2 WPS-F time 3 WPS-F time 4 WPS-F time 5 WPS-F time 6 WPS post-test

WPS test WPS-F probes

FIGURE 3. Performance data on word problem solving (WPS) pretest, word problem solving-fluency (WPS-F) probes, and
WPS posttest by students.

disorder; Matt had bipolar disorder and exhibited oppo- riculum-based measurement (see Table 1). The study was
sitional defiant and disruptive behavior disorders. Both implemented over 20 school weeks. Students received SBI
students were receiving instruction from a special educa- in solving one-step and two-step addition and subtraction
tion teacher in a private school for children and adolescents word problems during their regularly scheduled mathemat-
with challenging behavior problems. We selected these two ics instructional period for 45 min daily, 5 days per week,
students for the case study because they received support for a total of 225 min weekly. The classroom teacher who
from the same teacher in the same classroom. Matt was received in-service training in implementing SBI taught
functioning below grade level in mathematics, and Debbie all lessons with a high degree of treatment implementation
had chronically underachieved in school. These students fidelity (M ⫽ 98.9%; range ⫽ 92–100) as determined by
were pretested on their mathematics computational skills independent observations that were conducted nine times
and were similar in their performance as measured by cur- during the study.
150 Preventing School Failure Vol. 54, No. 3

Case Study Findings because it is one of the few explicit approaches advocated
Figure 3 presents the performance data for problem solv- by the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008) that
ing by both Matt and Debbie across the study. Matt’s perfor- addresses effective problem solving and reasoning skills that
mance on the word problem-solving 16-item test improved tend to develop over a long period of time. At the same time,
substantially from 27% at pretest to 97% for an overall future research with larger samples and rigorous experimen-
improvement of 70%; Debbie’s performance indicated an tal designs should be conducted to determine not only the
increase of 25%. Her scores improved from 73% at pretest effectiveness of SBI for improving the problem solving per-
to 98% at posttest. Results of an eight-item word problem- formance of students with EBD, but also to examine transfer
solving fluency (WPS-F) probe administered every 3 weeks effects and outcomes over time.
following instruction to monitor student progress indicated a AUTHOR NOTES
mean performance of 59% for Matt and 93% for Debbie. For
Asha K. Jitendra is the Rodney Wallace Professor for the
Matt, data during intervention indicated somewhat variable Advancement of Teaching and Learning in the Department of
performance, with an increasing trend. Although the perfor- Educational Psychology at the University of Minnesota. Her
mance on the probe at Time 6 showed a decrease, the score research interests include academic and curricular strategies in
was higher than at Time 1 (see Figure 3). In contrast, data for mathematics and reading for students with learning disabilities,
Debbie during the intervention were stable, with an increas- assessment practices to inform instruction, as well as instruc-
tional design and textbook analysis. Michael P. George is the
ing trend. These data are noteworthy given that the progress director of the Centennial School of Lehigh University. His
made by both students during the intervention was either research interests are the design and development of effective
maintained or increased at posttest to a level that is deemed special education programs and prevention and intervention in
proficient (less than 95% correct). Evidently, proficient levels the field of emotional and behavioral disorders. Sheetal Sood is
of performance are necessary for maintenance and general- an assistant professor at the University of Hartford. Her research
interests include early mathematics and reading intervention.
ization of problem solving skills. Further, the mean percent- Kelly Price is the elementary program coordinator at the Centen-
age of on-task behavior during the intervention was 96% nial School of Lehigh University. Her research interests include
for Matt and 97% Debbie, which is encouraging given that school and classwide positive behavior support systems, preven-
students with EBD characteristically experience behavioral tion and intervention strategies, direct instruction, and active
problems that interfere with their learning. engagement techniques.

Summary REFERENCES

The case study results suggest that students with EBD can Gersten, R. (2005). Behind the scenes of an intervention
research study. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,
successfully learn problem-solving skills when instruction 20, 200–212.
is designed to promote understanding. It appears that the Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., & Monk, C. A. (1995). Comprehension
schematic diagrams in SBI provided a “level of concreteness of arithmetic word problems: A comparison of successful and
and support to help understand key concepts” (Gersten, 2005, unsuccessful problem solvers. Journal of Educational Psychol-
p. 203) necessary for successful problem solving. Three ogy, 87, 18–32.
Hodge, J., Riccomini, P. J., Buford, R., & Herbst, M. H. (2006).
factors seemed to contribute to the effectiveness of SBI for A review of instructional interventions in mathematics for
improving the two students’ problem-solving performance students with emotional and behavioral disorders. Behavioral
in the present case study. Using SBI, the teacher (a) empha- Disorders, 31, 297–311.
sized problem comprehension and representation (conceptual Jitendra, A. K. (2007). Solving math word problems: Teaching stu-
knowledge) as well as linked the algorithmic procedure (e.g., dents with learning disabilities using schema-based instruction.
Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
adding and subtracting) to the conceptual idea (e.g., the sum Jitendra, A. K., Griffin, C., Haria, P., Leh, J., Adams, A., &
of the parts is equal to the whole), (b) appropriately scaf- Kaduvetoor, A. (2007). A comparison of single and multiple
folded instruction (e.g., consistent, logical, and complete strategy instruction on third grade students’ mathematical prob-
explanations; schematic diagrams; self-regulation strategy lem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 115–127.
checklists), and (c) focused on ongoing progress monitoring Jitendra, A. K., Sczesniak, E., & Deatline-Buchman, A. (2005).
Validation of curriculum-based mathematical word problem
to inform subsequent instruction. Although SBI in this case solving tasks as indicators of mathematics proficiency for third
study was effective and represents a promising approach for graders. School Psychology Review, 34, 358–371.
teachers to meet the provisions of the No Child Left Behind Kilpatrick, J., Swafford, J., & Findell, B. (2001). Adding it up:
Act (2001) with regard to closing the achievement gap for Helping children learn mathematics. Washington, DC: National
students with EBD, the intensity of the 20-week interven- Academy Press.
Leh, J., Jitendra, A. K., Caskie, G., & Griffin, C. (2007). An evalu-
tion for the two students with EBD may call into question ation of CBM mathematics word problem solving measures
the efficiency of SBI. Nonetheless, if the goal of instruction for monitoring third grade students’ mathematics competence.
is to promote conceptual understanding, SBI is appropriate Assessment for Effective Intervention, 32, 90–99.
Vol. 54, No. 3 Jitendra, George, Sood, & Price 151

Marshall, S. P. (1995). Schemas in problem solving. New York: Nelson, R. J., Benner, G. J., Lane, K., & Smith, B. W.
Cambridge University Press. (2004). Academic achievement of K–12 students with
Mayer, R. E. (1999). The promise of educational psychology: emotional and behavioral disorders. Exceptional Children,
Learning in the content areas (Vol. 1). Upper Saddle River, NJ: 71, 59–73.
Merrill/Prentice Hall. No Child Left Behind Act of 2001. 20 U.S.C. § 6319 (2008).
Mayer, R. E., & Hegarty, M. (1996). The process of understanding Psychological Corporation (2001). WIAT II: Wechsler Individual
mathematics problems. In R. J. Sternberg & T. Ben-Zeev (Eds.), Achievement Test (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Author.
The nature of mathematical thinking (pp. 29–53). Hillsdale, NJ: Reid, R., Gonzalez, J. E., Nordness, P. D., Trout, A., & Epstein, M. H.
Erlbaum. (2004). A meta-analysis of the academic status of students with
National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (2000). Principles emotional/behavioral disturbance. Journal of Special Educa-
and Standards for School Mathematics. Reston, VA: Author. tion, 38, 130–143.
National Mathematics Advisory Panel. (2008). Foundations for Van de Walle, J. A. (2004). Elementary and middle school math-
success: The final report of the National Mathematics Advisory ematics: Teaching developmentally (5th Ed.). Boston: Allyn &
Panel. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education. Bacon.
Copyright of Preventing School Failure is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd. and its content may not be
copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written
permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like