You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/325224502

The contrasting influences of incidental anger and fear on responses to a


service failure

Article  in  Psychology and Marketing · May 2018


DOI: 10.1002/mar.21114

CITATIONS READS

13 269

3 authors:

Lei Su Lisa C. Wan


Hong Kong Baptist University The Chinese University of Hong Kong
54 PUBLICATIONS   346 CITATIONS    18 PUBLICATIONS   615 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jr. Robert S. Wyer


Chinese Universiy of Hong Kong
239 PUBLICATIONS   15,704 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The contrasting influences of incidental anger and fear on responses to a service failure View project

Cultural mindsets View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Lei Su on 24 August 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


DOI: 10.1002/mar.21114

RESEARCH ARTICLE

The contrasting influences of incidental anger and fear


on responses to a service failure

Lei Su1 ∗ Lisa C. Wan2 ∗ Robert S. Wyer Jr3 †

1 School of Business, Hong Kong Baptist

University, Hong Kong, China Abstract


2 School of Hotel and Tourism Management, The Customers’ disposition to register a formal complaint about an inferior product or poor service is
Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, often mediated by attributions of responsibility. However, the anger or fear that people happen to
China be experiencing for totally irrelevant reasons can also influence this disposition. Two field studies
3 Marketing Department, The Chinese University
and four laboratory experiments indicate that when people feel angry at the time they encounter a
of Hong Kong, Hong Kong, China
service failure, they are more likely to blame the service provider for the failure and more likely to
Correspondence
Lei Su, WLB504, Hong Kong Baptist University,
register a complaint. When they experience fear, however, they are uncertain about the cause of
Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong, China. their misfortune and decrease their negative reactions relative to conditions in which fear is not
Email: lsu@hkbu.edu.hk experienced. The effects of these incidental emotions are evident both when a service failure is
∗ The first two authors contributed equally to this
personally experienced and when it is only observed. These effects are eliminated, however, when
article.
individuals do not have the cognitive resources available to assess the reasons for the service fail-
† Robert Wyer is currently a visiting professor at
ure and the conditions surrounding it.
the University of Cincinnati.

KEYWORDS
attribution, complaint, incidental emotion, purchase intention, service failure

1 INTRODUCTION Furthermore, according to the data from the Beijing Consumer


Association (2012), consumers’ complaints about the service they
Consumers’ complaints can often provide managers with insight into received increased significantly in the weeks that followed.
how to improve their business. Several features of a consumption sit- A quite different incident occurred a year earlier, on May 15, 2011,
uation can influence individuals’ likelihood of complaining indepen- when an earthquake in eastern Japan caused major damage to the
dently of their satisfaction with the product or service itself, including Fukushima Nuclear Power Plant. Chinese people panicked as a result
the nature of their personal relationship with the provider (Wan, Hui, of the radiation leakage, often stocking up basic supplies in anticipa-
& Wyer, 2011) and whether they perceive that the failure was avoid- tion of food shortages. In this case, data from the same source indi-
able (Bitner, 1990; Chan & Wan, 2008). However, consumers’ com- cated a significant decrease in consumers’ complaints following the
plaint behavior can also be influenced by factors that are objectively incident.
irrelevant to the consumption experience. For example, consumers’ The aforementioned examples raise the possibility that people's
emotions at the time they encounter a service failure could affect their incidental emotional reactions to a consumption situation can influ-
disposition to complain or simply to withdraw from the situation inde- ence their complaint behavior independently of the characteristics
pendently of the circumstances that gave rise to these emotions. The of the situation itself. For example, individuals who happen to feel
present research explored this possibility. angry at the time they have a negative experience might attribute
Two commonly experienced basic emotions, anger and fear, are the cause of the experience to others and this attribution might
associated with attributions of blame (Berkowitz, 1990; Tiedens & stimulate an action against the perpetrator. In contrast, individu-
Linton, 2001). Two incidents provide graphic evidence that people's als who experience fear might be uncertain about the cause of the
experience of these emotions for totally irrelevant reasons can affect event and feel out of control, leading them to avoid the situation
their subsequent complaint behavior. On September 10, 2012, the rather than confronting it. The effects of these emotional reactions
Japanese government announced that it had purchased the Senkaku could occur independently of the events that have given rise to
Islands from “private owners,” thus creating conflict and animosity them.
between Japan and China. The anger of the Chinese people was evi- The research to be reported investigated this possibility. Partici-
dent throughout the country, as they smashed Japanese cars, and pants were induced to feel either angry or fearful at the time they
protested outside the Japanese Embassy. encountered a service failure. People who felt angry were expected to

Psychol Mark. 2018;1–10. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/mar 


c 2018 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 1
2 SU ET AL .

attribute responsibility for the situation to the service provider, lead- (e.g., Betancourt & Blair, 1992; Weiner, 1980). Anger, for example,
ing them to register a complaint against the provider. In contrast, peo- is characterized by a high degree of certainty and a disposition to
ple who experienced fear at the time they encountered the situation approach (e.g., to “attack”) the perpetrator of the event in order to pun-
were expected to be uncertain of the cause of the event, decreasing ish the person or obtain redress (Berkowitz, 1990). In contrast, fear
their likelihood of complaining and increasing their disposition to avoid is characterized by uncertainty and a disposition to avoid the situa-
the situation. The effects of these incidental emotions could be evi- tion that gave rise to it (Tiedens & Linton, 2001). In the present con-
dent both when a service failure is personally experienced and when text, these considerations suggest that consumers who attribute a ser-
it is only observed. In the latter case, angry observers might decrease vice failure to a feature of the situation that was under the provider's
their likelihood of patronizing the provider in the future whereas fear control may experience anger, whereas those who attribute it to an
observers might increase this disposition. unstable feature of the situation may experience uncertainty and
anxiety.
Emotions have both a feeling component and a cognitive compo-
nent (Wyer, 2004; Wyer, Clore, & Isbell, 1999). Consequently, it is often
2 THEORETICAL BACKGROUND difficult to separate the effects of the feelings that people experience
from the effects of the cognitions that accompany them. The present
Most research assumes that consumers base their decisions to com- research was not concerned with the effects of the cognitive appraisal
plain about an unfavorable service experience on features of the sit- of a service failure on the emotions it elicits. Rather, it investigated the
uation itself. These features can influence inferences that the service effect of the feelings that individuals happen to be experiencing for
failure was either intentional or beyond the provider's control (Bit- reasons that have nothing to do with a service failure can affect their
ner, 1990; Chan & Wan, 2008) and perceptions of the likelihood of disposition to complain about the failure and their patronage inten-
receiving fair compensation (Blodgett, Hill, & Tax, 1997; Maxham & tions. Although the effects of emotions on judgments and behavior
Netemeyer, 2002). Complaint behavior can also be influenced by cus- have been previously investigated (for a review, see So et al., 2015), a
tomers’ personal relationship with the provider (Wan et al., 2011). consideration of these effects in the present context raises issues that
However, although customers’ responses to a negative consumption have rarely been investigated.
experience seem likely to depend in part on the type and intensity of Of particular relevance to the present concerns is the appraisal-
their emotional reactions to it, little research has confirmed this intu- tendency framework proposed by Lerner and Keltner (2001) and Han
ition (Smith & Bolton, 2002). That is, the extent to which complaint et al. (2007). According to their formulation, features of the cogni-
behavior is induced by emotions that customers happen to be experi- tive appraisal that stimulate an emotion can carry over to situations
encing for reasons that are unrelated to the service failure itself has in which the emotion is elicited, leading the later situation to be inter-
not been examined. Furthermore, although past research has focused preted in terms of these features. Thus, the subjective experience of an
on consumers’ reactions to a service failure that they personally expe- emotion serves as a prime, activating concepts that influence the inter-
rience (e.g., Bitner, 1990; Wan, 2013; Wan, Hui, et al., 2011), little pretation of a new situation to which they are applicable and a behav-
research has examined their reactions to a service failure that they ioral disposition that is associated with this interpretation. For exam-
observe. Online complaints on social networking sites, which have ple, previous research has found that when participants were induced
increased by 800% between 2014 and 2015 (Causon, 2015), capture to feel angry in one situation, these feelings elicited a motive to blame
many observers’ attention and affect their purchase decisions. There- that carried over to other unrelated situations (Quigley & Tedeschi,
fore, it is important to examine whether the effects of incidental emo- 1996). Similarly, incidental fear can elicit feelings of uncertainty that
tions on attributions of blame carry over to both suffering customers influence consumers’ willingness to make a choice in an unrelated sit-
and observing customers. uation they encounter later (Coleman, Williams, Morales, & White,
In considering this possibility, two emotions, anger and fear, are par- 2017).
ticularly likely to play a role. Although these emotions share a similar These considerations have implications for the effect of emotions
valence, they are linked to distinctive patterns of cognitive appraisal, on responses to a service failure. If the perception that another person
especially attributions of responsibility (Berkowitz, 1990; Tiedens & has been responsible for one's misfortune has become associated with
Linton, 2001). Most theory and research on the relation between emo- feelings of anger, concepts associated with these feelings are likely to
tions and behavior assumes that emotional reactions to an event are be reactivated in a later situation to which they are applicable. These
elicited by a cognitive appraisal of the circumstances that surround its concepts, in turn, could influence the interpretation of this situation
occurrence (Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988; Roseman, 1991; Smith & and the behavior that results from it. Thus, the anger that happens to
Ellsworth, 1985). For example, if people attribute responsibility for a be experienced at the time a service failure occurs might influence the
negative event to another person and perceive that the other could interpretation of the events surrounding the service failure and the dis-
have prevented the event's occurrence, they may respond with anger position to complain about it. In contrast, if a fear-evoking experience
(Han, Lerner, & Keltner, 2007; Ortony et al., 1988). If they attribute the has elicited a disposition to avoid the situation, and if concepts asso-
event to an unstable situational factor, however, they may experience ciated with this disposition are salient at the time a service failure is
anxiety and fear. Numerous studies confirm the behavioral manifes- encountered, these concepts could give rise to avoidance behavior in
tations of these attributions and the emotions associated with them this situation as well.
SU ET AL . 3

In summary: 1983) and these responses could occur without a deliberate analysis
of the situation at hand.
H1: The experience of incidental anger at the time individuals
To evaluate this possibility, we capitalized on evidence that the
encounter a service failure will increase the likelihood of com-
effect incidental emotions on the cognitive deliberation involved con-
plaining about the failure whereas the experience of incidental
sumer decision making and judgment is diminished under cognitive
fear at the time the failure is experienced will decrease this like-
load (Drolet & Luce, 2004; Kron, Schul, Cohen, & Hassin, 2010). Specif-
lihood.
ically, participants in some conditions were put under cognitive load
To reiterate, Hypothesis 1 assumes that the effects of emotions by asking them to keep some extraneous material in mind while they
on responses to a service failure are mediated by the effects of con- responded to information describing a service failure. If the effects
cepts associated with these emotions on cognitive appraisals of the of incidental anger and fear on these responses are mediated by con-
failure situation and perceptions of the reasons for its occurrence scious cognitive appraisals of the service failure situation, putting
(Lerner & Keltner, 2001). If this is so, the appraisal-relevant con- participants under cognitive load should eliminate these effects. If,
cepts associated with an emotion could affect the interpretation of contrary to expectations, the effects of emotions reflect conditioned
not only an event that one personally experiences but also an event approach and avoidance responses to the situation, inducing cognitive
that occurs to someone else. Defensive attribution theory (Shaver, load should have little effect. The former was expected.
1970) suggests that when observers witness a negative event in a
H3: Putting participants under cognitive load when they consider a
context that is personally relevant, they assign responsibility to the
service failure situation will reduce the effects of the emotions
perpetrator of the event rather than the victim. The perception of
they are experiencing on responses to the situation.
relevance, in turn, is influenced by similarity between the circum-
stances of the person involved and the observer (Burger, 1981). In
the service encounters, observers could include both (a) other cus-
tomers who are involved in the same service setting and (b) individ- 3 PRELIMINARY STUDIES
uals who simply learn about the incident through social media (e.g.,
online review). They clearly share situational similarity with the suffer- Two field studies, noted briefly earlier in this article, provided pre-
ing customer in a service failure. To this extent, incidental anger and liminary support for the correlational relationship between incidental
fear might influence the interpretation of not only a service failure emotions of anger or fear and complaint behavior.
that one personally encounters but also a failure that one happens to
observe.
Of course, people who observe a service failure that occurs to oth-
3.1 Responses to anger
ers are unlikely to complain themselves. If they attribute the failure On September 10, 2012, the Japanese government announced that
to the service provider, however, they may decrease their intention it had purchased the Senkaku Islands from “private owners.” This
to use the service again. Prior research has confirmed that attribu- announcement provoked widespread anger among Chinese people.
tions of blame to a service provider will decrease observers’ likelihood Two independent judges coded people's posts posted on the major Chi-
of patronizing the provider in the future (Wan, Chan, & Su, 2011). If nese chatting website (www.baidu.com) regarding to this news dur-
this is so, and if incidental anger is associated with appraisals of indi- ing the first week after the announcement, by categorizing each of the
vidual control, it should decrease observers’ likelihood of patroniz- posts based the major emotional type that it involved. The researchers
ing the provider. Correspondingly, if incidental fear is associated with used the coding scheme developed by Richins (1997) for such cate-
appraisals of uncertainty and situational control, it might increase this gorization. The intercoder agreement rate was 78%. The results indi-
disposition relative to conditions in which this fear was not experi- cated that anger is the major elicited emotion (80.97% for anger; other
enced. elicited emotions included sadness, disgust, etc.).
The effect of incidental emotions on consumers’ complaint behavior
H2: The attribution of responsibility for a service failure that occurs
was inferred from the monthly number of complaint letters received
to another person will be affected by the anger and fear that
from Beijing residents to the Beijing Consumer Association (2011,
one personally experiences at the time the failure is observed.
2012) during the month that the incident occurred and the months
Specifically, anger will decrease the likelihood of patronizing the
both preceded it and followed it. These frequencies were compared to
provider in the future whereas the experience of incidental fear
the number of complaints received in identical months of the year pre-
at the time the failure is observed will increase this disposition.
ceding the incident (2011). To remove the effect of year-to-year fluc-
These considerations raise an alternative possibility that has not tuation, the number in September 2011 was adjusted according to the
typically been considered. That is, the effect of an emotion on ratio of the average number of monthly complaint letters received in
responses to a service failure might not be mediated by a cognitive 2012 to that in 2011.
appraisal at all but rather, might reflect a conditioned response to the The results are summarized in Table 1. Consistent with the pre-
emotion that occurs automatically, with little if any cognitive delibera- diction, there was a significant rise in the number of complaint let-
tion. Fear, for example, might elicit an automatic avoidance response ters in September 2012, with 1,605 letters of complaint received in
(Foa & Kozak, 1986) whereas anger might elicit aggression (Averill, September 2012 compared with only 1,374 in September 2011. Also,
4 SU ET AL .

TA B L E 1 Comparison of number of consumer complaints (anger condition)—preliminary studies

Ratio Ratio
Number of Complaint Letters August September October Sep/Aug Sep/Oct
Year 2011 (Adjusted)* 1,313 1,374 1,273 1.05 1.08
Year 2012 1,129 1,605 1,048 1.42 1.53

Ratio of monthly average in Year 2012 to that in Year 2011: 0.88.

the ratios of number of complaint letters in September to August and guise of a “memory test” (Lerner, Gonzalez, Small, & Fischhoff, 2003),
to October are consistently higher in year 2012, than those in year participants were asked to describe a past experience that made them
2011. These comparisons consistently indicate that incidental anger very angry (anger conditions), an experience that made them very fear-
increases complaints toward service provider. ful (fear conditions) or the activities they typically engaged in dur-
ing the evening (control conditions). They then reported how angry or
3.2 Responses to fear fearful they felt along scales of one (not at all) to seven (very strong)

The Great East Japan Earthquake that caused major damage to the (Dunn & Hoegg, 2014; Lerner & Keltner, 2001).

Fukushima Nuclear Power Plant on March 15th, 2011, led Chinese cit- In the second task, participants provided feedback on an online

izens to panic over the radiation leakage and to stock up on supplies in ordering system used by a local restaurant. They were shown pictures

anticipation of food shortages. Independent judges’ coding of people's of several foods and beverages, together with name and price informa-

emotional responses to this event during the first week after its occur- tion and asked to place their orders by clicking a button. However, they

rence (using the same coding scheme; interjudge agreement = 73%) experienced a 10-s delay after pressing the check-out button. A pretest

indicated that fear is the major elicited emotion (52.97% for fear; other (N = 50) confirmed that participants had a stronger perception of ser-

elicited emotions included sad, curious, etc.). vice failure under 10-s delay conditions than under no-delay conditions

A similar procedure was used in evaluating the effects of earth- (M delay = 7.72 vs. M no-delay = 2.80, respectively; F(1, 48) = 92.49;

quake disaster. In this case, however, the month in which the precipi- P < 0.001).

tating event occurred (February, the month of Chinese Lunar New Year Participants then wrote down their thoughts and feelings about

in which many celebrations occur) was excluded. the system and reported their intentions to complain by respond-

These results, shown in Table 2, indicate that a substantial decrease ing to a single item (i.e., “You have strong intention to complain to

in consumer complaints during the month the incident occurred, with the restaurant”) along three measurement scales from 1 (strongly dis-

1,220 complaint letters received in March 2011 (adjusted by year-to- agree/definitely not/never) to 7 (strongly agree/definitely will/always);

year fluctuation) compared to 1,873 in March 2012. Similarly, the ratio 𝛼 = 0.98. Finally, they reported their agreement with three items con-

of number of complaint letters received in March to April is much lower cerning the cause of the technical problems (e.g., “The restaurant is

in year 2011 than that in year 2012, indicating a significant drop in responsible for the technical problems of the system”) along scales

complaint intention among the society. from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree); 𝛼 = 0.76.

In summary, the preliminary studies provided initial evidence


that consumer complaint behavior changed in opposite directions in
4.2 Results
response to events that elicited widespread anger and fear. Because
complaint letters only represent a small proportion of total complaint 4.2.1 Manipulation check
cases, these studies provide a conservative test of the hypothesis. The Participants reported being more angry in the anger conditions
different data sources for the independent and dependent variables (M = 4.58, SD = 1.89) than in the fear conditions (M = 2.22, SD = 1.07;
also decreased the possibility of demand effects. Finally, the longitu- F(1, 95) = 54.09, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.38) and the control conditions
dinal nature of the dependent variable and the field setting demon- (M = 1.24, SD = 0.50; F(1, 95) = 109.68, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.61), and
strated the strong external validity of the effect. reported being more fearful in the fear conditions (M = 4.39, SD = 1.82)
than in the anger conditions (M = 2.29, SD = 1.27; F(1, 95) = 40.65,
P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.31) and the control conditions (M = 1.33, SD = 0.54;
4 EXPERIMENT 1
F(1, 95) = 88.83, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.57).

Experiment 1 investigated the effects of incidental anger and fear on


complaint behavior in a laboratory setting in which participants per- 4.2.2 Complaint intention
sonally experienced a service failure. Participants’ intention to complain to the service provider was signifi-
cantly greater in anger conditions (M = 4.42, SD = 1.21) than in control
4.1 Method
conditions (M = 3.61, SD = 1.23; F(1, 95) = 6.81, P = 0.011, 𝜂p2 = 0.10)
One hundred Hong Kong university students (30 men; M age = 20.99, and was significantly lower in fear conditions (M = 2.85, SD = 1.29) than
SD = 2.48) participated in exchange for a small monetary incentive. Par- in control conditions (M = 3.61; SD = 1.23; F(1, 95) = 6.10, P = 0.015,
ticipants were told that there were two unrelated studies. Under the 𝜂p2 = 0.08).
SU ET AL . 5

TA B L E 2 Comparison of number of consumer complaints (fear condition)—preliminary studies

Ratio Ratio
Number of Complaint Letters February March April (Mar/Feb) (Mar/Apr)
Year 2011 (Adjusted)* – 1,220 1,369 – 0.89
Year 2012 – 1,873 1,578 – 1.19

Ratio of monthly average in Year 2012 to that in Year 2011: 0.88.

4.2.3 Complaint behavior out being present in the situation. In this case, the failure is personally
irrelevant. The effects of emotions on attributions in both situations
Two research assistants coded each listed thought as positive (e.g.,
were examined.
“The menu is colorful and eye-catchy”), negative (e.g., “The process-
ing time is long”) or neutral (e.g., “Maybe you can include an option of
not ordering anything”). All negative comments were treated as com-
plaints. The intercoder agreement rate was 87.6%.
5 EXPERIMENT 2A
The number of complaints generated by each participant was sig-
nificantly greater in anger conditions (M = 2.84, SD = 1.24) than in
According to Hypothesis 2, incidental emotions can affect the interpre-
control conditions (M = 2.00, SD = 1.37; F(1, 95) = 7.25, P = 0.008,
tation of not only a service failure that one personally encounters but
𝜂p2 = 0.10), and was significantly lower in fear conditions (M = 1.33,
also a failure that one happens to observe. Experiment 2A determined
SD = 1.11) than in control conditions (F(1, 95) = 4.73, P = 0.032,
whether incidental anger and fear would influence participants’ attri-
𝜂p2 = 0.07). Analyses of the proportion of complaints that participants
butions of a failure that occurs in a situation in which they imagined
listed (relative to the total number of comments) yielded similar con-
being personally involved.
clusions. That is, the proportion of complaints was greater in anger
conditions (M = 0.83, SD = 0.22) than in control conditions (M = 0.63,
SD = 0.28; F(1, 95) = 7.36, P = 0.008, 𝜂p2 = 0.14). Whereas, the pro- 5.1 Method
portion of complaints in fear conditions was much lower (M = 0.48,
One hundred five participants from the United States (37 men, M
SD = 0.35) (F(1, 95) = 4.91, P = 0.029, 𝜂p2 = 0.06).
age = 34.18, SD = 14.40) were recruited from Amazon Mechanical
Turk (MTurk) and were assigned to anger, fear, and control conditions.
4.2.4 Attributions To prime different emotions, the same recall task used in study 1 was
Participants’ tendency to blame the service provider was greater in employed. Then, in a second study, participants were asked to imag-
anger conditions (M = 5.10, SD = 0.91) than in control conditions ine that they were visiting a travel agency and saw another customer
(M = 4.50, SD = 1.10; F(1, 95) = 4.93, P = 0.029, 𝜂p2 = 0.09) but lower not being serviced after waiting for 20 min. After reading the scenario,
in fear conditions (M = 3.90, SD = 1.16) than in control conditions participants made attributions along the same scale used in study 1
(M = 4.50, SD = 1.10; F(1, 95) = 5.16, P = 0.025, 𝜂p2 = 0.07). (𝛼 = 0.89).
A bootstrapping analysis confirmed that attributions of blame to
service provider mediated the effect of incidental emotion on com-
plaint intention. Based on 5,000 samples, the indirect effect of attri- 5.2 Results
butions was significant (95% CI from 0.0335 to 0.3523). 5.2.1 Manipulation check
Participants’ report of both their anger and fear varied over conditions

4.3 Discussion as expected. Participants reported being more angry in anger condi-
tions (M = 4.06, SD = 1.93) than in either control (M = 2.06, SD = 1.48;
The effects of incidental emotions on responses to an actual failure F(1, 103) = 23.83, P < 0.001) or fear conditions (M = 1.89, SD = 1.64;
(study 1) provide converging support for the hypothesis. As noted ear- F(1, 103) = 29.38, P < 0.001). In addition, participants reported being
lier, however, these emotions could have similar effects on attribu- more fearful in fear conditions (M = 3.44, SD = 1.70) than in either con-
tions of responsibility for outcomes that others experience (Burger, trol (M = 1.58, SD = 1.00; F(1, 103) = 27.72, P < 0.001) or anger condi-
1981). To this extent, the effects of incidental anger and fear on par- tions (M = 2.14, SD = 1.59; F(1, 103) = 14.15, P < 0.001).
ticipants’ attributions for a service failure they observe should be sim-
ilar to the effects that occur when they personally experience the fail-
ure. These observations could occur in two conditions. In some cases, 5.2.2 Attributions
people might observe a service failure that occurs to another cus- Attributions of responsibility to the service provider were greater in
tomer in the same situation they are in. In this case, observers might anger conditions (M = 5.19, SD = 1.13) than in control conditions
interpret the failure as personally relevant because they are using the (M = 4.51, SD = 1.36; F(1, 103) = 4.82, P = 0.030) but was significantly
same service provider. In other conditions, however, consumers might lower in fear conditions (M = 3.71, SD = 1.36) than in control conditions
encounter a service failure in the social media (e.g., on YouTube) with- (F(1, 103) = 6.54, P = 0.012).
6 SU ET AL .

6 EXPERIMENT 2B ence of these emotions on attributions of responsibility for the fail-


ure's occurrence. Cognitive load has been used in several studies to dis-
Experiment 2A determined that incidental emotions affect attribu- tinguish between the effects of cognitive deliberation and the effects
tions of responsibility for a service failure that occurred in a situation in of automatic responses to stimuli (Albarracín & Wyer, 2000; Gilbert,
which participants were present. Experiment 2B determined whether 1989; Shiv & Fedorikhin, 1999). Putting participants under cognitive
these can be generalized to conditions in which the service failure hap- load when they consider a service failure should prevent them from
pens to another customer in a situation in which observers are not engaging in the cognitive effort required to make these attributions
involved at all. To test the generality of the effects over different meth- and should reduce the effects of the emotions they are experiencing
ods for priming incidental emotions (e.g., Lee & Andrade, 2011), a pro- on responses to the situation.
cedure developed by Rottenberg, Ray, and Gross (2007) was used to Experiment 3 also examined the consequences of these attri-
manipulate incidental anger and fear. butions. For example, if people attribute the failure to the service
provider, they may decrease their intention to use the service again.
Because “intentions to patronize” has significant marketing implica-
6.1 Method
tions (Maxham, 1999; Smith & Bolton, 1998), intentions to patronize
Eighty undergraduate students (22 men; M age = 20.43, SD = 3.08) were evaluated.
from a Hong Kong university participated in this study in exchange for
credit. Participants were told that they would participate in a “movie
showcase” and would view and evaluate two short movie clips. The
7.1 Method
first clip was used to prime the target emotion using materials similar
to those employed by Rottenberg et al. (2007). That is, participants in Two hundred five Hong Kong undergraduate students (104 men; M
anger conditions watched a scene from My Bodyguard (Simon, Devlin, age = 21.70, SD = 2.87) participated in exchange for a small amount
Goldfarb, & Bill, 1980) and participants in fear conditions watched a of money. They were assigned to cells of a 3 (emotion conditions: anger
scene from Silence of the Lambs (Goetzman et al., 1991). In control con- vs. fear vs. control) x 2 (cognitive load) design.
ditions, participants saw a clip of scenery. Anger, fear, and neutral emotions were primed as in Experiments 1
Next, the participants watched a video. The film described a cus- and 2A. Then, under the guise of a memory test, participants in high cog-
tomer in a camera shop who rented a camera but he discovered the nitive load conditions were shown pictures of 12 products along with
camera did not work after he paid and left. After watching the film, par- their prices and were given 1 min to remember the price of each prod-
ticipants reported their agreement with three items along a scale from uct. They were told that they would be asked to recall the information
1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree) “The shop assistant is respon- at the end of the study. Participants in the low cognitive load conditions
sible for the accident captured in the video.” They then responded to did not perform this task.
questions concerning the quality of the movie (e.g., filming technique, Then, in an ostensibly unrelated task, participants imagined that
lighting, etc.) and completed an emotion manipulation check. they observed a restaurant customer fail to get a sea-view table that
he had requested earlier. After doing so, they reported their willing-
ness to patronize the restaurant along three scales from 1 (strongly
6.2 Results
disagree/definitely not/very unlikely) to 7 (strongly agree/definite will/very
Participants reported being angrier in anger conditions (M likely); 𝛼 = 0.94. Their attribution of responsibility was then assessed
anger = 4.96, SD = 1.32) than in either control (M = 3.83, SD = 1.69; F(1, using four items (e.g., “The restaurant is responsible for the incident
74) = 6.71, P = 0.012, 𝜂p2 = 0.13) or fear conditions (M = 3.12, SD = 1.66; you observed”). Finally, participants in high cognitive load conditions
F(1, 74) = 18.70, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.28), and reported being more fearful recalled the prices of the products they had seen earlier. The average
in fear conditions (M = 5.35, SD = 1.77) than in either control (M = 3.25, rate of accurate recall was 28.35% and there were no significant dif-
SD = 1.68; F(1, 74) = 18.22, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.09) or anger conditions ferences across three emotion conditions.
(M = 3.63, SD = 1.76; F(1, 74) = 12.97, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.20). The effect
of incidental emotions on attributions of responsibility was significant
(F(2,74) = 10.33, P < 0.001). Attributions of responsibility to the shop
7.2 Results
assistant were greater in anger conditions (M = 3.59, SD = 1.09) than
in control conditions (M = 3.00, SD = 0.90; F(1, 74) = 5.68, P = 0.020, 7.2.1 Manipulation check
𝜂p2 = 0.09) but were less in fear conditions (M = 2.49, SD = 0.75) than in Participants reported being angrier in anger conditions (M = 3.83,
control conditions (F(1, 74) = 4.18, P = 0.044, 𝜂p2 = 0.09). SD = 2.11) than in fear conditions (M = 2.60, SD = 1.81; F(1,
199) = 14.40, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.09) and control conditions (M = 2.26,
SD = 1.70; F(1, 199) = 23.36, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.15). They reported being
7 EXPERIMENT 3 more fearful in fear conditions (M = 3.79, SD = 1.93) than in anger con-
ditions (M = 2.35, SD = 1.73; F(1, 199) = 22.46, P < 0.001, 𝜂p2 = 0.14) and
Experiments 1 and 2 supported the hypothesis that the impact of inci- control condition (M = 2.18, SD = 1.59; F(1, 199) = 28.55, P < 0.001,
dental emotions on reactions to a failure is mediated by the influ- 𝜂p2 = 0.17).
SU ET AL . 7

TA B L E 3 Consumer reactions to service failure as a function of inci- on observer's purchase intention (95% CI from –0.2322 to –0.0342).
dental emotion and cognitive load—Experiment 3 In high cognitive load conditions, however, the mediating effect of
Incidental Emotions attributions was not evident (based on 5,000 samples, 95% CI from
Anger Neutral Fear –0.0383 to 0.0603).
Attribution to Service Provider
Low cognitive load 4.51a (1.07) 4.05b (0.81) 3.64c (0.71)
7.3 Discussion
High cognitive load 4.04b (0.76) 4.03b (0.91) 4.08b (0.74) The effect of incidental emotions on observers’ purchase intentions
Patronage Intention disappeared when participants were prevented from engaging in the
Low cognitive load 3.03a (1.03) 3.63b (1.10) 4.11c (0.89) cognitive appraisals associated with the emotions they are experienc-
High cognitive load 3.57b (1.12) 3.55b (1.17) 3.61b (0.92) ing. These findings are consistent with the hypothesis that the effects
of incidental emotions on reactions to an undesirable service situation
Note. Numbers in parentheses indicate standard deviations. Means with
unlike subscripts are significantly different at P < 0.05. do not simply reflect previously acquired aggression and avoidance
responses that were conditioned to these emotions and elicited spon-
taneously. Rather, the judgments that participants made required con-
7.2.2 Attributions scious cognitive appraisals of the situation and attributions of respon-
Analyses of responsibility attributions also yielded a significant effect sibility for the failure that occurred.
of emotion conditions (F(2, 195) = 3.99; P = 0.020) that was quali-
fied by an interaction of emotion conditions and cognitive load (F(2,
195) = 4.84; P = 0.009, 𝜂p2 = 0.05). Table 3 shows that when cognitive
8 GENERAL DISCUSSION
load was low, participants assigned more blame to the service provider
Consumers’ incidental emotional state at the time they encounter a
under anger conditions (M = 4.51, SD = 1.07) than under control con-
service failure can affect their likelihood of complaining about the
ditions (M = 4.05, SD = 0.81; F(1, 195) = 17.43, P = 0.024, 𝜂p2 = 0.06)
failure and their willingness to patronize the service provider again.
but assigned less blame to the provider in fear conditions (M = 3.64,
Whereas incidental anger increases consumers’ disposition to com-
SD = 0.71) than in control conditions (F(1, 195) = 4.18, P = 0.042,
plain about a service failure, incidental fear leads them to react less
𝜂p2 = 0.07). This effect disappeared in high cognitive load conditions,
negatively to the service failure than they otherwise would. This was
however (4.04, 4.08, and 4.03 in anger, fear, and control conditions,
true both when a service failure was personally experienced, and when
respectively, F < 1).
it was only observed. These effects are mediated by the impact of inci-
dental emotions on cognitive appraisals of the situation.
7.2.3 Intentions to patronize
Note that student samples were used in experiments conducted in
Participants’ willingness to patronize the restaurant is summarized lab settings with better control. Although student samples cannot rep-
in Table 3 as a function of cognitive load and emotion conditions. resent the consumer population as a whole, the preliminary study illus-
The effect of emotion conditions was significant (F(2, 195) = 6.69; trated the effect in a real business setting and Experiment 2A examined
P = 0.002) but was qualified by an interaction of emotion conditions the effect in an online sample that better represents the general popu-
and cognitive load (F(2, 195) = 3.21; P = 0.043, 𝜂p2 = 0.04). When partic- lation.
ipants were not under cognitive load, they had significantly less inten- Although other research provides evidence that incidental emo-
tions to visit the restaurant in anger conditions (M = 3.03, SD = 1.03) tions can influence the appraisal of a situation (So et al., 2015), the
than in control conditions (M = 3.63, SD = 1.10; F(1, 195) = 6.16; present experiments are the first to demonstrate that incidental anger
P = 0.014, 𝜂p2 = 0.08), but had greater intentions to do so in fear con- and fear influence consumers’ reactions to a service failure. Previous
ditions (M = 4.11, SD = 0.89) than in control conditions (M = 3.63, research, however, has assumed that consumers base their decision
SD = 1.10; F(1, 195) = 3.92; P = 0.049, 𝜂p2 = 0.05). In high cognitive to complain about a service failure on features of the service expe-
load conditions, however, participants did not differ in their intentions rience (e.g., Bitner, 1990; Maxham & Netemeyer, 2002; Wan, 2013).
(3.57, 3.61, and 3.65 in anger, fear, and control conditions, respectively, The present research is consistent with this assumption but extends
F < 1). its implications. That is, it indicates that concepts activated by the emo-
Note that in the absence of cognitive load, fear appeared to increase tions that people happen to be experiencing at the time they encounter
the intention to patronize the restaurant relative to control conditions. a service failure can influence their complaint behavior.
This could indicate that in the absence of any specific emotion, par- Incidental emotions have received attention in past research (So
ticipants were inclined to react negatively to the restaurant and that et al., 2015). However, this research has focused on the impact of inci-
inducing fear decreased the magnitude of this negative reaction. dental anger and fear on risk perceptions (Han et al., 2007; Lerner
& Keltner, 2001; Lerner et al., 2003). The present research is the
7.2.4 Mediation first to investigate both the incidental anger and fear on consumer
A moderated mediation analysis (model 8, Hayes, 2013) revealed that and observer reactions to service failures. In addition, because anger
in the low cognitive load conditions, consumer's attribution for the is associated with heuristic processing (Bodenhausen, Sheppard, &
observed service failure mediated the effect of incidental emotion Kramer, 1994), cognitive load might seem unlikely to influence its
8 SU ET AL .

impact on judgments and behavior. In the conditions constructed, how- Bitner, M. J. (1990). Evaluating service encounters: The effects of physical
ever, cognitive load attenuated the impact of both anger and fear on surroundings and employee responses. Journal of Marketing, 54(2), 69–
82. https://doi.org/10.2307/1251871
consumer attributions.
Blodgett, J. G., Hill, D. J., & Tax, S. S. (1997). The effects of dis-
Finally, previous research has typically focused on consumers’ reac-
tributive, procedural, and interactional justice on postcomplaint
tions to a service failure that they personally experience (e.g., Bitner, behavior. Journal of Retailing, 73(2), 185–210. https://doi.org/
1990; Tax, Brown, & Chandrashekaran, 1998; Wan, 2013; Wan et al., 10.1016/S0022-4359(97)90003-8
2011). The evidence that people's emotions affect their interpretation Bodenhausen, G. V., Sheppard, L. A., & Kramer, G. P. (1994). Nega-
of the service failures that others encounter, and that this interpreta- tive affect and social judgment: The differential impact of anger
tion affects their own likelihood of patronizing the service provider, and sadness. European Journal of Social Psychology, 24(1), 45–62.
https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420240104
could have practical implications. That is, service providers may need
Burger, J. M. (1981). Motivational biases in the attribution of
to think of ways to restore not only victims’ confidence in them but
responsibility for an accident: A meta-analysis of the defensive-
also observers’ confidence as well. That is, offering compensation or
attribution hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 90(3), 496–512.
an apology to the victims may not always be sufficient to restore confi- https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.90.3.496
dence in persons who have observed or otherwise become aware of the Causon, J. (2015). Customer complaints made via social media on the rise.
service failure, particularly if the failure is communicated in the social The Guardian, Retrieved from https://www.theguardian.com/media-
media (Facebook, Twitter, etc.). network/2015/may/21/customer-complaints-social-media-rise/

Prior research (Keltner, Gruenfeld, & Anderson, 2003) indicated Chan, H., & Wan, L. C. (2008). Consumer responses to service failures: A
resource preference model of cultural influences. Journal of International
that different power levels were associated with the incidental emo-
Marketing, 16(1), 72–97. https://doi.org/10.1509/jimk.16.1.72
tions of anger and fear. That is, people with high power tend to become
Coleman, N. V., Williams, P., Morales, A. C., & White, A. E. (2017). Atten-
angry easily, whereas people with low power more easily become fear-
tion, attitudes, and action: When and why incidental fear increases
ful. It is conceivable that people with high power are relatively more consumer choice. Journal of Consumer Research, 44(2), 283–312.
likely to blame the service provider in service failures that they experi- https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx036
ence or observe. Several factors (e.g., social status, company–customer Drolet, A., & Luce, M. F. (2004). The rationalizing effects of cognitive load on
relationships) can lead to different levels of consumer power. To this emotion-based trade-off avoidance. Journal of Consumer Research, 31(1),
63–77. https://doi.org/10.1086/383424
extent, the proposed investigations suggest service failure recovery
strategies that managers might use to deal with different types of con- Dunn, L., & Hoegg, J. (2014). The impact of fear on emotional
brand attachment. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(1), 152–168.
sumers.
https://doi.org/10.1086/675377
Furthermore, many studies have found that both music and color
Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Expo-
can arouse different emotions (e.g., Schulkind, Hennis, & Rubin, 1999; sure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20–35.
Valdez & Mehrabian, 1994). Based on similar logic, it is possible that https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20
environmental settings such as music and color would lead to con- Gilbert, D. T. (1989). Thinking lightly about others: Automatic components
sumer emotions arousal and this would potentially influence their per- of the social inference process. In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unin-
ception toward a service failure. This would be an interesting direction tended thought (pp. 189–211). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

for future research. Goetzman, G., Bozman, R. M., Saxon, E., Utt, K., Blake, G. (Producers), &
Demme, J.. (Director) (1991). The silence of the lambs [Motion picture],
United States: Orion Pictures.
REFERENCES Han, S., Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2007). Feelings and consumer
Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2000). The cognitive impact of decision making: The appraisal-tendency framework. Journal of
past behavior: Influences on beliefs, attitudes, and future behav- Consumer Psychology, 17(3), 158–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/
ioral decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(1)5–22. S1057-7408(07)70023-2
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.5 Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional pro-
Averill, J. R. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications cess analysis: A regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 1145–1160. Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H., & Anderson, C. (2003). Power,
https://doi.org/10.1037/0003- 066X.38.11.1145 approach, and inhibition. Psychological Review, 110(2), 265–284.
Beijing Consumer Association (2011). E-Netcom Beijing Technol- https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.2.265
ogy and Business Service Platform [Data file]. Retrieved from Kron, A., Schul, Y., Cohen, A., & Hassin, R. R. (2010). Feelings
https://www.baic.gov.cn/xxgk/fenxi/index.htm don't come easy: Studies on the effortful nature of feelings.
Beijing Consumer Association (2012). E-Netcom Beijing Technol- Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(3), 520–534.
ogy and Business Service Platform [Data file]. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020008
https://www.baic.gov.cn/xxgk/fenxi/index.htm Lee, C. J., & Andrade, E. B. (2011). Fear, social projection, and finan-
Berkowitz, L. (1990). On the formation and regulation of anger and aggres- cial decision making. Journal of Marketing Research, 48, S121–S129.
sion: A cognitive-neoassociationistic analysis. American Psychologist, https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.48.SPL.S121
45(4), 494–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.45.4.494 Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003).
Betancourt, H., & Blair, I. (1992). A cognition (attribution)–emotion model Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A
of violence in conflict situations. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(2), 144–150.
letin, 18(3), 343–350. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167292183011 https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01433
SU ET AL . 9

Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in
Personality and Social Psychology, 81(1), 146–159. https://doi.org/ emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813–838.
10.1037/0022-3514.81.1.146 https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.4.813
Maxham, J. G. (1999). Service recovery's influence on consumer So, J., Achar, C., Han, D., Agrawal, N., Duhachek, A., & Maheswaran,
satisfaction, positive word-of- mouth, and purchase inten- D. (2015). The psychology of appraisal: Specific emotions and
tions. Journal of Business Research, 54(1), 11–24. https://doi.org/ decision-making. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(3), 359–371.
10.1016/S0148-2963(00)00114-4 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2015.04.003
Maxham, J. G., & Netemeyer, R. G. (2002). Modeling customer percep- Tax, S. S., Brown, S. W., & Chandrashekaran, M. (1998). Customer
tions of complaint handling over time: The effects of perceived jus- evaluations of service complaint experiences: Implications
tice on satisfaction and intent. Journal of Retailing, 78(4), 239–252. for relationship marketing. Journal of Marketing, 62(2), 60–76.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4359(02)00100-8 https://doi.org/10.2307/1252161
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emo- Tiedens, L. Z., & Linton, S. (2001). Judgment under emotional certainty
tions. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. and uncertainty: The effects of specific emotions on information pro-
Quigley, B. M., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1996). Mediating effects of blame attri- cessing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(6), 973–988.
butions on feelings of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.6.973
22(12), 1280–1288. https://doi.org/10.1177/01461672962212008 Valdez, P., & Mehrabian, A. (1994). Effects of color on emotions.
Richins, M. L. (1997). Measuring emotions in the consumption experi- Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(4), 394–409.
ence. Journal of Consumer Research, 24(2), 127–146. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.123.4.394
10.1086/209499 Wan, L. C. (2013). Culture's impact on consumer complaining responses to
Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. embarrassing service failure. Journal of Business Research, 66(3), 298–
Cognition & Emotion, 5(3), 161–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/ 305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.08.009
02699939108411034 Wan, L. C., Chan, E. K. Y., & Su, L. (2011). When will customers care
Rottenberg, J., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Emotion elicitation using about service failures that happened to strangers? The role of personal
films. In J. A. Coan & J. J. B. Allen (Eds.), The handbook of emotion elicita- similarity and regulatory focus and its implication on service evalua-
tion and assessment (pp. 9–28). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. tion. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 30(1), 213–220.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhm.2010.07.004
Schulkind, M. D., Hennis, L. K., & Rubin, D. C. (1999). Music, emotion, and
autobiographical memory: They're playing your song. Memory & Cogni- Wan, L. C., Hui, M. K., & Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2011). The role of relationship
tion, 27(6), 948–955. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201225 norms in responses to service failures. Journal of Consumer Research,
38(2), 260–277. https://doi.org/10.1086/659039
Shaver, K. G. (1970). Defensive attribution: Effects of severity
and relevance on the responsibility assigned for an accident. Weiner, B. (1980). A cognitive (attribution)-emotion-action model
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 14(2), 101–113. of motivated behavior: An analysis of judgments of help-giving.
https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028777 Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(2), 186–200.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.39.2.186
Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of
affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Wyer, R. S., Jr. (2004). The cognitive organization and use of general knowl-
Research, 26(3), 278–292. https://doi.org/10.1086/209563 edge. In J. T. Jost, M. R. Banaji, & D. A. Prentice (Eds.), Perspectivism
in social psychology: The yin and yang of scientific progress (pp. 97–112).
Simon, M., Devlin, D., Goldfarb, P. (Producers), & Bill, T. (Director) (1980). My Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
bodyguard [Motion picture]. United States: 20th Century Fox.
Wyer, R. S. Jr., Clore, G. L., & Isbell, L. M. (1999). Affect and informa-
Smith, A. K., & Bolton, R. N. (1998). An experimental investigation tion processing. Advances in experimental social psychology, 31, 1–77.
of customer reactions to service failure and recovery encoun- https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08x)60271-3
ters: Paradox or peril? Journal of Service Research, 1(1), 65–81.
https://doi.org/10.1177/109467059800100106
Smith, A. K., & Bolton, R. N. (2002). The effect of customers’ emotional How to cite this article: Su L, Wan LC Wyer RS. The
responses to service failures on their recovery effort evaluations and contrasting influences of incidental anger and fear on
satisfaction judgments. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science,
responses to a service failure. Psychol Mark. 2018;1–10.
30(1), 5–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/03079450094298
https://doi.org/10.1002/mar.21114
10 SU ET AL .

APPENDIX 1
Phrasing of the Questions and Scales Used for the Reported Findings

Experiment 1

Manipulation Check for Incidental Emotion Priming

• How much do you feel angry now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)
• How much do you feel fearful now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)

Complaint Intention

• You have strong intention to complain to the restaurant. (1 = strongly disagree/definitely not/very unlikely; 7 = strongly agree/definitely will/very likely)

Attributions

• The restaurant is responsible for the technical problem of the system. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• You blame the restaurant for the technical problem of the system. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• The technical problem of the online system is due to uncontrollable factors. (R) (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)

Experiments 2A and 2B

Manipulation Check for Incidental Emotion Priming

• How much do you feel angry now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)
• How much do you feel fearful now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)

Attributions

• The travel agency (shop assistant) is responsible for the problem that the customer encountered. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• You blame the travel agency (shop assistant) for the problem that the customer encountered. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• The problem that the customer encountered is due to some uncontrollable factors. (R) (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)

Experiment 3

Manipulation Check for Incidental Emotion Priming

• How much do you feel angry now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)
• How much do you feel fearful now? (1 = not at all, 7 = very strong)

Intentions to Patronize

• You will patronize the restaurant again in the future. (1 = strongly disagree/definitely not/very unlikely; 7 = strongly agree/definitely will/very likely)

Attributions

• The restaurant is responsible for the incident you observed. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• You blame the restaurant for the incident you observed. (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• The incident was beyond the restaurant's control. (R) (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)
• The incident was due to uncontrollable factors. (R) (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree)

View publication stats

You might also like