You are on page 1of 20

J Autism Dev Disord

DOI 10.1007/s10803-015-2623-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

Is Social Categorization the Missing Link Between Weak Central


Coherence and Mental State Inference Abilities in Autism?
Preliminary Evidence from a General Population Sample
Daniel P. Skorich1 • Adrienne R. May1 • Louisa A. Talipski1 • Marnie H. Hall1 •

Anita J. Dolstra1 • Tahlia B. Gash1 • Beth H. Gunningham1

 Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Abstract We explore the relationship between the ‘the- ests that are present from an early developmental period,
ory of mind’ (ToM) and ‘central coherence’ difficulties of and that cause significant impairment in general function-
autism. We introduce covariation between hierarchically- ing (APA 2013). Outside these diagnostic features, people
embedded categories and social information—at the local with ASD also exhibit other cognitive-perceptual abnor-
level, the global level, or at both levels simultaneously— malities, including increased speed and accuracy in block
within a category confusion task. We then ask participants design (Shah and Frith 1993), increased speed on the
to infer the mental state of novel category members, and Embedded Figures Test (EFT; Morgan et al. 2003), and
measure participants’ autism-spectrum quotient (AQ). unusual homonym pronunciation (Happé 1997). The social
Results reveal a positive relationship between AQ and the and communication deficits of ASD have traditionally been
degree of local/global social categorization, which in turn explained as the outcome of dysfunction in the ‘theory of
predicts the pattern of mental state inferences. These mind’ system (Baron-Cohen 1994), while the non-diag-
results provide preliminary evidence for a causal relation- nostic features have often been attributed to a general
ship between central coherence and ToM abilities. Impli- tendency towards ‘weak central coherence’ (Frith and
cations with regard to ToM processes, social Happé 1994). In the current paper, we explore the rela-
categorization, intervention, and the development of a tionship between the theory of mind and weak central
unified account of autism are discussed. coherence features of ASD, and provide preliminary evi-
dence that the theory of mind difficulties are an outcome of
Keywords Autism  Weak central coherence  Theory of weak central coherence with regard to the categorization of
mind  Categorization  Social categorization  Self- social information.
categorization

‘Theory of Mind’ Difficulties in ASD


Introduction
The social and communication impairments of ASD
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a pervasive develop- include specific deficits in social-emotional reciprocity,
mental disorder, diagnosed on the basis of social and non-verbal communication, the spontaneous sharing of
communication deficits and repetitive behaviors or inter- interests or affect, and the initiation, maintenance and
understanding of relationships. These features—which are
thought to fall on a continuum and to define a broader
phenotype rather than a categorically distinct disorder (e.g.,
Sucksmith et al. 2011)—have traditionally been understood
& Daniel P. Skorich as outcomes of a deficit in the theory of mind system
Daniel.Skorich@anu.edu.au
(Baron-Cohen et al. 1985). People with ASD have been
1
Research School of Psychology, The Australian National shown to perform more poorly relative to typically devel-
University, Canberra, ACT 0200, Australia oping (TD) people and other clinical groups on a whole

123
J Autism Dev Disord

range of theory of mind tasks, including: understanding the ‘Weak Central Coherence’ in ASD
relationship between seeing and knowing (Baron-Cohen
and Goodhart 1994); recognizing that others can have false Alongside these theory of mind difficulties, a whole range
beliefs about the physical world (Baron-Cohen et al. 1985; of cognitive and perceptual differences have also been
Happé 1995) and about the beliefs of others (Tager-Flus- observed in ASD. Relative to controls, people with ASD
berg and Sullivan 1994); deducing complex emotional have been shown to exhibit: increased speed and accuracy
states from facial expressions (Baron-Cohen et al. 1997); in block design (Shah and Frith 1993); increased speed on
and recognizing the mental states of people in uncomfort- radial frequency search tasks (Almeida et al. 2010) and on
able social situations (Heavey et al. 2000). More recently, the EFT (Jolliffe et al. 1997; Shah and Frith 1983);
it has been shown that these theory of mind difficulties decreased susceptibility to visual illusions (Happé 1996);
arise particularly in spontaneous or implicit mentalizing decreased ability to detect unusual elements of visual
tasks, but less so in explicit tasks where compensatory scenes (Jolliffe and Baron-Cohen 2001); decreased accu-
mechanisms can be invoked (Schuwerk et al. 2015; Senju racy in homonym pronunciation (Happé 1997); and a
et al. 2009; Sodian et al. 2015). In all cases, however, the preference for processing the local letters in Navon fig-
ability of people with ASD to represent the minds of others ures (Plaisted et al. 1999). These empirical results show
has been shown to be impaired, or slower to develop, rel- that people with ASD have unusual cognitive-perceptual
ative to TD individuals. strengths in certain areas, as well as unusual cognitive-
Baron-Cohen (1994) has argued that the fundamental perceptual weaknesses in others.
deficit leading to these outcomes is one of shared atten- This spiky pattern of strengths and weaknesses (Brosnan
tion. In his Empathizing System model (Baron-Cohen et al. 2004) has traditionally been understood as a tendency
2005), he argues that the theory of mind system can be towards weak central coherence. Frith (1989) suggested that
separated into six cognitive components, each of which information processing in TD individuals is characterized by
matures during specific developmental periods. The a tendency to integrate stimulus information into a coherent
Intentionality Detector (ID), the Emotion Detector (TED) whole. Frith and Happé (1994) further suggested that this
and the Eye Direction Detector (EDD) develop early on, general tendency for constructing ‘‘higher-level meaning in
and allow for the development of the Shared Attention context’’—or what these researchers referred to as ‘‘central
Mechanism (SAM). Baron-Cohen argues that ID, TED coherence’’—was ‘‘weak’’, in people with ASD; a fact, they
and EDD are all intact in ASD, but that SAM is affected, argued, that could explain the whole range of non-diagnostic
leading to delayed development of, or dysfunction in, the features exhibited by people with ASD. The increased speed
final two components of the system: the Empathizing in block design and the EFT, failures in detecting unusual
System (TESS) and the Theory of Mind Mechanism elements of a visual scene, and poorer global processing in
(TOMM). SAM is a system that allows for triadic ‘self- Navon tasks, for example, can all be explained as a decreased
other-object’ representations, where a perceiver comes to tendency to construct a global whole. There is some con-
understand that his or her attention is directed toward the tention regarding the exact nature of the processing differ-
same object as another, or directed elsewhere relative to ence—with evidence emerging that weak central coherence
the other’s focus of attention. Baron-Cohen argues, with might be better understood as a tendency toward enhanced
the support of a variety of empirical findings (Adamson perceptual functioning, rather than decreased information
et al. 2010; Cornew et al. 2012; Osterling and Dawson integration (Mottron et al. 2006)—but there is robust evi-
1994; Osterling et al. 2002), that it is precisely these ‘self- dence for some form of locally-oriented processing in ASD,
other-object’ representations, and the resulting ability to and for some variant of weak central coherence (Happé and
share attention with others, with which people with ASD Booth 2008; Happé and Frith 2006).
have trouble. As a result of delayed or impaired triadic
representations, empathy—the primary function of
TESS—and the ability to represent the full range of The Relationship Between ‘Theory of Mind’
complex mental states—the primary function of ToMM— and ‘Weak Central Coherence’
are also delayed or impaired in ASD. A specific deficit in
SAM, and a general deficit in the theory of mind system— Since the advent of weak central coherence theory, the
particularly at an implicit level (Schuwerk et al. 2015)—is relationship between weak central coherence and the theory
therefore a plausible, and widely accepted explanation for of mind deficits has been explored, first theoretically, and
the social and communication difficulties observed in then empirically in a handful of studies. In Frith’s (1989)
ASD (Yirmiya et al. 1998). original conception, weak central coherence was the cogni-

123
J Autism Dev Disord

tive dysfunction at the root of all the features of ASD, measures of theory of mind and weak central coherence
including impaired theory of mind. She suggested that the performance, rather than exploring the cognitive mecha-
human cognitive system is composed of a general informa- nisms that might unite these features. Without a falsifiable
tion processing mechanism, the primary function of which is cognitive account of the dissociation between weak central
the extraction of higher-level meaning from stimulus arrays. coherence and theory of mind processes, it is premature to
She suggested that theory of mind abilities also rely on this derive any strong conclusions about their relationship. As
ability to extract higher-level meaning in context, such that a such, we argue that it is necessary to evaluate the cognitive
dysfunction in the general information processing system process accounts of the theory of mind system in the social
(i.e., weak central coherence) would by necessity result in a cognitive literature, in order to identify any unifying
more specific dysfunction in mentalizing abilities. mechanisms that might exist.
Since this early theorizing, Frith and others (Brunsdon Similar to the literature on theory of mind abilities in
and Happé 2014; Frith and Happé 1994; Happé and Ronald ASD, the general social cognitive literature has explored
2008) have abandoned the notion that weak central each of the dissociable components involved in mentaliz-
coherence might account for the theory of mind features of ing, including shared attention, empathy, and mental state
ASD, going so far as to suggest that it might be ‘‘time to inference. With regard to shared attention, Shteynberg
give up on a single explanation for autism’’ (Happé et al. (2010) has shown that an individual’s attention will be
2006). These researchers all argue that ASD is better oriented towards those stimuli experienced by similar tar-
understood as ‘‘fractionable’’, such that the each of the get others, but not towards those stimuli experienced by
features of the disorder represent separate, but sometimes dissimilar target others (see also Shteynberg 2015). Simi-
co-occurring, outcomes of a modular cognitive system. In larly, Tarrant et al. (2009) have found that empathic
making this argument, Brunsdon and Happé (2014) point to reactions to target others are increased if those target others
absent (e.g., Burnette et al. 2005), qualified (e.g., Pelicano are similar to the self, or members of the same social
et al. 2006), and low to moderate (Jarrold et al. 2000) category as the self (see also Thomas et al. 2009; Cialdini
correlations between weak central coherence and theory of et al. 1997). Of most interest to the current research,
mind features. Jarrold et al. (2000) report the only set of however, Ames (2004) has shown that human beings will
experiments showing a consistent and robust pattern of project their own thoughts onto similar target others and
correlations across measures of theory of mind and weak will use stored stereotypes about common social groups to
central coherence. In their first experiment, they showed a understand the thoughts of dissimilar target others.
relationship between performance on Baron-Cohen et al.’s In Ames’ first experiment, participants were presented
(1997) ‘‘reading the eyes in the mind’’ test and perfor- with a number of vignettes in which the central character
mance on the EFT, in a general population sample. In their was confronted with a simple scenario. This character was
second and third experiments, they showed a similar rela- described in terms of a number of personal characteristics,
tionship—though qualified by verbal mental age—between as well as in terms of his or her membership in one of a
a combined theory of mind score, and block design and number of common social groups (e.g., medical student,
EFT performance, in both a general population sample lawyer etc.). Participants were then asked to rate how
(Expt. 2) and in a group diagnosed with ASD (Expt. 3). similar they felt to the character, and how likely it was that
These results suggest that a relationship between weak each of the characters, they themselves, and a typical
central coherence and theory of mind remains conceivable, member of the relevant social group was thinking a variety
but, at present, the literature, such as it is, must be inter- of thoughts. Results revealed that participants’ own
preted as favoring a ‘‘fractionable’’ account of the features thoughts were predictive of the thoughts they attributed to
of the disorder, as argued by Happé and her colleagues. the character to the extent that they felt similar to that
character. When participants felt dissimilar to the charac-
ter, the thoughts they attributed to a typical member of the
Social Cognitive Accounts of the ‘Theory of Mind’ social group to which the character belonged was predic-
System tive of the thoughts they attributed to the character. In a
second and third experiment, Ames (2004) replicated these
Despite this evidence in favor of separable deficits, how- results with direct manipulations of the similarity between
ever, we argue that there remain sound reasons to suggest the participants and the characters. Ames interpreted the
that weak central coherence and theory of mind processes results of these three studies as demonstrating that mental
might be related. With the notable exception of Frith’s state inferences are derived from perceptions of similarity
(1989) early theorizing, the research thus far conducted has between the self and a target other, where the thoughts of
focused entirely on uncovering correlations between similar others will be projected from one’s own thoughts,

123
J Autism Dev Disord

and the thoughts of dissimilar others will be inferred from Categorization in ASD
their group memberships (see also Ames et al. 2012).
These examples are by no means exhaustive with For social categorization to be a plausible link between
regard to the general social cognitive literature on theory weak central coherence and theory of mind abilities in
of mind abilities, but they do represent important and ASD, it would be desirable to have some evidence for its
prototypical findings within that literature. In each case, relationship with weak central coherence, and for its dis-
perceptions of self-other similarity, and the related process ruption in people diagnosed with the disorder. Unfortu-
of social categorization, emerge as fundamental precursors nately, with the exception of some indirect evidence from
to theory of mind abilities. Social categorization is defined the face processing domain (e.g., Hobson 1983, 1987;
here as the process of using socially-relevant cognitive Strauss et al. 2012; Teunisse and de Gelder 2001), and
organizing principles (e.g., age, gender, ethnicity, and from research on the self-concept (e.g., Hobson 1990;
many more subtle stimulus groupings) to make sense of Hobson and Meyer 2005; Lee and Hobson 1998; Meyer
social stimulus arrays (McGarty et al. 2015; Skorich and and Hobson 2004), direct empirical evidence for the latter
Mavor 2013). In each of the above cases, the perceiver is point does not currently exist. Indirectly, however, theo-
engaged in a process of calculating similarities and dif- retical and empirical research on the more general process
ferences between him or herself and others in a psycho- of categorization provides credence to both points above.
logical frame of reference, and thereby deriving an First, at a theoretical level, the concept of central coher-
appropriate social categorical representation of the stim- ence, as defined by Frith and Happé (1994), is almost
ulus frame on which to base his or her inferences and identical to working definitions of categorization as used
empathy judgments. By categorizing the social world in by researchers in social cognition. Frith and Happé (1994)
terms of known representations stored in memory, the describes central coherence as ‘‘the tendency to draw
perceiver is able to go beyond the information available in together diverse information to construct higher-level
the situation, and make further predictions about the meaning in context’’ (pp. 121), while Oakes et al. (1994)
thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of category members. If suggest that categorization has a ‘‘meaning-giving func-
a target other is categorized as similar to the self, then tion’’ (pp. 124) that it fulfils by ‘‘selectively drawing out
mental states are projected from self to other (Ames aspects of structure […] in stimulus information […]
2004), attention is shared with the other (Shteynberg within the stimulus context as a whole.’’ (pp. 113). In both
2010), and empathy is felt for the other (Tarrant et al. cases, the process is described as being concerned princi-
2009). If a target other is categorized as dissimilar to the pally with the construction or creation of meaning, with the
self, then mental states will be based on other non-self- drawing together or structuring of information, and with
categories (Ames 2004), and attention will not be shared understanding stimuli within a specific context. Catego-
with (Shteynberg 2010), and empathy will not be felt for rization—like central coherence—is, therefore, a process
(Tarrant et al. 2009), the other. of organizing or grouping stimulus information in terms of
While these results clearly go further into the cognitive some higher-level structure, in a context-dependent fash-
processing domain than much of the ASD literature on the ion, and is thus plausibly related to central coherence.
topic, it is important to note that these findings are also Second, at an empirical level, a number of researchers
entirely consistent with understandings of theory of mind (e.g., Gastgeb et al. 2012; Klinger and Dawson 2001) have
abilities in that literature, and indeed with Baron-Cohen’s shown that individuals with ASD display impairments in
(2005) model of the mind-reading system. Baron-Cohen forming prototypes during category learning; a general
does not propose a clear cognitive mechanism through categorization impairment that these researchers attribute
which SAM achieves its purpose, but his conception of to weak central coherence. Klinger and Dawson (2001)
SAM as representing ‘self-other-object’ relations points to presented ASD, TD and Down’s syndrome participants
the same social categorical processing mechanism under- with a category learning task, in which participants were
lying shared attention as the social cognitive literature in asked to make sense of a number of categories of pretend
general, where self and other must be seen as categorically animals based on a series of exemplars. In the prototype
similar for shared attention to arise. Fundamentally, then, condition, the animal categories were defined by a proto-
each of the key dissociable components of the theory of type, such that no single rule (and no single feature) was
mind system—and particularly SAM—relies on a social defining of category membership. In the rule-based cate-
categorization mechanism for its proper functioning, and, gorization conditions, the animal categories were defined
as such, it is reasonable to further propose that dysfunction by a single rule, such that possessing a single feature of a
in the social categorization mechanism could lead directly given size was defining of category membership. Results
to disruption in theory of mind abilities. showed that ASD children were unable to extract the

123
J Autism Dev Disord

prototype from the category learning task, and were only coherence) to be explored. Similarly, the mental state
able to successfully complete the task when a categoriza- inference task, adapted from Ames’ (2004) dissimilar-other
tion rule was available. Providing similar evidence, Gast- conditions, was developed to explore the extent to which
geb et al. (2012) presented high-functioning ASD and local versus global social categories would be used to make
matched TD individuals with a series of dot patterns rep- mental state inferences, and how these local/global ten-
resenting exemplars of a category. Following familiariza- dencies were related to the degree of autistic-like traits.
tion with the dot-pattern category, participants were asked The AQ was employed as a measure of participants’
to classify a series of stimuli as members of the previously autistic-like traits, and as a proxy operationalization of
learnt category or as non-category members. Results ASD in the current experiment’s general population sam-
revealed that ASD individuals accurately endorsed fewer ple, such that the current experiment was capturing the
category members than TD individuals, suggesting that broader autism phenotype (Sucksmith et al. 2011), rather
individuals with ASD had been less able to learn the cat- than ASD per se.
egory prototype than TD individuals (see also Brosnan In the first phase of the experiment, the category con-
et al. 2004; Gastgeb et al. 2006, 2009; Plaisted et al. 1998). fusion paradigm was employed to present participants with
The results of Klinger and Dawson (2001) and Gastgeb social information, which took the form of written state-
et al. (2012) together imply that individuals with ASD have ments of ostensibly performed behaviors. Each of these
an impairment in their categorization abilities, character- statements was attributed to a particular individual who
ized by a specific deficit in prototype formation. These was said to be a member of two social categories. Once the
findings are consistent with the notion from weak central participant was presented with all of these behavioral
coherence theory (Frith and Happé 1994) that individuals statements one-by-one, he or she was then presented with
with ASD have a tendency to integrate stimulus informa- the same statements again, but with the identity and social
tion less thoroughly than TD individuals. Given that social category information removed. The participant’s task was
categorization is simply a special case of the more general then to attribute each of the behavioral statements to the
process of categorization (McGarty et al. 2015), it is rea- individual who was initially said to have performed the
sonable to assume that the weak central coherence that behavior. Participants’ patterns of errors—either within-
manifests itself in impairments in general categorization category or between-category—could then be used as an
abilities among individuals with ASD could also result in unobtrusive measure of the extent to which the participant
impairments in social categorization. These social catego- was using the social categories to organize the social
rization impairments, were they shown to be present in information.
individuals with ASD, could in turn result in impaired In the current experiment, unlike a typical category
theory of mind, for reasons discussed above. The link confusion paradigm, the social categories were hierarchi-
between weak central coherence and theory of mind defi- cally embedded, such that participants had both global and
cits, via the cognitive mechanism of social categorization, local categories available for use in organizing the social
would then be plausibly, though preliminarily, established. information. This global/local category structure mirrors
that of a Navon figure, where individuals with ASD have
been shown to have a tendency to process at the local level
The Current Experiment (Plaisted et al. 1999). In order to give participants an
opportunity to form one or more category prototypes at
The current experiment is designed to test the proposition either or both of the global and local levels, the principles
that the theory of mind difficulties associated with ASD are of self-categorization theory (Turner et al. 1987; Turner
an outcome of weak central coherence via a social cate- et al. 1994) were employed. Self-categorization theory is a
gorization mechanism. In order to test this proposition, we widely-used model of social category use, which argues
employed a widely-used social categorization paradigm, that social categorization is an outcome of an interaction of
Taylor et al. (1978) category confusion paradigm, followed perceiver factors—termed ‘‘perceiver readiness’’—and
by a mental state inference task, and Baron-Cohen et al. situational factors—termed ‘‘fit’’, of which there is a
(2001) Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ). The category ‘‘comparative’’ aspect and a ‘‘normative’’ aspect (Blanz
confusion paradigm allows for participants’ patterns of 1999). For the current experiment, comparative fit was
social categorization to be uncovered. In the current particularly invoked, as it relates most directly to the pro-
experiment, the paradigm was adapted in such a way that cess of prototype formation. Comparative fit refers to the
social category prototypes could be made available at both degree to which stimuli that co-vary with an organizing
the global and local levels—mirroring a Navon figure— principle (such as a category label) are on average more
thus allowing the relationship between AQ and local/global similar to one another than those stimuli are to all the
social categorization tendencies (and weak central remaining stimuli in the frame of reference. To have high

123
J Autism Dev Disord

comparative fit is to have a high degree of covariation traits, where a high AQ would indicate more autistic-like
between the organizing principle and the stimulus features, traits.
while to have low comparative fit is to have minimal Based on the proposal that the theory of mind difficulties
covariation. In the current experiment, each of the social of ASD are an outcome of weak central coherence with
categories was made to co-vary with a personality trait regard to the categorization of social information, we
dimension, such that there was a high degree of compar- propose four hypotheses. For each hypothesis, we specifi-
ative fit for the social categories, which gave participants cally highlight the pattern in the Local ? Global Fit con-
the opportunity to form category prototypes. A hierarchical dition, because it is in this condition that social category
model of personality (DeYoung et al. 2007) was used in prototypes are available at both the local and global levels,
order to allow for comparative fit at both the global and and it is, therefore, in this condition that the effects of
local levels simultaneously (Local ? Global Fit condition), participants’ AQ scores should be detectable. First, we
the global level only (Global Fit condition), the local level hypothesize that AQ will interact with fit condition in
only (Local Fit condition), or at neither level (No Fit determining the extent to which participants organize the
condition). In this way, participants were given the social information at the local level, the global level, both
opportunity to extract prototypes at either the local level, levels, or neither. More specifically, in the Local ? Global
the global level, both levels at once, or neither level; again, Fit condition, we hypothesize that AQ will be related with
in order to mirror the global/local processing opportunities a tendency to use the local categories to organize the social
available in Navon figures, where weak central coherence information, because individuals with ASD (and, relatedly,
in ASD has been particularly evident. individuals with a high AQ) have been shown to favor the
Following the category confusion task, participants were local level in equivalent Navon tasks. In the remaining
asked to complete a mental state inference task based on three fit conditions, no specific relationship is predicted
Ames’ (2004) dissimilar-other conditions. Participants between AQ and participants’ tendencies to use local or
were presented with a number of new individuals, each of global categories to organize the social information.
whom was said to be a member of one of the local cate- Second, we hypothesize that AQ will interact with
gories and one of the global categories from the previous comparative fit condition in determining the extent to
category confusion task. Each of these individuals was said which participants make their mental state inferences at the
to be confronted with a simple scenario. Participants were local level, the global level, both levels, or neither. More
then asked to rate the extent to which they believed these specifically, in the Local ? Global Fit condition, we
new individuals were likely to be thinking a variety of hypothesize that AQ will be related with a tendency to use
thoughts, each of which mapped onto the personality trait the local categories to make mental state inferences,
dimensions used in the category confusion task. As such, because individuals with ASD have been shown to favor
participants were being asked to use the category proto- the local level in equivalent Navon tasks. In the remaining
types at either the local level or the global level—should three fit conditions, no specific relationship is predicted
they have formed one at either of these levels during the between AQ and participants’ tendencies to use local or
previous category confusion task—to make their mental global categories to make mental state inferences.
state inferences.1 In the final phase of the experiment, Third, we hypothesize that there will be a positive
participants—drawn from the general population—were relationship between the relative (local/global) level at
asked to complete the AQ as a measure of their autistic-like which participants organize the social information and the
relative (local/global) level at which participants make
their mental state inferences. We hypothesize further that
1
In the current experiment, we have operationalized mental state this relationship will be particularly evident in the
inferences as falling somewhere on a continuum from local to global. Local ? Global Fit condition, because it is in this condi-
We do not believe, however, that mental state inferences so simply
tion that differences in social categorization and mental
and straightforwardly map onto this local–global continuum in all
situations. Rather, if mental state inferences rely crucially on social state inference should be most evident.
categorization as we propose, then all those factors that affect which Finally, we hypothesize that the relationship between
particular social categories become salient (or activated) in a given AQ and the relative level at which participants make their
context should equally impact on the activation of particular mental
mental state inferences that we predict in the Local ?
state inferences in context (including, for example, the accessibility of
particular categories in memory relative to others, the degree of Global Fit condition will be eliminated when the relative
covariation between stimulus inputs and categories stored in memory, level at which participants organize the social information
the perceiver’s goals in the current situation etc.). We chose to is taken into account.
operationalize social categorization—and by extension mental state
Should all four hypotheses be supported, it would be
inferences—in the way we have in the current experiment simply
because it most closely mirrors common operationalizations of weak reasonable to conclude that the relationship between AQ
central coherence in the ASD literature. and the level at which participants made their mental state

123
J Autism Dev Disord

inferences was mediated by the level at which participants were embedded within group Plibb, thus creating a hier-
organized the social information. Were this mediation archical category structure reminiscent of a Navon figure,
relationship to emerge, it would provide preliminary evi- with the groups at the more inclusive global level, and the
dence that the theory of mind dysfunction in ASD is an situations at the less inclusive local level. Three of the
outcome of weak central coherence with regard to the individual female names were associated with each of the
categorization of social information. situations at the local level, such that six of the individual
female names were members of each of the groups (see
Table 1 for the 12 possible person-group-situation strings).
Method In order to manipulate comparative fit at both the local
and global levels, the structure of DeYoung et al.’s (2007)
Participants hierarchical version of the Big Five personality inventory
was used. This instrument divides each of the big five
One hundred and three participants were recruited from a factors of Openness to Experience, Conscientiousness,
pool of undergraduate psychology students, and via Extraversion, Agreeableness and Neuroticism into a further
snowball sampling through various social media outlets two aspects, thus creating an embedded hierarchical
(e.g., Facebook). Sixty-two participants were female, and structure with the five factors at the more inclusive global
forty-one were male. The mean age was 24.64 years with a level, and 10 aspects at the less inclusive local level. For
range from 18 to 65. the purposes of the experiment, the factors of Agreeable-
ness and Conscientiousness were used to introduce the
Design desired covariation. Within DeYoung et al.’s (2007) model,
these factors are further divided into the aspects of
The experiment employed a four condition (fit: local ? politeness and compassion, and orderliness and industri-
global/global/local/no fit) between-subjects design, with ousness, respectively. Ten behavioral statements mapping
participants’ AQ included as a second continuous predic- onto each of these four aspects were created, such that
tor. The dependent variables were local–global catego- there were 20 behavioral statements mapping onto each of
rization on the category confusion attribution task, and the Agreeableness and Conscientiousness factors. The
local–global mental state inference for the mental state remaining eight behavioral statements were created from
inference task. the other six aspects of the DeYoung et al. (2007) model,
and were deemed filler statements, not made to systemat-
Procedure ically co-vary at either the local or global levels in any of
the fit conditions (see Table 2 for example statements).
The entire experiment was conducted online, on partici- In order to create the Local ? Global fit condition, eight
pants’ own time, on a computer of their own choosing. of the 12 statements associated with situation Yof were
Qualtrics survey software was used to present all stimuli politeness statements, eight of the 12 statements associated
and to collect all data. with situation Nuk were compassion statements, eight of

Phase 1: Category Confusion Task


Table 1 The 12 person-group-situation strings used in the category
confusion task
The first phase of the experiment was an adapted version of
Name Group Situation
Taylor et al.’s (1978) category confusion task. Participants
were presented with 48 behavioral statements of the form Louise A member of group Smedd In situation Yof
‘‘[Female Name], a member of group [five-letter non- Cathy A member of group Smedd In situation Yof
word], in situation [three-letter non-word], [behavior]’’. Anna A member of group Smedd In situation Yof
The 12 female names used were chosen from a list of the Holly A member of group Smedd In situation Nuk
25 most common English language female names. The Kristen A member of group Smedd In situation Nuk
five-letter non-words used to designate the groups were Nicole A member of group Smedd In situation Nuk
Smedd and Plibb. The three-letter non-words used to des- Emma A member of group Plibb In situation Teg
ignate the situations were Yof, Nuk, Teg, and Faz. Non- Laura A member of group Plibb In situation Teg
words were used to designate both the groups and the sit- Sophie A member of group Plibb In situation Teg
uations in order to remove the possibility that participants Alice A member of group Plibb In situation Faz
could use any pre-existing expectations to make sense of Sarah A member of group Plibb In situation Faz
the social information. Situations Yof and Nuk were Susan A member of group Plibb In situation Faz
embedded within group Smedd, and situations Teg and Faz

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 2 Example behaviors


Big five aspect Example behaviors
used in the category confusion
task Politeness Intentionally avoids ill-mannered and discourteous behavior
Takes the time to gently respond to others without aggression or antagonism
Compassion Expresses empathy and offers support and comfort to her peers
Wholeheartedly contributes to the well-being of others
Orderliness Chooses to follow established guidelines rather than improvise solutions
Endeavours to keep the environment around her neat and tidy
Industriousness Sets her own goals and keeps trying until she achieves them
Meets her deadlines and works with initiative and independence

the 12 statements associated with situation Teg were condition were swapped with the industriousness state-
orderliness statements and eight of the 12 statements ments associated with situation Faz in that condition. The
associated with situation Faz were industriousness state- remaining statements retained the same pattern of associ-
ments. The remaining four statements for each of the sit- ation as in the Local ? Global fit condition. This pattern of
uations were taken from the remaining pool of statements, covariation meant that, at the local level, situation Yof was
which covered all ten aspects (and all five factors) of the relatively polite, situation Nuk was relatively industrious,
Big Five. This covariation pattern meant that, at the local situation Teg was relatively orderly, and situation Faz was
level, situation Yof was relatively polite, situation Nuk was relatively compassionate, and, at the global level, there was
relatively compassionate, situation Teg was relatively no difference in agreeableness or conscientiousness
orderly, and situation Faz was relatively industrious, and, at between group Smedd and group Plibb. In this way,
the global level, group Smedd was relatively agreeable, and comparative fit existed only at the local level in this
group Plibb was relatively conscientious. In this way, condition.
comparative fit existed at both the local and global levels In the No fit control condition, the politeness, compas-
simultaneously in this condition. A covariation level of sion, orderliness and industriousness statements were
66 %—which was used across all 3 conditions in which a divided evenly between all the situations, such that there
pattern of covariation was desired—was chosen over a was no pattern of covariation at either the local or the
perfect covariation pattern of 100 % to remove the possi- global level in this condition (see Fig. 1 for a graphical
bility that an explicit rule could be used to make sense of representation of the covariation pattern in each condition).
the social information (cf Gastgeb et al. 2012; Klinger and In all conditions, the 48 statements were randomized
Dawson 2001). and presented on-screen one-by-one. Each statement stayed
In the Global fit condition, four of the 12 statements on-screen for 11 s, with a 500 ms inter-statement interval.
associated with situation Yof were politeness statements
and four were compassion statements. The number of Phase 2: Attribution Task
politeness and compassion statements for situation Nuk
was the same as for situation Yof. Four of the 12 statements In the second phase of the experiment, participants com-
associated with situation Teg were orderliness statements, pleted an adapted version of the attribution component of
and four were industriousness statements. The number of Taylor et al.’s (1978) category confusion task. Participants
orderliness and industriousness statements for situation Faz were presented with the 48 behavioral statements from the
was the same as for situation Teg. The remaining four previous task, along with a further 24 filler statements, with
statements for each of the situations were taken from the all person, group and situation information removed. Par-
remaining pool of statements, which covered all ten aspects ticipants were then asked to indicate, for each statement, if
(and all five factors) of the big five. This covariation pat- the statement was one they had seen before, by clicking
tern meant that, at the local level, there was no difference with their mouse on ‘‘seen before’’ or ‘‘not seen before’’
in politeness or compassion between situations Yof and options displayed on-screen below the statement. If par-
Nuk, and no difference in orderliness or industriousness ticipants indicated that the statement was not a statement
between situations Teg and Faz, but, at the global level, they had seen before, they moved onto the next statement.
group Smedd was relatively agreeable, and group Plibb If participants indicated that it was a statement they had
was relatively conscientious. In this way, comparative fit seen before, they were then asked to attribute the statement
existed only at the global level in this condition. to the person-group-situation string with which it had been
In the Local fit condition, the compassion statements associated in the previous task. The person-group-situation
associated with situation Nuk in the Local ? Global fit strings were presented in four columns below each

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Fig. 1 The local/global covariance structure of the four comparative fit conditions

behavioral statement, in a different random order for each unlikely’’ to (7) ‘‘very likely’’. The scenarios were ran-
statement. Participants indicated their response by clicking domized and presented on-screen, one-by-one. The same
with their mouse on the string of their choice. The 72 set of ten thoughts was always associated with the same
statements were randomized and presented on-screen one- scenario, but the order of the thoughts for each scenario
by-one. Each statement stayed on-screen until participants was randomized (see Table 3 for an example scenario).
made a response.
Phase 4: Autism-Spectrum Quotient and Demographic
Phase 3: Mental State Inference Task Information

In the third phase of the experiment, participants were In the fourth and final phase of the experiment, participants
presented with eight novel people, each of whom was said were asked to complete Baron-Cohen et al.’s (2001) Aut-
to be confronted by a different scenario. The eight people ism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ). The AQ is a 50-item mea-
were all female, with names chosen from the same list of sure designed to assess the degree of autistic-like traits that
the 25 most popular English language female names as those in the general population possess. Participants were
used for the names in phase 1. Two of the people were said asked to indicate their agreement with each of the 50
to be in situation Yof, two in situation Nuk, two in situation statements by clicking with their mouse on one of the four
Teg, and two in situation Faz. As in phase 1, the people points on a scale from ‘‘Disagree Strongly’’ to ‘‘Agree
in situations Yof and Nuk were said to be members of Strongly’’. The 50 items were presented in the same order
group Smedd, and those in situations Teg and Faz were as used by Baron-Cohen et al. (2001). Following the AQ,
said to be members of group Plibb, such that 4 of the novel participants were asked to indicate their gender, their age,
people were in each of the groups. The scenarios with and whether English was their first language.
which each person was confronted consisted of a short
description of an event about which that person could have
had any of a variety of thoughts. Below each scenario was Results
a list of ten thoughts—a different set of ten thoughts for
each scenario—each of which was created to capture each Calculation of Measures
of the ten aspects of DeYoung et al.’s (2007) hierarchical
Big Five model. Participants were asked to rate the like- In order to test the four hypotheses, it was necessary to
lihood that each of the novel people would have each of calculate three measures from the raw data: local–global
these ten thoughts on a 7-point scale from (1) ‘‘very categorization index, the local–global mental state

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 3 Example scenario and accompanying thoughts used in the mental state inference task
Scenario Thoughts

Julie is a member of group Plibb and finds herself in situation Teg. She ‘‘I shouldn’t stare for too long because it’s rude’’ (politeness)
sees someone getting pulled over for speeding ‘‘I feel really sorry for them, they must feel really embarrassed’’
(compassion)
‘‘Road rules are important because they create stability out of chaos’’
(orderliness)
‘‘Sometimes you might need to speed to get tasks done on time’’
(industriousness)
‘‘It’s interesting to consider whether laws and regulation are always the
best way to achieve social justice’’ (intellect)
‘‘There is real beauty in the way speed cameras use the same medium
as something as wonderful as music’’ (openness)
‘‘If I got caught speeding I would have such a good laugh with the cop’’
(enthusiasm)
‘‘If that were me I’d convince the cop not to charge me’’ (assertiveness)
‘‘The thought of getting caught speeding really angers me’’ (volatility)
‘‘I’m so worried about getting caught speeding, it would be the worst
thing in the world’’ (withdrawal)

inference index, and participants’ Autism-Spectrum Quo- score would indicate that information was being organized
tient (AQ). The categorization index was created to capture at the local level. A global error difference score was
the way in which participants categorized/organized the derived by subtracting the between-group errors from the
social information presented in the category confusion task. corrected within-group errors, such that a higher score
The mental state inference index was created to capture the would indicate that information was being organized at the
way in which participants used the local and global cate- global level. Finally, the (local–global) categorization
gories to make their mental state inferences. The AQ was index was created by subtracting the global error difference
borrowed from Baron-Cohen et al. (2001) in order to assess from the local error difference. A positive score on the
the degree of autistic-like traits possessed by participants. categorization index would therefore indicate that partici-
These measures were calculated as follows: pants were organizing the social information more at the
local level and less at the global level. A negative score
Categorization Index would indicate that participants were organizing the social
information more at the global level and less at the local
Taylor et al.’s (1978) error difference measure was adapted level. A score of zero would indicate that participants were
for use within the hierarchically-embedded category con- organizing the social information equally at the local and
fusion task used in the current experiment. Each partici- global levels.
pant’s errors in the attribution phase were counted. These
were then divided into within-situation errors (incorrect Mental State Inference Index
person, correct situation), between-situation/within-group
errors (incorrect person, incorrect situation, correct group), In order to assess the extent to which participants were
and between-group errors (incorrect person, incorrect sit- using the local categories or the global categories to make
uation, incorrect group). The number of within-situation their mental state inferences, meta-contrast ratios were
errors was then corrected for the higher probability of calculated. Within self-categorization theory (Turner et al.
between-situation than within-situation errors by dividing 1987, 1994), the meta-contrast ratio is defined as the ratio
them by the baseline ratio of possible within-situation to of the average inter-category differences to intra-category
between-situation errors. The number of within-group differences; as such, it captures the extent to which the
errors was corrected for the higher probability of between- members of a category are perceived to be relatively
group than within-group errors by dividing them by the similar to each other and relatively different from the
baseline ratio of possible within-group to between-group members of a comparison category. First, a local meta-
errors. A local error difference score was then derived by contrast score was derived by following three steps: (1) the
subtracting the uncorrected between-situation errors from average absolute difference between likelihood ratings for
the corrected within-situation errors, such that a higher those people who were said to be in the same situations was

123
J Autism Dev Disord

calculated across the trait dimensions relevant to the fit a mean of 16.74. Baron-Cohen at el. (2001) propose that a
condition; (2) the average absolute difference between score of 32 is a statistically useful cut-off between the TD
likelihood ratings for people who were said to be in dif- population and the ASD diagnostic category. This suggests
ferent situations (but in the same group) was calculated that the current sample would likely contain few, if any,
across the trait dimensions relevant to the fit condition; and participants who would meet the diagnostic criteria for
(3) the average absolute differences between the likelihood ASD.
ratings for people in different situations was divided by the
average absolute differences between the likelihood ratings Categorization of Social Information
for people in the same situations. A higher local meta-
contrast ratio would indicate that the thoughts of the novel In order to ascertain the extent to which participants were
category members were being derived from knowledge of organizing the social information from the category con-
the content of the local categories. fusion task at the local or the global level, an overall
Next, a global meta-contrast score was derived by fol- analysis and a number of planned comparisons by condi-
lowing four steps: (1) the average likelihood rating across tion were conducted on the categorization index. An
people who were said to be in the same situation was cal- overall ANCOVA revealed a significant effect of fit con-
culated for each of the trait dimensions; (2) the average dition, F (3, 95) = 7.89, p = .00009, g2 = .199, a mar-
absolute difference between the average likelihood ratings ginally significant positive relationship between AQ and
for the situations within the same group was calculated the categorization index, F (1, 95) = 2.89, p = .09,
across the trait dimensions relevant to the fit condition; (3) g2 = .03, and a significant interaction between fit condition
the average absolute difference between the average likeli- and AQ, F (3, 95) = 8.28, p = .00006, g2 = .207. Overall,
hood ratings for the situations in different groups was cal- participants organized the social information more at the
culated across the trait dimensions relevant to the fit global than the local level across all conditions, but this
condition; and (4) the average absolute difference between was particularly evident in the Local ? Global fit
the likelihood ratings for the situations in different groups (M = -1.71) and No fit (M = -1.53) conditions, relative
was divided by the average absolute differences between the to the Global fit (M = -.41) and the Local fit (M = -.56)
likelihood ratings for the situations in the same group. A conditions. Of more theoretical interest, however, was the
higher global meta-contrast ratio would indicate that the significant interaction, which was deconstructed by fit
thoughts of the novel category members were being derived condition:
from knowledge of the content of the global categories.
Finally, the mental state inference index was created by Local ? Global Fit
subtracting the global meta-contrast from the local meta-
contrast. A positive score on the mental state inference A significant effect of AQ on the categorization index
index would therefore indicate that participants were emerged in the Local ? Global fit condition, F (1,
inferring the thoughts of the novel people more from the 22) = 12.03, p = .002, g2 = .353. Figure 2 shows that the
local category and less from the global category. A nega- relationship between AQ and the categorization index is
tive score would indicate that participants were inferring positive, which suggests that higher AQ is associated with
the thoughts of the novel people more from the global more local and less global organization of the social
category and less from the local category. A score of zero information.
would indicate that participants were inferring the thoughts
of the novel people equally from the local and global Global Fit
categories (for more information on calculating meta-
contrast ratios, see McGarty 1999). A significant effect of AQ on the categorization index
emerged in the Global fit condition, F (1, 30) = 9.19,
Autism-Spectrum Quotient p = .005, g2 = .235. Figure 2 shows that the relationship
between AQ and the categorization index is negative,
The AQ was calculated as described in Baron-Cohen et al. which suggests that higher AQ is associated with less local
(2001). One point was given for responses of ‘‘Disagree’’ and more global organization of the social information.
and ‘‘Disagree Strongly’’ on the 26 reverse-coded items.
One point was given for responses of ‘‘Agree’’ and ‘‘Agree Local Fit
Strongly’’ for the remaining 24 items. These points were
then added together to create an aggregate AQ score, such No significant effect of AQ on the categorization index
that a higher score would indicate more autistic-like traits. emerged in the Local fit condition, F (1, 24) = .46,
AQ scores in the current sample ranged from 2 to 32, with p = .51, g2 = .019.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Fig. 2 The relationship between Autism-Spectrum Quotient and local–global categorization across the four comparative fit conditions

No Fit inferences more on the basis of the global than the local cate-
gories. This pattern was evident in the Local ? Global fit
No significant effect of AQ on the categorization index (M = -2.9), the Global fit (M = -.21) and the Local fit
emerged in the No fit condition, F (1, 19) = .804, p = .38, (M = -.15) conditions. Only in the No Fit condition was there
g2 = .041. a tendency for participants to make inferences more on the
basis of the local than the global categories (M = .75). Of more
Mental State Inference theoretical interest, however, was the significant interaction,
which was deconstructed by fit condition:
In order to ascertain the extent to which participants were
inferring the thoughts of the novel category members more Local ? Global Fit
from the local or the global categories, an overall analysis and a
number of planned comparisons by condition were conducted A significant effect of AQ on the mental state inference
on the mental state inference index. An overall ANCOVA index emerged in the Local ? Global fit condition, F (1,
revealed a significant effect of fit condition, F (3, 94) = 5.01, 22) = 7.61, p = .011, g2 = .257. Figure 3 shows that the
p = .003, g2 = .138, no effect of AQ, F (1, 94) = .002, relationship between AQ and the mental state inference
p = .961, g2 = .000026, and a significant interaction between index is positive, which suggests that higher AQ is asso-
fit condition and AQ, F (3, 94) = 4.79, p = .004, g2 = .133. ciated with more locally-derived and less globally-derived
Overall, there was a tendency for participants to make mental state inference.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Global Fit Mediation Analysis

No significant effect of AQ on the mental state inference A mediation analysis was conducted in order to test whe-
index emerged in the Global fit condition, F (1, ther the relationship between AQ and the mental state
30) = 1.26, p = .271, g2 = .04. inference index observed in the Local ? Global fit condi-
tion was mediated by the categorization index in that
Local Fit condition. The first two criteria as set out by Baron and
Kenny (1986)—that is, (1) a significant relationship
No significant effect of AQ on the mental state inference between the IV and the DV, and (2) a significant rela-
index emerged in the Local fit condition, F (1, 24) \1, tionship between the IV and the mediator—were fulfilled
p = .997, g2 = .00000072. by way of the significant relationship between AQ and the
mental state inference index and the significant relationship
No Fit between AQ and the categorization index described above.
The third criterion—that is, a significant relationship
No significant effect of AQ on the mental state inference between the mediator and the DV—was also fulfilled: a
index emerged in the No fit condition, F (1, 18) = .299, bivariate correlation analysis revealed a significant positive
p = .101, g2 = .143. correlation between the categorization index and the

Fig. 3 The relationship between Autism-Spectrum Quotient and local–global mental state inference across the four comparative fit conditions

123
J Autism Dev Disord

mental state inference index in the Local ? Global fit AQ scores predicted their tendencies to organize the social
condition, r (24) = .471, p = .02. The final criterion was information at the local level relative to the global level
also fulfilled: the previously significant relationship when there was competing covariation at these levels. This
between AQ and the mental state inference index became first finding is consistent with research on the more general
non-significant when the categorization index was entered process of categorization (Klinger and Dawson 2001), and
as a covariate, F (1, 21) = 2.39, p = .137, g2 = .1. Taken on the processing of Navon figures (Plaisted et al. 1999),
together, these results suggest that the relationship between that individuals with ASD (in this case, individuals with a
AQ and the mental state inference index is fully mediated higher AQ) have a tendency to engage in less integrated
by the categorization index in the Local ? Global fit and more local processing. It is also quite clear from these
condition. results that participants’ AQ scores predicted their patterns
of mental state inference. This finding is consistent with the
general and widely-supported notion that individuals with
Discussion ASD show differences in their theory of mind abilities
(e.g., Baron-Cohen et al. 1985). Finally, these results also
The current experiment was designed to test the notion that make it quite clear that participants’ patterns of social
the theory of mind difficulties associated with ASD are a categorization are related directly to their patterns of
result of a difference in social categorization tendencies, mental state inferences, and that, in fact, the relationship
which itself is related to weak central coherence. We between AQ and the patterns of mental state inferences is
proposed specifically that the degree of autistic-like largely due to the way in which participants organized the
traits—captured by the AQ—would predict the pattern of social information. This latter finding represents the novel
social categorization in the Local ? Global Fit condition, contribution of the current research, and points to the
which in turn would predict the pattern of results on the possibility that at least some of the so-called ‘‘fractionable’’
mental state inference task in that condition. All four components of the disorder could be united under a single
hypotheses were broadly supported. AQ interacted with fit cognitive framework. In addition, the current experiment
condition in determining the extent to which participants has a number of important implications for our under-
organized the social information at the local level relative standing of general theory of mind abilities and of social
to the global level, particularly in the Local ? Global fit categorization, and should also open up new avenues of
condition, where AQ positively predicted local social cat- enquiry with regard to diagnosis and intervention.
egorization at the expense of global social categorization.
AQ also interacted with fit condition in determining the Implications
extent to which participants made their mental state
inferences at the local level relative to the global level, The implications of this research can be divided into those
particularly in the Local ? Global fit condition, where AQ relevant to general cognitive processes, and those relevant
positively predicted local mental state inference at the to ASD. Given that the results of this experiment are pre-
expense of global mental state inference. The pattern of liminary—a point to which we return in the limitations
social categorization also predicted the pattern of mental section below—all the implications discussed here should
state inferences, with more local social categorization be interpreted with appropriate care.
predicting more local mental state inference and more
global social categorization predicting more global mental General Theory of Mind Abilities
state inference. This relationship between categorization
and mental state inference was particularly evident in the First, the current experiment has important implications for
Local ? Global fit condition. Finally, the relationship our understanding of general theory of mind abilities.
between AQ and the pattern of mental state inference in the These results support the general consensus in the social
Local ? Global fit condition was fully mediated by the cognitive literature that theory of mind abilities rely cru-
pattern of social categorization, such that the positive cially on a process of social categorization (e.g., Ames
relationship between AQ and local mental state inference 2004). The positive relationship between participants’
was eliminated when local/global social categorization was patterns of social categorization and their patterns of
taken into account. mental state inferences demonstrates that human beings
Taken together, these results provide preliminary evi- employ social categories to infer the thoughts of category
dence for the central proposition of the current research members. Importantly, the current experiment goes beyond
that the theory of mind difficulties associated with ASD are existing understandings by linking mental state inference
an outcome of weak central coherence via a social cate- directly to the more fundamental cognitive process of
gorization mechanism. It is quite clear that participants’ comparative fit detection which underlies social

123
J Autism Dev Disord

categorization. In so doing, the current research suggests (e.g., Van de Cruys et al. 2014). Happé and colleagues
that the pattern of covariation in a social stimulus set lar- (Brunsdon and Happé 2014; Happé and Booth 2008)
gely determines the way in which a social category pro- describe ASD as ‘‘fractionable’’ suggesting that it consists
totype is formed, which in turn affects the nature and of dissociable components, at each of the behavioral,
content of the thoughts that are inferred. The current cognitive and neurological levels of analysis. The current
experiment also extends previous understandings by experiment, however, suggests that at least some of the
showing that there can be a conflict between competing features—that is, those associated with weak central
category prototypes—in this case at different levels of coherence and theory of mind dysfunction—could be uni-
abstraction (i.e., global vs. local)—which can be resolved ted within a more parsimonious cognitive model, in which
by the perceiver’s information processing tendencies—in social categorization plays a central role.
this case related to autistic-like traits. Understanding theory A more unified cognitive account of ASD based around
of mind processes in this way allows for highly developed a social categorization dysfunction could lead to a variety
connectionist models of category learning (e.g., Love et al. of new avenues for research, novel hypotheses, innovative
2004; Van Rooy 2012) to be applied where previously their diagnostic tests, and improved intervention. By linking
relevance had not been recognized. Overall, these results ASD to social categorization, topics commonly explored in
provide direct evidence for Frith’s (1989) contention that social cognition—such as impression formation, stereo-
the theory of mind system is embedded within a more typing, self-construal etc.—could also be explored in ASD.
general information processing system, the main function Understanding ASD as a disorder of social categorization
of which is extracting higher-level meaning in context. could also lead to novel hypotheses with regard to the
significance of the over- or under-connectivity in neural
Social Categorization circuitry observed in the disorder (see, for example,
Casanova et al. 2002), and also its genetic basis. Similarly,
In establishing a direct relationship between ASD, theory it may be possible to develop diagnostic tests for ASD
of mind abilities and social categorization, the current based around reliable categorization/prototype formation
experiment also has the potential to provide new insights paradigms, such as the category confusion paradigm used
into the process of social categorization. The current results in the current experiment. Finally, by linking ASD to social
can be interpreted as demonstrating that ASD is, at least in and self-categorization, promising new interventions based
part, a disorder of social categorization. This novel char- around social and self-categorization principles—includ-
acterization of ASD means that the process of social cat- ing, for example, self-complexity buffering (Linville 1987)
egorization can be better understood by exploring the and the social identity approach to health and well-being
consequences that arise when its functioning is disrupted in (Jetten et al. 2012)—could be employed to decrease the
the disorder. It is likely that studying ASD in this way much higher rates of anxiety (Gillott et al. 2001) and
could lead to a better understanding of, at the very least, the depression (Ghaziuddin et al. 2002) among individuals
process of prototype formation, and the related cognitive with ASD. Overall, the implications for ASD of the more
process of comparative fit detection. Examining the con- unified account of the disorder implied by the current
sequences that arise following disruption of a cognitive research could be profound.
system is a common way for cognitive phenomena to be
studied—for example, memory and amnesia (Schachter Limitations
1987); face processing and prosopagnosia (Palermo et al.
2011); attention and unilateral neglect (Aimola-Davies As noted at the beginning of the previous section, the
2004)—but one that has so far been impossible for social results of the current experiment, though promising and
categorization. The current results, and research building with the potential for important implications, provide only
on them, suggest that this gap could soon be filled. preliminary evidence for the central proposition that the
theory of mind deficits of ASD are an outcome of weak
Implications for ASD central coherence. As such, it is important to note the most
significant limitations in the current research, both as a
The most important implications of the current research cautionary note, but also as the foundation for future
relate to existing understandings of ASD. Currently, ASD directions.
is characterized as a multi-faceted condition, consisting of
a variety of distinct but co-occurring features. It is most Non-clinical Sample
widely viewed as a disorder of theory of mind processes
(Baron-Cohen 2005), but the idea that it is also a disorder First, and most obviously, the current results were obtained
of general information processing is also gaining traction with a non-clinical sample. As such, the generalizability of

123
J Autism Dev Disord

the results to individuals with ASD is not immediately 2001), local/global processing tendencies (e.g., Plaisted
clear. The AQ was, however, developed to assess the extent et al. 1999), and as a priori differences in the threshold
to which TD adults possess the characteristics associated necessary for identification of factors such as radial fre-
with ASD, and, as such, these results should be taken as quency (e.g., Almeida et al. 2010). In the current experi-
having some relevance to ASD itself, if only as very pre- ment, weak central coherence was operationalized through
liminary evidence of the proposition in question. The AQ is the category confusion paradigm in a way most consistent
commonly used in research on ASD as a proxy measure for with the notion that it is a preference for (or tendency
clinical diagnosis (e.g., Almeida et al. 2010; Jarrold et al. towards) local processing, which maps most directly onto
2000), and is commonly interpreted as providing prelimi- the categorization and local/global processing operational-
nary evidence for phenomena that affect those on the ASD izations of Klinger and Dawson (2001) and Plaisted et al.
spectrum, with the caveat that caution should always be (1999) respectively. Had we operationalized weak central
taken when generalization is attempted. We argue that the coherence in a different way, the pattern of results could
same degree of caution should be applied when attempting have been profoundly altered. It is important, therefore, to
to generalize these results beyond the TD population. interpret the results explicitly in terms of the specific defi-
nition, and the specific operationalization, of weak central
Confounding Factors coherence that we chose. It is equally important to note,
however, that the lack of clarity surrounding the concept of
Relatedly, in the current research we did not control for weak central coherence arises from the literature on the
potential confounding/covarying factors that are typically topic—where its fundamental nature is still being debated—
taken into account in similar research. IQ, general range of rather than from our own experimental shortcomings.
vocabulary, individual differences in processing style, and
other similar factors could all plausibly have impacted on Theory of Mind Deficit Versus Differential Mental State
the results of the current experiment. The use of a highly- Inference
educated non-clinical sample, who are not likely to have
had any trouble completing the experimental tasks, means, Finally, it should be acknowledged that the current results
however, that many of these factors are less likely to have do not in fact demonstrate a deficit in theory of mind
had particularly potent influences on these results. Addi- processes, but rather reveal only different patterns of
tionally, the inclusion of these factors in most research mental state inference as a function of AQ. In part this is
commonly results in a suppressor effect, thus increasing the due to the general population sample, such that no specific
effects of the centrally important theoretical variables, deficit would a priori be expected. It is plausible,
rather than decreasing them. In the current research, this nonetheless, that the current results can be reconciled with
would result in stronger relationships between AQ, social the notion of theory of mind deficits in ASD, and therefore
categorization, and patterns of mental state inference, with the more central proposal that weak central coherence
rather than weaker ones. Nonetheless, the absence of these is a precursor to theory of mind deficits. When a person is
control factors means that caution should be taken in engaging in social categorization, their task is to categorize
interpreting these results. other people and their behaviors in terms of relevant and
meaningful social categories, in order to make inferences
Lack of Clarity Surrounding Weak Central Coherence about their thoughts, feelings, and behaviors. Categorizing
other people in this way is a more complex task than cat-
More importantly than the absence of certain control vari- egorizing objects or other static sets of stimuli, because
ables in the current research is the lack of clarity sur- people have the ability to self-categorize (Turner et al.
rounding the concept of weak central coherence. Weak 1987). The fact that others can self-categorize means that,
central coherence has variously been described as a deficit in unlike a static object, the fundamental nature of the stim-
global processing; a preference for, or tendency towards, ulus to be categorized changes. The task then for a per-
local processing; a deficit in holistic processing; a relative ceiver engaged in social categorization is to capture the
absence of Gestalt grouping; a decreased ability to draw way in which the target is self-categorizing in that moment.
together component features into a coherent whole; and, For example, if a perceiver is attempting to predict the
more recently, as a tendency towards enhanced perceptual emotional reactions of a person on a soccer field when a
functioning (Mottron et al. 2006). These different under- goal is scored, he or she will need to capture how that
standings of the concept have led to a variety of distinct (but person is self-categorizing; that is, as a member of one
related) operationalizations, which variously tap into dis- team rather than the other. Only by capturing a target’s
embedding abilities (e.g., Morgan et al. 2003), group- self-categorization can the thoughts, feelings and behavior
ing/categorization abilities (e.g., Klinger and Dawson of that target be inferred (McGarty 1999).

123
J Autism Dev Disord

The fact that mental state inference requires a perceiver central coherence could be operationalized as the degree of
to capture a target’s self-categorization further entails that comparative fit—where it may be that individuals with
the task of mental state inference will be easier to the ASD require a higher level of fit before a prototype can be
extent that there is a match between any general tendency detected (Almeida et al. 2010)—or as the ratio of signal to
in the (local/global) level of social categorization at which noise—where individuals with ASD may in fact be more
the perceiver categorizes another and any general tendency adept at disembedding the signal from the noise, just as
in the level of self-categorization at which the target cat- they are more adept at the EFT (e.g., Morgan et al. 2003).
egorizes him or herself. That is, if a perceiver has a general Relatedly, it is also important to establish empirically that
tendency to categorize others at a more global level, and social categorization is in fact related to weak central
those others also have a general tendency to categorize coherence, which could be achieved by exploring the
themselves at a more global level, then the perceiver’s relationship between patterns of social categorization in a
mental state inferences are more likely to be accurate. To category confusion paradigm and traditional measures of
the extent that there is a mismatch between these general weak central coherence, such as the EFT and Navon tasks.
tendencies, there will be a decrease in accuracy. The cur- Finally, future research should be directed toward
rent results suggest that a higher AQ is associated with a exploring shared attention, as a precursor to mental state
tendency to categorize others at a more local level, and a inference and as the cognitive process thought to be most
lower AQ is associated with a tendency to categorize others directly affected in ASD (Baron-Cohen 2005). This could
at a more global level. If these general tendencies were also be achieved by adapting the category confusion paradigm
reflected in self-categorization tendencies, then the so- to include the self, such that a participant would be given
called deficit in theory of mind abilities among individuals the opportunity to self-categorize. In the current experi-
with ASD could arise simply as a result of an a priori, ment, all the targets were other individuals, such that no
baseline mismatch between the level at which people with self-categorization—that is, understanding the categorical
ASD categorize others and the level at which TD indi- relationship between self and others in a psychological
viduals generally categorize themselves. This suggestion is frame of reference—was possible. By adapting the cate-
obviously contentious; in the absence of direct measures of gory confusion paradigm for self-categorization, partici-
self-categorization tendencies among individuals diag- pants’ patterns of local versus global self-categorization
nosed with ASD, the results of the current experiment could be measured. The relationship between these patterns
cannot speak to them directly, but they nonetheless allude of self-categorization and patterns of shared attention could
to intriguing avenues for new research in this area. then be explored. Research of this sort would fundamen-
tally bolster the central claim of the current paper that weak
Future Directions central coherence is a crucial precursor to all aspects of
theory of mind, including SAM, the component thought to
The limitations noted above suggest a number of avenues be specifically disrupted during development.
for future research. First, it is crucially important to repli-
cate the current experiment with a sample of participants
diagnosed with ASD. The current results only hint at the Conclusion
possibility that weak central coherence and theory of mind
processes might be causally related in ASD. The AQ is a In this paper, we explored the notion that the theory of mind
commonly used and reliable tool for gathering preliminary difficulties observed in ASD were the result of weak central
evidence for phenomena associated with ASD, but coherence with regard to the process of social categorization.
nonetheless, direct evidence for the causal relationship of We presented an experiment in which we manipulated com-
interest can plainly only arise from exploring that rela- parative fit between a number of hierarchically embedded
tionship among those diagnosed with the disorder. Relat- social categories and social information—at the local level,
edly, it is important that any research conducted with a the global level, or at both levels simultaneously—within a
clinical sample of this sort also controls for other possible category confusion task. We then asked non-clinical partici-
confounding factors—such as IQ, verbal abilities etc.—for pants to infer the mental state of novel category members, and
which we did not control in the current experiment. measured participants’ AQ. The experiment showed that AQ
Future research exploring the relationship between weak was positively associated with the degree of local relative to
central coherence and theory of mind processes should also global categorization and the degree of local relative to global
attempt to pin down the true nature of weak central mental state inferences, when there was competing covaria-
coherence, and should operationalize it in a variety of ways tion at the local and global levels. We also found that the
to ensure that the relationship remains, irrespective of the pattern of social categorization predicted the pattern of mental
specific operationalization. In future experiments, weak state inferences in this condition, thus providing preliminary

123
J Autism Dev Disord

evidence for a direct, causal relationship between weak central Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997).
coherence and theory of mind abilities. These findings have Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very
high functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome.
important implications for the development of a more unified Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(7), 813–822.
account of ASD, thus challenging the currently dominant Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator
‘‘fractionated’’ view of the disorder. These results must be variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual,
replicated—particularly with a clinically diagnosed sample of strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182.
individuals with ASD, and in similar paradigms exploring Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic
other theory of mind processes such as shared attention, and child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition, 21, 37–46.
with other measures of weak central coherence—before any Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley,
strong conclusions about the singular versus multi-faceted E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from
Asperger Syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females,
nature of ASD can be drawn. Nonetheless, we hope that the scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Develop-
current paper will motivate other researchers in the field to mental Disorders, 31(1), 5–17.
reconsider whether it really is yet ‘‘time to give up on a single Blanz, M. (1999). Accessibility and fit as determinants of the salience
explanation for autism.’’ of social categorizations. European Journal of Social Psychol-
ogy, 29(1), 43–74.
Brosnan, M. J., Scott, F. J., Fox, S., & Pye, J. (2004). Gestalt
Acknowledgments We would like to thank Anne Aimola-Davies, processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships
Laura Birchall, Alex Haslam, Ken Mavor, Katie Stalker, Lidan and the implications for perceptual understanding. Journal of
Zheng, Carla Mazefsky and two anonymous reviewers for their Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 459–469.
insightful comments on this research. Brunsdon, V. E. A., & Happé, F. (2014). Exploring the ‘fractionation’
of autism at the cognitive level. Autism, 18(1), 17–30.
Author Contributions All authors made substantial contributions Burnette, C. P., Mundy, P. C., Meyer, J. A., Sutton, S. K., Vaughan,
to the design of the experiment reported in this paper; to the acqui- A. E., & Charak, D. (2005). Weak central coherence and its
sition, analysis, and interpretation of the data; to the drafting of the relations to theory of mind and anxiety in autism. Journal of
manuscript, and to all revisions made; and all authors have given final Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(1), 63–73.
approval of the version to be published. Casanova, M. F., Buxhoeveden, D. P., & Brown, C. (2002). Clinical
and macroscopic correlates of minicolumnar pathology in
autism. Journal of Child Neurology, 17(9), 692–695.
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L.
References (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy–altruism relationship: When
one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social
Adamson, L. B., Deckner, D. F., & Bakeman, R. (2010). Early Psychology, 73(3), 481–494.
interests and joint engagement in typical development, autism, Cornew, L., Dobkins, K. R., Akshoomoff, N., McCleery, J. P., &
and down syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Carver, L. J. (2012). A typical social referencing in infant
Disorders, 40(6), 665–676. siblings of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Aimola Davies, A. M. (2004). Disorders of spatial orientation and Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(12), 2611–2621.
awareness: Unilateral neglect. In J. Ponsford (Ed.), Cognitive DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between
and behavioural rehabilitation: From neurobiology to clinical facets and domains: 10 aspects of the big five. Journal of
practice (pp. 175–223). New York: Guilford Press. Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 880–896.
Almeida, R. A., Dickinson, J. E., Maybery, M. T., Badcock, J. C., & Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.
Badcock, D. R. (2010). A new step towards understanding Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond ‘theory of mind’.
embedded figures test performance in the autism spectrum: The Cognition, 50, 115–132.
radial frequency search task. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 374–381. Gastgeb, H. Z., Dundas, E. M., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss, M. S.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical (2012). Category formation in autism: Can individuals with
manual of mental disorders: DSM-5TM (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: autism form categories and prototypes of dot patterns? Journal
American Psychiatric Publishing Inc. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(8), 1694–1704.
Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind readers’ tool kit: Projection and Gastgeb, H. Z., Rump, K. M., Best, C. A., Minshew, N. J., & Strauss,
stereotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality M. S. (2009). Prototype formation in autism: Can individuals
and Social Psychology, 87(3), 340–353. with autism abstract facial prototypes? Autism Research, 2(5),
Ames, D. R., Weber, E. U., & Zou, X. (2012). Mind-reading in 279–284.
strategic interaction: The impact of perceived similarity on Gastgeb, H. Z., Strauss, M. S., & Minshew, N. J. (2006). Do
projection and stereotyping. Organizational Behavior and individuals with autism process categories differently? The
Human Decision Processes, 117(1), 96–110. effect of typicality and development. Child Development, 77(6),
Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). The mindreading system: New directions for 1717–1729.
research. Current Psychology of Cognition, 13, 724–750. Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. (2002). Depression in
Baron-Cohen, S. (2005). The empathizing system: A revision of the persons with autism: Implications for research and clinical care.
1994 model of the mindreading system. In B. Ellis & D. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(4),
Bjorklund (Eds.), Origins of the social mind: Evolutionary 299–306.
psychology and child development (pp. 468–492). New York: Gillott, A., Furniss, F., & Walter, A. (2001). Anxiety in high-
Guildford Press. functioning children with autism. Autism, 5(3), 277–286.
Baron-Cohen, S., & Goodhart, F. (1994). The ‘seeing-leads-to- Happé, F. G. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of
knowing’ deficit in autism: The Pratt and Bryant probe. British mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Develop-
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12(3), 397–401. ment, 66(3), 843–855.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Happé, F. G. (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: Morgan, B., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2003). Weak central
Children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A coherence, poor joint attention, and low verbal ability: Indepen-
research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, dent deficits in early autism. Developmental Psychology, 39(4),
37(7), 873–877. 646–656.
Happé, F. G. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J.
Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Develop- (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update,
mental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12. and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and
Happé, F. G., & Booth, R. D. (2008). The power of the positive: Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.
Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. The Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 50–63. social reality. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishing.
Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail- Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with
focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(3), 247–257.
Happé, F. G., & Ronald, A. (2008). The ‘fractionable autism triad’: A Osterling, J. A., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. A. (2002). Early
review of evidence from behavioral, genetic, cognitive and recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder
neural research. Neuropsychological Review, 18, 287–304. versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology,
Happé, F. G., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single 14(2), 239–251.
explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218–1220. Palermo, R., Willis, M. L., Rivolta, D., McKone, E., Wilson, C. E., &
Heavey, L., Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (2000). The Calder, A. J. (2011). Impaired holistic coding of facial expres-
awkward moments test: A naturalistic measure of social sion and facial identity in congenital prosopagnosia. Neuropsy-
understanding in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental chologia, 49(5), 1226–1235.
Disorders, 30(3), 225–236. Pelicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple
Hobson, R. P. (1983). The autistic child’s recognition of age-related cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spec-
features of people, animals and things. British Journal of trum disorder: ‘Weak’ central coherence and its relationship to
Developmental Psychology, 1(4), 343–352. theory of mind and executive control. Development and
Hobson, R. P. (1987). The autistic child’s recognition of age-and sex- Psychopathology, 18, 77–98.
related characteristics of people. Journal of Autism and Devel- Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced
opmental Disorders, 17(1), 63–79. discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with
Hobson, R. P. (1990). On the origins of self and the case of autism. autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child
Development and Psychopathology, 2(2), 163–181. Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 765–775.
Hobson, R. P., & Meyer, J. A. (2005). Foundations for self and other: Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism
A study in autism. Developmental science, 8(6), 481–491. show local precedence in a divided attention task and global
Jarrold, C., Butler, D. W., Cottington, E. M., & Jimenez, F. (2000). precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child
Linking theory of mind and central coherence bias in autism and Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 733–742.
in the general population. Developmental Psychology, 36(1), Schacter, D. L. (1987). Memory, amnesia, and frontal lobe dysfunc-
126–138. tion. Psychobiology, 15(1), 21–36.
Jetten, J., Haslam, C., & Alexander, S. A. (Eds.). (2012). The social cure: Schuwerk, T., Vuori, M., & Sodian, B. (2015). Implicit and explicit
Identity, health and well-being. New York: Psychology Press. theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum disorders: The
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Are people with autism and impact of experience. Autism, 19(4), 459–468.
Asperger Syndrome faster than normal on the embedded Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind
figures test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger
38(5), 527–534. syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885.
Jolliffe, T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). A test of central coherence Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A
theory: Can adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
syndrome integrate objects in context? Visual Cognition, 8(1), 24(4), 613–620.
67–101. Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show
Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child
Development and Psychopathology, 13(1), 111–124. Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(8), 1351–1364.
Lee, A., & Hobson, R. P. (1998). On developing self-concepts: A Shteynberg, G. (2010). A silent emergence of culture: The social
controlled study of children and adolescents with autism. tuning effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(8), 1131–1144. 99(4), 683–689.
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against Shteynberg, G. (2015). Shared attention. Perspectives on Psycholog-
stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personality and ical Science, 10(5), 579–590.
Social Psychology, 52(4), 663–676. Skorich, D. P., & Mavor, K. I. (2013). Cognitive load privileges
Love, B. C., Medin, D. L., & Gureckis, T. M. (2004). SUSTAIN: A memory-based over data-driven processing, not group-level over
network model of category learning. Psychological Review, person-level processing. British Journal of Social Psychology,
111(2), 309–332. 52(3), 469–488.
McGarty, C. (1999). Categorization in social psychology. London: Sodian, B., Schuwerk, T., & Kristen, S. (2015). Implicit and
Sage. spontaneous theory of mind reasoning in autism spectrum
McGarty, C., Mavor, K. I., & Skorich, D. P. (2015). Social disorders. In M. Fitzgerald (Ed.), Autism spectrum disorder—
categorization. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia recent advances. In Tech.
of the social and behavioral sciences (2nd ed., pp. 186–191). Strauss, M. S., Newell, L. C., Best, C. A., Hannigen, S. F., Gastgeb,
New York, NY: Elsevier. H. Z., & Giovannelli, J. L. (2012). The development of facial
Meyer, J. A., & Hobson, R. P. (2004). Orientation in relation to self gender categorization in individuals with and without autism:
and other: The case of autism. Interaction Studies, 5(2), The impact of typicality. Journal of Autism and Developmental
221–244. Disorders, 42(9), 1847–1855.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Sucksmith, E., Roth, I., & Hoekstra, R. A. (2011). Autistic traits motivating action for social change. Personality and Social
below the clinical threshold: Reexamining the broader autism Psychology Review, 13(4), 310–333.
phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychology Review, 21(4), Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherell,
360–389. M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-catego-
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (1994). Predicting and explaining rization theory. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
behavior: A comparison of autistic, mentally retarded and Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. (1994). Self
normal children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and
35(6), 1059–1075. Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463.
Tarrant, M., Dazeley, S., & Cottom, T. (2009). Social categorization Van de Cruys, S., Evers, K., Van der Hallen, R., Van Eylen, L., Boets,
and empathy for outgroup members. British Journal of Social B., de-Wit, L., & Wagemans, J. (2014). Precise minds in
Psychology, 48(3), 427–446. uncertain worlds: Predictive coding in autism. Psychological
Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T., Etcoff, N. L., & Ruderman, A. J. (1978). Review, 121(4), 649–675.
Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and Van Rooy, D. (2012). A connectionist ABM of social categorization
stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, processes. Advances in Complex Systems, 15(6), 1250077.
36(7), 778–793. Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M., & Solomonica-Levi, D. (1998).
Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2001). Impaired categorical Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals
perception of facial expressions in high-functioning adolescents with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally
with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 7(1), 1–14. developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3),
Thomas, E. F., McGarty, C., & Mavor, K. I. (2009). Transforming 283–307.
‘‘apathy into movement’’: The role of prosocial emotions in

123

You might also like