You are on page 1of 9

REVIEWS

Depression and Somatization: a Review


Part I

WAYNE KATON, M.D. The authors describe the relationship between the major depressive
ARTHUR KLEINMAN, M.D. disorder and somatitation. A literature review documenting the in-
GARY ROSEN, M.D. cidence and prevalence of depression in primary care and the
Seattle, Washington rate of misdiagnosis is presented. Evidence is collated that points
to several factors in misdiagnosis. The patient often selectively
complains about the somatic manifestations of depression, mini-
mizes the affective and cognitive components and is treated
symptomatically. This is due to the physician’s lack of recognition
that the patient may have major dept’essive disorder and yet not
recognize and report the mood component. The authors develop a
conceptual model that elucidates the mechanism behind the se-
lective perception and focus by the patient on the somatic mani-
festations of depression. In this first part, the influence of socio-
cultural and childhood experience on the ability of the patient to
recognize and report mood changes is delineated. Understanding
this model is crucial in preventing misdiagnosis and potential iat-
rogenic harm to the patient.

The purpose of this paper is to describe the intricate relationship


between somatization and depression. We present a conceptual model
to elucidate the relationship between the psychobiologic disease of
depression and the use of somatization by the patient. We use the term
depression in this paper to describe the major depressive disorder,
which is a syndrome involving mood, vegetative symptoms of neu-
roendocrine and autonomic nervous system dysfunction and specific
cognitive schemata [ 11. Somatization in patients with the major de-
pressive disorder can be defined as the selective perception and focus
on the somatic manifestations of depression with denial or minimi-
zation of the affective and cognitive changes.
Patients who somatize either have no discernible biologic abnor-
mality but recurrently present with somatic complaints, or have dis-
cernible disease but amplify their symptoms and frequently utilize
medical services. Although we emphasize depression as one of the
most common causes of somatization, other psychiatric disorders
associated with it include somatization disorder (Briquet’s syndrome,
From the Department of Psychiatryand Behavioral formerly called hysteria), anxiety disorders, obsessive-compulsive
Sciences, University of Washington, Seattle, disorder, grief reactions, hysterical personality disorder, the borderline
Washington.Reprint requests should be addressed personality disorder, chronic factitious illness, malingering, conversion
to Dr. Wayne Katon, Department of Psychiatry and
Behavioral Sciences, RP-10, University of disorders and hypochondriasis [2]. Somatization frequently expresses
Washington, Seattle, Washington 98195. Manu- social as well as emotional distress, but may at times represent only
script accepted August 12, 1981. social problems [3].

January 1992 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72 127


DEPREsslob AN0 SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

Somatization is one of the most common and trou- the prevalence of mental disorder meeting Research
blesome problems physicians in primary care face. Diagnostic Criteria to be 26.7 percent with depressive
Studies have repeatedly demonstrated that as many as disorders (major, intermittent and minor) and anxiety
50 percent of patients utilizing primary care clinics (phobic and generalized), again representing more than
actually have psychosocial precipitants as opposed to 80 percent of the cases. Major depressive disorder
biomedical ones as the main cause of their clinic visits alone made up 21.7 percent of the psychiatric diag-
[4-61. Further, Reigier et al. [7] have demonstrated that noses. In a study of 300 medical inpatients and outpa-
most people with emotional disorders are seen and tients in a general hospital (utilizing the Beck Depression
treated in primary care settings rather than by psychi- Inventory [ 181 and the Zung Self-Rating Scale [ 191)
atrists. Goldberg and Blackwell [8] have pointed out that Raft et al. 1201 estimated the rate of depression to be
in addition to the substantial numbers of patients whose 35 percent. Schuman and Ramesar and their co-
emotional problems are recognized by their physicians, workers 121,221, in two longitudinal studies using a
there are many (termed the “hidden psychiatric mor- structured interview and a self-rating checklist, found
bidity”) whose emotional disorders go unrecognized. the incidence of affective disorder (depression, anxi-
These are predominantly patients who present with ety-depression) in a large family practice to be five to
somatic symptoms and are treated symptomatically. six cases per 100 patients per year.
These same observations have been made by Lipsitt Patients with depression and anxiety in these studies
[9] and Jacobs et al. [ lo]. Not only does mental dis- experienced substantial physical symptoms and thus
order commonly present as physical disorder in primary often found their way to medical practitioners for con-
care, but also studies have revealed that early detection sultation and treatment. Yet two studies [23,24] re-
is especially beneficial for the more severe disorders vealed that primary care physicians diagnose depres-
[ 111. In the case of affective disorders, early detection sion in only 0.5 percent to 4.5 percent of their cases;
and treatment not only potentially shorten the course, thus, significant numbers of cases appear to go undi-
as there is highly effective treatment with psychother- agnosed. This was well demonstrated in Nielson and
apy, medication and electroconvulsive therapy, but also Williams’ [25] recent study of depression in ambulatory
spare the patient unnecessary physical investigations medical patients in a prepaid health program. Utilizing
that tend to reinforce hypochondriacal symptom pat- the Beck Depression Inventory [ 181 as well as psy-
terns and carry a significant risk of iatrogenesis. chiatric interview, they found 12.2 percent of patients
There are several studies that indicate the incidence to have at least mild depression and 5.5 percent mod-
and prevalence of affective disorders in primary care erate depression; the primary care physicians failed to
patients. Although most suffer from a lack of diagnostic diagnose depression in 50 percent of the cases.
clarity, there is some correlation as to the prevalence Goldberg [ 141 has shown that more than half the
of depression and anxiety despite the utilization of dif- patients presenting to primary care physicians had
fering diagnostic techniques. We give statistics for both significant somatic symptoms as part of their depressive
depression and anxiety because they often coexist in disorder. Too often the patient’s physical complaint is
the same patient. In a large computer-based study in treated symptomatically whereas the underlying disease
Virginia of 526,196 patient visits to 118 family practi- is left untreated. The main problem here is the inability
tioners, depression and anxiety constituted 86.8 percent of the physician to conceptualize depression as existing
of the psychiatric problems seen by these physicians unless the patient perceives an affective state and re-
[ 121. In two studies of more than 2,000 primary care ports it to the physician. Thus, a patient who has not
patients in Philadelphia [ 131 and London [ 141, a family developed the language to label and report his emo-
physician in the first study and a psychiatrist in the latter tional states, or who utilizes defenses or coping styles
each found that 87 percent of the combined 492 pa- that minimize affects or who believes his problem is a
tients with a psychiatric diagnosis had an affective physical one will not be recognized as depressed by
disorder, i.e., depression, mixed anxiety and depression, many physicians. Perhaps the prototype is the patient
anxiety state and affective psychosis. Thus, these with chronic pain. Sternbach [ 261 has shown that 100
studies indicate that depression and anxiety constitute patients with low back pain averaged two standard
the two major psychiatric syndromes seen by primary deviations above the norm on the depression subscale
care physicians but do not indicate the prevalence or of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory.
incidence of depression. The recent study by Hoeper Blumer et al. [27] further convincingly demonstrated
et al. [ 151 did reveal the prevalence of mental disorder that a large percentage of patients with pain are de-
in a primary care clinic population utilizing a standard pressed and respond to tricyclic antidepressants with
psychiatric interview, the Schedule for Affective Dis- partial to complete alleviation of pain and depression.
orders and Schizophrenia [ 161, which is based on Re- Most of the patients with “pain” overtly denied the af-
search Diagnostic Criteria [ 171. Hoeper’s group found fective component of depression despite having all of

128 January 1982 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

the vegetative symptoms, i.e., insomnia, anorexia, dominant affects. Moreover, characteristic somatic
anhedonia, weight loss, loss of libido, or minimized symptoms occur including anorexia, weight loss, in-
depressed mood as being only “secondary” to their somnia or hypersomnia, psychomotor agitation or re-
physical pain. For many, this belief and its associated tardation, decreased energy and libido, decreased ability
treatment expectations are shared by their physicians to think and concentrate, dry mouth, constipation,
who frequently reify “pain” as the disorder and tech- headaches and backaches [32]. Chronic pain also
nologic intervention targeted at physical symptoms as seems to be a common somatic complaint. Watts [33]
the treatment. reported that 27 percent of his depressive patients in
Unfortunately, the end result of the misconception a primary care clinic presented with pain as a com-
that depressed patients will perceive their affective plaint, while Von Knorring [34] found that 60 percent
state and report it to physicians is that many patients of his patients who presented with depression had ex-
who have chronic pain and other patients who recog- perienced some form of somatic pain. Ward et al. [35]
nize only the somatic symptoms of depression fre- described a 100 percent incidence of pain complaints
quently receive treatments that result in iatrogenic harm in a study of depressed psychiatric patients. Hill and
and do not receive specific treatment for depression. Blendis [36], in a study of persons with persistent
The average patient visiting a pain clinic usually pre- “nonorganic” abdominal pain, and Turkington [37], in
sents with several unnecessary surgical procedures and patients with diabetic neuropathy, reported a high in-
is often addicted to minor tranquilizers and/or narcotics cidence of depression. Ward et al. [ 351, Hill and Blendis
[28,29]. Because the hypochondriacal patterns many [36] and Turkington [37] all found marked alleviation
patients use to cope with dysphoric affect and social of both pain and depression after treatment with tricyclic
stress are reinforced by physician interest, lengthy antidepressants.
workups and costly treatment, eventually secondary Along with the characteristic affective change, at
gain occurs that further sustains these symptoms. least four of these somatic and cognitive symptoms
Secondary gain commonly takes the form of disability (suicidal ideation and thoughts of self-reproach,
payments and change in family systems that enable worthlessness, hopelessness, guilt) must be present
patients to avoid stressful situations like work and to every day for a period of two weeks to fit the diagnostic
fulfill dependency needs by becoming passive recipi- criteria for the major depressive disorder [ 11. Most
ents of care by others. Other secondary gains include dysphoric individuals not demonstrating these symp-
the sanctioning of failure, “addiction” to the health care toms fall into the categories of dysthymic disorder
system for social support and the manipulation of social (formerly depressive neurosis or reactive depression),
relationships. Most physicians are aware of the extreme cyclothymic disorder, or adjustment disorder [ 11.
difficulty that is basic to working with patients in whom In physiologic perspective, depression is a psycho-
substantial secondary gain is an active disincentive to biologic disorder with multi-level impairment in brain
getting well. neurotransmitters [38], hormonal systems [39], the
automatic nervous system [ 391, circadian rhythms and
rapid-eye-movement (REM) sleep [40]. As we dem-
DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION: A COGNITIVE
MODEL onstrate later on, depression is also a social phenom-
enon; hence it fits Engel’s [41] model of disease as
The major depressive syndrome, besides its social always expressing biopsychosocial interrelation-
significance, has cognitive, affective and somatic ships.
components. Patients with depression have charac- The perceptions of a group of depressive patients
teristic cognitive schemata described and validated by about their illness vary widely. Many seem to selectively
Beck’s studies in which they recurrently interpret perceive various vegetative symptoms and therefore
themselves, their experiences and the future in an idi- seek the help of physicians for somatic complaints.
osyncratic manner [30,31]. For instance, a person with Singh [42] noted that 65 percent of a depressed out-
depression tends to see himself as defective, inade- patient group presented with a physical complaint.
quate, diseased or deprived-he attributes his un- Widmer and Cadoret [43,44] noted that, compared with
pleasant experiences to psychological, moral or control subjects, in the records of 154 depressed family
physical defects in himself. He believes he is undesir- practice patients in the seven months prior to the di-
able and worthless because of these putative defects, agnosis of depression, there was (1) an increase in the
and he underestimates and criticizes himself because number of patient-initiated visits, (2) an increased in-
of them. cidence of hospitalizations, (3) an increased number of
The affective state of depression is similar to sadness presenting complaints of three types: ill-defined
but is longer lasting and more pervasive. In some pa- “functional” complaints, pain of undertermined etiology
tients, irritability and loss of interest may be the pre- and complaints of tension and anxiety.

January 1982 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72 129


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

I
DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION
__-___

DEPRESSION

\\
\\
\\
‘.
.N --em__
Y
Social consequences
I e seconddry gam

Figure 1. Chart of the components of depression and somatization

Other patients present to psychiatrists, psychologists vocabulary for emotions the patient has learned and on
and other physicians with complaints about their so- the cognitive mechanisms he has developed to manage
matic as well as affective symptoms. There seems to feelings. Similarly, perception is the basis for feedback
be a spectrum here with some patients rigidly adhering molding illness experience in terms of social conse-
to a somatic model of their illness, others feeling as quences (Figure 1).
though it is primarily psychological due to the affective
and cognitive changes and the vast majority somewhere SOCIOCULTURAL FACTORS
in between, perceiving both the somatic and affective
components and not sure which is primary. Historically, somatization of depression was the norm
The patient’s subjective experience of the depressive in Western society. Patients did not go to indigenous
syndrome is influenced by culture, past family experi- healers and physicians with existential depressions.
ence, his cognitive coping mechanisms and his current They did not complain of mood or affective changes but
social environment, i.e., medical care, work-disability, presented with somatic and vegetative complaints. The
family and larger social network and the sociopolitical development of “psychological mindedness” is largely
system (Figure 1). The key concept here is cognition. a 20th-century phenomenon in Western culture. A
Cognition refers both to the content of thought and to majority of cultures in the world even today do not value
thinking processes [45,46]. It includes ways of per- and often stigmatize the expression and perception of
ceiving the environment and processing information emotions. In many cultures, stoicism is considered a
from it, problem-solving strategies, attitudes and attri- virtue. Thus, the ability to perceive an affective state
butions and the mechanisms and content of recall, and report it to a physician seems to be a very recent
memory and communication [46]. development. Cross-cultural evidence points to the fact
The affective component of depression can only be that depression occurs in all cultures, but the form it
known to the patient through cognition: perceiving, la- takes varies [47,48]. Cross-cultural studies show the
beling, ordering and giving value to internal feelings. core of vegetative symptoms of depression are similar
Without cognitive perception of the feeling component, but the cognitive changes like intense guilt and self-
the depressive disorder exists as a somatic syndrome depreciation seen in Western culture are absent or di-
expressed by vegetative symptoms. The ability to minished, as is the expression of depressed affect
perceive the associated affective state depends on the [47,48]. Racy [49] studied depression in the Middle

130 January 1992 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

East and discovered that traditional Arabs presented found that the Yoruba had no words for depression as
with somatic complaints such as gastrointestinal an emotional state or a single concept of depression as
symptoms, loss of appetite and decreased weight. Guilt a syndrome or disorder. Tseng and Hsu [ 591 reported
and self-depreciation were rare. However, as Arabs a similar observation among Chinese. Leff [60] re-
became Westernized, the form of depression became viewing the cross-cultural literature concludes that the
closer to that seen in the West. Pfeiffer [50] reviewed language of emotions in most societies evolves from
40 reports of depression in 22 non-European countries vague somatic terms, conveying both somatic and af-
and found that the vegetative symptoms were always fective complaints, to more highly differentiated somatic
found, including insomnia, decreased libido, decreased and affective complaints, with fine effective distinctions
appetite and weight loss, but self-depreciation, hope- only as a late stage associated with modernization and
lessness and guilt were rarely found. Mezzich and Raab Westernization. Benoist et al. [61] studied French-
[51] compared depressive symptoms and their so- Canadians and found that only 5 percent labeled a case
ciocultural context in a Peruvian sample of 93 depres- involving sadness, insomnia, fatigue and loss of interest
sive patients and a United States sample of 64 de- as depression. The defined depression as a “nervous
pressive patients. A basic common core of depressive condition,” like a nervous breakdown with the emphasis
signs and symptoms was found in both samples (in- on “nerves” as a physical problem. Kleinman [62]
cluding vegetative, cognitive and affective symptoms), found that of 100 patients suffering “neurasthenia” in
but more complaints and higher scores on somatic the outpatient psychiatric clinic at the Hunan Medical
symptoms were found in the Peruvian group and higher College in China, 87 had major depressive disorder; all
scores on suicidal manifestations were noted in the presented with physical symptoms as their chief
United States group. Somatization also correlates in the problem, and most regarded their disorder as neuro-
United States with lower socioeconomic class, tradi- logic, owing to nervous exhaustion.
tionally oriented ethnic groups that discourage and Secondly, in many cultures, there are strong sanc-
disparage undisguised expression of emotions, blue- tions against talking about and perceiving emotional
collar workers, rural living and lower educational levels states like depression, and specific cognitive coping
[2,3,52-561. styles are utilized that deflect affective complaints along
It appears then that patients in most non-Western channels of physical symptoms. These coping devices
cultures still present with physical complaints as the block introspection as well as direct expression of
primary symptom of depression and that even in feelings. They place dysphoric affects in a nonpsy-
Western culture it is probably only recently that patients chological idiom. Unlike the “internal” idiom used by
present with selfdepreciating thoughts, hopelessness Westerners, it is an “external” idiom that communicates
and helplessness, and are able to perceive and articu- affects indirectly as somatic, situational or dissociated
late depression as a discreet affect. At present, primary metaphors.
care physicians, not psychiatrists, still see most of the Chinese patients often present with a symptom
patients with mental illness [7,57], and the Goldberg hsing-thing pu hao, a term that refers to general emo-
[ 141 and Widmer and Cadoret [43,44] studies of pri- tional upset [63]. This vague term functions to reduce
mary care clinic populations suggest that most de- the intensity of anxiety and depression by keeping the
pressed patients somatize. Therefore, in Western so- emotions undifferentiated. Leff [64] studied Chinese
ciety as well as in most other cultures, somatization and Nigerian subjects and found that they did not dis-
represents a powerful method of coping with psycho- tinguish between anxiety and depression. Thus, in some
social distress. Symptoms are communications of cultures, patients use undifferentiated terms to cope
distress, and in many cultures depression connotes with depressive states. Further evidence was provided
weakness, moral culpability and loss of face; besides, by Tanaka-Matsumi and Marsella [65], who compared
there are no psychiatrists and mental illness is highly word associations to the equivalent words depression
stigmatized. Moreover, physical complaints are legiti- and yuutsu (depression in Japanese) in three groups of
mate cues for obtaining care, love, sympathy, time off patients: Japanese nationals, Japanese Americans and
and time out-they possess social efficacy. Therefore, Caucasian Americans. The Japanese nationals asso-
it is not surprising that the affective component of de- ciated more external referent terms such as rain and
pression is minimized or overtly suppressed, denied or cloud and somatic references such as headache to the
perhaps not experienced, and the vegetative or somatic word yuutsu. Japanese Americans and Caucasian
experiences are highlighted. Americans associated predominantly mood-state terms
Cultures influence depression in three major ways. such as sad and lonely to the word depression. The
In many cultures, there are essentially no words to de- association to external referents by Japanese nationals
scribe internal emotional states. For example, when suggests that they experience depressive feelings
Leighton et al. [58] studied the Yoruba of Nigeria, they “externally” in an indirect fashion as related to imper-

January 1982 The American Journal of Medicine Volume72 131


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

sonal Nature and its culturally established emotional CHILDHOOD EXPERIENCE


referents.
Other related mechanisms that cope with emotions Each family can be visualized as a distinct subcultural
like depression include minimization to overt denial, group in itself that has different norms and rules to in-
dissociation (affects are separated from consciousness terpret and cope with many problems, including un-
by mechanisms like trance states and hysterical be- pleasant emotions. Family therapists have demon-
havior) and somatization. Minimization is the process strated the importance of these rules and the family
in which the intensity of emotion is suppressed by structure in the family’s emotional homeostasis. There
minimizing its significance. Somatizers with depression is most likely a spectrum in Western families between
often use this coping device, with statements like, “Yes, families that cannot experience and express feelings
I’m depressed, but anyone with this pain would be.” This psychologically and those that perceive feelings and
process extends to frank denial whereby emotions are express them easily and readily. We would postulate
simply not acknowledged. When examined, patients that it is in families that have not developed psycho-
with somatized depression may appear visibly de- logical language for articulating unpleasant emotions
pressed, have all the vegetative symptoms of depres- that we would be most apt to see the selective focus on
sion and have family members who describe a change the somatic manifestations of depression. Somatization
in affect, yet they vehemently deny perception of de- is obviously one strategy that is used to cope with de-
pressive mood. It is important to point out that a group pression and appears to be one of the more powerful
of these patients seem not to be just using denial or ways to elicit nur-turance and caring from families in all
conscious suppression but appear not to perceive and cultures
label internal emotional states. Some may even lack Families are the units of the larger culture that so-
a language to express such states. Dissociation points cialize children to discern the difference between
to a whole range of coping mechanisms in which affect physical and emotional pain. They also teach a value
is separated from consciousness, cognition or behavior system to the child about the appropriateness of ex-
or the specific stimuli provoking it. This dissociated pressing emotions, and they directly model as well as
affect is expressed in isolation, most usually in a cul- reinforce sanctioned types of interpretive systems,
turally sanctioned way. Kleinman [63] has found that coping mechanisms and defenses to handle emo-
in Taiwan one sees angry, sad and anxious feelings tions.
expressed via trance states or hysterical behavior or At young ages, children do not appear to distinguish
displaced to other subjects, i.e., socially legitimate between emotional and physical distress [69]. It is the
anger at strangers and deviants. reaction of the child’s caretakers in labeling, classifying,
The third effect culture may have on the perception explaining and evaluating a child’s distress that teaches
and expression of depression is noted when there are the child to distinguish various mood states and separate
culturally idiosyncratic explanations for certain affective somatic from emotional ills. In many families, care is
states. For instance, in Central and South America in provided for somatic ills but not for emotional ills. In
people of Latin0 ancestry, there exists a disease called these families, emotional feelings are seldom labeled,
susto in which symptoms include weakness, decreased and there may be strong negative sanctions against
appetite, anxiety, insomnia, motor retardation and de- directly expressing these emotions. Zborowski [ 701
creased libido [47,66,67]. In our culture, this constel- stressed in his description of the “old American” family
lation of symptoms might be labeled as agitated de- the tendency of the mother to teach the child to take
pression; in Latin0 culture, it is labeled susto. Susto pain “like a man,” not to be a sissy and not to cry. Such
means “soul loss,” and the connotation of this disease training according to Zborowski does not discourage
in terms of treatment is that it has a supernatural or use of the physician but implies that such use will be
religious cause, i.e., breaking taboos, curses or based on physical needs not psychological. The child
witchcraft. Just as American cultural beliefs about de- learns quickly by this differential reinforcement how to
pression may lead individuals not only to perceive vague elicit nurturance from a caregiver, i.e., complaining
symptoms as a sign of depression but also to amplify about a headache rather than about being sad or de-
or “imagine” appropriate complaints, perhaps in susto pressed. Mechanic [3] has also emphasized that
and other culture-bound disorders, symptoms are learning influences the tendency of males as compared
construed and constructed in keeping with local beliefs with females to take more risks, to be less expressive
about how the disease should “be felt.” Thus, culture about illness, to appear more stoical and to seek
provides the sick person with illness beliefs, views medical care less frequently.
about how the body works, dominant metaphors and At early ages, children define being healthy as feeling
idioms that organize his interpretation of bodily change good, being able to do things they want and being happy.
[681. Feeling unhealthy is associated with not being able to

132 January 1992 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

engage in desired activities, and feeling moody and children. Apley et al. [75] also noted that children with
weak [7 I]. Parents identify illness in their children by recurrent fGnctional complaints were commonly
behavioral cues. Change in a child’s activity is an es- copying their symptoms from another family member,
sential cue and the most frequently noted indicators of usually the mother. Kreitman [74] noted that for the
illness are irritability, listlessness, crankiness and apathy patients with persistent somatic complaints and de-
[ 721. Parents differ in their perception of such cues, in pression, the internal statement “I am ill” might signify
their ability to identify illness in their children and in their “I am in some kind of distress but have learned no
own reaction [73]. Thus, a parent’s responses may language other than my body by which to convey this.”
create various degrees of reinforcement as well as Indeed, body language may be an effective way to in-
provide definitions to attribute meaning to internal directly express dysphoria among family members
states. [ 691. exquisitely sensitive to such bodily cues.
Thus, depending on parental attitudes, some children Minuchin et al. [76], Bruch [77] and Pallazzoli [78]
get a strong message that talking about emotions per in their work with psychosomatic families, i.e., families
se or about certain ones is taboo and such parents may with psychological conflict that is expressed via a
often not nurture and aid the child in discerning the member with somatic symptoms, point out the frequent
differences between mood states and physical symp- lack of a language of emotions, the inability of these
toms and talking about them. In such families, physical families to work out conflict verbally and the strong
symptoms may become the socially legitimate way to denial by all family members that psychological prob-
receive and accept nurturance. lems exist. Somatization by the child seems to stabilize
Several studies lend support to this hypothesis. a precarious family equilibrium, often at the expense
Krietman et a]. [74] showed that patients who pre- of the affected family member. It again serves to ex-
sented with persistent somatic symptoms and were ternalize family emotional problems like marital conflict
eventually recognized as depressed (due to their classic so that somatic treatments are often initially sought.
vegetative symptoms and response to tricydic anti- Many of the affected children in the case of anorexia
depressants) differed from patients who presented nervosa go on to have chronic patterns of somatic
primarily with depression in that significantly more complaints in multiple body systems. The learned and
mothers of those in the former group had the identical reinforced pattern of interpreting and coping with
somatic symptoms. The depression subgroup also had emotional problems via somatic complaints continues
significantly more classic psychosomatic illness as into adulthood [79].

REFERENCES
1. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and statistical 12. Marsland DW, Wood M. Mayo F: Content of family practice:
manual of mental disorders, 3rd ed. Washington, D.C.: a data bank for patient care, curriculum and research in
American Psychiatric Association, 1980; 210-218. family practice-526,196 patient problems. J Fam Pratt
2. Barsky AJ: Patients who amplify bodily sensations. Ann Intern 1976: 3: 25-68.
Med 1979; 91: 63-70. _ 13. Hesbacher PT, Rickels K, Goldberg 0: Social factors and
3. Mechanic D: Social psychologic factors affecting the pre- neurotic symptoms in family practice. Am J Public Health
sentation of bodily complaints. N Engl J Med 1972; 286: 1975; 65(2): 148-155.
1132-l 139. 14. Goldberg D: Detection and assessment of emotional disorders
4. Stoeckle JD, Zola IK, Davidson GE: The quantity and signifi- in a primary care setting. Int J Ment Health 1979; 8: 30-
cance of psychological distress in medical patients. J 48.
Chronic Dis 1964; 17: 959-970. 15. Hoeper EW, Nyczi GR, Cleary PD, Reigier DA, Goldberg ID:
5. Roberts BH, Norton NM: Prevalence of psychiatric illness in Estimated prevalence of RDC mental disorder in primary
a medical outpatient clinic. N Engl J Med 1952; 245: 82. care. Int J Ment Health 1979; 8: 6-15.
6. Mannucci M, Friedman SM. Kaufman MR: Survey of patients 16. Endicott J, Spitzer RL: The schedule for affective disorders
who have been attending nonpsychiatric outpatient de- and schizophrenia-lifetime version. New York: Biometrics
partment services for ten years or longer. J Mt Sinai Hosp Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1978.
1961; 28: 32. 17. Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E: Research diagnostic criteria:
7. Regier DA, Goldberg ID, Taube CA: The de facto U.S. mental rationale and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35:
health services system: a public health perspective. Arch 773-782.
Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35(6): 685-693. 18. Beck AT, Beamesderfer A: Assessment of depression: the
8. Goldberg D, Blackwell B: Psychiatric illness in general medical depression inventory. Mod Probl Pharmacopsychiatry 1974;
practice. Br Med J 1970: 2: 439-443. .-. .__.
7: 151-169
9. Lipsitt DR: Medical and .psychologic characteristics of 19. Zung WW: A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psychiatry
“crocks.” Psychiatr Med 1970; 1: 15-25. 1965; 12: 63.
10. Jacobs TJ, Gogelson S, Charles E: Depression ratings in hy- 20. Raft D, Davidson J, Toomey TC, Spencer RD, Lewis BF: In-
pochondria. NY State J Med 1968; 68: 3119-3122. patient and outpatient patterns of psychotropic drug pre-
11. Johnstone A, Goldberg D: Psychiatric screening in general scribing by non-psychiatrist physicians. Am J Psychiatry
practice. Lancet 1976; I: 605-608. 1975; 132: 1309-1312.

January 1982 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72 133


DEPRESSION AND SOMATIZATION-- KATON ET AL

21. Schuman SH, Kurtzman SB, Fisher JV, Groh MJ, Poston JH: psychology vol 5: psychopathology. Boston: Allyn and
Three approaches to the recognition of affective disorders Bacon, 1980.
in family practice: clinical, pharmacological and self-rating 48. Marsella AJ: Cross-cultural studies of mental disorders. in:
scales. J Fam Pratt 1978; 7: 705711. Marsella AJ, Tharp R, Ciborowski T, eds. Perspectives on
22. Ramesar S, Schuman SH. Groh MJ, Poston JH: Detection of cross-cultural psychology. New York: Academic Press,
affective disorders in family practice: 126 assessments. 1979.
J Fam Pratt 1980; 10: 819-828. 49. Racy J: Psychiatry in the Arab east. Acta Psychiatr Stand
23. Rawnsey K: Epidemiology of affective disorders, In: Coppen 1970; suppl21: 1-171.
A, Walk A, eds. Recent developments in affective disorders. 50. Pfeiffer W: The symptomatology of depression viewed
British Journal of Psychiatry special suppl 2. London: transculturally. Transcult Psychiatr Res Rev 1968; 5:
Headley Brothers, 1968: 27-36. 121-123.
24. Reifler B, Okomoto J, Heidrich F, lnui TS: Recognition of de- 51. Mezzich JE, Raab ES: Depressive symptomatology across
pression in a family medicine training program. J Fam Pratt the Americas. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980; 37: 818-823.
1979; 9: 623-628. 52. Pilowski I: Primary and secondary hypochondriasis. Acta
25. Nielson AC, Williams TA: Depression in ambulatory medical Psychiatr Stand 1970; 46: 273-285.
patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980; 37: 999-1004. 53. Bart PB: Social structure and vocabularies of discomfort: what
26. Sternbach RA: Pain patients, traits and treatment, New York: happened to female hysteria? J Health Sot Behav 1969;
Academic Press, 1974. 9: 188-193.
27. Blumer D, Heilbronn N, Pedraza E, Pope G: Systematic 54. Srole L, Langner TS, Michael ST, Opler MK, Rennie TAC:
treatment of chronic pain with antidepressants. Henry Ford Mental health in the metropolis: the midtown Manhattan
Hosp Med J 1980; 28. 15-21. study. New York: McGraw-Hill Book, 1962.
28. Newman R, Pointer J, Seres J: A therapeutic milieu for chronic 55. Hollingshead AB, Redlich FC: Social class and mental illness:
pain patients. J Human Stress 1978; 4: 8-12. a community study. New York: John Wiley and Sons,
29. Chapman CR. Psychological aspects of pain patient man- 1958.
agement. Arch Surg 1977; 112: 767-772. 56. Lerner J, Noy P: Somatic complaints in psychiatric disorders:
30. Kovacs M, Beck AT: Maladaptive cognitive structures in de- social and cultural factors. Int J Sot Psychiatry 1968; 14:
pression Am J Psychiatry 1978; 135: 525-533 145-150.
31. Beck AT, Rush JA. Shaw B, Emery G: Cognitive therapy of 57. Climent CE, Diop BSM, Harding TW, lbrahim HHA, Ladvido-
depression. New York: Guilford Press, 1979. lgnacio L, Wig NN: Mental Health in primary care. WHO
32. Klerman GL: Affective disorders. In: Nicholi AM, ed. The Chronicle 1980; 34: 231-236.
Harvard guide to modern psychiatry. Cambridge, Massa- 58. Leighton A, Lambo T, Hughes C, Leighton D, Murphy J, Macklin
chusetts: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press, 1978; D: Psychiatric disorder among the Yoruba. Ithaca, New
253-281. York: Cornell University Press, 1963.
33. Watts CAH:Depressive disorders in the community. Bristol, 59. Tseng W, Hsu J: Chinese culture, personality formation and
England: John Wright and Sons, 1966. mental illness. Int J Sot Psychiatry 1969; 16: 5-14.
34. Von Knorring L: The experience of pain in depressed patients. 60. Leff J: Transcultural psychiatry. New York: Marcel Dekker (in
Neuropsychobiology 1975; 1: 155-165. press).
35. Ward NG, Bloom VL, Friedel RO: The effectiveness of tricyclic 61. Benoist A, Roussin M, Fredette M, Rousseau S: Depression
antidepressants in the treatment of coexisting pain and among French Canadians in Montreal. Transcult Psychiatr
depression. Pain 1979; 7: 331-341. Rev 1965; 2: 52-54.
36. Hill OW, Blendis L: Physical and psychological evaluation of 62. Kleinman A: Neurasthenia and depression in the Hunan
“non-organic” abdominal pain. Gut 1967; 8: 221. Medical College, Changsha, Hunan, People’s Republic of
37. Turkington RW: Depression masquerading as diabetic neu- China (in press).
ropathy. JAMA 1980; 243: 1147-1150. 63. Kleinman A: Patients and healers in the context of culture: an
38. Goodwin FK, Sack RL: Affective disorders: the catechotamine exploration of the borderland between anthropology,
hypothesis revisited. In: Usdin E, Synder S, eds. Frontiers medicine and psychiatry. Berkeley: University of California
in catecholamine research. New York: Pergamon Press, Press, 1980.
1973; 1157-1164. 64. Leff J: Culture and the differentiation of emotional states. Br
39. De la Fuente JR: Endocrine changes in depressive illness. J Psychiatry 1973; 123: 299-306.
Psychiatr Ann 1979; 9: 37-50. 65. Tanaka-Matsumi J, Marsella AJ: Cross-cultural variations in
40. Vogel GW, Vogel F, McAbee RS, Thurmond AJ: Improvement the phenomenological experience of depression I. Word
of depression by REM sleep deprivation: new findings and association studies. J Cross-Cult Psycho1 1976; 7: 379-
a theory. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1980; 37: 247-253. 396.
41. Engel GL: Clinical application of the biopsychosocial model. 66. Kiev A: Curandismo: Mexican-American folk psychiatry. New
Am J Psychiatry 1980; 37: 535-544. York: Free Press, 1968.
42. Singh G: The diagnosis of depression. Punjab Med J 1968; 18: 67. Rubel AJ: The epidemiology of a folk illness: susto in Hispanic
53-59. America. Ethnology 1964; 3: 268-284.
43. Widmer RB, Cadoret RJ: Depression in primary care: changes 68. White GM: Cultural explanations of illness and adjustment: a
in pattern of patient visits and complaints during a devel- comparative study of American and Hong Kong Chinese
oping depression. J Fam Pratt 1978; 7: 293-302. students. Presented at the 79th annual meeting of the
44. Widmer RB, Cadoret RJ: Depression in family practice: American Anthropological Association, Ethnography of
changes in pattern of patient visits and complaints during Health Care Decisions, Washington, D.C., December
subsequent developing depressions. J Fam Pratt 1979; 1980.
9:1017-1021. 69. Mechanic D: Development of psychological distress among
45. Broadbent DE: Cognitive psychology: introduction. Br Med Bull young adults. Gen Psychiatry 1979; 36: 1233- 1239.
1979; 27: 191-194. 70. Zborowski M: Cultural components in responses to pain. J Sot
46. Neisser U: Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Cen- Issues 1952; 8(4): 16-30.
tury-crofts, 1967. 71. Natapoff J: Children’s view of health: a developmental study.
47. Marsella AJ: Depressive affect and disorder across cultures. Am J Public Health 1978; 68: 995-1000.
in: Triandis H, Draguns J. eds. Handbook of cross-cultural 72. Mechanic D: Religion, religiosity and illness behavior. The

134 January 1992 The American Journal of Medicine Volume 72


DEPRESSION AND SDMATIZATION-KATON ET AL.

special case of the Jews. Hum Organ 1963; 22: 202- 76. Minuchin S. Rosman BL. Baker L: Psychosomatic families:
208. anorexia nervosa in context. Cambridge, Massachusetts:
73. Mechanic D: The influence of mothers on their children’s Harvard University Press, 1978.
health attitudes and behavior. Pediatrics 1964; 33: 444- 77. Bruch H: Obesity, anorexia and the person within. New York:
453. Basic Books, 1973.
74. Krietman N, Swinsbury P, Pearce K, Costain WR: Hypo- 78. Pallazzoli MS: Self-starvation. New York: Jason Aronson.
chondriasis and depression in outpatients at a general 1978.
hospital. Br J Psychiatry 1965; 111: 607-615. 79. Theander S: Anorexia nervosa: a psychiatric investigation of
75. Apley J, MacKeith R, Meadow R: The child and his symptoms: 94 female patients. Acta Psychiatr Stand 1970; suppl
a.comprehensive approach. Oxford: Blackwell, 1978. 214.

January 1982 The Amerkan Journal of Medlclne Volume 72 135

You might also like