You are on page 1of 6

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403 doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2012.00774.

Somatic markers mediate the effect of serotonin


transporter gene polymorphisms on Iowa Gambling
Task

A. C. Miu∗,† , L. G. Crişan† , A. Chiş‡ , yielded higher immediate gains but also higher future losses.
L. Ungureanu§ , B. Drug㶠and R. Vulturar∗,‡ In healthy controls, the preference for long-term advanta-
geous decks in IGT was paralleled by the development of skin
† Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of conductance responses (SCR) prior to the selection of cards
(Bechara et al . 1997). Anticipatory SCR were higher before
Psychology, Babeş-Bolyai University, ‡ Department of Cell and
picking a card from the disadvantageous decks in healthy
Molecular Biology, Iuliu Haţieganu University of Medicine and
controls, but not in patients with ventromedial prefrontal
Pharmacy, § Department of Dermatology, Iuliu Haţieganu
cortex lesions (Bechara et al . 1999). These discoveries put
University of Medicine and Pharmacy, and ¶ Institute of
IGT in the center of the somatic marker hypothesis (Bechara
Biological Research, Cluj-Napoca, Romania
*Corresponding authors: Dr A. C. Miu, 37 Republicii, 400015
et al . 2000; Damasio 1996), which has argued that emotions,
as reflected by anticipatory SCR during IGT, guide decision
Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania. E-mail: andreimiu@psychology.ro;
making. Although this theory has been debated (Bechara
Dr R. Vulturar, 37 Republicii, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania.
E-mail: romanavulturar@yahoo.co.uk
et al . 2005; Dunn et al . 2006; Guillaume et al . 2009; Maia
& McClelland 2004), it remains highly influential in cognitive
psychology, cognitive neuroscience and neuroeconomics.
Recent interest in IGT has concerned the neurochemical
This study investigated whether somatic markers medi- mechanisms that underlie optimal performance, with impli-
ate the effect of serotonin transporter genotype on Iowa cations for understanding the decision-making dysfunctions
Gambling Task (IGT) performance. Participants (N = 135) in various neurological and psychiatric conditions (Bechara
were genotyped for the insertion/deletion and single- 2004; Verdejo-Garcia & Bechara 2009). It has been hypoth-
nucleotide (rs25531) polymorphisms in the promoter esized that the biasing action of somatic markers (e.g.
region of the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR ). anticipatory SCR) on IGT performance may involve changes
The results of mediation analyses indicated that skin
in neurotransmitter release in cortical and subcortical neural
conductance responses that anticipated IGT card selec-
structures associated with executive functioning, working
tions partially (i.e. 42% of the total effect) mediated the
memory, emotions and the implementation of behavioral
effect of genotype on IGT performance. In comparison
with high-functioning 5-HTTLPR genotypes, the low- decisions (Bechara & Damasio 2005; Li et al . 2010). One
functioning genotypes were associated with higher total of the candidate neurotransmitters involved in IGT is sero-
IGT scores. This suggests that the higher synaptic avail- tonin. It was shown that the administration of fluvoxamine,
ability of serotonin, associated with the low-functioning a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI), improved the
5-HTTLPR genotypes, may confer differential suscepti- selection of advantageous choices in IGT, and this effect
bility to decision making under risk, and that almost half was stronger in the second part of the task (Bechara et al .
of this effect is explained by facilitated somatic markers 2001). This part of IGT is thought to reflect decision making
during IGT. under risk because, by this stage, participants have probably
acquired some subjective sense of the outcome probabili-
Keywords: decision making, emotion, endophenotype, ties associated with the different decks of cards (Bechara
5-HTTLPR, somatic marker hypothesis
et al . 1997). Therefore, serotonin stimulation by SSRI may
specifically facilitate decision making under risk in IGT, but
Received 24 November 2011, revised 17 January 2012 and
12 February 2012, accepted for publication 16 February 2012 this hypothesis should be further investigated considering
that the preliminary findings of Bechara et al . (2001) have
not been replicated. However, it was also observed that
enhancing serotonin activity in humans by administering
The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) is extensively used in basic and its precursor, 5-hydroxytryptophan, reduced decision-making
clinical research on decision making. The original success of biases (i.e. the reflection effect) in a task similar to IGT (Mur-
IGT was due to its sensitivity to ‘myopia for the future’, a type phy et al . 2009). In contrast, tryptophan depletion in rats,
of functional impairment displayed by patients with ventro- which effectively reduced brain serotonin synthesis, impaired
medial prefrontal cortex lesions (Bechara et al . 1994). These decision-making performance in a murine analog of IGT (Koot
patients failed to develop a preference for the decks of cards et al . 2012). Psychopharmacological studies in animals and
yielding lower immediate gains but also smaller future losses humans seem to agree on the involvement of serotonin
during IGT, and persevered in choosing from the decks that in decision making, but it is yet unclear whether increased

398 © 2012 The Authors


Genes, Brain and Behavior © 2012 Blackwell Publishing Ltd and International Behavioural and Neural Genetics Society
5-HTTLPR, somatic markers and decision making

serotonin activity has enhancing or impairing effects on IGT studies in healthy participants (Ha et al . 2009; Stoltenberg
(Homberg 2012; Rogers 2011). et al . 2011) genotyped for rs25531 alleles, and this may
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor acts on the serotonin have contributed to their success in finding positive effects
transporter (5-HTT), which is the key molecule responsible for on IGT. Another unexplored aspect, which would greatly
the reuptake of serotonin. The human 5-HTT is encoded by increase the theoretical significance of this line of research,
a gene located on the chromosome 17 (17q11.1-q12). The is related to the effects of 5-HTT genotype on somatic
promoter of this gene (5-HTTLPR ) hosts two polymorphic markers during IGT. Recent evidence confirmed that the dif-
loci, which result in variations in 5-HTT gene expression ference between anticipatory SCR before advantageous and
and 5-HTT function in the brain. An insertion/deletion disadvantageous choices predicted IGT performance, and
polymorphism in the human 5-HTTLPR has been first suggested that somatic markers and declarative knowledge
reported (Lesch et al . 1996). This polymorphism has a short independently contribute to IGT (Guillaume et al . 2009).
(S ) allele comprising 14 copies of a 20–23 base pair unit, This study was designed to investigate the effects of
and a long (L) allele comprising 16 copies of the same repeat 5-HTT genotype, including 5-HTTLPR and rs25531, on IGT
unit. The short allele is associated with reduced expression performance and somatic markers during IGT. On the
of 5-HTT gene and consequently reduced availability of basis of the previous literature related to somatic marker
5-HTT in the brain (Little et al . 1998; Reimold et al . 2007). hypothesis, we specifically expected that anticipatory SCR
A more recently described polymorphism (rs25531) involves would mediate the effect of genotype on IGT performance.
the substitution of an adenine to a guanine in the L allele
of 5-HTTLPR (Hu et al . 2006). The LG allele functionally
resembles the S allele (Ehli et al . 2012; Hu et al . 2006), so Material and methods
it is necessary to genotype both 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms
(i.e. triallelic 5-HTTLPR ) in order to correctly categorize the
Experimental subjects
L alleles into low-functioning (LG ) and high-functioning (LA ). N = 135 participants (118 women) volunteered for this study,
Therefore, an alternative to pharmacological manipulations following in-class presentations. Prior to study participation, written
of 5-HTT by SSRI is to study the influence of low- (S and informed consent was obtained from all the volunteers. They were
LG ) and high-functioning (LA ) alleles of 5-HTT gene on IGT all Caucasians of Romanian descent, and came from the same
well-circumscribed geographical area. Age ranged from 16 to 42
performance. (M = 21.6 years). All the participants were compensated for their
In light of the potentially enhancing effects of SSRI on IGT, time. The study followed the recommendations of AMA’s Declaration
one would expect that the low-functioning alleles of 5-HTT of Helsinki, and it was approved by the Babeş-Bolyai University
facilitated decision making in this task. Genetic knockout of Research Council.
5-HTT in rats was indeed associated with a higher level of
performance in a murine analog of IGT (Homberg et al . 2008). Genotyping
However, the lack of 5-HTT by gene knockout in rats may DNA was extracted from leukocytes [ethylenediaminetetraacetic
have different effects on IGT, in comparison with low 5-HTT acid (EDTA)-anticoagulated blood] using Genomic DNA Extraction
functioning in humans. Indeed, the findings of recent genetic Kit (Fermentas, Vilnius, Lithuania) and kept at −20◦ C. Both biallelic
and triallelic (i.e. including rs25531) 5-HTTLPR genotyping were per-
association studies on 5-HTT and IGT in healthy humans have
formed using a protocol adapted after Lonsdorf et al . (2009) and
been heterogenous. Two studies were able to confirm that S Kosek et al . (2009). The polymerase chain reaction (PCR) assay con-
carriers made more advantageous choices in the first block of ditions were optimized as follows: each reaction was carried out
IGT (Stoltenberg et al . 2011; Stoltenberg & Vandever 2010), in a 25 μl volume [50 ng of genomic template, 12.5 μl PCR mas-
whereas another study found that only S carriers who also termix (2x)]; the forward primer (5 -GGCGTTGCCGCTCTGAATGC-3 )
and reverse primer (5 -GAGGGACTGAGCTGGACAACCAC-3 ), from
had a certain dopamine receptor type 4 genotype displayed Generi-Biotech (Hradec Kralove, Czech Republic), were used to
IGT advantage (Ha et al . 2009). In contrast, three studies amplify a region encompassing 5-HTTLPR. These primers yield
reported that S carriers showed reduced IGT performance amplicons of 529 (for L allele) or 486 bp (for S allele). Thermal
(van den Bos et al . 2009; He et al . 2010; Homberg et al . cycling consisted of 3 min of initial denaturation at 94◦ C followed
by 31 cycles of 94◦ C (40 seconds), 57◦ C (40 seconds) and 72◦ C (40
2008), and another study indicated no effect of 5-HTTLPR seconds), each with a final extension step of 4 min at 72◦ C. The LG
on IGT (Lage et al . 2011). Similar studies in clinical samples and LA alleles were subsequently studied by enzymatic digestion of
found that the S and LG alleles were associated with reduced 10 μl of PCR products that were digested by HpaII (an isoschizomer
IGT performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder (da Rocha of MspI ) type FastDigest (Fermentas) in a 30 μl reaction assay at
37◦ C for 5 min. The restriction enzyme MspI recognizes and cuts
et al . 2008), or had no influence on IGT in suicide attempters
a 5 -C/CGG-3 sequence resulting in the following fragments: 340,
(Jollant et al . 2007). 127 and 62 bp for the LA allele; 174, 166, 127 and 62 bp for the LG
Clearly, additional studies are crucial in order to elucidate allele; 297, 127 and 62 bp for the SA allele; and 166, 131, 127 and
the effects of 5-HTT on IGT, and set the stage for meta- 62 bp for the SG allele. Finally, 10 μl of remaining PCR product and
analyses. Different groups focused on various IGT outcomes 15 μl of restriction enzyme assay solution were loaded onto a 2.5%
agarose gel, run for 2 h at 160 V in 0.5 × TBE running buffer and
(e.g. total score, scores from a single 20-trial block and scores visualized by ethidium bromide for size estimation. The 5-HTTLPR
from the first block vs. the other four blocks), and these vari- allele frequencies were 0.45 for SA allele, 0.48 for LA allele and 0.05
ables need to be considered. The effects of 5-HTT may also for LG allele, similar to the ones reported by Hu et al . (2006) for Cau-
be different in clinical samples compared with healthy volun- casians. One of the participants carried an ultralong (>16 repeats)
allele (XL) (Ehli et al . 2012). The genotypes were categorized into low-
teers because the effects of the genotype may interact with functioning (i.e. carriers of two low-expressing alleles: SS, LG LG , SLG :
the duration of the disease or previous medication in patients N = 32), intermediate-functioning (i.e carriers of one low-expressing
with emotional disorders. Moreover, only two of the previous allele: SLA , LG LA : N = 66), and high-functioning (i.e. carriers of two

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403 399


Miu et al.

high-expressing alleles: LA LA , LA XL: N = 30). These genotypes were of the Sobel test was used to test whether the indirect effect was
in Hardy–Weinberg equilibrium (χ 2 = 0.13, not significant). significant (Aroian 1947; Baron & Kenny 1986). All the analyses were
run in SPSS V.13.0.

Iowa Gambling Task


The computerized version of IGT was used (Bechara et al . 1994).
Briefly, IGT involves four decks of cards (A–D), from which partici- Results
pants are allowed to choose 100 cards. Each card from decks A and B
is associated with a high gain ($100), whereas each card from C and D
comes with a lower gain ($50). However, choosing a card is followed
Iowa Gambling Task performance and somatic
by unpredictable losses in all decks: for every 10 cards from A and B, markers by 5-HTTLPR group
there is a $1000 gain and $1250 loss; for every 10 cards from C and A 3 (genotype: low- vs. intermediate- vs. high-functioning) ×
D, there is a $500 gain and $250 loss. Therefore, decks A and B are 5 (block of trials) ANCOVA, with sex as covariate, indicated sig-
disadvantageous because of the net $250/10 cards loss, and decks C
nificant main effects of genotype (F2,132 = 4.61, P = 0.01,
and D are advantageous due to the net $250/10 cards gain. The IGT
score is obtained by subtracting the total number of disadvantageous Cohen’s d = 0.96) and block (F4,130 = 5.79, P = 0.003,
choices from the total number of advantageous choices: [(C + D) – (A Cohen’s d = 1.56) on CD–AB scores, as well as a significant
+ B)]. Participants are allowed to choose 100 cards. Learning in IGT is interaction of genotype and block (F7,127 = 4.1, P = 0.01,
operationalized as the improvement of the [(C + D) – (A + B)] scores ohen’s d = 0.87). In comparisons with the high-functioning
from one block of 20 successive selections to another.
group, the low- and intermediate-functioning groups had sig-
nificantly higher CD–AB scores in blocks 2–4 (i.e. trials
Psychophysiology 21–80) (Fig. 2a). A similar ANCOVA analysis found a sig-
SCR was recorded during IGT, using a Biopac MP150 system nificant effect of genotype on CD–AB anticipatory SCR
(Biopac Systems, Goleta, CA, USA). Area under the curve (μS/s)
(F2,132 = 3.09, P = 0.04, Cohen’s d = 0.45), with higher SCR
was estimated from reward or punishment intervals (i.e. 5 seconds
after the result of each selection was displayed), and anticipatory in low- and intermediate-functioning genotypes compared
intervals (i.e. 5-second intervals before the result of each selection with high-functioning genotype (Fig. 2b). There was a sta-
was displayed). The difference between anticipatory SCR before tistically significant correlation between CD–AB anticipatory
disadvantageous and advantageous cards was computed. SCR and CD–AB scores (r = 0.29, P = 0.04). There were
no statistically significant effects on SCR during reward or
Statistical analyses punishment intervals.
Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to test for differences in
IGT performance and SCR by 5-HTTLPR group. Pearson correlations
were computed to determine associations between IGT performance Somatic markers mediating the association between
and SCR. Mediation was tested using multiple regression analyses 5-HTTLPR and Iowa Gambling Task performance
(Baron & Kenny 1986; Frazier et al . 2004; Hoyt et al . 2008). According Table 1 describes the results of the analyses that examined
to Baron and Kenny (1986), the model had to satisfy three conditions
in order to be confirmed (Fig. 1): (1) the initial variable (5-HTTLPR the mediation hypothesis (see also Fig. 1). As predicted,
group) was significantly related to the outcome variable (CD–AB genotype was significantly related to IGT performance (Path
score) (Path c ); (2) the initial variable was related to the mediator c ) and to anticipatory SCR (Path a). When regressing IGT
(anticipatory SCR difference between CD and AB cards) (Path a) performance on both genotype and anticipatory SCR, the
and (3) the mediator was significantly associated with the outcome
variable when regressed on both the mediator and the initial variable
coefficient associated with the relation between anticipatory
(Path b), and the effect of the initial variable (Path c ) was reduced SCR and IGT performance, while controlling for genotype
compared with that in the first regression (Path c ). The Aroian version (Path b), was significant. The coefficient associated with
the relation between genotype and IGT performance, while
controlling for anticipatory SCR (Path c ), remained significant.
A
However, Path’s c coefficient was smaller than Path’s c
and anticipatory SCR was a significant mediator (Aroian’s
z = 5.33, P = 0.00). The ratio a × b/c indicated that about
42% of the total effect of genotype on IGT performance was
mediated by anticipatory SCR. A reverse-causality analysis
that tested a path from IGT performance to anticipatory SCR
B indicated a consistently smaller indirect effect (18% of the
total effect). The mediation model was also tested after
excluding the fewer men from the sample, and the results
were closely similar (i.e. 41% mediation by anticipatory SCR).

Discussion

As hypothesized, we found that anticipatory SCR during


Figure 1: Links between genotype, somatic markers and IGT partially mediated the relation between 5-HTTLPR
gambling performance. Mediation model describing the direct genotype and IGT performance. In terms of causation,
effect of genotype on IGT performance (a) and the indirect effect genotype preceded both SCR development (the mediator)
of genotype on IGT performance through anticipatory SCR (b). and learning IGT (the outcome). In addition, we tested

400 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403


5-HTTLPR, somatic markers and decision making

A B

Figure 2: Gambling performance


and somatic markers by geno-
type. The effects of 5-HTTLPR
genotype (low- vs. intermediate-
vs. high-functioning) on IGT perfor-
mance (a) and difference in antici-
patory SCR between advantageous
and disadvantageous decks (T
scores) (b). Error bars represent
one standard error of the mean.

P < 0.05; ∗∗ P < 0.01.

Table 1: Path coefficients for the mediation by anticipatory SCR of the relation between genotype and IGT performance

Testing steps in mediation model B SE B 95% CI β

Testing step 1 (Path c )


Outcome: [(C + D) – (A + B)] score
Predictor: genotype (low- vs. intermediate- vs. high-functioning)∗ 1.70 0.32 1.30–1.98 0.21∗∗
Testing step 2 (Path a)
Outcome: anticipatory SCR level
Predictor: genotype 0.72 0.09 0.34–1.21 0.22∗∗
Testing step 3 (Paths b and c  )
Outcome: [(C + D) – (A + B)] score
Mediator: anticipatory SCR level (Path b) 1.01 0.14 0.87–1.12 0.36∗∗
Predictor: genotype (Path c  ) 0.93 0.19 0.59–1.17 0.19∗

B, unstandardized regression coefficient; β, standardized regression coefficient; CI, confidence interval; SE, standard error.

0 = high-functioning genotype (i.e. LA /LA and LA /XL genotypes); 1 = intermediate-functioning genotype (i.e. S /LA and LG /LA
genotypes); 2 = low-functioning genotype (i.e. S /S , LG /LG and S /LG genotypes).

P < 0.05; ∗∗ P < 0.01.

an alternative model in which IGT performance mediated has complete information regarding the stochastic relation-
anticipatory SCR and found that the indirect effect dropped ship between actions and outcomes (Rangel et al . 2008).
significantly. Therefore, these results support the view that We found that anticipatory SCR mediated 42% of the
low-functioning 5-HTTLPR genotypes are associated with total effect of genotype on IGT performance. This partial,
increased IGT performance (partially) because they lead to yet consistent mediation provides a new type of support
higher anticipatory SCR during IGT. for the somatic marker hypothesis (Bechara et al . 2000).
The present finding that low-functioning alleles of To our knowledge, this is the first study showing that a
5-HTTLPR facilitate IGT performance is in line with previ- genetic-driven difference in somatic markers has impact on
ous genetic association studies in humans (Stoltenberg et al . IGT performance. These results contribute to the recent lit-
2011; Stoltenberg & Vandever 2010). This effect is similar to erature on how genes contribute to individual differences in
that of SSRI, which may facilitate IGT performance and par- decision making (Ebstein et al . 2010), and may open a new
ticularly decision making under risk (Bechara et al . 2001), as and important line of research related to somatic marker
well as to observations from psychopharmacological manip- hypothesis and the contribution of emotions to IGT. In light of
ulations of tryptophan (Koot et al . 2012; Murphy et al . 2009). debates surrounding the somatic marker hypothesis, future
Our results are also supported by the previous observation studies might test a complementary model in which declar-
that 5-HTT knockout in rats increased performance in the ative knowledge acquired during IGT might mediate another
second half of a murine IGT analog (Homberg et al . 2008). portion of the effect of 5-HTTLPR on IGT performance.
Overall, these studies indicate that the reduced function Somatic markers and declarative knowledge independently
of 5-HTT, which probably leads to an increased synaptic contribute to IGT performance (Guillaume et al . 2009), and it
availability of serotonin, is beneficial to decision making in is thus possible that they are influenced by different genetic
IGT. 5-HTTLPR may thus illustrate differential susceptibility polymorphisms or perhaps gene × environment interactions.
(Belsky & Pluess 2009; Homberg & Lesch 2010) to IGT learn- To date, twin studies have not approached the genetic
ing environment, particularly in the second part of the task. and environmental influences on IGT and its underlying
From this perspective, carriers of low-functioning alleles of mechanisms. An intriguing possibility, although entirely spec-
5-HTTLPR may be particularly susceptible to learning from ulative, is that heritability contributes to IGT through somatic
environments that include risk, in which the decision maker markers, and individual environment influences IGT through

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403 401


Miu et al.

declarative knowledge. Therefore, subsequent research may questions and answers. Trends Cogn Sci 9, 159–162. discussion
investigate whether 5-HTTLPR interacts with other genetic 162-154.
polymorphisms (Ha et al . 2009; da Rocha et al . 2011) and Belsky, J. & Pluess, M. (2009) Beyond diathesis stress: differential
susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull 135,
environmental factors (e.g. socioeconomic status) in influ- 885–908.
encing the susceptibility to learning IGT. Somatic markers van den Bos, R., Homberg, J., Gijsbers, E., den Heijer, E. &
and IGT performance may qualify for endophenotypes of neu- Cuppen, E. (2009) The effect of COMT Val158 Met genotype on
ropsychiatric conditions (e.g. obsessive-compulsive disorder, decision-making and preliminary findings on its interaction with the
bipolar disorder, pathological gambling, addiction), and shed 5-HTTLPR in healthy females. Neuropharmacology 56, 493–498.
light on the pathogenesis of these conditions (Verdejo-Garcia Damasio, A.R. (1996) The somatic marker hypothesis and the
possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philos Trans R Soc
& Bechara 2009). Lond B Biol Sci 351, 1413–1420.
This design was based on a priori predictions, which Dunn, B.D., Dalgleish, T. & Lawrence, A.D. (2006) The somatic
were derived from an empirically supported theory. Our marker hypothesis: a critical evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev
hypotheses were also in line with previous experimental 30, 239–271.
results from psychopharmacological studies in humans and Ebstein, R.P., Israel, S., Chew, S.H., Zhong, S. & Knafo, A. (2010)
genetic engineering in animals. As expected, the results Genetics of human social behavior. Neuron 65, 831–844.
Ehli, E.A., Hu, Y., Lengyel-Nelson, T., Hudziak, J.J. & Davies, G.E.
supported our model. However, it is noteworthy that the (2012) Identification and functional characterization of three novel
association of low-functioning 5-HTTLPR alleles with higher alleles for the serotonin transporter-linked polymorphic region. Mol
IGT performance is not in line with several previous studies Psychiatry 17, 185–192.
(He et al . 2010; Homberg et al . 2008; van den Bos et al . Frazier, P.A., Tix, A.P. & Barron, K.E. (2004) Testing moderator and
2009). Most of these studies did not genotype rs25531, mediator effects in counseling psychology research. J Counsel
and the failure in categorizing L alleles into low-functioning Psychol 51, 115–134.
Guillaume, S., Jollant, F., Jaussent, I., Lawrence, N., Malafosse, A.
LG and high-functioning LA may explain the divergence of & Courtet, P. (2009) Somatic markers and explicit knowledge
their results from the theoretically and empirically supported are both involved in decision-making. Neuropsychologia 47,
view. Future studies might thus try to replicate these results 2120–2124.
by genotyping both 5-HTTLPR polymorphisms (Ehli et al . Ha, R.Y., Namkoong, K., Kang, J.I., Kim, Y.T. & Kim, S.J. (2009) Inter-
2012; Hu et al . 2006; Lesch et al . 1996), and measuring IGT, action between serotonin transporter promoter and dopamine
somatic markers and declarative knowledge. receptor D4 polymorphisms on decision making. Prog Neuropsy-
chopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 33, 1217–1222.
In conclusion, this study showed that anticipatory SCR
He, Q., Xue, G., Chen, C., Lu, Z., Dong, Q., Lei, X., Ding, N., Li, J.,
during IGT mediate part of the effect of 5-HTTLPR on Li, H., Chen, C., Li, J., Moyzis, R.K. & Bechara, A. (2010) Serotonin
IGT performance, with low-functioning alleles conferring an transporter gene-linked polymorphic region (5-HTTLPR) influences
advantage in learning IGT by their association with increased decision making under ambiguity and risk in a large Chinese
somatic markers. sample. Neuropharmacology 59, 518–526.
Homberg, J.R. (2012) Serotonin and decision making processes.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 36, 218–236.
Homberg, J.R. & Lesch, K.P. (2010) Looking on the bright side of
References serotonin transporter gene variation. Biol Psychiatry 69, 513–519.
Homberg, J.R., van den Bos, R., den Heijer, E., Suer, R. & Cuppen, E.
Aroian, L.A. (1947) The probability function of the product of two (2008) Serotonin transporter dosage modulates long-term decision-
normally distributed variables. Ann Math Stat 18, 265–271. making in rat and human. Neuropharmacology 55, 80–84.
Baron, R.M. & Kenny, D.A. (1986) The moderator-mediator variable Hoyt, W.T., Imel, Z.E. & Chan, F. (2008) Multiple regression and
distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic, correlation techniques: Recent controversies and best practices.
and statistical considerations. J Pers Soc Psychol 51, 1173–1182. Rehab Psychol 53, 321–339.
Bechara, A. (2004) The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence Hu, X.Z., Lipsky, R.H., Zhu, G., Akhtar, L.A., Taubman, J., Green-
from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn berg, B.D., Xu, K., Arnold, P.D., Richter, M.A., Kennedy, J.L., Mur-
55, 30–40. phy, D.L. & Goldman, D. (2006) Serotonin transporter promoter
Bechara, A. & Damasio, A.R. (2005) The somatic marker hypothesis: gain-of-function genotypes are linked to obsessive-compulsive dis-
a neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ Behav 52, order. Am J Hum Genet 78, 815–826.
336–372. Jollant, F., Buresi, C., Guillaume, S., Jaussent, I., Bellivier, F.,
Bechara, A., Damasio, A.R., Damasio, H. & Anderson, S.W. (1994) Leboyer, M., Castelnau, D., Malafosse, A. & Courtet, P. (2007)
Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human The influence of four serotonin-related genes on decision-making
prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50, 7–15. in suicide attempters. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (1997) Deciding 144B, 615–624.
advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Koot, S., Zoratto, F., Cassano, T., Colangeli, R., Laviola, G., van den
Science 275, 1293–1295. Bos, R. & Adriani, W. (2012) Compromised decision-making
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Damasio, A.R. & Lee, G.P. (1999) Different and increased gambling proneness following dietary serotonin
contributions of the human amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal depletion in rats. Neuropharmacology 62, 1640–1650.
cortex to decision-making. J Neurosci 19, 5473–5481. Kosek, E., Jensen, K.B., Lonsdorf, T.B., Schalling, M. & Ingvar, M.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A.R. (2000) Emotion, decision (2009) Genetic variation in the serotonin transporter gene (5-
making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 10, 295–307. HTTLPR, rs25531) influences the analgesic response to the short
Bechara, A., Damasio, H. & Damasio, A.R. (2001) Manipulation of acting opioid Remifentanil in humans. Mol Pain 5, 37.
Dopamine and Serotonin Causes Different Effects on Covert and Lage, G.M., Malloy-Diniz, L.F., Matos, L.O., Bastos, M.A., Abrantes,
Overt Decision-Making. Society for Neuroscience Annual Meeting, S.S. & Correa, H. (2011) Impulsivity and the 5-HTTLPR polymor-
San Diego, CA, (abstract 456.5). phism in a non-clinical sample. PloS ONE 6, e16927.
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Damasio, A.R. (2005) The Lesch, K.P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S.Z., Greenberg, B.D.,
Iowa Gambling Task and the somatic marker hypothesis: some Petri, S., Benjamin, J., Muller, C.R., Hamer, D.H. & Murphy, D.L.

402 Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403


5-HTTLPR, somatic markers and decision making

(1996) Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism da Rocha, F.F., Malloy-Diniz, L., Lage, N.V. & Correa, H. (2011) The
in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science 274, relationship between the Met allele of the BDNF Val66Met
1527–1531. polymorphism and impairments in decision making under
Li, X., Lu, Z.L., D’Argembeau, A., Ng, M. & Bechara, A. (2010) The ambiguity in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Genes
Iowa Gambling Task in fMRI images. Hum Brain Mapp 31, Brain Behav 10, 523–529.
410–423. da Rocha, F.F., Malloy-Diniz, L., Lage, N.V., Romano-Silva, M.A., de
Little, K.Y., McLaughlin, D.P., Zhang, L., Livermore, C.S., Dalack, Marco, L.A. & Correa, H. (2008) Decision-making impairment is
G.W., McFinton, P.R., DelProposto, Z.S., Hill, E., Cassin, B.J., related to serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism in a
Watson, S.J. & Cook, E.H. (1998) Cocaine, ethanol, and genotype sample of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behav
effects on human midbrain serotonin transporter binding sites and Brain Res 195, 159–163.
mRNA levels. Am J Psychiatry 155, 207–213. Rogers, R.D. (2011) The roles of dopamine and serotonin in decision
Lonsdorf, T.B., Weike, A.I., Nikamo, P., Schalling, M., Hamm, A.O. making: evidence from pharmacological experiments in humans.
& Ohman, A. (2009) Genetic gating of human fear learning and Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 114–132.
extinction: possible implications for gene-environment interaction Stoltenberg, S.F. & Vandever, J.M. (2010) Gender moderates the
in anxiety disorder. Psychol Sci 20, 198–206. association between 5-HTTLPR and decision-making under
Maia, T.V. & McClelland, J.L. (2004) A reexamination of the evidence ambiguity but not under risk. Neuropharmacology 58, 423–428.
for the somatic marker hypothesis: what participants really know Stoltenberg, S.F., Lehman, M.K., Anderson, C., Nag, P. & Anagnopou-
in the Iowa gambling task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101, los, C. (2011) Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and
16075–16080. childhood trauma are associated with individual differences in
Murphy, S.E., Longhitano, C., Ayres, R.E., Cowen, P.J., Harmer, C.J. decision making. Front Genet 2, e1–e7.
& Rogers, R.D. (2009) The role of serotonin in nonnormative Verdejo-Garcia, A. & Bechara, A. (2009) A somatic marker theory of
risky choice: the effects of tryptophan supplements on the addiction. Neuropharmacology 56 (Suppl. 1), 48–62.
‘‘reflection effect’’ in healthy adult volunteers. J Cogn Neurosci
21, 1709–1719.
Rangel, A., Camerer, C. & Montague, P.R. (2008) A framework for
studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Acknowledgements
Rev Neurosci 9, 545–556.
We are grateful to Simona Pană, Silviu Matu, Ioana Cocia, Bianca
Reimold, M., Smolka, M.N., Schumann, G., Zimmer, A., Wrase, J.,
Blaj and Julia Avram for help with data collection. This research
Mann, K., Hu, X.Z., Goldman, D., Reischl, G., Solbach, C.,
was supported by grant 411/2010 from the National Council of
Machulla, H.J., Bares, R. & Heinz, A. (2007) Midbrain serotonin
Scientific Research in Higher Education (CNCSIS) to A.C.M.
transporter binding potential measured with [11 C]DASB is affected
by serotonin transporter genotype. J Neural Transm 114, 635–639.

Genes, Brain and Behavior (2012) 11: 398–403 403

You might also like