You are on page 1of 8

SPECIAL REPORT MEMORY

Making
Connections
The essence of memory is linking one thought to another
by Anthony j. Greene

M
any people wish their memory, in the parlance of the field — it
memory worked like a is more burden than blessing.
video recording. How For most of us, memory is not like a
handy would that be? video recording— or a notebook, a photo-
Finding your car keys graph, a hard drive or any of the other
would simply be a matter of zipping common storage devices to which it has
back to the last time you had them and been compared. It is much more like a web
hitting “play.” You would never miss an of connections between people and things.
appointment or forget to pay a bill. You Indeed, recent research has shown that
p h O t O i l l U S t r At i O n b Y A A r O n G O O d M A n

would remember everyone’s birthday. some people who lose their memory also
You would ace every exam. lose the ability to connect things to each
Or so you might think. In fact, a other in their mind. And it is the connec-
memory like that would snare mostly tions that let us understand cause and ef-
useless data and mix them willy-nilly fect, learn from our mistakes and antici-
with the information you really needed. pate the future.
It would not let you prioritize or create The things we remember are the ones
the links between events that give them that experience teaches us will help us
meaning. For the very few people who make predictions; the newest work in
have true photographic recall — eidetic our laboratory reveals how we make use

22 Scientific AMericAn Mind j ul y/Au gu s t 2 010


© 2010 Scientific American
w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/M in d s c i e n t i f i c am e r i c a n m i n d   23
© 2010 Scientific American
To a mind that can’t make connections, each instant
fleeting and unrelated, each precept without relevance,
of this predictive ability. Other recent
studies show that imagining the future Creating Memories
involves brain processes similar to, but
when a memory forms in the brain, it alters the connections between nerve cells, such
distinct from, those involved in conjur- as these from a sea slug, aplysia. A sensory neuron (left) loses most of its connections,
ing the past. We also tend to remember or synapses, to an adjacent motor neuron when the slug learns that two things or events
the people and events that resonate emo- are no longer related (center). new synapses form when repetition reinforces an associ-
tionally, which is why forgetting an an- ation (right), creating a memory as neurons that fire together wire together.
niversary is such an offense: it is fair evi-
Motor
dence that the date is not as important as neuron
the ones we do remember.
The discovery that memory is all
about connections has revolutionary im- Sensory
plications for education. It means that neuron
memory is integral to thought and that
nothing we learn can stand in isolation;
we sustain new learning only to the de- Ready to go No associations New associations
gree we can relate it to what we already
know. The modern theory of memory er hard drives. Modern scientific dialogue roots in the 1930s and 1940s, when a se-

f r O M a N i N T r o D u C T i o N T o B r a i N a N D B e h aV i o r , t h i r d e d i t i O n , b Y b . kO l b A n d i . Q . w h i S h Aw, w O r t h p U b l i S h e r S , 2 0 0 9
can help us as we organize our experi- still sometimes refers to memory as a trace ries of discoveries, most notably by psy-
ences, teach our children and support (as in a wax imprint or a rubbing). Lately chologist Karl Lashley of the University
those with learning problems. we compare human memory to computer of Chicago and later Harvard University,
memory, and we draw from the same tax- revealed that learning and memory are
the history of Memory onomy. We speak of encoding and storage not sequestered in their own storage
For millennia, metaphors for memory for the learning of new memories, retriev- banks but are distributed across the en-
have marched in lockstep with the tech- al for the act of remembering, address for tire cerebral cortex. Lashley set out to
nology for recording thought. References the location of a memory in the brain, and discover the location of the learning cen-
in ancient Greece likened memory to trac- output for a remembered event— meta- ter of the brain by systematically discon-
ings in a wax tablet, in the Middle Ages to phors that persist even as our understand- necting different regions of the cerebral
parchment, and later to books, files, pho- ing of memory evolves. cortex in a number of different rats. To
tos, videos, audio recordings and comput- The modern view of memory has its his surprise, all the rats showed some de-
gree of mild learning impairment, but
none was seriously impaired.
FAST FACTS The significance of these fi ndings is
linking and learning profound. It means that memory is dis-
persed, forming in the regions of the

1>> brain cells that fire together during an experience can form perma-
nent connections.
brain responsible for language, vision,
hearing, emotion and other functions. It
means that learning and memory arise

2>> those connections let the same network of brain cells refire later
on, which we experience as memory.
from changes in neurons as they connect
to and communicate with other neurons
[see illustration above]. And it means

3>> new experiences can lead a network of cells to develop further con-
nections, adding to a memory and helping us learn but sometimes
modifying a recollection and creating false memories.
that a small reminder can reactivate a
network of neurons wired together in
the course of registering an event, allow-
ing you to experience the event anew.

4>> because knowledge derives from connections, the optimal strategy


for learning involves making meaningful associations among topics.
Remembering is reliving.
Another piece of the puzzle fell into
place in the 1950s, after some surpris-
ing observations of a few individuals

24 Scientific AMericAn Mind j ul y/Au gu s t 2 010


© 2010 Scientific American
is an isolated event without continuity, each thought
each person a stranger, every event unexpected.
with almost complete amnesia. In the time of the neural insult; the greater the create memories [see illustration below].
most compelling case, a 27-year-old damage, the more severe the amnesia. Recent amnesia research paints an
Connecticut man, known as HM but At first, this finding appeared to con- even bleaker picture of the condition,
identified as Henry Gustav Molaison tradict the discovery that learning and while yielding some startling insights
after his death in 2008, had severe epi- memory are distributed across the brain. into what memory really is. Hippocam-
lepsy that was not responding well to The hippocampus, though, turned out to pal amnesiacs appear to have impair-
medication. It was a sadly common be not the source or storehouse of mem- ments that go well beyond the loss of
practice in those days to treat epilepsy ory but rather an essential mediator in memory creation. They also have severe
by removing or disconnecting substan- its formation. In a very small brain, ev- difficulty imagining future events, living
tial portions of brain tissue. The perfor- ery neuron might be connected to every instead in a fragmented, disconnected re-
mance of a brain resection on HM re- other neuron. But a human brain that ality. In a study published in 2007 psy-
sulted in one of the most extreme cases worked on this model would require chologist Eleanor Maguire and her re-
of amnesia ever recorded. His case and that each of hundreds of billions of neu- search team at University College Lon-
others revealed that damage to the hip- rons be linked to every other neuron, an don asked participants with amnesia and
pocampus, a wishbone-shaped struc- impossibly unwieldy configuration. The those with normal memories to elaborate
ture located deep below the surface folds hippocampus solves this problem by on hypothetical scenarios of short, sim-
of the cerebral cortex, leaves people al- serving as a kind of neural switchboard, ple vignettes. (The researchers gave par-
most completely unable to acquire new connecting the distant cortical regions ticipants a cue card summarizing the
memories or to learn complex associa- for language, vision and other abilities main dramatic elements to ensure that
tions. Their minds remain frozen at the as synaptic networks take shape and no one would forget the setup.)
In one scenario, participants were to
imagine they were standing in the main
A Neural Switchboard hall of a museum with many exhibits.
Those with normal memory could gen-
erally develop a coherent narrative about
people and their activities. Amnesic par-
ticipants typically could visualize only a
few details, which were often discon-
nected and lacking in spatial and tempo-
ral coherence. One typical narrative:
“Well, there’s big doors.... There’d be the
exhibits.... I don’t know what they are....
There’d be people.” What is lost in am-
nesia, then, is the capacity to connect
things to one another and find any mean-
ing in them. To a mind that cannot make
connections, each instant is an isolated
event without continuity, each thought
fleeting and unrelated, each precept
mat h e w v e r b e r Medical College of Wisconsin

without relevance, each person a strang-


er, every event unexpected.

(The Author)
ANTHONY j. GREENE is associate pro-
From its central location, the hippocampus (yellow) connects far-flung regions of the
fessor of psychology at the University
cortex (red) that are involved in a particular recollection. The image, in which the
brain is rendered semitransparent, superimposes a functional MRI image of the of Wisconsin–Milwaukee, where he
cerebral activity on an MRI of its structure. runs a learning and memory lab.

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/M in d s c i e n t i f i c am e r i c a n m i n d   25
© 2010 Scientific American
The ability to form and retain connections makes
this critical aspect of learning: “I put two and two

Reimagining Is Reliving
These two fMRIs
show a few differ-
ences but great
overlap in the
regions of the brain
involved in recalling
a past event and
imagining a future
one. In each case,
a network of neu-
rons fires across
the cerebral cortex.

permi ssion from El se vier (brain scans); ge t t y image s (mallard); gk har t/ vikki har t Getty Images (goose); herber t spichtinger Corbis (whistling duck)
and el abor ation,” by Donna Rose Addis, Al ana T. Wong and Daniel L . Sc hac ter, in Neurop sychologia , Vol . 45; 20 07, reprinted with
Remembering the past Imagining the future

from “Remembering the pas t and imagining the future: Common and di stinc t neur al sub str ate s during e vent con struc tion
The connections across the brain also call an experience instead of inventing a nections gives us not just a record of
help us conceive the future, as recent im- new one. The researchers found that al- events but also the foundations of com-
aging studies have shown. Functional though the subjects’ brain activity was prehension. We have plenty of clichés to
magnetic resonance imaging detects similar to that of people imagining a new express this critical aspect of learning:
changes in blood oxygen in various re- scenario, the patterns were clearly dis- “I put two and two together” or “I just
gions of the brain. When participants per- tinct, suggesting that anticipating the fu- connected the dots.” We make use of
form memory tasks, the areas of the brain ture is not simply a matter of recasting these connections thousands of times a
that use more oxygen are assumed to be past experiences into a novel or hypothet- day. And knowing how they work is cru-
involved. In 2007 a team led by psycholo- ical form [see illustration above]. Every cial to understanding how we learn.
gist Kathleen McDermott of Washington new event has some novel element to it Connections progress from simple
University in St. Louis and, separately, by even if experienced under similar circum- relations between things to ever more
psychologist Daniel L. Schacter of Har- stances, so the pattern of neuronal firing complex cascades of inferences. Links
vard and his colleagues showed that a mo- will be different each time. Our brain has between things, events, people and our
saic of brain areas similar to those in- evolved not just for learning and memory actions — so-called item associations —
volved in memory is active when partici- but for the management of relations: past, are the reason certain objects evoke rem-
pants imagine details of hypothetical or present and future. iniscence and become keepsakes. Visit-
prospective events. In McDermott’s study, ing your alma mater, catching a whiff of
participants were told they would be Memory as Connection burning leaves or finding a letter from a
asked to imagine some future event. Once The ability to form and retain con- loved one can bring back vivid memories
the subjects were inside the scanner, a re-
searcher would announce a keyword—
“birthday,” say— and the participants
would then think (to themselves; talking
is not permitted during a scan) about how
and where and with whom they would
celebrate a future birthday. Just as memo-
ries do, those imaginings elicited activity
in the hippocampus as well as multiple re-
The neural connections that make up our memories also help us generalize and discrimi-
gions of the cerebral cortex.
nate. Generalization helps us see the common elements in, say, all waterfowl but blinds us
In a 2009 experiment with a similar to the differences between the mallard (left), the goose (center) and the white-faced whis-
setup, Schacter’s team asked people to re- tling duck (right). In time, we learn to tell one similar creature from another.

26  s c i e n t i f i c am e r i c a n m i n d J u l y/Au gu s t 2 010


© 2010 Scientific American
comprehension possible. Plenty of clichés express
together” or “I just connected the dots.”
Getting the Most from Your Memory

R
ather than cluttering your mind with arbitrary facts,
try these techniques for using your memory more ef-
fectively. You will understand concepts in greater
depth and have more confidence in what you know.

■ Think about relevant connections between what you


wish to learn and what you already know. Build as many
meaningful connections as you can.
■ Make sure you thoroughly understand what you want
to learn. If you try to memorize a formula, a foreign
phrase or a passage you have not fully grasped, you will
soon forget it and will create a cul-de-sac for future
learning.
■ Explain things to yourself as if you were teaching them
to someone who was unfamiliar with the subject. You
will clarify and consolidate your own understanding.
■ Organize your thinking, outlining complex topics. En- ■ Never cram or rush yourself. Spread out your learning.
sure that the outline is as logical as you can make it. There are no known upper limits on how much human
■ Summarize what you wish to learn so that you establish beings can learn, but there are significant limits on how
the main points. Then elaborate on them to establish much they can learn at the last minute.
depth and breadth. ■ Practice early and often. — A.G.

of events you might not have thought tions requires us to weigh multiple vari- mentary processes that ultimately work
about in years. Item associations let us ables, which in turn takes a brain big together to shape our associations.
remember that Italy has good wine, and enough to learn all the relations involved. As we amass knowledge over the
they help us connect people’s names to Indeed, social interactions can pose our course of our life and connect events in
their faces. Items can also be retrieval greatest predictive challenges and may our memory, we learn to model complex
cues. A buzzer reminds us that dinner is well have been a major impetus, among contingencies and make inferences about
still in the oven. The sight of a co-work- our prehuman ancestors, for the evolu- novel relations. In my own lab, we have
er in the hall prompts us to mark a meet- tion of astounding learning abilities. been exploring the ways people use
ing on the calendar. At the root of the flexibility of learn- learned relations to make predictions. In
Put enough of these item associations ing and memory is generalization. My one experiment, published in 2006, par-
together, and you will create a web of one-year-old son recently had a won­ ticipants lying inside a functional MRI
connections that can help you make pre- derful time feeding some ducks and soon machine viewed a computer screen on
dictions and navigate the world more ef- was able to point out a duck with no which we displayed various pairs of un-
fectively over time. At their simplest, pre- trouble, whatever its color or age. He familiar shapes, drawn from the Japanese
dictive associations remind us that wet also overgeneralizes: in his lexicon, geese hiragana alphabet [see top illustration on
JEFFRE Y C O O LID G E G e t t y I m a g e s

snow will clog the snowblower and that and swans are also ducks. Eventually he next page]. For each pairing, we asked
lewd humor will upset some people. But will learn to discriminate among water- participants to click on one shape or the
many predictive associations are more fowl and perhaps in time among differ- other, eliciting a message —“correct” or
nuanced. Your boss might get upset if ent types of ducks, as he will later learn “incorrect,” depending on the choice —
you tell a crude joke in the office but not to tell a cabbage from a lettuce and one letting them learn which symbol in a giv-
when you are out for a beer and even sort of lettuce from another. Generaliza- en pair was preferred. Then we showed
then may check to see who else is there tion and discrimination are the yin and them novel pairings and asked them to
before deciding to laugh. Making predic- yang of learning and memory— comple- click on the one they thought was correct,

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/M in d s c i e n t i f i c am e r i c a n m i n d   27
© 2010 Scientific American
Experiments reveal how easy it is to create false
incorrect information about a car accident, many
rize multiplication tables, the Preamble
Symbol Solutions to the U.S. Constitution, and poetry that
I did not understand at all. Although the
days of learning by repeating things over
and over are finally on the wane, educa-
tors still rely on old-school methods such
as the arbitrary mnemonic device — an
people were trained to recognize which of these symbols from the japanese hiragana
alphabet was the “preferred” one in a series of pairings. then the participants tried to unrelated acronym used as a memory
pick the “correct” symbol in new pairings. the learners who were able to infer a hierar- cue. This is not to say that facts need not
chy of symbols from the original symbol sets made more correct choices later on and be learned. Some, such as phone num-
displayed greater activity in the hippocampus. bers and people’s names, are inherently
arbitrary, and for them rote learning
based on the hierarchy implied during the light was green. Many then remem- may be appropriate. But the message of
the learning phase. Those who scored bered a yellow light that never existed— modern memory research is that the
well— the ones who used inference — dis- which is why eyewitness testimony after brain is wired to recognize and organize
played much greater activity in the hip- police interrogation can be so unreliable. coherent connections, not arbitrary

f r O M “A n f M r i A n A lY S i S O f t h e h U M A n h i p p O c A M p U S : i n f e r e n c e , c O n t e X t, A n d tA S k AwA r e n e S S ,” b Y A . j . G r e e n e , w. l . G r O S S ,
c . l . e l S i n G e r A n d S . M . r AO, i n J o u r N a l o F C o g N i T i V e N e u r o s C i e N C e , v O l . 1 8 ; 2 0 0 6 (t o p) ; w i l l & d e n i M C i n t Y r e C o r b i s (b o t t o m)
pocampus than those who scored nearer To avoid this kind of malleability, smart ones. By tying new learning to existing
the level of chance — the ones who merely lawyers tell clients to write down what associations— by engaging in contextual
guessed— suggesting memory networks happened as soon as possible and before learning— we greatly improve results.
are fully engaged across the brain when discussing it. [For more on the reliability Say, for instance, you were teaching
people use what they know to make pre- of eyewitness testimony, see “Do the Eyes students about a historical novel such as
dictions. This study and others like it Have It?” by Hal Arkowitz and Scott O. The Scarlet Letter. Before you uttered
give insight into how we cobble together Lilienfeld; Scientific American Mind, the name “Hester Prynne,” you might
bits of information learned over many January/February 2010.] fi rst discuss how Puritan society mir-
years and use them to navigate our course rored truths your students already know:
through life. teach, Memory religious leaders do not always live up to
The accretive and adaptive quality of In my own early education during their convictions; the judgment of peers
memory can sometimes cause us prob- the 1970s, schools often taught through is weighty and lasting; concealed shame
lems by altering our memories instead of rote memorization. I was told to memo- eats at the soul. You could next intro-
augmenting them. During the 1990s psy-
chologist Elizabeth Loftus, now at the
University of California, Irvine, pro-
duced an impressive amount of research
showing how easy it is to create false
memories of past events. In one study,
participants watched a film of a car acci-
dent. Researchers asked some subjects
how fast they thought the cars were going
when they “smashed into” each other
and asked other subjects how fast the cars
were going when they “hit” each other.
The subjects who heard the word
“smashed” gave significantly higher esti-
mates of the speed. In other experiments,
subjects were fed incorrect information
about an accident after watching the film;
they might, for instance, be asked repeat-
edly whether a traffic light had turned new brain insights suggest that rote learning is usually ineffective. Students are more likely
yellow before the collision when in fact to retain new information when they can relate it to what they already know.

28 Scientific AMericAn Mind j ul y/Au gu s t 2 010


© 2010 Scientific American
memories of past events. When people were fed
remembered a yellow light that never existed.
duce ways in which the Puritan universe
was different: living on the verge of sur-
vival necessitated collective conformity;
technology was primitive; religious con-
victions were a community affair. Your
students can then engage their imagina-
tions to paint a picture of the characters’
way of life, from family relations to dogs
chasing pigs down the streets. As their
knowledge of the story’s setting deep-
ens, they can begin to take in abstract
ideas, such as the political and legal
structures of Puritan society. By this
time, the students will have formed an
intricate web of associations that will let
them weave the lessons of the book into
their own thinking. They can live the Rather than learning multiplication tables by endless repetition, students should figure
out how many wheels they would need to equip a fleet of cars.
story and grasp its significance.
Contextual learning can even help
with tasks that seem intractably a mat- from another, and from there exceptions Memory springs from the connections.
ter of rote memorization, such as learn- to those conditions, and in time more Despite our imperfect metaphors,
ing the times tables. You can better teach complicated relations still. we must have long known that memory
a child that 3 times 4 equals 12 by bring- Connections help us anchor an ever was more than a mere repository of ex-
ing the idea into the real world. Ask a more complicated body of knowledge perience. Consider the ancient Greeks.
girl who likes cars how many wheels she about how the world works and negoti- The muses were not simply the goddess-
would need for three racers with four ate the complex structures all around us. es who inspired poetry, music and all
wheels apiece. As she memorizes the ta- Memory is a dynamic aspect of our in- forms of artistic creativity and for whom
bles, she will learn what multiplication tellect. And as our understanding of temples were erected. They were also
actually does, which will help her solve memory grows deeper, we see that the patrons of the liberal arts and the well-
related problems later on. When rich in connections we make between the peo- spring of philosophy, knowledge,
context, science becomes an extension ple, places and things in our lives, be- thought and wisdom. And the mother
of a student’s natural curiosity to figure tween the past, present and future, do and queen of all the muses? Mnemos-
out how things work, just as history, as not themselves spring from memory. yne, the muse of memory. M
collective memory, connects students to
the continuum of civilization, and fine (Further Reading)
arts connects them to the mind of the
artist. If the connections between sub- ◆ ◆ Metaphors of Memory: A History of Ideas about the Mind. Douwe Draaisma.
Translated by Paul Vincent. Cambridge University Press, 2000.
ject matter and students are relevant and
◆ ◆ Contextual Teaching and Learning: What It Is and Why It’s Here to Stay. Elaine B.
personal to them, the learned material Johnson. Corwin Press, 2002.
becomes part of their beings. ◆ ◆ Synaptic Self: How Our Brains Become Who We Are. Joseph LeDoux. Penguin
The lessons of associative learning Books, 2002.
JEFFRE Y C O O LID G E G e t t y I m a g e s

can also give educators a fresh approach ◆ ◆ What’s New with Amnesic Patient HM? Suzanne Corkin in Nature Reviews Neuro-
to early learning and to teaching children science, Vol. 3, pages 153–160; 2002.
◆ ◆ From Conditioning to Conscious Recollection: Memory Systems of the Brain.
with learning deficits. Simplified contin-
Howard Eichenbaum and Neal J. Cohen. Oxford University Press, 2004.
gencies can help everyone in the first
◆ ◆ The Science of False Memory. C. J. Brainerd and V. F. Reyna. Oxford University
stages of learning. For example, a young Press, 2005.
child who thinks that all swimming birds ◆ ◆ The Hippocampus Book. Edited by Per Andersen, Richard Morris, David Amaral,
are ducks may later learn conditions that Tim Bliss and John O’Keefe. Oxford University Press, 2007.
distinguish one kind of swimming bird

w w w. S c i e nti f i c A m e r i c an .c o m/M in d s c i e n t i f i c am e r i c a n m i n d   29
© 2010 Scientific American

You might also like