You are on page 1of 12

Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

A dual perspective on risk perception and its effect on safety behavior: A T


moderated mediation model of safety motivation, and supervisor’s and
coworkers’ safety climate
Nini Xiaa,*, Qiuhao Xieb, Xiaowen Huc, Xueqing Wangd, Hao Menge
a
Department of Construction and Real Estate, School of Civil Engineering, Southeast University, No. 2 Southeast Road, Jiangning District, Nanjing 211189, China
b
Business School, Hohai University, No. 8 West Focheng Road, Jiangning District, Nanjing 211100, China
c
Queensland University of Technology, GPO Box 2434, Brisbane, QLD 4001, Queensland, Australia
d
College of Management and Economics, Tianjin University, No. 92 West Weijin Road, Nankai District, Tianjin 300072, China
e
Bank of Tianjin Co., Ltd., No.15 Youyi Road, Hexi District, Tianjin 300202, China

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Previous studies have acknowledged the impact of risk perception on safety behavior, but were largely con-
Risk perception troversial. This study aims to clarify this conflict and the mechanism through which risk perception can have an
Safety behavior impact on safety behavior. From the perspective of the dual attribute of the job demand concept in job de-
Job demands mands–resources theory, we posit that risk perception can be considered as a job hindrance or a job challenge
Safety motivation
depending on the context, thereby resulting in a negative or positive impact on safety behavior, respectively. The
Safety climate
current research context is the construction industry and the hypotheses were tested using hierarchically nested
Construction industry
data collected from 311 workers in 35 workgroups. Risk perception was demonstrated to be a job hindrance
exerting a negative impact on safety behavior and safety motivation mediated this effect. In addition, two di-
mensions of group-level safety climate––supervisor’s and coworkers’––were expected to alleviate or even reverse
the detrimental effects of hindrance risk perception on safety motivation and on safety behavior via motivation.
A moderation model and a first-stage moderated mediation model were established, respectively, for testing the
moderating roles of safety climate in the relationship between risk perception and safety motivation, and in the
indirect relationship of risk perception with safety behavior via motivation. Surprisingly, contrary to the hy-
potheses, when supervisor’s safety climate changed from a low level to a high level, the impact of risk perception
on safety motivation changed from positive to negative, and the negative effect of risk perception on safety
behavior via safety motivation was not alleviated but worsened. As expected, for workers in a positive cow-
orkers’ safety climate, the negative effect of risk perception on motivation and the indirect negative effect of risk
perception on behavior were both reversed to the positive. This indicates that coworkers’ safety climate helped
to change perceived risk from a job hindrance to a challenge. This research contributes to workplace risk per-
ception and safety behavior research by theoretically viewing risk perception as a dual job hindrance–challenge
concept and proposing two competing hypotheses concerning the impact of risk perception on safety behavior.
The empirical investigation confirmed the hindrance attribute of risk perception in the construction context. It
provides a theoretical framework and empirical evidence for future research to synthesize the conflict risk
perception–safety behavior relationship. We also contribute to the literature by pointing out the potential ne-
gative role of certain supervisor safety activities such as paternalistic leadership in influencing employee safety.

1. Introduction occupational accident or disease (International Labour Organization,


2018). Safety accidents in various industries not only cause personal
Injuries and accidents in the workplace remain a serious safety injuries, but also great economic losses (Beus et al., 2015; Hofmann
concern worldwide (Christian et al., 2009; Li et al., 2013; Nahrgang et al., 2003; Zou et al., 2014). Unsafe acts on the part of employees are
et al., 2011). Every day, approximately 7600 people die from an the main cause of industrial accidents (Heinrich et al., 1980). Haslam


Corresponding author.
E-mail addresses: ninixia@seu.edu.cn (N. Xia), xieqiuhao@hhu.edu.cn (Q. Xie), xiaowen.hu@qut.edu.au (X. Hu), wxqlab@126.com (X. Wang),
menghao@bankoftianjin.com (H. Meng).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2019.105350
Received 3 February 2019; Received in revised form 28 August 2019; Accepted 25 October 2019
0001-4575/ © 2019 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

et al. (2005) analyzed 100 accidents in the construction industry and 2. Theoretical background and hypotheses
found that 70 % of them were related to human factors, especially the
unsafe behavior of employees. Thus, reduction in the occurrence of 2.1. Job demands–resources theory and the link between risk perception and
occupational injuries and accidents largely depends on improvement in safety behavior
employees’ safety behavior.
Employees, as front-line personnel in the organization, are directly Workplace risks can be defined as the extent to which individuals
exposed to workplace risks, injuries, and accidents. If they perceive are exposed to danger when working (Frone, 1998), and risk perception
their work to be at a high level of risk, they are likely to behave safely to refers to “individuals’ subjective judgment of the risk” (Aven and Renn,
avoid risk to their own safety (Didla et al., 2009). If they do not behave 2009, p. 1). Safety behaviors are those activities ensuring that in-
safely, they increase their likelihood of suffering workplace injuries and dividuals or the workplace are free from physical threat or harm (Beus
accidents (Christian et al., 2009). Some studies confirmed the positive et al., 2015). Following job performance theory, Griffin and Neal
relationship between risk perception and safety behavior (e.g., (2000) defined and differentiated two components of safety behavior:
Kouabenan et al., 2015; Seo, 2005; Tomás et al., 1999; Xia et al., 2017). safety compliance and safety participation. Safety compliance corre-
However, the current findings concerning the impact of risk perception sponds to task performance and refers to “the core activities that in-
on safety behavior are still contradictory. For example, Nahrgang et al. dividuals need to carry out to maintain workplace safety” (Griffin and
(2011) considered perceived level of workplace risks and hazards as Neal, 2000, p. 349), such as adhering to safety norms. Safety partici-
one type of job hindrance demands (i.e., stresses), which requires sus- pation corresponds to contextual performance and refers to “behaviors
tained physical and/or mental effort and thereby leaves limited per- that do not directly contribute to an individual’s personal safety but
sonal resources for handling safety behavior (Bakker and Demerouti, that do help to develop an environment that supports safety” (Griffin
2017; Hobfoll, 2001). Thus, additional research is warranted for clar- and Neal, 2000, p. 349), such as participating in voluntary programs for
ifying the impact of risk perception on safety behavior. improving workplace safety.
We argue that the conflict between risk perception and safety be- We propose that the conflict findings concerning the impact of risk
havior can be explained by drawing on the dual attribute of the job perception on safety behavior can be clarified by drawing on JD–R
demand concept in job demands–resources (JD–R) theory (Bakker and theory, especially its perspective of hindrance and challenge demands.
Demerouti, 2017). Nahrgang et al. (2011) considered perceived level of Put forward in 2001, JD–R theory was frequently used for explaining
workplace risks as a job hindrance demand; however, recent develop- job stress (Demerouti et al., 2001). The first proposition of JD–R theory
ments in JD–R theory have found that job hindrances can also be ex- was that all types of job characteristics can be classified into one of two
perienced as job challenges. Job challenges can cost effort but si- categories: job demands and job resources. Job demands are defined as
multaneously can contribute to goal achievement or learning “those physical, psychological, social, or organizational aspects of the
(Breevaart and Bakker, 2017). Studies have found that a job hindrance, job that require sustained physical and/or psychological effort and are
such as work pressure experienced in one occupation, can be considered therefore associated with certain physiological and/or psychological
as a challenge in other occupations (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017; costs” (Demerouti et al., 2001, p. 501). They are the specific stresses
Bakker and Sanz-Vergel, 2013). Whether a job demand can be experi- related to the work context (Ten Brummelhuis and Bakker, 2012). In
enced as a job hindrance or a job challenge depends on the context. high-risk settings, examples of job demands include exposure to risks
Using this lens, we posit that perceived risk may be considered as a job and hazards, physical demands, and complexity of the work (Nahrgang
hindrance and one that inhibits employees’ safety behavior, while in et al., 2011). Job demands as work stresses will reduce individuals’ job
other contexts it may serve as a job challenge and thus elicits em- performance (e.g., safety behavior) by exhausting personal resources or
ployees’ safety behavior for the achievement of safety needs. Basically, energy for displaying organization-expected behaviors (Bakker and
the hindrance or challenge attribute of perceived risk, as well as the Demerouti, 2017; Hobfoll, 2001).
negative or positive relationship between risk perception and safety JD–R theory has been developed for approximately 20 years, and
behavior are empirical questions that depend on the context. two further types of job demands have been distinguished: hindrance
This dual hindrance and challenge job demand perspective on the job demands and challenge job demands. Hindrance job demands are
concept of risk perception can provide a theoretical framework for defined as “job demands or work circumstances that involve excessive
synthesis of the current conflict in the risk perception–safety behavior or undesirable constraints that interfere with or inhibit an individual’s
relationship. The construction industry is characterized by high risk ability to achieve valued goals,” thereby exerting a negative impact on
(Barbaranelli et al., 2015; Fang and Wu, 2013; Murie, 2007; Yu et al., work engagement and performance (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017, p.
2014), and the unsafe behavior of workers appears to be a critical factor 277). Challenge job demands are defined as “demands that cost effort
in a large proportion of construction accidents (Fogarty and Shaw, but that potentially promote personal growth and achievement of the
2010; Xia et al., 2017). Thus, analysis of a sample of construction employee,” exerting a positive impact on work engagement and per-
workers should add empirical evidence to the existing risk percep- formance similar to job resources (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017, p.
tion–safety behavior relationship research. Furthermore, this study will 277). Certain job demands are job hindrances such as role conflict while
explore the moderated mediation effect of individual safety motivation others are job challenges such as high levels of responsibility. Fur-
and group-level supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate in the risk thermore, studies have found that a job hindrance such as work pres-
perception–safety behavior relationship. This will contribute to re- sure experienced in one occupation can be considered as a challenge in
vealing the underlying mechanism through which risk perception can other occupations (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017; Bakker and Sanz-
impact safety behavior, as most previous studies have merely in- Vergel, 2013). That is, job demands can be experienced as either a job
vestigated the direct relationship between them (e.g., Seo, 2005; Tomás hindrance demand or job challenge demand depending on the context.
et al., 1999; Xia et al., 2017). Furthermore, we distinguish the two Drawing on the dual hindrance and challenge attribute of job de-
safety climate dimensions, which will help to examine whether they mands, we posit that employees’ risk perception varies as a job hin-
play the same role in the risk perception–safety motivation–behavior drance or challenge in different contexts, which then decreases or in-
relationship. The entire conceptual model is shown in Fig. 1. Details of creases safety behavior in different population samples from different
the development of the hypotheses are presented in the following sec- occupations, respectively. The hindrance or challenge attribute of risk
tion. perception and its subsequent negative or positive relationship with
safety behavior is basically an empirical question. For the current re-
search sample, construction workers, we propose two competing hy-
potheses.

2
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Fig. 1. Conceptual model.

Hypothesis 1. Risk perception will have a negative effect on safety The moderating role of supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate
compliance (H1a) and safety participation (H1b). can be explained by JD–R theory. According to this theory, job re-
sources refer to “those physical, psychological, social, or organizational
Hypothesis 1’. Risk perception will have a positive effect on safety
aspects of the job that are functional in achieving work goals, reduce
compliance (H1a’) and safety participation (H1b’).
job demands and the associated physiological and psychological costs,
Safety motivation refers to an individual’s willingness to exert effort
or stimulate personal growth, learning, and development” (Bakker and
to enact safety behaviors and the valence associated with those beha-
Demerouti, 2017, p. 274). One typical example of job resources in high-
viors (Chen and Chen, 2014; Neal and Griffin, 2006; Vinodkumar and
risk settings is a positive safety climate, because it reduces job demands
Bhasi, 2010). Perceptions of workplace risks likely influence workers’
and their associated physiological and psychological costs and moti-
motivation to act safely first and then promote their safety behavior. It
vates the development of safety behavior expected, supported, and re-
can also be inferred that safety motivation plays a mediating role in the
warded by the organization (Nahrgang et al., 2011; Schaufeli et al.,
impact of risk perception on safety behavior, given the confirmed job
2009). JD–R theory proposes that job resources can buffer the negative
demands–work engagement–job performance relationship in the JD–R
impact of job hindrance demands on work engagement and perfor-
theory (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017, p. 277). While work engagement
mance (Bakker and Demerouti, 2017). That is, employees who have
is often defined as a generic positive motivation state of vigor, ded-
access to sufficient resources can better cope with demanding situa-
ication, and absorption toward work (Bakker et al., 2014), safety mo-
tions. This is because job resources refill energetic resources that are
tivation refers to safety-specific motivation toward work (Neal and
consumed when meeting job hindrances, by providing employees with
Griffin, 2006). In the workplace safety research, safety motivation and
emotional support or new tools to cope with stresses at work (Bakker
knowledge have been demonstrated to be the two determinants of
and Demerouti, 2014). In the context of workplace safety, Bronkhorst
safety behavior, while other factors influence safety behavior via these
(2015) focused on health care employees and revealed that the safety
two determinants (Christian et al., 2009; Griffin and Neal, 2000).
climate buffers the negative impact of a job hindrance demand (i.e., job
Therefore, we propose the following hypotheses.
insecurity) on safety behavior. Similarly, Kao et al. (2016) con-
Hypothesis 2. Risk perception is negatively associated with safety ceptualized supervisor safety priority as a job resource and found that it
motivation. can alleviate the deleterious impact of insomnia on safety behavior and
injuries.
Hypothesis 3. Safety motivation mediates the impacts of risk
Furthermore, according to the standpoint of conservation of re-
perception on safety compliance (H3a) and safety participation (H3b).
sources theory (COR) (Hobfoll, 1989, 2002), both supervisor’s and
coworkers’ safety climate are a positive interpersonal activity related to
2.2. The moderating role of safety climate safety, which is likely to generate psychological resources and positive
affect by fulfilling human needs such as relatedness and competence
In high-risk settings, the safety climate is an important environ- (Koopman et al., 2016; Weinstein and Ryan, 2010). As a positive event,
mental factor and has been widely confirmed to affect safety behavior supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate can also reinforce self-eva-
(Brondino et al., 2012; Christian et al., 2009; Zohar and Polachek, luations (Williamson and Clark, 1989). These increased relatedness,
2014). The safety climate can be defined as shared perceptions among competence, self-evaluations, and associated psychological resources
members of a group or organization concerning policies, procedures, and positive affect may also help workers to better cope with workplace
and practices related to safety in the workplace (Dov, 2008; Morrow risks acting as job hindrances, or to change their perceptions of work-
et al., 2010; Zohar, 1980). Brondino et al. (2012) proposed three di- place risks from negative (a job hindrance) to positive (a job challenge),
mensions of safety climate, namely, top management’s safety climate, thereby motivating them to demonstrate safety behavior. Empirical
supervisor’s safety climate, and coworkers’ safety climate, reflecting the research has found that demands are sometimes considered as both
actual priority given to safety by top management, supervisors, and hindering and challenging (Searle and Auton, 2015).
coworkers, respectively (Zohar, 2003). Owing to the short tenure For simplicity and clarity, we assume that there is a negative re-
characteristic of workers on construction sites, complex subcontracting, lationship between risk perception and safety behavior for construction
and the temporality of projects in the construction industry, construc- workers. In line with the proposition in JD–R theory and the empirical
tion workers are prone to disconnect with top management (Schwatka findings, we conceptualize supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate as
and Rosecrance, 2016). In reality, construction workers tend to work in a job resource that will moderate the impact of risk perception as a job
groups and interact frequently with their supervisors and coworkers hindrance demand on safety behavior. When the safety climate is po-
within the group. Research has found significant effects of both su- sitive, employees are in a workplace where safety policies, procedures,
pervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate on construction workers’ safety and practices are clearly established and supervisors and coworkers
behavior (Fang et al., 2015; Liang et al., 2018). To capture the parti- show concern for safety. In this situation, workers are likely to be better
cularity of the setting, we consider supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety at dealing with contextual hindrance demands, as the emotional and/or
climate as a group-level moderator in the relationship between risk behavioral safety support provided by supervisors and coworkers help
perception and safety behavior. to replenish effort lost when meeting job hindrances (Ten Brummelhuis

3
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

and Bakker, 2012). That is, job resources relating to a positive safety completing questionnaires and to respond to questions. At the begin-
climate can counter the tendencies of workers to demonstrate unsafe ning of the survey, the participants were informed that their responses
behavior by perceiving workplace risks as a job hindrance. By further were anonymous, that there were no right or wrong answers, and that
considering the standpoint of COR and the finding in Searle and Auton they should answer questions as honestly as possible. All of the parti-
(2015), we expect that the negative effect of risk perception on safety cipants were assured that their responses would remain confidential
motivation and behavior can be alleviated or even reversed by a posi- and be kept by the authors for research purposes only.
tive safety climate. On the other hand, when the safety climate in the A total of 490 copies of the questionnaire were distributed. Because
workplace is poor, there is a lack of job resources supporting safety the survey was held on site, all questionnaires were returned. After
behavior, probably manifested by ambiguous safety policies, proce- checking the answers, 179 questionnaires were deemed unusable or
dures, and practices, supervisors’ insufficient concern for safety, and careless and thus removed from future use. To ensure the quality of our
coworkers’ demonstration of unsafe conduct. Lacking job resources, data, we applied three criteria to remove unusable or careless response
workers are likely to suffer from resource loss spirals, becoming more data: (a) multiple occurrence of two options were chosen for one item
sensitive to resource loss and tending to perceive workplace risks as (N = 50); (b) questionnaires were finished with more than 5 % missing
hindrances consuming their personal resources (Halbesleben et al., items, for example, one respondent gave no response to items on one
2014), which consequently reduces their safety motivation and then entire page (N = 36; this criterion was used by Seo (2005)); and (c)
behavior (Bakker and Demerouti, 2014). Therefore, we expect that with no variations across negatively and positively worded items on a
supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate will moderate the direct personality measure, for example, one respondent provided the same
relationship of risk perception to safety motivation and the indirect response (e.g., “strongly agree”) to the item “extraverted, enthusiastic”
relationship of risk perception to safety behavior. and the item “reserved, quiet” (N = 93; this criterion was used by
Griffin and Hu (2013)).
Hypothesis 4a. Supervisor’s safety climate moderates the relationship
Our final sample for the analyses comprised 311 workers nested in
between risk perception and safety motivation. Specifically, when
35 workgroups, producing a valid response rate of 63.47 %. This rate
supervisor’s safety climate is positive, the negative effect of risk
was considered high as most questionnaire surveys in the construction
perception on safety motivation is weaker or may change to be
industry normally range from 20 % to 30 % (Akintoye, 2000). Such a
positive, compared to when supervisor’s safety climate is negative.
relative lower rate compared to other industries may be due to the fact
Hypothesis 4b. Coworkers’ safety climate moderates the relationship that our subjects are construction workers, the majority of whom were
between risk perception and safety motivation. Specifically, when of low educational status and might struggle to answer the questions
coworkers’ safety climate is positive, the negative effect of risk (Xia et al., 2017).
perception on safety motivation is weaker or may change to be Nonetheless, deleting data may create sampling bias. To reduce this
positive, compared to when coworkers’ safety climate is negative. concern, we further implemented the following strategies. First, we
conducted t-tests for comparing demographic variables of the 179 de-
Hypothesis 5. Supervisor’s safety climate moderates the indirect
leted respondents with those of the 311 final respondents. Results show
negative effects of risk perception on safety compliance (H5a) and
that demographics between the two groups, on the whole, did not show
participation (H5b) (via safety motivation). Specifically, supervisor’s
any significant difference: gender (t = .528, p = .598, n.s.), age (t =
safety climate is positive, the indirect negative relationship of risk
−1.303, p = .194, n.s.), work experience (t = −.752, p = .232, n.s.),
perception with safety behavior via safety motivation is weaker or may
and work position (t = .971, p = .332, n.s.). Second, we calculated the
change to be positive, compared to when supervisor’s safety climate is
binary correlations between the response rates at workgroups and all
negative.
the study variables and found none of the relationships was statistically
Hypothesis 6. Coworkers’ safety climate moderates the indirect significant at the .05 level. Based on these tests, it can be concluded that
negative effects of risk perception on safety compliance (H6a) and sampling bias should not be a problem in contaminating the results. The
participation (H6b) (via safety motivation). Specifically, when retained sample was eligible for further analyses. Details of the final
coworkers’ safety climate is positive, the indirect negative participants’ demographics are presented in Table 1.
relationship of risk perception with safety behavior via safety
motivation is weaker or may change to be positive, compared to
when coworkers’ safety climate is negative. Table 1
Final participant demographics (N = 311).
Number Percent(%)
3. Methods
Gender
Male 286 6.7
3.1. Procedures and participants
Female 21 92.0
Missing 4 1.3
This study was supervised by an independent board at Southeast Age
University for the ethical review. Questionnaires were distributed on 15∼29 years 41 13.2
site in 35 construction projects in China, including housing projects, 30∼39 years 102 32.8
40∼49 years 121 38.9
subway projects, waterway projects, highway projects, etc. In one
50 years and over 43 13.8
project, we randomly chose one workgroup as a survey unit, and within Missing 4 1.3
each group 8–15 workers were randomly selected as participants. From Work experience
August 2017 to October 2017, 15 workgroups in 15 projects were 0∼5 years 56 18.0
6∼10 years 96 30.9
surveyed; from June 2019 to July 2019, another 20 workgroups in 20
11∼15 years 46 14.8
projects were surveyed. We approached the participants by contacting 16∼20 years 52 16.7
and getting permission from the project manager. Participation was 21 years and over 55 17.7
voluntary and the participants had the right to withdraw from the study Missing 6 1.9
at any time. Work position
Foreman 49 15.8
The participants completed the questionnaires during the lunch
Ordinary worker 256 82.3
break in the project meeting room. This process lasted for about 20 min, Missing 6 1.9
during which the authors participated to provide guidance on

4
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

3.2. Measures adopted (Li, 2013). This scale comprised measures on five dimensions:
extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, and
3.2.1. Risk perception openness to experience. All personality items were rated on a 5-point
Risk perception was assessed using nine items based on the scale Likert scale, with responses ranging from 1 (very inaccurate) to 5 (very
developed by Gyekye and Salminen (2005). One sample item was “I feel accurate).
that my work is very dangerous.” All items were rated on a 5-point
Likert scale, with 1 indicating “strongly disagree” and 5 indicating 3.3. Analytic strategy
“strongly agree.” The Cronbach’s alpha of the current sample was .931.
As indicated by Nunnally (1978), when Cronbach’s alpha is greater SPSS 19.0 was used to perform descriptive analyses, correlation
than .70, the items for measuring a variable show an adequate relia- analyses, and reliability tests. Because the study variables were mea-
bility. sured by workers’ self-reports, we assessed the construct validity of our
measures by examining structure validity, criterion-related validity,
3.2.2. Safety behavior convergent validity, and discriminant validity. Individual perceptions
The two types of safety behavior, namely, safety compliance and of safety climate were aggregated to the group level; thus, we checked
safety participation, were assessed using the scale developed by Neal the viability of the group-level variables by examining within-group
and Griffin (2006). Safety compliance was measured by four items; a interrater reliability, intra-class correlation (ICC[1]), and reliability of
sample item was “I use all the necessary safety equipment to do my the mean (ICC[2]).
job.” Safety participation was measured by three items; a sample item Because of the nested nature of the data, we estimated a null model
was “I put in extra effort to improve the safety of the workplace.” Re- that had no predictors at either level 1 (the individual level) or level 2
sponses were recorded on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (the group level) to partition the safety motivation variance into within-
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alphas of the and between-group components. The level 2 residual variance of the
current sample were .869, and .809 for safety participation and safety intercept was .259 (p < .01). The ICC(1) was .59, indicating that 59%
compliance, respectively. of the variance in safety motivation resided between groups, and 41%
of the variance resided within groups. For safety compliance, the level 2
3.2.3. Safety motivation residual variance of the intercept was .273 (p < .01). The ICC(1) was
Safety motivation was assessed using five items based on the scale .58, indicating that 58% of the variance in safety motivation resided
developed by Vinodkumar and Bhasi (2010). A sample item was “I between groups, and 42% of the variance resided within groups. For
think the safety of the workplace is very important.” All items were safety participation, the level 2 residual variance of the intercept was
rated on a 5-point Likert scale, with responses ranging from 1 (strongly .319 (p < .01). The ICC(1) was .55, indicating that 55% of the var-
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alpha of the current iance in safety motivation resided between groups, and 45% of the
sample was .864. variance resided within groups.
These results consistently indicated that ICC values were at accep-
3.2.4. Safety climate table levels (Bliese, 2000; Cheung and Au, 2005) and that between-
We adopted the safety climate subscales developed by Brondino groups’ variances in safety motivation, safety compliance, and safety
et al. (2012) to measure the supervisor’s safety climate and coworkers’ participation were significantly different from zero. Thus, hypotheses
safety climate. In each surveyed workgroup, one supervisor was as- were tested by means of hierarchical linear modeling (Raudenbush and
signed for the worker participants to assess the degree to which safety is Bryk, 2002) using Mplus 7.11 (Muthén and Muthén, 2013). Tests of the
a priority of the workgroup’s supervisor. The supervisor’s safety climate main, mediating, moderating, and moderated mediating effects are
was measured by ten items; a sample item was “The supervisor cares for described below.
safety rules when a delay in production occurs.” To assess the im-
portance given to safety by coworkers, coworkers’ safety climate was 3.3.1. Tests of main and mediation effects
measured by four dimensions, including safety communication, safety Hierarchical linear models were established in Mplus 7.11 to test
mentoring, safety values, and safety systems. The four dimensions the main effects, namely, the effects of risk perception on safety com-
contained 12 items; sample items were “Team members discuss incident pliance (H1a), safety participation (H1b), and safety motivation (H2),
prevention” for safety communication, “Team members emphasize as well as the mediation effects, namely, the mediating roles of safety
safety care to peers when tired” for safety mentoring, “Team members motivation in the effects of risk perception on safety compliance (H3a)
engage in safety care when tired” for safety values, and “Team members and safety participation (H3b). Moreover, to test the mediation effects,
care for other workers’ safety equipment” for safety systems. The re- we used a percentile bootstrap approach to obtain bias-corrected con-
sponse coding used was a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly fidence interval estimates (MacKinnon et al., 2004). The bootstrap
disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). method can overcome the problems of non-normal sampling distribu-
We operationalized group-level supervisor’s safety climate and tions of mediating effects estimates (Preacher and Hayes, 2004; Zhao
coworkers’ safety climate by aggregating the individual worker scores et al., 2010).
to the group level and testing the within-group agreement. As suggested
by Sirotnik (1980), the internal consistency reliability was computed at 3.3.2. Tests of moderation effects
the group level. The Cronbach’s alphas of the current sample were .968 We tested the moderating effects of group-level supervisor’s safety
and .965 for supervisor’s safety climate and coworkers’ safety climate, climate (H4a) and coworkers’ safety climate (H4b) in the relationship
respectively. between risk perception and safety motivation by establishing hier-
archical linear models in Mplus 7.11. For the current sample, the two
3.2.5. Control variables proposed moderators were correlated (r = .807, p < .01); thus, these
To maximize internal validity and rule out alternative explanations, two moderators were included simultaneously in one moderation
we controlled variables. We included individual characteristics of model.
workers: gender, age, work experience, and work position. Personality
traits are also important factors influencing employees’ safety motiva- 3.3.3. Tests of moderated mediation
tion and behavior and the “big five” have been the most acknowledged To test H5 and H6 (i.e., the moderated mediation effects), we es-
measures of personal traits (Beus et al., 2015). Therefore, a ten-term tablished a two-level first-stage moderated mediation model (Bauer
personality inventory that was suitable for Chinese employees was et al., 2006) in Mplus 7.11 and used a bootstrap method to estimate the

5
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Table 2
Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.
Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

Individual level, construction worker


1. Safety compliance 4.027 .672 (.869)
2. Safety participation 3.841 .750 .667** (.809)
3. Risk perception 2.592 .859 −.234** −.234** (.931)
4. Safety motivation 4.053 .648 .649** .530** −.203** (.864)
5. Extraversion 3.471 .817 .045 .191** .025 .089 –
6. Agreeableness 2.500 .825 −.111 −.143* .091 −.075 .350** –
7. Conscientiousness 3.722 .724 .157** .256** −.128* .189** .071 −.308** –
8. Emotional stability 2.564 .819 −.116* −.159** .058 −.162** .219** .591** −.359** –
9. Openness to experience 3.259 .685 .182** .271** −.009 .234** .201** −.287** .458** −.263** –
10. Gender .930 .253 .046 .007 .070 .021 .142* .022 .022 .043 .167** –
11. Age 2.541 .893 −.046 −.040 −.031 −.086 −.025 −.041 .033 −.027 −.072 .033 –
12. Work experience 2.849 1.387 −.049 −.035 .019 −.058 .024 −.081 .053 −.086 .021 .158** .581** –
13. Work position 1.840 .368 −.049 −.125* .099 −.111 −.079 .113* −.198** .085 −.182** −.085 −.087 −.139*
Group level, workgroup
1. Supervisor’s safety climate 3.887 .609 (.968)
2. Coworkers’ safety climate 3.722 .588 .807** (.965)

Notes: N = 311 at the individual level; N = 35 at the group level. Gender: 0 = female, 1 = male. Age: 1 = 15∼29 years, 2 = 30∼39 years, 3 = 40∼49 years,
4 = 50 years and over. Work experience: 1 = 0∼5 years, 2 = 6∼10 years, 3 = 11∼15 years, 4 = 16∼20 years, 5 = 21 years and over. Work position: 1 = foreman,
2 = ordinary worker. Cronbach’s alphas are in the parentheses on the diagonal. *p < .05, **p < .01, two-tailed.

conditional indirect relationship of risk perception to safety behavior coworkers’ safety climate (r =.807, p < .01). As these correlations
via safety motivation at higher (+1 standard deviation) and lower le- were consistent with theories about and empirical evidence on safety
vels (-1 standard deviation) of safety climate as a moderator. Similar to behavior and safety climate (e.g., Brondino et al., 2012; Neal and
the model for testing H4, the two moderators were included simulta- Griffin, 2006), the results provided criterion-related validity evidence
neously in one first-stage moderated mediation model. for the measures.
Third, the convergent validity of the measures was examined by
4. Results three indices suggested by Hair et al. (2009), including item reliability
(standardized factor loading, SFL), composite reliability (CR), and
4.1. Descriptive analyses and correlations average variance extracted (AVE). Table 4 shows the result of con-
vergent validity examination. The SFLs range (.528–.897) exceeded the
Table 2 shows the means, standard deviations, and correlations critical value of .50 suggested by Hair et al. (2009). The CRs range
among all the variables. For the controls, safety compliance was related (.752–.936) exceeded the critical value of .60 suggested by Fornell and
to conscientiousness (r = .157, p < .01), emotional stability (r = Larcker (1981). The AVEs range (.504–.617) was above the value of .50
−.116, p < .05), and openness to experience (r = .182, p < .01); suggested by Fornell and Larcker (1981). Thus, the convergent validity
safety participation was related to extraversion (r = .191, p < .01), of our measures was demonstrated. The values of CRs and AVEs also
agreeableness (r = −.143, p < .05), conscientiousness (r = .256, indicated good reliabilities of the measures (Fornell and Larcker, 1981).
p < .01), emotional stability (r = −.159, p < .01), openness to Fourth, the discriminant validity of the measures was assessed by
experience (r = .271, p < .01), and work position (r = −.125, p < comparing the square root of a variable’s average variance extracted
.05); safety motivation was related to conscientiousness (r = .189, p < (AVE) and correlations involving the variable (Fornell and Larcker,
.01), emotional stability (r = −.162, p < .01), and openness to ex- 1981). Results in Table 5 indicated that for all the individual variables,
perience (r = .234, p < .01). In the tests of related hypotheses, ex- the square root of AVE for safety compliance, safety participation, risk
traversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability, perception, and safety motivation was .785, .710, .782, and .761,
openness to experience, and work position were included. Other con- greater than the correlations involving the variable. The correlation
trols were not included in our analyses as their inclusion had no sub- between supervisor’s safety climate and coworkers’ safety climate
stantive effect on the results (Becker, 2005). (.807), however, was greater than the square root of AVE for coworkers’
safety climate (.773) and supervisor’s safety climate (.744). This was
acceptable as the two dimensions of safety climate are distinct but
4.2. Validity of measures
highly correlated (Brondino et al., 2012). Thus, discriminant validity of
measures was largely demonstrated.
We conducted the following analyses to assess the validity of our
Collectively, the above results demonstrated that the current mea-
measures in this data.
sures had a six-factor structure, good criterion-related validity, and
First, we conducted confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to examine
reasonable convergent and discriminant validity, thus demonstrating
the structure validity of the measures (DiStefano and Hess, 2005). As
the validity of our measures.
shown in Table 3, our hypothesized six-factor model had a reasonable
fit to the data (Browne and Cudeck, 1992; Hu and Bentler, 1999).
Compared to alternative models, our baseline model provided sub- 4.3. Aggregation of group-level variables
stantial improvement in fit indexes. Thus, the structure validity of our
measures was demonstrated. For supervisor’s safety climate, the median value of within-group
Second, we examined the criterion-related validity. As reported in interrater reliability (Rwg) was .98 (Rwg > .70; George and
Table 2, at the individual level, safety compliance was significantly Bettenhausen, 1990), intra-class correlation (ICC(1)) was .69 (ICC(1) ≥
correlated with safety participation (r =.667, p < .01), and safety .12; James et al., 1993), and ICC(2) was .95 (ICC(2) ≥ .60; Bartko,
motivation was significantly correlated with safety compliance (r = 1976). There was also significant between-group variance in super-
.649, p < .01) and participation (r =.530, p < .01); at the group visor’s safety climate, F(34, 270) = 20.768, p < .001. For coworkers’
level, supervisors’ safety climate was significantly correlated with safety climate, the median value of Rwg was .98, ICC(1) was .68, and

6
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Table 3
Confirmatory factor analysis for testing structure validity.
Model χ2 (df, p) Δχ2 CFI TLI SRMR RMSEA

Six factors (baseline model): safety compliance, safety participation, risk perception, safety motivation, 1798.016 .912 .908 .068 .065
supervisor’s safety climate, coworkers’ safety climate (803, p < .01)
Five factors: collapsing safety compliance and safety participation 2883.255 1085.239** .786 .773 .080 .091
(850, p < .01)
Five factors: collapsing supervisor’s safety climate and coworkers’ safety climate 3073.997 1275.981** .766 .752 .080 .095
(850, p < .01)
One factor: collapsing all the variables studied 5636.505 3838.489** .598 .473 .141 .138
(860, p < .01)

** p < .01, two-tailed.

Table 4 Table 5
Results of convergent validity examination. Results of discriminant validity examination.
Variable Indicator Standardized Composite Average Variables 1 2 3 4
factor loading reliability variance
(SFL) (CR) extracted Individual level, construction worker
(AVE) 1. Safety compliance (.785)
2. Safety participation .667** (.710)
Safety compliance SC1 .730 .866 .617 3. Risk perception −.234** −.234** (.782)
SC2 .820 4. Safety motivation .649** .530** −.203** (.761)
SC3 .790 Group level, workgroup
SC4 .800 1. Supervisor’s safety climate (.733)
Safety SP1 .791 .752 .504 2. Coworkers’ safety climate .807** (.744)
participation SP2 .653
SP3 .678 Notes: **p < .01, two-tailed. The square roots of AVEs are reported in par-
Risk perception PER1 .599 .933 .611 entheses along the diagonal of the correlation of variables.
PER2 .755
PER3 .883
PER4 .773 p < .05; see Model 1 (H1a)) and participation (B = −.214, SD = .061,
PER5 .727 p < .01; see Model 1 (H1b)), thereby supporting H1a and H1b, re-
PER6 .868 spectively. Accordingly, the competing hypothesis H1’ was rejected.
PER7 .779 Risk perception was significantly and negatively related to safety mo-
PER8 .812
tivation (B = −.187, SD = .080, p < .01; see Model 1 (H2)), thereby
PER9 .802
Safety motivation SM1 .770 .872 .579 supporting H2.
SM2 .842 For the mediation effects, after controlling for safety motivation as a
SM3 .710 mediator, the relationship between risk perception and safety com-
SM4 .686
pliance was reduced (B = −.115, SD = .071, p < .05; see Model 2
SM5 .785
Supervisor’s safety SSC1 .773 .920 .537 (H3a) in Table 6) and the relationship between risk perception and
climate SSC2 .735 safety participation was reduced (B = −.130, SD = .086, p < .05; see
SSC3 .780 Model 2 (H3b) in Table 6), providing evidence of a partial mediation. In
SSC4 .528 addition, if the 95% bias-corrected confidence interval of a mediation
SSC5 .805
effect excludes zero, the mediation effect is significant (Preacher et al.,
SSC6 .762
SSC7 .679 2004). The 95% bias-corrected confidence interval for the mediation
SSC8 .793 effect in H3a was (-.121, -.032) and was (-.134, -.029) for the mediation
SSC9 .713 effect in H3b. Thus, H3a and H3b were supported.
SSC10 .722
According to Table 7, the interaction effects of risk perception with
Coworkers’ safety CSC1 .732 .936 .554
climate CSC2 .748
supervisor’s safety climate (B = −1.851, SD = .658, p < .05) and
CSC3 .735 coworkers’ safety climate (B = 1.930, SD = .356, p < .05) on safety
CSC4 .580 motivation were both significant.
CSC5 .731 To further identify how each moderator moderates the relationship
CSC6 .830
between risk perception and safety motivation, we drew simple slopes
CSC7 .667
CSC8 .555 for the two moderating effects in Figs. 2 and 3. According to Fig. 2,
CSC9 .764 under a positive supervisor’s safety climate, risk perception was nega-
CSC10 .817 tively related to safety motivation; however, under a negative super-
CSC11 .897
visor’s safety climate, risk perception was positively related to safety
CSC12 .801
motivation. That is, although the moderating effect of supervisor’s
safety climate was significant, this effect was contrary to H4a (Super-
ICC(2) was .95. There was also significant between-group variance in visor’s safety climate moderates the relationship between risk percep-
coworkers’ safety climate, F(34, 276) = 9.551, p < .001. Hence, it tion and safety motivation. Specifically, when supervisor’s safety cli-
was appropriate to aggregate individual perceptions of supervisor’s mate is positive, the negative effect of risk perception on safety
safety climate and coworkers’ safety climate to the group level. motivation is weaker or may change to be positive, compared to when
supervisor’s safety climate is negative). Taken together, H4a was not
supported. According to Fig. 3, under a positive coworkers’ safety cli-
4.4. Hypotheses testing
mate, risk perception was positively related to safety motivation;
however, under a negative coworkers’ safety climate, risk perception
For the main effects, results in Table 6 show that risk perception was
was negatively related to safety motivation. Hence, this study
negatively related to safety compliance (B = −.167, SD = .042,

7
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Table 6
Hierarchical linear modeling estimates for the relationships among risk perception, safety motivation, and safety behaviors (main and mediation effects).
Predictor Safety motivation Safety compliance Safety participation

Model 1 (H2) Model 1 (H1a) Model 2 (H3a) Model 1 (H1b) Model 2 (H3b)

Intercept 3.705*** (.769) 3.920*** (.368) 2.466** (1.170) 3.651*** (.425) 2.490*** (1.814)
Control variable
Extraversion .143 (.101) .140 (.107)
Agreeableness −.124 (.052) −.195 (.164)
Conscientiousness .042 (.104) .025 (.041) .044 (.166) .063 (.057) .055 (.211)
Emotional stability −.063 (.098) −.048 (.038) −.042 (.092) −.048 (.102) −.007 (.169)
Openness to experience .110* (.068) .060 (.075) −.017 (.296) .065 (.079) .076 (.083)
Work position .039 (.215) .047 (.137)
Direct effect
Risk perception −.187** (.080) −.167* (.042) −.115* (.071) −.214** (.061) −.130* (.086)
Safety motivation .283** (.034) .243** (.143)

Notes: N = 311 at the individual level; N = 35 at the group level. Entries are standardized estimates. Standard deviations are in parentheses. *p < .05, **p < .01,
***p < .001, two-tailed.

Table 7
Hierarchical linear modeling estimates for the moderating effects of super-
visor’s safety climate and coworkers’ safety climate in the relationship between
risk perception and safety motivation (moderation effects).
Predictor Safety motivation

B SD

Intercept 4.153* 1.754


Level 1 direct effect
Risk perception −.806 .527
Level 2 direct effect
Supervisor’s safety climate (clim_supervisor) 1.738** 1.237
Coworkers’ safety climate −1.968** 1.336
(clim_cowo)
Interaction effect
Risk perception × clim_supervisor −1.851* .658
Risk perception × clim_cowo 1.930* .356

Notes: N = 311 at the individual level; N = 35 at the group level. B represents Fig. 3. Moderating effect of coworkers’ safety climate on the relationship be-
standardized estimates. SD represents standard deviations. Controls were not tween risk perception and safety motivation. clim_cowo = coworkers’ safety
reported in this table. *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, two-tailed. climate.

perception to safety compliance via safety motivation at higher levels


(+1 standard deviation) was −1.044 (SD = .521, p < .05) and was
−.647 (SD = .336, p = .063) at lower levels (-1 standard deviation) of
supervisor’s safety climate as a group-level moderator. Moreover,
bootstrapping results revealed that the indirect effect from risk per-
ception to safety compliance (through safety motivation) was sig-
nificant at high levels (95% CI = [−2.193, −0.064]) but not sig-
nificant at low levels (95% CI [−1.212, 0.004]) of supervisor’s safety
climate. This means that at both lower and higher levels of supervisor’s
safety climate, the effect of risk perception on safety compliance via
safety motivation was negative, and the negative effect was significant
and greater at higher levels of supervisor’s safety climate. To sum-
marize, contrary to our hypothesis (H5a), supervisor’s safety climate
did not alleviate but worsened the negative effect of risk perception on
safety compliance.
Similar results were also obtained for the test of H5b: The estimate
of the conditional indirect relationship of risk perception to safety
Fig. 2. Moderating effect of supervisor’s safety climate on the relationship be-
participation via safety motivation at higher levels (+1 standard de-
tween risk perception and safety motivation. clim_supervisor = supervisor’s
viation) was −.563 (SD = .312, p < .05) and was −.321 (SD = .187,
safety climate.
p < .05) at lower levels (-1 standard deviation) of supervisor’s safety
climate as a group-level moderator. Moreover, bootstrapping results
supported H4b (Coworkers’ safety climate moderates the relationship revealed that the indirect effect from risk perception to participation
between risk perception and safety motivation. Specifically, when (through safety motivation) was significant at both high levels (95% CI
coworkers’ safety climate is positive, the negative effect of risk per- = [−1.341, −0.036]) and low levels (95% CI = [−0.876, −0.005])
ception on safety motivation is weaker or may change to be positive, of supervisor’s safety climate. This means that at both lower and higher
compared to when the coworker safety climate is negative). levels of supervisor’s safety climate, the effect of risk perception on
The estimate of the conditional indirect relationship of risk

8
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

safety participation via safety motivation was significant and negative, of risk perception on behavior were both reversed to be positive. This
and the negative effect was greater at higher levels of supervisor’s implies that a positive coworkers’ safety climate helped to change
safety climate. That is, contrary to our hypothesis (H5b), supervisor’s workers’ perceptions of workplace risks from negative (hindrances) to
safety climate did not alleviate but worsened the negative effect of risk positive (challenges). However, an adverse moderation effect of su-
perception on safety participation. pervisor’s safety climate was identified.
The estimate of the conditional indirect relationship of risk per-
ception to safety compliance via safety motivation at higher levels (+1 5.1. Theoretical contributions
standard deviation) was .932 (SD = .516, p < .05) and was −.533
(SD = .321, p = .115) at lower levels (-1 standard deviation) of cow- This study offers a new theoretical perspective on risk perception
orkers’ safety climate as a group-level moderator. Moreover, boot- and its impact on safety behavior. The previous findings concerning this
strapping results revealed that the indirect effect from risk perception to impact are largely contradictory. Some research has supported a posi-
safety compliance (through safety motivation) was significant at high tive impact (Kouabenan et al., 2015; Seo, 2005; Tomás et al., 1999; Xia
levels (95% CI = [0.015, 2.155]) but not significant at low levels (95% et al., 2017), while other studies have supported a negative impact
CI = [−0.008, 1.379]) of coworkers’ safety climate. This means that at (Goldenhar et al., 2003; Nahrgang et al., 2011). It can be concluded
lower levels of a coworkers’ safety climate, the effect of risk perception that the former studies mainly consider safety behavior as risk-avoiding
on safety compliance via safety motivation was negative; however, at behavior that employees implement to reduce their risk. The latter re-
higher levels of coworkers’ safety climate, the effect of risk perception search mainly reviews perceived risk as job hindrance demands (i.e.,
on safety behavior via safety motivation was significantly positive. stresses) that exhaust the personal resources required for demonstrating
Hence, as we expected in H6a, coworkers’ safety climate alleviated the safety behavior. We propose that this conflict can be addressed by
negative effect of risk perception on safety compliance via safety mo- drawing on the recent development of the job demand concept, namely,
tivation: when coworkers’ safety climate was at higher levels, the the hindrance demand–challenge demand distinction (Bakker and
moderation effect was strong and reversed this indirect impact from Demerouti, 2017; Searle and Auton, 2015). Risk perception is a dual
negative to positive. concept by nature, and whether perceived risk is viewed as a job hin-
Similar results were obtained for the test of H6b: The estimate of the drance or challenge and, subsequently, whether it negatively or posi-
conditional indirect relationship of risk perception to safety participa- tively impacts safety behavior is basically an empirical question. Our
tion via safety motivation at higher levels (+1 standard deviation) was findings showed that perceived risk was viewed as a job hindrance
.497 (SD = .314, p < .05) and was −.302 (SD = .170, p = .108) at demand by construction workers, and thus high-risk perceptions led to
lower levels (-1 standard deviation) of coworkers’ safety climate as a low levels of safety behavior. The dual hindrance and challenge de-
group-level moderator. Moreover, bootstrapping results revealed that mand perspective on the concept of risk perception provides a new
the indirect effect from risk perception to safety participation (through theoretical reference for conceptualizing risk perception and synthe-
safety motivation) was significant at high levels (95% CI = [0.032, sizing the conflict relationship between risk perception and safety be-
1.289]) but not significant at low levels (95% CI = [−0.005, 0.785]) of havior examined in different occupations.
coworkers’ safety climate. This means that at lower levels of a cow- This study broadens our understanding of the role of supervisor
orkers’ safety climate, the effect of risk perception on safety participa- safety supervision and leadership in protecting and boosting employee
tion via safety motivation was negative; however, at higher levels of safety. Most previous studies have underscored the positive effect of
coworkers’ safety climate, the effect of risk perception on safety beha- safety-oriented supervisors in preventing unsafe behavior and pro-
vior via safety motivation was significantly positive. Thus, as we ex- moting safety behavior (e.g., Clarke, 2013; Fang et al., 2015; Zohar and
pected in H6b, coworkers’ safety climate indeed alleviated the negative Polachek, 2014). It was also found that the safety climate, including
effect of risk perception on safety participation via safety motivation: supervisor’s and coworkers’ climate, buffered the deleterious impact of
when coworkers’ safety climate was at higher levels, the moderating job demands (i.e., work–family conflict and job insecurity) on safety
effect of coworkers’ safety climate was strong and reversed this indirect behavior (Bronkhorst, 2015). Our findings further supported the ex-
impact from negative to positive. isting literature by confirming that coworkers’ safety climate reversed
the negative impacts of the risk perception on safety motivation and
5. Discussion behavior to be positive. However, contrary to the safety literature and
our expectations, when supervisors’ safety climate changed from a low
In targeting construction workers, this study investigated the direct to a high level, the impact of risk perception on safety motivation
effect of risk perception on safety behavior and the indirect effect from changed from positive to negative, and the negative effect of risk per-
risk perception to safety behavior via safety motivation. Moreover, we ception on safety behavior via safety motivation was not alleviated but
explored whether two salient safety climate dimensions––supervisor’s worsened.
and coworkers’––in the construction industry moderated the relation- The different effects of supervisor’s safety climate and coworkers’
ship between risk perception and safety motivation and the mediated safety climate may be explained from the perspective of work stress.
relationship. Our study is unique in that we drew on the dual nature of Work stress can be derived from work requirements (Nahrgang et al.,
the job demand concept, and proposed that risk perception can be 2011) and interpersonal relations (Weiss et al., 1976). In the present
viewed as a job hindrance demand or a job challenge demand de- study, most of the items for measuring supervisors’ safety climate were
pending on the context, which will have either a negative or positive imperatives, such as “The supervisor will ask us to abide by all safety
impact on safety behavior. Accordingly, two competing hypotheses rules” and “The supervisor will still strictly comply with the safety rules
concerning the impact of risk perception on safety behavior were pro- when we are tired.” These items reflect the characteristics of a pa-
posed. For the construction workers studied, it was found that risk ternalistic leader defined as “a style that combines strong discipline and
perception was a job hindrance that reduced safety motivation and, authority with fatherly benevolence” (Farh and Cheng, 2000, p. 91).
consequently, both safety compliance and participation behavior. We Paternalistic leadership imposes work stress on employees through
expected that both supervisor’s and coworkers’ safety climate would strict work requirements and/or interpersonal tensions, thus worsening
alleviate the negative impact of risk perception on safety motivation the negative effects of risk perception on safety motivation and beha-
and then on behavior, or may change the impact from negative to po- vior (Chao and Kao, 2005; Chen and Kao, 2009). On the contrary,
sitive. Our results demonstrated this function of coworkers’ safety cli- coworkers’ safety climate items such as “Team members will actively
mate: for workers in a positive coworkers’ safety climate, the negative discuss how to prevent accidents” and “Team members often remind me
effect of risk perception on motivation and the indirect negative effect to stay safe” show that workers maintain a good relationship with each

9
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

other. These activities helped to ease the stress (risk perception as job substantive factors + 1 method factor) has poorer fit indexes (χ2=
hindrance demands) placed on workers and subsequent consequences 2255.955, df = 802, p < .01; CFI = .847; TLI = .828; SRMR = .064;
produced by the stress, or even changed their negative perceptions of RMSEA = .079) than the baseline six-factor model (χ2 = 1798.016,
workplace risks as hindrances (followed by a negative reaction, namely, df = 803, p < .01; CFI = .912; TLI = .908; SRMR = .068; RMSEA =
a reduction in safety motivation and behavior) to positive perceptions .065). Furthermore, Evans (1985) conducted a Monte Carlo study and
as challenges (followed by a positive reaction, namely, an improvement concluded that common method variance can only alleviate true in-
in safety motivation and behavior). In addition, using the lens of the teractions rather than generating artificial interactions. Spector (1987)
JD–R theory, paternalistic supervisor’s safety climate can be viewed as also argued that interactions were unlikely to be a consequence of
a job hindrance demand while friendly coworkers’ safety climate tends method variance. As moderation effects were found in our research,
to be a job resource, as we hypothesized. A job hindrance demand will common method variance should not pose a serious threat to the study’s
worsen the negative effects of another job hindrance on work engage- validity. Nevertheless, to mitigate common method variance, we sug-
ment and performance, while a job resource will alleviate these effects gest that future research should try to collect data from different
(Bakker and Demerouti, 2017). We contribute to the literature by sources (objective observation or supervisor-assessment of worker
pointing out the potential negative role of certain supervisor safety safety behavior) to examine and validate the relationships found in the
activities in influencing employee safety. present study.
Last, our study also contributes to the hindrance–challenge job de- We suggest two future research directions. To the best of our
mand framework. The concept of job demands was first proposed as job knowledge, the dual job hindrance and challenge perspective on the
hindrances that decreased employees’ work engagement and perfor- concept of risk perception was first proposed here, hence, more work is
mance (Demerouti et al., 2001). However, recent empirical studies have warranted on this issue. For example, the perception of workplace risks
found that some job demands, such as job responsibility, can promote and its impact on safety behavior should be widely examined in various
work engagement and performance (Crawford et al., 2010). Such job high-risk contexts. These empirical studies may help to further reveal
demands are redefined as challenge demands that cost effort but si- the conditions under which perceived risk serves as a job hindrance
multaneously contribute to learning and goal achievement (Bakker and demand versus a job challenge demand; eventually, the synthesis of the
Demerouti, 2017; Breevaart and Bakker, 2017). Furthermore, a demand conflict risk perception–safety behavior relationship from the dual job
such as work pressure can be experienced as a job hindrance or chal- hindrance and challenge perspective can be achieved. The role of su-
lenge in different contexts (Searle and Auton, 2015). Research has also pervisor’s safety climate in the relationship between risk perception and
found that demands are sometimes considered as both hindering and safety behavior should also be further clarified. As we focused mainly
challenging (Searle and Auton, 2015). Thus, our research fine-tunes the on paternalistic safety leadership, other leadership styles, such as
hindrance–challenge job demand distinction in JD–R theory, by con- transformational leadership, should be investigated. In so doing, we can
ceptualizing risk perception as a concept that can be a job hindrance or develop a holistic picture of the role of supervisors in influencing em-
challenge, as well as by demonstrating that job demands (risk percep- ployee safety.
tion) can be both hindrances and challenges with the moderating roles
of coworkers’ safety climate. 6. Conclusions

5.2. Implications for practice This study draws on the hindrance–challenge job demand concept,
proposing that perceived risk is by nature a dual concept. Whether
Given the hindrance attribute of risk perception for construction perceived risk is viewed as a job hindrance or challenge and, subse-
workers and its negative impact on workers’ safety behavior, the quently, whether it negatively or positively impacts safety behavior is
management should assess and take measures to reduce risk perception basically an empirical question that depends on context. Empirical data
levels in construction sites. Possible measures include providing suffi- collected from construction workers demonstrated that risk perception
cient individual protection equipment, removing obstacles in con- served as a job hindrance, which had a further negative impact on
struction sites, and revealing safety management systems for removing safety motivation and behavior. This study also confirmed that cow-
workers’ misperceptions of workplace risk. Considering the potential orkers’ safety climate helped workers to change their perceptions of
dual attribute of risk perception, it is suggested that the management in workplace risks from negative (a job hindrance) to positive (a job
other occupations should first investigate the relationship between challenge), thereby motivating them to demonstrate safety behavior.
perceived risk and safety behavior, and then decide to take measures to However, when supervisor’s safety climate changed from a low level to
increase or decrease employees’ risk perception levels. a high level, the impact of risk perception on safety motivation changed
Regarding the buffer effect of coworkers’ safety climate against the from positive to negative, and the negative effect of risk perception on
deleterious impacts risk perception has on safety motivation and then safety behavior via safety motivation was not alleviated but worsened.
on safety behavior, management should commit itself to creating po- Overall, this study provides a theoretical framework and empirical
sitive cooperation and communication concerning safety among evidence for the risk perception concept and its relationship to safety
workers. More importantly, the management should realize that safety- behavior, and indicates the potentially different role of supervisor’s and
oriented supervisors will not always promote workers’ safety behavior; coworker’s safety climate in the formulation of employee safety.
for example, paternalistic safety leadership may produce negative im-
pacts on safety motivation and behavior. Declaration of Competing Interest

5.3. Limitations and future directions None. The authors claim that this paper is only being considered for
Accident Analysis & Prevention. All of the authors and their responsible
One limitation in our research design is that our data were workers’ authorities have approved the manuscript and agreed its final submis-
self-reports. Thus, common method variance may exist. As suggested by sion.
Podsakoff et al. (2003), we used statistical remedies to test common
method variance. First, Harman’s single-factor test showed that the one- Acknowledgements
factor model fit the data poorly (χ2 = 5636.505, df = 860, p < .01;
CFI = .598; TLI = .473; SRMR = .141; RMSEA = .138). We also used This work was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for
the technique of controlling for the effects of a single unmeasured latent the Central Universities (Grant Nos. 2242019R10, 2019B69014),
method factor. The CFA results indicated that the revised model (6 Research Foundation of Humanities and Social Sciences of Ministry of

10
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

Education of China (18YJC630121), the National Natural Science Organizations. Management and Organizations in the Chinese Context. Palgrave
Foundation of China (Grant Nos. 71772736, 71722004), and the Macmillan, London, pp. 84–127.
Fogarty, G.J., Shaw, A., 2010. Safety climate and the theory of planned behavior: towards
Postgraduate Research & Practice Innovation Program of Jiangsu the prediction of unsafe behavior. Accid. Anal. Prev. 42 (5), 1455–1459.
Province (Grant No. SJKY19_0399). We would like to thank the parti- Fornell, C., Larcker, D.F., 1981. Structural equation models with unobservable variable
cipants who provided us with valuable empirical data. Particular thanks and measurement error: algebra and statistics. J. Mark. Res. 18 (3), 382–388.
Frone, M.R., 1998. Predictors of work injuries among employed adolescents. J. Appl.
also go to the editor and anonymous reviewers for their work and Psychol. 83 (4), 565–576.
contributions. George, J.M., Bettenhausen, K., 1990. Understanding prosocial behavior, sales perfor-
mance, and turnover: a group–level analysis in a service context. J. Appl. Psychol. 75
(6), 698–709.
References Goldenhar, M.L., Williams, L.J., Swanson, N.G., 2003. Modelling relationships between
job stressors and injury and near-miss outcomes for construction labourers. Work
Akintoye, A., 2000. Analysis of factors influencing project cost estimating practice. Stress 17 (3), 218–240.
Constr. Manage. Econ. 18, 77–89. Griffin, M.A., Hu, X., 2013. How leaders differentially motivate safety compliance and
Aven, T., Renn, O., 2009. On risk defined as an event where the outcome is uncertain. J. safety participation: the role of monitoring, inspiring, and learning. Saf. Sci. 60,
Risk Res. 12 (1), 1–11. 196–202.
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., 2014. Job demands-resources theory. In: Cooper, C., Chen, P. Griffin, M.A., Neal, A., 2000. Perceptions of safety at work: a framework for linking safety
(Eds.), Wellbeing: A Complete Reference Guide. Wiley-Blackwell, Chichester, UK. climate to safety performance, knowledge, and motivation. J. Occup. Health Psychol.
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., 2017. Job demands–resources theory: taking stock and 5 (3), 347–358.
looking forward. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 22 (3), 273–285. Gyekye, S.A., Salminen, S., 2005. Are “good soldiers” safety conscious? An examination of
Bakker, A.B., Demerouti, E., Sanz-Vergel, A.I., 2014. Burnout and work engagement: the the relationship between organizational citizenship behaviors and perception of
JD-R approach. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 1, 389–411. workplace safety. Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 33 (8), 805–820.
Bakker, A.B., Sanz-Vergel, A.I., 2013. Weekly work engagement and flourishing: the role Hair, J.F., Black, W.C., Babin, B.J., Anderson, R.E., 2009. Multivariate Data Analysis.
of hindrance and challenge job demands. J. Vocat. Behav. 83 (3), 397–409. Prentice-Hall, Upper Saddle River, NJ.
Barbaranelli, C., Petitta, L., Probst, T.M., 2015. Does safety climate predict safety per- Halbesleben, J.R.B., Neveu, J.P., Paustian-underdahl, S.C., Westman, M., 2014. Getting to
formance in Italy and the USA? Cross-cultural validation of a theoretical model of the “COR”: understanding the role of resources in conservation of resources theory. J.
safety climate. Accid. Anal. Prev. 77, 35–44. Manage. 40 (5), 1334–1364.
Bartko, J.J., 1976. On various intraclass correlation reliability coefficients. Psychol. Bull. Haslam, R.A., Hide, S.A., Gibb, A.G.F., Gyi, D.E., Pavitt, T., Atkinson, S., Duff, A.R., 2005.
83, 762–765. Contributing factors in construction accidents. Appl. Ergon. 36 (4), 401–415.
Bauer, D.J., Preacher, K.J., Gil, K.M., 2006. Conceptualizing and testing random indirect Heinrich, H.W., Petersen, D.C., Roos, N.R., Hazlett, S., 1980. Industrial Accident
effects and moderated mediation in multilevel models: new procedures and re- Prevention: A Safety Management Approach. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
commendations. Psychol. Methods 11 (2), 142. Hobfoll, S.E., 1989. Conservation of resources: a new attempt at conceptualizing stress.
Becker, T.E., 2005. Potential problems in the statistical control of variables in organiza- Am. Psychol. 44, 513–524.
tional research: a qualitative analysis with recommendations. Organ. Res. Methods 8 Hobfoll, S.E., 2001. The influence of culture, community, and the nested-self in the stress
(3), 274–289. process: advancing conservation of resources theory. Appl. Psychol. 50 (3), 337–421.
Beus, J.M., Dhanani, L.Y., McCord, M.A., 2015. A meta-analysis of personality and Hobfoll, S.E., 2002. Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Rev. Gen. Psychol.
workplace safety: addressing unanswered questions. J. Appl. Psychol. 100 (2), 6, 307–324.
481–498. Hofmann, D.A., Morgeson, F.P., Gerras, S.J., 2003. Climate as a moderator of the re-
Bliese, P.D., 2000. An introduction to multilevel modeling techniques. Pers. Psychol. 53 lationship between leader-member exchange and content specific citizenship: safety
(4), 1062–1065. climate as an exemplar. J. Appl. Psychol. 88 (1), 170–178.
Breevaart, K., Bakker, A.B., 2017. Daily job demands and employee work engagement: the Hu, L.T., Bentler, P.M., 1999. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure ana-
role of daily transformational leadership behavior. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 23 (3), lysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Eq. Model.- Multidiscip. J.
338–349. 6 (1), 1–55.
Brondino, M., Silva, S.A., Pasini, M., 2012. Multilevel approach to organizational and International Labour Organization, 2018. Safety and Health at Work. Retrieved from:.
group safety climate and safety performance: co-workers as the missing link. Saf. Sci. https://www.ilo.org/global/topics/safety-and-health-at-work/lang–en/index.htm.
50 (9), 1847–1856. James, L.R., Demaree, R.G., Wolf, G., 1993. Rwg: an assessment of within-group inter-
Bronkhorst, B., 2015. Behaving safely under pressure: the effects of job demands, re- rater agreement. J. Appl. Psychol. 78, 306–309.
sources, and safety climate on employee physical and psychosocial safety behavior. J. Kao, K.Y., Spitzmueller, C., Cigularov, K., Wu, H., 2016. Linking insomnia to workplace
Safety Res. 55, 63–72. injuries: a moderated mediation model of supervisor safety priority and safety be-
Browne, M.W., Cudeck, R., 1992. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol. Methods havior. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 21 (1), 91–104.
Res. 21 (2), 230–258. Koopman, J., Lanaj, K., Scott, B.A., 2016. Integrating the bright and dark sides of OCB: a
Chao, A., Kao, S., 2005. Paternalistic Leadership and subordinated stress in Taiwanese daily investigation of the benefits and costs of helping others. Acad. Manage. J. 59
enterprises. Res. Appl. Psychol. 27 (1), 111–131. (2), 414–435.
Chen, C.F., Chen, S.C., 2014. Measuring the effects of safety management system prac- Kouabenan, D.R., Ngueutsa, R., Mbaye, S., 2015. Safety climate, perceived risk, and in-
tices, morality leadership and self-efficacy on pilots’ safety behaviors: safety moti- volvement in safety management. Saf. Sci. 77 (3), 72–79.
vation as a mediator. Saf. Sci. 62, 376–385. Li, F., Jiang, L., Yao, X., Li, Y.J., 2013. Job demands, job resources and safety outcomes:
Chen, H.Y., Kao, H.S.R., 2009. Chinese paternalistic leadership and non-Chinese sub- the roles of emotional exhaustion and safety compliance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 51,
ordinates’ psychological health. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manage. 20 (12), 2533–2546. 243–251.
Cheung, M.L., Au, K., 2005. Applications of multilevel structural equation modeling to Li, J.D., 2013. Psychometric properties of ten-item personality inventory in China. China
cross-cultural research. Struct. Equ. Model. 12 (4), 598–619. J. Health Psychol. 21, 1688–1692. https://doi.org/10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2013.11.
Christian, M.S., Bradley, J.C., Wallace, J.C., Burke, M.J., 2009. Workplace safety: a meta- 008. (in Chinese).
analysis of the roles of person and situation factors. J. Appl. Psychol. 94 (5), Liang, H.K., Lin, K.Y., Zhang, S.J., Su, Y.K., 2018. The impact of coworkers’ safety vio-
1103–1127. lations on an individual worker: a social contagion effect within the construction
Clarke, S., 2013. Safety leadership: a meta-analytic review of transformational and crew. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15 (4), 773.
transactional leadership styles as antecedents of safety behaviours. J. Occup. Organ. MacKinnon, D.P., Lockwood, C.M., Williams, J., 2004. Confidence limits for the indirect
Psychol. 86 (1), 22–49. effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behav. Res.
Crawford, E.R., LePine, J.A., Rich, B.L., 2010. Linking job demands and resources to 39 (1), 99–128.
employee engagement and burnout: a theoretical extension and meta-analytic test. J. Morrow, S.L., McGonagle, A.K., Dove-Steinkamp, M.L., Walker Jr, C.T., Marmet, M.,
Appl. Psychol. 95 (5), 834. Barnes-Farrell, J.L., 2010. Relationships between psychological safety climate facets
Demerouti, E., Bakker, A.B., Nachreiner, F., Schaufeli, W.B., 2001. The job demands- and safety behavior in the rail industry: a dominance analysis. Accid. Anal. Prev. 42
resources model of burnout. J. Appl. Psychol. 86 (3), 499–512. (5), 1460–1467.
Didla, S., Mearns, K., Flin, R., 2009. Safety citizenship behaviour: a proactive approach to Murie, F., 2007. Building safety: an international perspective. Int. J. Occup. Environ.
risk management. J. Risk Res. 12 (3–4), 475–483. Health 13 (1), 5–11.
DiStefano, C., Hess, B., 2005. Using confirmatory factor analysis for construct validation: Muthén, L.K., Muthén, B.O., 2013. Mplus 7.11. Los Angeles, CA.
an empirical review. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 23 (3), 225–241. Nahrgang, J.D., Morgeson, F.P., Hofmann, D.A., 2011. Safety at work: a meta-analytic
Dov, Z., 2008. Safety climate and beyond: a multi-level multi-climate framework. Saf. Sci. investigation of the link between job demands, job resources, burnout, engagement,
46 (3), 376–387. and safety outcomes. J. Appl. Psychol. 96 (1), 71–94.
Evans, M.G., 1985. A Monte Carlo study of the effects of correlated method variance in Neal, A., Griffin, M.A., 2006. A study of the lagged relationships among safety climate,
moderated multiple regression analysis. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 36 (3), safety motivation, safety behavior, and accidents at the individual and group levels.
305–323. J. Appl. Psychol. 91 (4), 946–953.
Fang, D.P., Wu, C.L., Wu, H.J., 2015. Impact of the supervisor on worker safety behavior Nunnally, J.O., 1978. Psychometric Theory. McGraw-Hill, New York, NY.
in construction projects. J. Manag. Eng. 31 (6), 04015001. Podsakoff, P.M., MacKenzie, S.B., Lee, J.-Y., Podsakoff, N.P., 2003. Common method
Fang, D.P., Wu, H.J., 2013. Development of a Safety Culture Interaction (SCI) model for biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended
construction projects. Saf. Sci. 57, 138–149. remedies. J. Appl. Psychol. 88, 879–903.
Farh, J.L., Cheng, B.S., 2000. A Cultural Analysis of Paternalistic Leadership in Chinese Preacher, K.J., Hayes, A.F., 2004. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects

11
N. Xia, et al. Accident Analysis and Prevention 134 (2020) 105350

in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 36 (4), 717–731. prosocial behavior and its influence on well-being for the helper and recipient. J.
Raudenbush, S.W., Bryk, A.S., 2002. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 222–244.
Analysis Methods. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA. Weiss, S., Baker, G., Gupta, R., 1976. Vibrational residual stress relief in a plain carbon
Schaufeli, W.B., Bakker, A.B., van Rhenen, W., 2009. How changes in job demands and steel weldment. Welding Res. Suppl. 55 (2) 47s–51s.
resources predict burnout, work engagement, and sickness absenteeism. J. Organ. Williamson, G.M., Clark, M.S., 1989. Providing help and desired relationship type as
Behav. 30, 893–917. determinants of changes in moods and self-evaluations. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 56,
Schwatka, N.V., Rosecrance, J.C., 2016. Safety climate and safety behaviors in the con- 722–734.
struction industry: the importance of co-workers commitment to safety. Work 54 (2), Xia, N., Wang, X., Griffin, M.A., Wu, C., Liu, B., 2017. Do we see how they perceive risk?
401–413. An integrated analysis of risk perception and its effect on workplace safety behavior.
Searle, B.J., Auton, J.C., 2015. The merits of measuring challenge and hindrance ap- Accid. Anal. Prev. 106, 234–242.
praisals. Anxiety Stress Coping 28 (2), 121–143. Yu, Q., Ding, L., Zhou, C., Luo, H., 2014. Analysis of factors influencing safety manage-
Seo, D.C., 2005. An explicative model of unsafe work behavior. Saf. Sci. 43 (3), 187–211. ment for metro construction in China. Accid. Anal. Prev. 68, 131–138.
Sirotnik, K.A., 1980. Psychometric implications of the unit of analysis problem (with Zhao, X., Lynch Jr, J.G., Chen, Q., 2010. Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: myths and
examples from the measurement of organizational climate). J. Educ. Meas. 17 (4), truths about mediation analysis. J. Consum. Res. 37 (2), 197–206.
245–282. Zohar, D., 1980. Safety climate in industrial organizations: theoretical and applied im-
Spector, P.E., 1987. Method variance as an artifact in self-reported affect and perceptions plications. J. Appl. Psychol. 65 (1), 96–102.
at work: myth or significant problem? J. Appl. Psychol. 72 (3), 438–443. Zohar, D., 2003. Safety climate: conceptual and measurement issues. In: Quick, J.C.,
Ten Brummelhuis, L.L., Bakker, A.B., 2012. A resource perspective on the work–home Tetrick, L. (Eds.), Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology. American
interface: the work–home resources model. Am. Psychol. 67 (7), 545–556. Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Tomás, J.M., Melia, J.L., Oliver, A., 1999. A cross-validation of a structural equation Zohar, D., Polachek, T., 2014. Discourse-based intervention for modifying supervisory
model of accidents: organizational and psychological variables as predictors of work communication as leverage for safety climate and performance improvement: a
safety. Work Stress 13 (1), 49–58. randomized field study. J. Appl. Psychol. 99 (1), 113–124.
Vinodkumar, M., Bhasi, M., 2010. Safety management practices and safety behaviour: Zou, P.X.W., Sunindijo, R.Y., Dainty, A.R.J., 2014. A mixed methods research design for
assessing the mediating role of safety knowledge and motivation. Accid. Anal. Prev. bridging the gap between research and practice in construction safety. Saf. Sci. 70,
42 (6), 2082–2093. 316–326.
Weinstein, N., Ryan, R.M., 2010. When helping helps: autonomous motivation for

12

You might also like