You are on page 1of 7

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Can Anger Be Helpful?


Soldier Perceptions of the Utility of Anger
Amy B. Adler, PhD,* Daniel F. Brossart, PhD,*† and Robin L. Toblin, PhD, MPH*

Given the potential negative impact of anger reactions on health


Abstract: Studies have found that soldiers returning from combat deployment and adjustment, it is important to understand the role that anger reac-
report elevated levels of anger and aggression. The present study examined the tions play in the military occupational context. As Castro and Adler
Downloaded from https://journals.lww.com/jonmd by BhDMf5ePHKav1zEoum1tQfN4a+kJLhEZgbsIHo4XMi0hCywCX1AWnYQp/IlQrHD3i3D0OdRyi7TvSFl4Cf3VC1y0abggQZXdtwnfKZBYtws= on 09/15/2021

perception that anger was helpful in performing occupationally related duties (2011) have documented, there are unique characteristics associated
and whether this perception was associated with mental health problems, somatic with emotional expression in the military. In the case of PTSD, for ex-
symptoms, and functioning. Soldiers (N = 627) completed a survey 4 months af- ample, whereas numbing may be a diagnostic focus, anger and aggres-
ter their deployment to Afghanistan and again 3 months later. When examining sion are key components of combat-related PTSD. Such reactions also
anger over time, findings revealed four groups of different latent classes: low sta- may be compounded by the acceptance of anger in the larger military
ble (resilient), high stable (chronic), decreasing over time (improved), and in- culture. Consistent with this perspective, one qualitative study found
creasing over time (delayed problems). For two of the groups (chronic and that when soldiers were asked about their peritraumatic response to a
delayed problems), perceiving anger as helpful was closely related to anger reac- combat-related event, the most frequent response was that they reacted
tions. Perceiving anger as helpful was also associated with worse mental health based on occupational training, and the second most frequent response
symptoms. Further work in understanding how to mitigate this positive percep- was that they responded with anger (Adler et al., 2008). These angry re-
tion of anger in prevention initiatives may be useful in addressing anger reactions. sponses typically reflected a drive to payback or punish others and
Key Words: Anger reactions, aggression, military, mental health, seemed to energize the soldiers responding to a combat-related event.
latent class analysis
(J Nerv Ment Dis 2017;205: 692–698) The Positive Role of Anger
Despite being generally regarded as a negative emotion associ-

E xposure to deployment-related stressors has been linked to numer-


ous transition difficulties (Adler et al., 2011) including mental
health problems such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and de-
ated with increased risk of mental health problems, anger can, in fact,
be adaptive. If anger is aligned with a task, then anger may be useful:
emotional resources may end up being directed toward a goal, and an-
pression (Hoge et al., 2004), physical health problems (Jakupcak ger may help performance (e.g., Lazarus, 2000). In contrast, if anger
et al., 2008), difficulties with functioning (Wright et al., 2013), and an- is misaligned, then anger may interfere with performance. As Barsade
ger and aggression (e.g., Elbogen et al., 2010; Heesink et al., 2015; and Gibson (2007) suggest, there are opportunities for negative emo-
MacManus et al., 2015). In the military context, anger reactions may tions such as anger to have an important and positive role given the right
be particularly important because of their association with unethical occupational context.
conduct during deployment (Wilk et al., 2013), risky behaviors and un- Likewise, anger may be useful for leaders to resolve problems. In
healthy habits after deployment (Adler et al., 2011), and greater symp- a qualitative study of workers in the construction industry, for example,
tom severity overall (Forbes et al., 2003; Gonzalez et al., 2016). Anger Lindebaum and Fielden (2011) found that project managers enacted an-
also does not seem to decrease over the course of the postdeployment ger to ensure progress. Anger can also be used to restore equity or en-
period (Bliese et al., 2007; Heesink et al., 2015; Reijnen et al., 2015), sure fair treatment (van Doorn and Zeelenberg, 2014) and prompt
and trait anger may even contribute to the risk of developing PTSD in individuals to protest an unfair situation (Tagar et al., 2011). Studies
soldiers exposed to high levels of combat (Wilk et al., 2015). have also shown a positive link between anger and the tendency to ex-
Besides the link to deployment, anger reactions in military sam- press anger outward rather than suppress it and individual physical per-
ples have also been associated with relationship problems (Novaco et al., formance (Davis et al., 2010).
2012), intent to harm others (Gonzalez et al., 2016), and violence Given that anger can be associated with benefits, it may be that
(MacManus et al., 2013). Moreover, there are indications that serious individuals in an occupational context like the military see anger as a
problems with anger, such as intermittent explosive disorder, are positive emotion. This positive perception introduces a potential layer
associated with risk for suicide (Kessler et al., 2014). Anger reactions of complexity that would need to be understood to develop interven-
are also associated with being less likely to respond to evidence- tions designed to reduce the deleterious effects of anger in this context.
based treatments for PTSD (Lloyd et al., 2014).
The Present Study
*Center for Military Psychiatry and Neuroscience, Walter Reed Army Institute of Re-
Using a military sample, the first goal of the present study was to
search, Silver Spring, Maryland; and †Department of Educational Psychology, characterize the perception of anger as helpful in performing occupationally
Texas A&M, College Station, Texas. related duties. The goal was to identify the degree to which military person-
Send reprint requests to Amy B. Adler, PhD, Center for Military Psychiatry and nel perceived anger as a useful tool for furthering performance and how that
Neuroscience, Walter Reed Army Institute of Research, 503 Robert Grant Avenue,
Silver Spring, MD 20910. E‐mail: amy.b.adler.civ@mail.mil.
perception shifted over the course of the period after a combat deployment.
Written work prepared by employees of the Federal Government as part of their official The postdeployment period is known to be a dynamic time with potential
duties is, under the U.S. Copyright Act, a “work of the United States Government” for increasing difficulties (e.g., Adler et al., 2011; Bliese et al., 2007).
for which copyright protection under Title 17 of the United States Code is not The second goal of our study was to determine the degree to
available. As such, copyright does not extend to the contributions of employees
of the Federal Government.
which the perception of anger as helpful varied between subgroups of
ISSN: 0022-3018/17/20509–0692 soldiers based on their change in well-being over time. Specifically,
DOI: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000712 we examined mental health, somatic symptoms, and functioning

692 www.jonmd.com The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017 Can Anger Be Helpful?

because problems in each of these domains have been associated with to rate their frequency of experience in the past month on a 5-point
serving on a combat deployment (e.g., Hoge et al., 2004; Jakupcak scale (1 = never; 2 = one time; 3 = two times; 4 = three or four times;
et al., 2008; Wright et al., 2013). This analysis was designed to identify 5 = five or more times). The items, which are behaviorally anchored and
whether there is a health-related cost associated with viewing anger as have face validity for soldiers, are as follows: “get angry at someone and
helpful even if soldiers believe there is a short-term benefit in job per- yell or shout at them,” “get angry at someone and kick or smash something,
formance. Such a health-related cost is important to consider because slam the door, punch the wall, etc.,” “threaten someone with physical vio-
persistently believing in the benefits of anger may inure individuals to lence,” and “get into a fight with someone and hit the person.” These items
attempts to help them regulate their emotions. This perception may also were based on existing measures of anger, aggression, and hostility (Kulka
reinforce anger reactions that, in turn, increase relationship conflict and et al., 1990; Spector and Jex, 1998; Spielberger, 1999). Previous work by
detachment from others. Individuals who maintain a belief that anger is Wilk et al. (2015) has found two items from the trait anger scale
adaptive may be at even greater risk for health-related problems and (Spielberger et al., 1983) correlated 0.51 with these four items. In a sepa-
functional impairment. rate, unpublished analysis of different survey data with 2036 soldiers, the
Finally, the third goal of the study was to examine potential cor- four-item scale correlated 0.68 (p < 0.001) with the Dimensions of An-
relates of these subgroups to develop a more specific profile. We com- ger Reactions-5 (Forbes et al., 2004). Internal consistency on the initial
pared the different subgroups in terms of combat experiences and and follow-up survey was 0.72 and 0.78, respectively.
health-related behaviors (alcohol and sleep problems) given the link be-
tween these variables and postdeployment difficulties (e.g., Adler et al., Deployment Experiences
2009; Sundin et al., 2014; Wright et al., 2011a). A 34-item list of combat-related events such as “being attacked
or ambushed” and “handling human remains” was measured at time 1
METHODS and adapted from Hoge et al. (2004) and Adler et al. (2009). Items were
rated on a 4-point scale (never, one time, two to four times, five or more
Participants and Procedure times). Responses denoting any level of exposure to the event were
summed to create a total score ranging from 0 to 34. Coefficient alpha
Study participants were active-duty US soldiers in a brigade was not estimated for the scale because the combat exposure items are
combat team who had returned from a 12-month combat deployment formative rather than reflective measures.
to Afghanistan 4 months earlier. Participants gave informed consent be-
fore enrollment, and 1110 of the 1322 soldiers briefed on the study Mental Health
consented to participate (83.9%). The recruitment briefing, conducted PTSD symptoms were measured at 4 months after redeployment
without senior leadership present, occurred in platoon-sized groups and 3 months later using the 17-item PTSD Checklist (PCL; Weathers
(i.e., 30–40 soldiers) in classrooms on the installation. Initial (time 1) et al., 1993). This version has been used in other major postdeployment
and follow-up (time 2) survey administrations were also conducted studies (e.g., Adler et al., 2009; Hoge et al., 2004) and has been vali-
in platoon-sized groups and in installation classrooms. Time 2 oc- dated with military samples (Bliese et al., 2008; Riviere et al., 2011).
curred 3 months after the initial survey. Items are rated on a 5-point Likert scale (not at all to extremely). Sam-
Because of the nature of brigade combat teams, the sample was ple items included “repeated, disturbing memories, thoughts, or images
all male, with the majority of the sample ranging in age from 20 to of the stressful experience” and “feeling distant or cut-off from other
24 years old (68%). In terms of education, most were high school grad- people.” Internal consistency on the initial and follow-up survey was
uates (62%) or had some college or an associate's degree (32%). The 0.94 and 0.95, respectively.
majority had only been in the military for 2 years (73%), and 83% were Depression symptoms were measured on the initial and
of junior enlisted rank (E1–E4). follow-up survey using the nine-item Patient Health Questionnaire
At follow-up, soldiers were surveyed again (N = 627 or 56% of the for Depression (PHQ-9; Spitzer et al., 1999), scored on a 4-point
original sample). This attrition rate is consistent with previous studies scale (not at all to nearly every day). The PHQ-9 has been validated
(Adler et al., 2009, 2011) and reflects the procedure of tracking units rather in primary care samples (Spitzer et al., 1999) and used in postdeployment
than individuals over time to preserve anonymity. Individuals may have studies (Adler et al., 2009). Sample items included “little interest or
been unavailable at follow-up due to competing training or duty require- pleasure in doing things” and “feeling down, depressed, or hopeless.”
ments and normal personnel rotation. This study was approved by an in- Internal consistency on the initial and follow-up survey was 0.86 and
stitutional review board at the Walter Reed Army Institute of Research. 0.88, respectively.
Measures
Somatic Symptoms
Demographics Somatic symptoms were measured using 12 items from the Pa-
Demographic variables, asked in the initial survey, included sex, tient Health Questionnaire (PHQ-15; Kroenke et al., 2002). Symptoms
age category, educational background, years in the military, and rank. were rated in terms of the past 4 weeks using a three-item response scale
(1 = not bothered at all to 3 = bothered a lot). The PHQ-15 is typically
Perceptions of Anger as Helpful scored with two items that are shared with the PHQ-9 (i.e., feeling tired
One item, developed for the present study, assessed perceptions of or having low energy and trouble falling asleep, staying asleep, or
anger as helpful with the following statement, “Does anger help you in any sleeping too much). To avoid using these same items on two scales,
way to perform your duties now (such as: helps you focus, helps motivate the two items were only scored for the PHQ-9. The item regarding men-
you)?” was rated on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all; 2 = rarely; 3 = some- strual problems was also omitted given that the sample only included
times; 4 = often; 5 = very often). This item was assessed at times 1 and 2. men. Thus, analyses were conducted on the 12 remaining items, such
as “stomach pain” and “back pain.” Internal consistency on the initial
Anger Reactions and follow-up survey was 0.77 and 0.76, respectively.
The four-item measure of anger reactions, which has been the
standard measure used in large-scale studies with soldiers Functional Impairment
(e.g., Cabrera et al., 2010; Cabrera et al., 2016; Killgore et al., The Walter Reed Functional Impairment Scale (WRFIS; Herrell
2008; Thomas et al., 2010; Wilk et al., 2013, 2015), asked soldiers et al., 2014) is a 14-item measure used to assess functioning in an

www.jonmd.com 693
Adler et al. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017

occupational context like the military. Items are scored using five response 10.9% endorsed “rarely,” 27.8% endorsed “sometimes,” 13.4% en-
options (1 = no difficulty at all to 5 = extreme difficulty). Sample items in- dorsed “often,” and 7.1% endorsed “very often.” At time 2, the ratings
cluded rating “the accuracy of your work” and “your ability to multitask.” were 45.8%, 17.8%, 29.3%, 4.0%, and 3.0%, respectively. Thus, at time
Internal consistency was 0.92 on both the initial and follow-up survey. 1, nearly half of the soldiers (48.3%) felt that anger was at least some-
times helpful; this proportion dropped at time 2 to 36.3%.
Health-Related Behaviors As with previously published studies, mental health symptoms
Alcohol problems were measured using the three-item Alcohol correlated with one another. For example, at time 1, PTSD scores were
Use Disorders Identification Test–Consumption scale (AUDIT-C; Bush correlated with depression symptoms (r = 0.79, p < 0.01) and with an-
et al., 1998). These items were a) “How often do you have a drink contain- ger reactions (r = 0.50, p < 0.01); similarly, depression symptoms were
ing alcohol?” b) “How many drinks containing alcohol do you have on a correlated with anger reactions (r = 0.46, p < 0.01). Likewise, some de-
day when you are drinking?” and c) “How often do you have six or more mographic differences in symptom reporting were significant. For ex-
drinks on one occasion?” Internal consistency on the initial and follow-up ample, analyses of variance (ANOVAs) run on years of military
survey was 0.88 and 0.90, respectively. Sleep problems were measured service in terms of PTSD and depression symptoms revealed that those
with four items adapted from the Insomnia Severity Index (Bastien with 4 to 6 years of service reported more PTSD and depression symp-
et al., 2001), which has been used in other studies with soldiers toms than those with less (1–3 years) or more (7–10, 11+) years of ser-
(Adler et al., 2009; Wright et al., 2011b). The items included difficulty vice (F [3, 621] = 4.62, p < 0.01; F [3, 621] = 4.22, p < 0.01,
falling or staying asleep (1 = none to 5 = very severe), sleep satisfaction respectively). In terms of rank, non-commissioned officers (NCOs;
(1 = very satisfied to 5 = very dissatisfied), and the degree to which E5–E9) reported more PTSD and depression symptoms than those at
sleep interfered with functioning (1 = not at all to 5 = very much). In- junior or officer ranks (F[2, 618] = 4.02, p < 0.02; F[2, 618] = 3.56,
ternal consistency on the initial and follow-up survey was 0.92 and p < 0.03, respectively). However, in terms of anger reactions, there were
0.91, respectively. no significant differences for rank or years of service.
Analytic Strategy Latent Profile Analysis
First, we calculated the frequency of soldiers' perception of anger We began by determining the appropriate number of latent clas-
as helpful, and then examined relationships between mental health mea- ses at both time points (based on how helpful anger is in performing
sures and possible differences in rank and years of service. For the main one's duties, the PHQ-9, the PCL, the anger reactions scale, somatic
analysis, we selected a statistical method that could do three things: symptoms, and the WRFIS) by comparing latent profile models with
a) account for intraindividual changes in soldiers' perceptions of anger two to four classes. Latent profile analysis is a cross-sectional method
as helpful, b) examine how anger may be related to other mental health “used to estimate membership in unobserved groups” (Newsom,
problems over time, and c) examine interindividual differences in po- 2015, p. 264). Table 1 provides the results of the latent profile analysis
tential groups of soldiers that were similar in how they perceived anger at time 1 and time 2. Lower AIC, BIC, and SABIC (Schwarz, 1978)
and the symptoms they were experiencing. Latent transition analysis al- values and higher entropy values indicate better model fit. The LMRT
lows examination of potential latent groups based on the distributional (Lo et al., 2001) compares the model being tested to a model with one
properties of the variables under investigation and allows individuals to less class; a statistically significant result indicates that the model being
change from one group or status to another over time. Adapting the ap- tested fits better than the model with one less class. A nonsignificant
proach taken by Lanza et al. (2010), when discussing the “latent clas- result suggests that a model with one less class is a better explanation
ses” produced by the latent transition analysis, we used the term of the data.
“group” instead of “class” to highlight the fact that individuals may Evaluating multiple indicators of fit is necessary because not all
change membership in class over time. The variables used to deter- fit indicators will suggest a model with the same number of classes as
mine latent class at each time point and in the subsequent latent tran- the best fitting model. For example, in Table 1 the AIC, BIC, and
sition analysis consisted of the perceptions of anger as helpful, the SABIC all suggest a four-class model at time one. However, entropy
PHQ-9, the PCL, the anger reactions scale, somatic symptoms, and values are highest for two and three classes, and the LMRT results sug-
the WRFIS. Mplus version 7.4 was used to test these models (Muthén gest a three-class model. Overall, a case can be made for three classes at
and Muthén, 1998–2015). time 1 given the higher entropy value and the LMRT results. The same
Using a common strategy, we first determined the optimal num- type of discrepancy can be seen for time 2. AIC suggests four classes,
ber of latent classes appropriate at each time point by running a number whereas the BIC and SABIC suggest that three classes will give the best
of latent profile models, while varying the number of latent classes and fit. Entropy was highest for two classes, and the LMRT suggests that
then examining the resulting model fit indices such as Akaike's Infor- only two classes are needed. Simulation studies show that methods
mation Criterion (AIC), Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC), Lo- (e.g., LMRT) that compare two models differing by one class may be
Mendell-Rubin Adjusted Likelihood Ratio Test (LMRT; Lo et al., preferable to solely examining fit indices (AIC, BIC, SABIC; Lo
2001), and the sample size-adjusted Bayesian information criteria et al., 2001). Overall, it seems that a case can be made for two classes
(SABIC; Schwarz, 1978). Then, the latent transition model was ana- at time 2 due to a higher entropy value and the LMRT results. In addi-
lyzed using the number of classes determined in the earlier step. Finally, tion to examining fit values, one final criterion for determining the
we examined the relationship across latent groups on one variable asso- number of classes is that the final model selected should be informative
ciated with increased risk (combat experiences) and two health-related and interpretable as demonstrated in other studies (e.g., McBride et al.,
behaviors (drinking problems and sleep problems) associated with poor 2014; Rodgers et al., 2014). When the three-class solution was graphed
adjustment to determine whether these variables could be used to clarify at time 2, the additional third class accounted for individuals who fell
differences across latent groups over time. between those who scored low and those who scored high across all
the variables. Thus, the conceptual and explanatory power gained by
using a three-class model instead of a two-class model seemed to
RESULTS be minimal.
To summarize, the latent profile analysis was conducted at both
Descriptive Statistics time points (time 1 and time 2) to determine how many classes to pro-
In terms of the single item regarding perception of anger as help- duce in the latent transition analysis, which allows for the identification
ful, at time 1, 39.4% of participants surveyed endorsed “not at all,” of classes at both time points and changes in class over time. Based on

694 www.jonmd.com
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017 Can Anger Be Helpful?

Latent Group by Combat Experiences, Drinking and


TABLE 1. Latent Profile Analysis of Anger Reactions and Health
Scores of US Soldiers at 4 and 7 Months Postdeployment (N = 627) Sleep Problems Over Time
Further exploration of the latent class patterns involved a series
2 Classes 3 Classes 4 Classes of ANOVAs to see if they could shed light on the differences between
those in each group. In terms of combat experiences, there was a statis-
Time 1 tically significant difference among the groups: F(64, 626) = 2.25,
AIC 18,923 18,256 18,047 p < 0.001. Examination of the group means revealed that those with
BIC 19,034 18,425 18,273 the lowest scores for combat experiences were in the resilient group
SABIC 18,955 18,304 18,112 (mean = 48.16, SD = 13.19), and those with the highest scores of combat
Entropy 0.90 0.90 0.85 experiences were in the chronic group (mean = 58.5, SD = 13.94); those
Lo, Mendell, Rubin 2 vs. 1 3 vs. 2 4 vs. 3 participants with the most combat experiences were doing the worst at
Value = 2327 Value = 685 Value = 221 times 1 and 2. Interestingly, the delayed problems group had fewer com-
p < 0.01 p = 0.04 p = 0.39 bat experiences (mean = 51.74, SD = 12.51) than the improved class
n for each class C1 = 334 C1 = 248 C1 = 129 (mean = 54.04, SD = 12.35), suggesting that other kinds of life stressors
may have accounted for this decline.
C2 = 293 C2 = 155 C2 = 214
When examining drinking problems at the initial time period,
C3 = 224 C3 = 196 there were statistically significant differences between the latent class
C4 = 88 patterns, F(14,626) = 5.76, p < 0.001. Therefore we examined the
Time 2 drinking scores for each latent group and found that those in the resil-
AIC 19,664 18,977 18,968 ient group drank the least at time 1 (mean = 3.04; SD = 2.87) and even
BIC 19,775 19,146 19,194 slightly less at follow-up (mean = 2.81; SD = 2.85). Consistent with the
SABIC 19,696 19,025 19,032 latent class patterns, those in the improved group also reduced their
Entropy 0.94 0.89 0.848 drinking at follow-up (meanT1 = 3.97, SDT1 = 3.79; meanT2 = 2.72,
Lo, Mendell, Rubin 2 vs. 1 3 vs. 2 4 vs. 3 SDT2 = 2.84), whereas those in the delayed problems group increased
Value = 2452 Value = 705 Value = 35 their drinking (meanT1 = 3.95, SDT1 = 3.45; meanT2 = 4.26, SDT2 =
3.58). Finally, those in the chronic group reported the most drinking
p < 0.01 p = 0.49 p = 0.58
at both time points (meanT1 = 5.86, SDT1 = 3.99; meanT2 =
n for each class C1 = 275 C1 = 181 C1 = 8 5.22, SDT2 = 4.17).
C2 = 352 C2 = 277 C2 = 162 A similar pattern was found for sleep problems. At time 1, there
C3 = 169 C3 = 198 was a statistically significant difference among the latent class patterns,
C4 = 259 F(16, 626) = 24.37, p < 0.001. Examining the sleep problem scores for
each latent pattern showed a similar pattern to that of drinking. Those in
Note. AIC lower values indicate better model fit. BIC lower values indicate
better model fit. SABIC lower values indicate better model fit. Higher entropy
the resilient group reported the fewest problems with sleeping overall
values indicate better model fit. The Lo-Mendell-Rubin test compares models (meanT1 = 6.48, SDT1 = 2.44; meanT2 = 6.70, SDT2 = 2.62). Those in
with one less class. If statistically significant, it suggests that a model with more the improved group reported a reduction in sleep problems (meanT1 =
classes should be investigated, not that adding more classes will necessarily pro- 10.39, SDT1 = 4.04; meanT2 = 8.88, SDT2 = 3.30), those in the delayed
duce better fit. If nonsignificant, it suggests that a model with one less class is a problems group reported an increase in sleep problems (meanT1 = 9.15,
better explanation of one's data. SDT1 = 3.57; meanT2 = 10.96, SDT2 = 3.85), and those in the chronic
C1, C2, C3, and C4 indicate to which class an individual belongs: C1, class 1; group reported the greatest level of sleep problems (meanT1 = 13.10,
C2, class 2; C3, class 3; C4, class 4. SDT1 = 4.24; meanT2 = 13.17, SDT2 = 3.78), although the level stayed
the same across the two time points.

our results, the latent profile analysis suggested a different number of DISCUSSION
classes for both time 1 and time 2. We selected a parsimonious two- Overall, nearly half of the soldiers surveyed reported that anger
class model using latent transition analysis to allow for differences at was at least sometimes helpful to them in performing their occupational
both times while also accounting for potential changes in class member- duties. The perception of anger as helpful, however, was not randomly
ship over time.
It can be confusing to read about a two-class model and yet have
four groups in the latent transition analysis. Recall that the latent profile TABLE 2. Counts, Proportions, and Latent Transition Probabilities for
analysis only examined a single time point, whereas the latent transition the Latent Class Patterns of Anger Reactions and Health Scores of US
analysis looks at group membership over time. Thus, with only a two- Soldiers at 4 and 7 Months Postdeployment (N = 627)
class model, there are actually four groups based on whether one stays
in the same group or transitions into another group at time 2. Latent Class Number Proportion of Latent Transition
The number of participants in each latent group, the proportion Patterna in Class Sample in Class Probabilities
of the sample in each group, and the latent transition probabilities are
Resilient: low-low 208 0.33 0.63
presented in Table 2. The largest groups are those that do not transition
from one class to another across time but are stable at both time 1 and Delayed problems: 126 0.20 0.37
low-high
time 2. To better understand what these latent class patterns mean, the
group means are presented for each variable in Table 3. Those low on Improved: high-low 72 0.11 0.24
measures at both time periods were termed “resilient” (n = 208), and Chronic: high-high 221 0.35 0.76
those high on the measures at both time periods were described as Note. Proportion rounded to two decimals.
“chronic” (n = 221). Those low at time 1 and high at time 2 were termed a
Latent class pattern description followed by relative time 1 and time 2 scores
the “delayed problems” group (n = 126), and those scoring high at time (e.g., low or high).
1 and low at time 2 were regarded as “improved” (n = 72).

www.jonmd.com 695
Adler et al. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017

TABLE 3. Latent Class Pattern and Means on Anger Reactions and Health Scores at Time 1 and Time 2 of US Soldiers at 4 and 7 Months
Postdeployment (N = 627)

Latent Class Pattern Mean Scores


Resilient Delayed Problems Improved Chronic
Variable (Low-Low) (n = 208) (Low-High) (n = 126) (High-Low) (n = 72) (High-High) (n = 221)
Anger as helpful T1 1.35 1.43 1.61 2.02
Anger as helpful T2 1.69 1.93 1.81 2.40
PHQ-9 T1 0.98 2.82 4.24 8.60
PHQ-9 T2 0.87 5.31 1.87 9.47
PCL T1 20.52 24.53 28.59 41.20
PCL T2 19.33 29.40 22.04 41.53
Anger reactions T1 0.82 1.56 2.78 5.24
Anger reactions T2 0.95 2.98 1.12 6.08
PHQ-15 T1 13.35 14.55 15.68 17.80
PHQ-15 T2 13.34 15.41 13.93 17.22
WRFIS T1 14.84 16.13 18.89 24.34
WRFIS T2 14.96 21.10 15.86 27.83

distributed across the sample. Specifically, latent transition analysis group, suggesting the possibility this group may be negatively
identified two at-risk groups of soldiers. We termed the first group of responding to other life-related events.
at-risk soldiers “chronic.” The “chronic” group represented more than The fact that perceiving anger as helpful is not associated with
a third of the sample (35%) and was likely to agree that anger helped positive mental health adjustment offers an important insight into how
them fulfill their occupational role. This group also reported high levels anger may function to undermine individual well-being in the military
of anger reactions, mental health problems, somatic symptoms, and context. Individuals may mislead themselves and believe that anger is
functional impairment. This perception of anger as helpful persisted of benefit rather than realize that anger may lead to a spiral of negative
roughly 3 months later as did their other symptoms. The other at-risk health, compounded by unhealthy behaviors. Given previous evidence
group accounted for 20% of the sample and was characterized as “de- that anger may be accepted in the military culture (e.g., Adler et al.,
layed problems.” Soldiers in this group initially reported moderately 2008; Castro and Adler, 2011), it may be that individuals who integrate
low levels of perceptions of anger as helpful and lower scores for anger the belief that anger is helpful into their experience, without caveat, may
reactions, mental health problems, somatic symptoms, and functional not manage their emotional response during the postdeployment phase
impairment. Nonetheless, at follow-up, their symptoms increased as did appropriately. Emphasizing the importance of emotion regulation may
their perception of anger as helpful. The other two latent class patterns forestall a negative spiral.
consisted of those with low scores on both the initial survey and on the From another clinical vantage point, it may be worth determining
follow-up survey (33%; “resilient”) or those whose symptoms and per- if anger is the soldier's “go to” emotion. If anger serves to energize and
ceptions of anger as helpful decreased over time (11%; “improved”). get things done while also serving as one's primary emotion, it may be
These differences across groups can be characterized as mean- easy for soldiers to default to this response as a way to navigate life cir-
ingful given the consistency with which the mean scores differed across cumstances. The downside is that the anger may obscure other impor-
groups and the consistency with which the two shifting groups (the de- tant emotions and may also disrupt relationships. Teaching soldiers
layed and improved group) changed scores over time. The extent of the strategies to increase emotion regulation may aid in developing alter-
group differences are seen most clearly by comparing the resilient and nate ways of interacting and interpreting events.
chronic group; in general, individuals in the chronic group reported at
least double what was reported by the resilient group, and the chronic
group's mean PCL score was near the standard cutoff score of 50 Limitations and Future Directions
(e.g., Hoge et al., 2004), a marker of clinical distress. There are several limitations to the present study. First, only one
Taking all four latent groups into account, the perception of an- item was used to assess the perception of anger as helpful. One item
ger as helpful was not associated with positive adjustment after deploy- does not represent a complex assessment of the construct, although
ment; rather the perception of anger as helpful was comorbid with one-item questions can be useful in assessing some constructs, espe-
higher levels of overall anger reactions, mental health problems, so- cially if it has good face validity (Wanous et al., 1997). Second, there
matic symptoms, and lower levels of functioning. Furthermore, sub- were no women in the sample and no soldiers from support units; thus,
groups characterized by the perception of anger as helpful and other the findings may not generalize to the way in which anger is perceived
symptoms were also more likely to report alcohol and sleep problems. by women or soldiers in support units. Third, this study relied on self-
Thus, although there have been previous indications that anger can be report scales to measure clinical outcomes, although anonymous sur-
useful in the right context (Barsade and Gibson, 2007), it seems in this veys may result in more reporting than formal interviews (Warner
particular occupational context—the postdeployment phase—the per- et al., 2011). Fourth, the measure of anger reactions used in the present
ception of anger as helpful is not associated with better health study is not a well-validated measure. Although frequently used in stud-
or functioning. ies with service members, follow-on research should rely on more tra-
In addition, although those in the chronic group reported the ditional measures of anger and aggression. Finally, there is no way to
highest levels of combat experiences as would be expected from previ- assess directionality. That is, it is unclear the extent to which the percep-
ous research (e.g., Adler et al., 2009; Wilk et al., 2015), the delayed tion of anger as helpful is a consequence of anger reactions and the ex-
problem group reported fewer combat experiences than the improved tent to which it is a cause.

696 www.jonmd.com
The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017 Can Anger Be Helpful?

There are several additional implications for future research. The DISCLOSURE
results of this study are important in that each latent class identified sug- Funding was provided by the US Army's Military Operational
gests a potentially different approach to treatment for mental health Medicine Research Program and was not part of a grant.
problems after deployment. For example, it may be useful to consider The authors declare no conflict of interest.
the service member's perception of anger in the context of treatment
planning. Given that over a third of the sample perceived anger as help-
ful initially and at follow-up, an additional study on these individuals is REFERENCES
merited to develop targeted methods of addressing their confluence of Adler AB, Bliese PB, McGurk D, Hoge CW, Castro CA (2009) Battlemind debriefing
mental health problems, somatic symptoms, and anger reactions. The and battlemind training as early interventions with soldiers returning from Iraq:
belief that anger is useful may result in the persistence of symptoms Randomization by platoon. J Consult Clin Psychol. 77:928–940.
and may benefit from being addressed directly in modules added to Adler AB, Britt TW, Castro CA, McGurk D, Bliese PD (2011) Effect of transition home
evidence-based treatments. Another group that warrants further exami- from combat on risk-taking and health-related behaviors. J Trauma Stress. 24:381–389.
nation is the class of service members that was worse on a range of mea- Adler AB, Wright KM, Bliese PD, Eckford R, Hoge CW (2008) A2 diagnostic crite-
sures at follow-up. Several studies have identified this drop in adjustment rion for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress. 21:301–308.
(e.g., Bliese et al., 2007; Bonanno et al., 2012). It may be that the belief
Barsade SG, Gibson DE (2007) Why does affect matter in organizations? Acad Manag
that anger is useful and other life stressors that occur after the deployment
Perspect. 21:36–59.
fuel a downward spiral. Identifying what drives this drop in adjustment
would be useful for informing interventions to prevent such a decline. Fu- Bastien CH, Vallieres A, Morin CM (2001) Validation of the Insomnia Severity Index
ture research should also target the improved group to gain insight into as an outcome measure for insomnia research. Sleep Med. 2:297–307.
what leads to a decrease in symptoms and in a decrease in finding anger Bliese PD, Wright KM, Adler AB, Cabrera O, Castro CA, Hoge CW (2008) Validating
helpful over time. In addition, follow-on work should examine the degree the primary care posttraumatic stress disorder screen and the posttraumatic stress dis-
to which these group-based characteristics predict episodes of violence, order checklist with soldiers returning from combat. J Consult Clin Psychol. 76:272.
identify other variables that may be contributing to aggression, and use Bliese PD, Wright KM, Adler AB, Thomas JL, Hoge CW (2007) Timing of post-
an expanded measure of the positive perception of anger and of anger combat mental health assessments. Psychol Serv. 4:141–148.
and aggression, including reports from unit or family members. Bonanno GA, Mancini AD, Horton JL, Powell TM, Leardmann CA, Boyko EJ, Wells
Research should also examine the degree to which the findings TS, Hooper TI, Gackstetter GD, Smith TC (2012) Trajectories of trauma symp-
replicate in women who are serving in the military. Although Worthen toms and resilience in deployed U.S. military service members: Prospective cohort
et al. (2014) have found similarly high rates of anger between men and study. Br J Psychiatry. 200:317–323.
women in a National Guard sample, other studies have documented Bush K, Kivlahan DR, McDonell MB, Fihn SD, Bradley KA (1998) The AUDIT al-
sex differences in anger (e.g., Martin et al., 2013). Thus, it is not clear cohol consumption questions (AUDIT-C): An effective brief screening test for
if there are sex differences in anger in the military occupational context. problem drinking. Ambulatory Care Quality Improvement Project (ACQUIP). Al-
It is also not clear whether the perception of anger as helpful is associ- cohol Use Disorders Identification Test. Arch Intern Med. 158:1789–1795.
ated with more difficult adjustment in women as it is with men.
Cabrera OA, Adler AB, Bliese PD (2016) Growth mixture modeling of post combat
Furthermore, research should explore whether having a positive aggression: Application to soldiers deployed to Iraq. Psychiatry Res. 246:539–544.
perspective of anger may result in individuals being less receptive to
standardized attempts at anger management or early intervention. If this Cabrera OA, Bliese PD, Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC (2010) Aggressiveness and
is the case, then adjustments to existing interventions may need to be con- perceived marital quality: The moderating role of a family-supportive work cli-
mate. Mil Psychol. 22:57–67.
sidered to address this misperception of anger's utility. Although anger
may be useful under some circumstances, holding this perception seems Castro CA, Adler AB (2011) Re-conceptualizing combat-related posttraumatic stress
to come at a significant cost. Such interventions should acknowledge the disorder as an occupational hazard. In Adler AB, Bliese PD, Castro CA (Eds), De-
positive role anger may have played during combat and specifically iden- ployment psychology: Evidence-based strategies to promote mental health in the
tify how these benefits may become detrimental after returning home. military (pp 217–242). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Finally, future work should assess the larger role of anger in this Davis PA, Woodman T, Callow N (2010) Better out than in: The influence of anger
high-risk occupational context. Studies should address the degree to which regulation on physical performance. Pers Indiv Diff. 49:457–460.
the positive perception of anger is unidimensional or present before join- Elbogen EB, Wagner HR, Fuller SR, Calhoun PS, Kinneer PM, Mid-Atlantic Mental
ing the military. In addition, future research should examine how anger Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center Workgroup, Beckham JC (2010)
is perceived within teams and managed by small team leaders. Correlates of anger and hostility in Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans. Am J
Psychiatry. 167:1051–1058.
Forbes D, Creamer M, Hawthorne G, Allen N, McHugh T (2003) Comorbidity as a
CONCLUSIONS predictor of symptom change after treatment in combat-related posttraumatic
Perceiving anger as helpful is not associated with better mental stress disorder. J Nerv Ment Dis. 191:93–99.
health adjustment and may be an indicator of risk. Considering how an- Forbes D, Hawthorne G, Elliott P, McHugh AF, Biddle D, Creamer MC, Novaco RW
ger is perceived has the potential to identify a range of potential points of (2004) A concise measure of anger in combat-related posttraumatic stress disor-
intervention for individuals working in a high-risk occupational context. der. J Trauma Stress. 17:249–256.
Gonzalez OI, Novaco RW, Reger MA, Gahm GA (2016) Anger intensification with
combat-related PTSD and depression comorbidity. Psychol Trauma. 8:9–16.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank Charles W. Hoge, Richard Herrell, Jonathan Heesink L, Rademaker A, Vermetten E, Geuze E, Kleber R (2015) Longitudinal mea-
Metzler, Jennifer Auchterlonie, Laura Bean, Kristina Clarke-Walper, sures of hostility in deployed military personnel. Psychiatry Res. 229:479–484.
Rachel Eckford, Paul Kim, Brian Kok, Carla Kreilein, Nadia Marroquin, Herrell RK, Edens EN, Riviere LA, Thomas JL, Bliese PD, Hoge CW (2014)
Julie Merrill, Randall Mills, Leonard Skipper, Michael Wood, and Kristin Assessing functional impairment in a working military population: The Walter
Young for their research support. The views expressed in this article Reed Functional Impairment Scale. Psychol Serv. 11:254–264.
are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC, McGurk D, Cotting DI, Koffman RL (2004) Com-
policy or position of the US Army Medical Command or the Depart- bat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care.
ment of Defense. N Engl J Med. 351:13–22.

www.jonmd.com 697
Adler et al. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease • Volume 205, Number 9, September 2017

Jakupcak M, Luterek J, Hunt S, Conybeare D, McFall M (2008) Posttraumatic stress and Riviere LA, Edens EN, Adler AB, Bliese PD, Klocko RP, Hoge CW (2011) Modifying
its relationship to physical health functioning in a sample of Iraq and Afghanistan War instructions on the posttraumatic stress disorder checklist for military populations
veterans seeking postdeployment VA health care. J Nerv Ment Dis. 196:425–428. does not change symptom reporting. J Nerv Ment Dis. 199:199–202.
Kessler RC, Heering SG, Stein MB, Colpe LJ, Fullerton CS, Hwang I, Naifeh JA, Rodgers S, Ajdacic-Gross V, Müller M, Hengartner MP, Holtforth MG, Angst J,
Nock MK, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Schoenbaum M, Zaslavsky AM, Ursano Rössler W (2014) The role of sex on stability and change of depression symptom
RJ, Army STARRS Collaborators (2014) Thirty-day prevalence of DSM-IV men- subtypes over 20 years: A latent transition analysis. Euro Arch Psychiatry Clin
tal disorders among non-deployed soldiers in the U.S. Army: Results from the Neurosci. 264:577–588.
Army Study to Assess Risk and Resilience in Servicemembers (Army STARRS). Schwarz G (1978) Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann Stat. 6:461–464.
JAMA Psychiatry. 71:487–489.
Spector PE, Jex SM (1998) Development of four self-report measures of job stressors
Killgore WDS, Cotting DI, Thomas JL, Cox AL, McGurk D, Vo AH, Castro CA, and strain: Interpersonal conflict at work scale, organizational constraints scale,
Hoge CW (2008) Post-combat invincibility: Violent combat experiences are asso- quantitative workload inventory, and physical symptoms inventory. J Occup
ciated with increased risk-taking propensity following deployment. J Psychiatr Health Psychol. 3:356.
Res. 42:1112–1121.
Spielberger CD (1999) The state-trait anger expression inventory-2 (STAXI-2): Profes-
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB (2002) The PHQ-15: Validity of a new measure sional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.
for evaluating the severity of somatic symptoms. Psychosom Med. 64:258–266. Spielberger CD, Jacobs G, Russell S, Crane R (1983) Assessment of anger: The state-
Kulka RA, Schlenger WE, Fairbank JA, Hough RL, Jordan BK, Marmar CR, Weiss D, trait anger scale. In Butcher JN, Spielberger CD (Eds), Advances in personality as-
Cranston A (1990) Trauma and the Vietnam War generation: Report of findings sessment (Vol. V3, pp 112–134). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
from the national Vietnam veterans readjustment study. New York: Bruner/Mazel. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB (1999) Validation and utility of a self-report ver-
Lanza ST, Patrick ME, Maggs JL (2010) Latent transition analysis: Benefits of a latent var- sion of PRIME-MD: The PHQ primary care study. Primary Care Evaluation of
iable approach to modeling transitions in substance use. J Drug Issues. 40:93–120. Mental Disorders. Patient Health Questionnaire. JAMA. 282:1737–1744.
Lazarus RS (2000) How emotions influence performance in competitive sports. Sport Sundin J, Herrell RK, Hoge CW, Fear NT, Adler AB, Greenberg N, Riviere LA,
Psychol. 14:229–252. Thomas JL, Wessely S, Bliese PD (2014) Mental health outcomes in US and
UK military personnel returning from Iraq. Br J Psychiatry. 204:200–207.
Lindebaum D, Fielden S (2011) ‘It's good to be angry’: Enacting anger in construction
project management to achieve perceived leader effectiveness. Hum Relat. 64: Tagar MR, Federico CM, Halperin E (2011) The positive effect of negative emotions
437–458. in protracted conflict: The case of anger. J Exp Soc Psychol. 47:157–164.
Thomas JL, Wilk JE, Riviere LA, McGurk D, Castro CA, Hoge CW (2010) Preva-
Lloyd D, Nixon RD, Varker T, Elliott P, Perry D, Bryant RA, Creamer M, Forbes D
lence of mental health problems and functional impairment among active compo-
(2014) Comorbidity in the prediction of cognitive processing therapy treatment
nent and national guard soldiers 3 and 12 months following combat in Iraq. Arch
outcomes for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Anxiety Disord. 28:
Gen Psychiatry. 67:614–623.
237–240.
van Doorn J, Zeelenberg M (2014) Anger and prosocial behavior. Emot Rev. 6:261–268.
Lo Y, Mendell NR, Rubin DB (2001) Tresting the number of components in a normal
mixture. Biometricka. 88:767–778. Wanous JP, Reichers AE, Hudy MJ (1997) Overall job satisfaction: How good are
single-item measures? J Appl Psychol. 52:247–252.
MacManus D, Dean K, Jones M, Rona RJ, Greenberg N, Hull L, Fahy T, Wessely S,
Fear NT (2013) Violent offending by UK military personnel deployed to Iraq Warner CH, Appenzeller GN, Grieger T, Belenkiy S, Breitbach J, Parker J, Warner
and Afghanistan: A data linkage cohort study. Lancet. 381:907–917. CM, Hoge C (2011) Importance of anonymity to encourage honest reporting in
mental health screening after combat deployment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 68:
MacManus D, Rona R, Dickson H, Somaini G, Fear N, Wessely S (2015) Aggressive and 1065–1071.
violent behavior among military personnel deployed to Iraq and Afghanistan: Preva-
Weathers FW, Litz BT, Herman DS, Huska JA, Keane TM (1993) The PTSD Checklist
lence and link with deployment and combat exposure. Epidemiol Rev. 37:196–212.
(PCL): Reliability, validity, and diagnostic utility. San Antonio: Paper presented at
Martin LA, Neighbors HW, Griffith DM (2013) The experience of symptoms of de- the annual meeting of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
pression in men vs women: Analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey Replica-
Wilk JE, Bliese PD, Thomas JL, Wood MD, McGurk D, Castro CA, Hoge CW (2013)
tion. JAMA Psychiatry. 70:1100–1106.
Unethical battlefield conduct reported by soldiers serving in the Iraq war. J Nerv
McBride O, Adamson G, Cheng HG, Slade T (2014) Changes in drinking patterns in Ment Dis. 201:259–265.
the first years after onset: A latent transition analysis of National Epidemiologic Wilk JE, Quartana PJ, Clarke‐Walper K, Kok B, Riviere LA (2015) Aggression in US
Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions (NESARC) data. Psychol Addict soldiers post‐deployment: Associations with combat exposure and PTSD and the
Behav. 28:696–709. moderating role of trait anger. Aggress Behav. 41:556–565.
Muthén LK, Muthén BO (1998–2015) Mplus (7.4) [Computer software]. Los Angeles, Worthen M, Rathod SD, Cohen G, Sampson L, Ursano R, Gifford R, Fullerton C, Galea
CA: Muthén & Muthén. S, Ahern J (2014) Anger problems and posttraumatic stress disorder in male and
Newsom JT (2015) Longitudinal structural equation modeling: A comprehensive in- female National Guard and Reserve Service members. J Psychiatr Res. 55:52–58.
troduction. New York, NY: Routledge. Wright KM, Britt TW, Bliese PD, Adler AB (2011a) Insomnia severity, combat expo-
Novaco RW, Swanson RD, Gonzalez OI, Gahm GA, Reger MD (2012) Anger and sure and mental health outcomes. Stress Health. 27:325–333.
postcombat mental health: Validation of a brief anger measure with US soldiers Wright KM, Britt TW, Bliese PD, Adler AB, Picchioni D, Moore D (2011b) Insomnia
postdeployed from Iraq and Afghanistan. Psychol Assessment. 24:661–675. as predictor versus outcome of PTSD and depression among Iraq combat veterans.
Reijnen A, Rademaker AR, Vermetten E, Geuze A (2015) Prevalence of mental health J Clin Psychol. 67:1240–1258.
symptoms in Dutch military personnel returning from deployment to Afghanistan: Wright KM, Cabrera OA, Adler AB, Bliese PD (2013) Functional impairment as a var-
A 2-year longitudinal analysis. Eur Psychiatry. 30:341–346. iable in adjustment post-combat. Mil Psychol. 25:545–556.

698 www.jonmd.com

You might also like