You are on page 1of 44

BEHAVIORAL RESEARCH IN ACCOUNTING American Accounting Association

Vol. 23, No. 1 DOI: 10.2308/bria.2011.23.1.1


2011
pp. 1–43

A Proposed Framework for Behavioral


Accounting Research
Jacob G. Birnberg
University of Pittsburgh
ABSTRACT: Behavioral accounting research 共BAR兲 is richer today, in the topics cov-
ered, the methods used, and the range of sub-areas of accounting in which it is per-
formed, than ever before. This paper offers a framework within which BAR literature can
be viewed as a whole rather than in segments, such as by accounting sub-areas or by
research method. The framework classifies BAR by the focus of the research: the
individual, group, organization, or the society within which accounting exists. The pur-
pose of the framework is to help researchers in BAR to appreciate the insights to their
research questions that can be found in BAR using another research method or study-
ing a similar issue in another sub-area of accounting. Existing research in each of these
four areas is discussed to illustrate the usefulness of the framework. In addition, be-
havioral research in other disciplines that could impact BAR and areas of potential
future research are discussed.
Keywords: behavioral accounting research.

INTRODUCTION
n the 20 or so years since Birnberg and Shields 共1989兲 reviewed behavioral accounting re-

I search 共BAR兲, the area of applied behavioral research in general and BAR in particular has
burgeoned. The BAR literature has grown in breadth, depth, and complexity. This change
reflects an important trend in BAR: the reference disciplines and the object of accounting and
nonaccounting behavioral researchers have broadened.
The behavioral decision-making and cognitive psychology literatures that stimulated a sig-
nificant portion of the emerging BAR research up to the late 1980s continue to have a significant
influence on BAR 共e.g., Camerer 2001兲. In addition, the role of behavioral research has grown in
other social science disciplines. Experimental economics has moved into the mainstream 共e.g.,
McCaffery and Slemrod 2006兲. This literature has had an impact on BAR 共Moser 1998; Callahan
et al. 2006兲. Legal researchers, heavily influenced by the writings of Kahneman and Tversky 共e.g.,
Kahneman and Tversky 1979兲, have begun to actively pursue behavioral issues 共see Sunstein
2000兲. A strong behavioral school even has developed within finance 共e.g., Thaler 1993; Barberis
and Thaler 2003兲. Medical researchers have joined with behavioral researchers to investigate

The author thanks the two reviewers for their insightful comments, the editor for the paper, Bryan Church, not only for all
his help, but also for his patience, and numerous colleagues for their help along the way.
A dagger 共†兲 at the end of select references indicates a review of the literature or a paper that includes an extensive set of
references.

Published Online: February 2011

1
2 Birnberg

issues such as how individuals react to prospective changes in the state of their health 共Udel et al.
2005兲. Even philosophy has developed a set of experimental researchers 共Knobe 2003; Appiah
2007兲 and a journal. Emerging methods for researching old questions are altering the form of
behavioral research, such as neuroeconomics 共Knudsen et al. 2007兲. These new tools permit
researchers to go beyond the observed behaviors of the decision makers and penetrate the “black
box”: that is, observe the brain’s activity during decision making. Finally, these new behavioral
researchers include economic modelers who have developed richer models of economic decision
makers 共“economic man”兲 intended to explain behaviors such as cooperation 共e.g., Rabin 1993,
1998兲, and empiricists who have utilized aggregated data to test these models 共e.g., La Porta et al.
1997; Ittner 2007兲.
The burgeoning of BAR and the expansion of disciplines that in one form or another have
added “behavioral” as an adjective to one of their sub-disciplines has enriched the extant research
on which BAR can draw 共e.g., Dickhaut et al. 2003; Hannan 2005兲. However, the increased
interest and diversity of methods used to research behavioral issues also leads to a blurring of the
definition of “behavioral research” in general and the boundaries of BAR in particular. What was
relatively clear 20 years ago is less clear today. The proliferation of research methods has meant
that BAR is more than laboratory experiments, surveys, and the occasional field study. A variety
of archival databases have been used to investigate essentially behavioral issues 共Banker et al.
2000b; Ittner 2007兲. Even efficient markets researchers, who would not be considered part of the
BAR community, are identifying and researching issues that clearly are intended to understand
individual investors’ behaviors: most notably, anomalous behavior relative to the predictions of the
efficient market 共Sloan 1996兲.
This blurring of boundaries between research thrusts has led to an often unrecognized degree
of commonality across BAR thrusts. While this has obvious potential benefits that will be dis-
cussed latter, it means the boundaries used in this paper necessarily are arbitrary and subjective. In
general, the questions studied and the papers cited will be related to the actual behavior of people,
whether it is as individuals or collectivities of varying degrees of size or complexity 共e.g., groups
or organizations兲, as they interact with each other and/or their environment. The test used in this
paper is analogous to one offered as an operational definition of obscenity: We know BAR when
we see it. At the margin different people will draw the line in different places. However, there is
little disagreement in the core of the research.
Given the growth in BAR, any attempt to provide a detailed review of BAR in general would
lead to a paper far beyond one this author could be expected to competently produce. Moreover,
recently a significant number of specialized reviews have been published offering the potentially
interested reader a wide variety of in-depth studies of BAR by both research topic 共e.g., auditing,
management accounting兲 and research method 共e.g., laboratory experiments, field research兲. These
reviews are cited in this paper where appropriate and review papers, or those with particularly
useful reviews of the literature, are identified in the reference section of this paper.
What would appear to be needed at this point in time is a framework within which the reader
can integrate the diverse studies making up BAR. To do this, I will present a framework that
focuses on the reference group of the studies, highlighting examples of research conducted in each
focal domain using different research methods and from different accounting sub-fields within
BAR. This approach not only is more parsimonious, but also permits the highlighting of a critical
facet of any research: complementarities of BAR across accounting sub-fields and methods. For
example, a paper dealing with audit teams may inform researchers interested in teams in manage-
ment accounting, and a field study may provide a laboratory researcher with the insight needed to
design a better experiment.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 3

The paper consists of six sections. The first provides an overview of the paper and the
framework used. The second through fifth sections discuss each of the broad categories of studies
in the framework. The final section offers a brief summary of the paper.

ORGANIZATION AND SCOPE OF THE REVIEW


The approach used in this paper to categorize BAR is the behavioral unit that is the object of
the research. Does the research study the behavior of an individual, group, etc.? Organizing studies
in this manner highlights the similarities across otherwise diverse studies and is intended to
facilitate intellectual exchange among accounting researchers. To do this, I must necessarily re-
strict the depth of the review in any section to accommodate the desired breadth of coverage. The
framework is described in the next section. Like BAR, the boundaries between these categories at
times are subjective. For example, a paper may cover issues appropriate for understanding both
groups and organizations 共Anderson et al. 2002兲.

Framework
I have elected to view the extant BAR by what I have labeled its “focus.” I define focus as the
unit used to analyze the research question共s兲. The units range from the study of individuals to the
study of the environment that acts upon accounting or that accounting helps to shape. The four
categories used in this review were selected because they define distinct sets of research
questions.1 The categories include:
• individuals,
• small groups,
• organizations, and
• environmental conditions.
Because a study’s classification is determined by the set of individuals it considers in the
research question共s兲 and/or the analysis, the categories can be viewed as constituting a series of
concentric circles, with the innermost circles representing the more micro studies. The outer
“rings” represent more macro studies reflecting the broader focus of the research question共s兲. The
environmental conditions category can be interpreted as the “world” within which all other events
occur. Two important points should be noted. First, within the categories, particularly the indi-
vidual category, there may be sub-categories. Second, studies from one category may inform
studies in another, likely adjacent category.

Definition and Discussion of the Categories


Individuals. These studies focus on the characteristics of a single actor and/or that actor’s
response to a particular accounting data set, accounting-related stimulus, or accounting-related
setting. It is by far the most active of the BAR categories discussed in this paper and can be
viewed as consisting of its own sub-categories. One line of individual research can be character-
ized by a concern with how individuals solve problems. I label these “pure choice” studies because
they focus on how well any actor can solve a problem without consideration being given to the
behavior of other actor共s兲. Recently, many of these studies have investigated the manner in which
the economic model 共“economic man”兲 in some significant way does not fit the behavior we
observe.
The second line of research explicitly considers the role of strategic behavior in the actor’s

1
This organization is similar to Hopwood’s 共1976, 5兲 Figure 1.1 describing the social context of accounting. He had four
categories: individual needs, group pressures and control, organizational structures and control strategies, and the social
economic environment. The organization used here differs from Hopwood’s by recognizing differences within the group
pressures and control categories between individuals and groups. This reflects changes in BAR over the decades.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
4 Birnberg

decision. In these studies the actor explicitly 共should兲 consider the behavior of a second actor who
actually is present in the setting. These studies would include negotiation 共e.g., Fisher et al. 2000兲
or “cheap talk” 共e.g., Zhang 2008兲. I label these “strategic studies.”
Groups. Research classified as covering groups includes those studies where the relevant unit
of analysis consists of a small number of individuals. Typically, the members will be viewed by
the organization as affiliated 共i.e., as acting in concert in some significant way兲. Thus, what
differentiates group research from research studying participants individually or strategically in-
teracting in dyads is the affiliation of the members. The actors are assumed to be in the same unit
at the time of the study. This would exclude studies such as those where the individuals are located
in different levels in a hierarchy. It is distinguished from research on organizations on two dimen-
sions. One is pragmatic. Groups are small enough to permit the researcher to study the interaction
among the multiple participants. As the size of the group increases, researchers find it more
difficult to create and/or analyze the interactions 共process兲 and the focus of the research shifts from
the members of the group/organization to the organization itself. The other distinction is the focus
of the research. While group research is concerned with the activities of the group’s members,
organization research is concerned with the role of policy or the effect of characteristics of the
organization or its environment on the organization’s accounting policy or the organization as a
whole. This reflects a higher level of aggregation where the behavior of the individuals is lost. For
practical purposes the upper limit of group research usually is relatively small, typically four.
Organizations. As noted above, the focus of this research is on the characteristics of the unit.
The entity studied may be described by the legal boundaries of a firm or a division within a larger
entity. The research question often is the role played by structural characteristics such as task
complexity or the organization’s accounting system design. These studies move us farther away
from the characteristics of the individual discussed in the two previous categories. It identifies the
individuals/groups that compose the organization by the roles they occupy rather than by focusing
on the characteristics/actions of the individuals who occupy them.
Environmental conditions. These studies examine the role of accounting in society. Studies
included in this category reflect the interaction between accounting and society: that is, the broader
world of which accounting is a part. The interaction can take the form of the external forces that
shape accounting, as well as studies of the role accounting has played in shaping the world in
which we live. The former may be closely related to BAR studies in organizations. For example,
Prime Minister Margaret Thatcher’s intention to privatize British Rail affected the relative roles of
accounting and engineering within the organization 共Dent 1991兲, or the potential impact of the
whistleblower provisions of Sarbanes-Oxley 共Hunton and Rose 2010; DeZoort et al. 2008兲. How
the institution of standards for outputs led to the establishing of standard sizes for clothing 共Jeacle
2003a兲 is an example of how developments in accounting 共standard costs兲 can lead to changes in
the environment 共standard sizes兲.

INDIVIDUALS
The earliest BAR studies across all accounting areas were of this type and it continues to be
the dominant form of BAR. Shields 共2007兲 reported that 90 percent of the papers published in
BRIA from 2004 to 2007 studied the behavior of the individual. As noted earlier, studies of the
individual are of two types: individual choice studies and strategic studies. While the two share a
common core of issues such as the selection of participants and the research methods utilized, they
are significantly different in many other ways. Thus, this section of the paper is organized in a
slightly different manner than those discussing the other elements of the framework. The first
sub-section discusses issues common to both. The second sub-section discusses elements specific
to individual choice studies, and the third sub-section does the same for strategic choice studies.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 5

Common Issues
The two types of individual choice studies share many common features. These include the
research method selected and the choice of participants. Each of these is discussed below. The
section also discusses differences between the traditional economic model of self-interested be-
havior and recent findings in the areas of interpersonal utility, trust, and cooperation found in this
research.

Research Methods
The individual choice studies consist predominately of experiments, though some utilize
surveys 共Shields 2007兲. Experiments are particularly appropriate when the relevant dimensions of
the decision environment in which the decision maker interacts with the stimulus and makes the
decision are well known. Experiments have been used in BAR to examine a wide variety of
questions, including internal policies, external policies, tax reporting policies, incentive systems,
various types of resource allocation decisions, ethical issues, and various types of reports. The
responses measured have varied from objective outcomes such as investment decisions 共Libby and
Tan 1999兲 to more subjective perceptions such as fairness 共Evans et al. 2005兲 or trust 共Coletti et
al. 2005兲. Overall, studies of this type are the predominant form of research in BAR, particularly
North American BAR, and can be found across a wide variety of topics, accounting sub-areas, and
settings.
Individual choice studies also utilize surveys 共e.g., Chalos and Poon 2000; Clinton and
Hunton 2001兲 and archival data 共e.g., Banker et al. 2000a兲. Archival studies often reflect a natu-
rally occurring experiment that permits the researcher to study behavior before and after the
change 共“stimulus”兲 has taken place.

Participants
A significant shift has taken place in the nature of the participants used in experimental
studies. Participants in the early studies most often were students 共undergraduate business majors
and/or M.B.A. students兲. BAR studies of the individual over the past two decades, however, have
required and utilized professionals as participants to a far greater degree. This is a significant
difference from the disciplines from which BAR draws its theories 共e.g., psychology兲, where the
generic participant remains the norm. This reflects the differences in the two groups’ reference
populations for external validity. The use of professionals as participants became necessary when
BAR shifted from its initial focus of “how participants respond while playing a particular role” to
“whether the skills accumulated by professionals insulate them from the negative effects of heu-
ristics and biases when performing complex tasks” 共e.g., Libby and Trotman 1993; Kennedy
1993兲. Students cannot simulate that accumulated experience or professional knowledge, nor can
a mundane experimental task provide insight into the professional’s work.
The use of professional participants in BAR implicitly assumes that the professional’s behav-
ior in an experimental setting accurately reflects their behavior “on the job.” Fehr and Leibbrandt
共2008兲 address this issue. They examine the cooperating behavior of fishermen both in a labora-
tory trust experiment and their level of cooperation to avoid over-fishing a given area. They find
that the participants’ behavior in the experimental setting accurately predicted their work behavior.
The broadening of the issues covered by BAR has expanded the type of professional partici-
pants required. The revival of interest in financial BAR now requires participants possessing
accounting expertise. BAR investigating proposed changes in the accounting rules requires
sophisticated/expert participants to test the validity of the hypotheses and enhance the study’s
external validity 共e.g., Hirst and Hopkins 1998兲. This also is true of BAR investigating anomalies

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
6 Birnberg

found in archival financial accounting research to generate BAR hypotheses 共e.g., Joe 2003兲,2 as
well as studies of the behavior of information providers in financial markets, such as security
analysts 共e.g., Libby and Tan 1999兲. However, many financial accounting-oriented BAR studies
continue to utilize M.B.A. students as surrogates for the “naïve investor” 共e.g., Tan and Tan 2009兲.
For a review of these studies and a discussion of the issues, see Libby et al. 共2002兲 and Koonce
and Mercer 共2005兲.
An exception to the use of professionals as participants is found in experiments in manage-
ment accounting. What we have learned from use of auditors and investors as participants would
suggest that manager participants likely would exhibit many of the same cognitive biases as
student participants 共e.g., Kennedy 1993; Gilad and Kliger 2008兲. However, this comparability
may not carry over to activities such as budgeting behavior and negotiation. See Vance et al.
共2008兲 for an auditing example.
Some researchers utilizing student participants attempt to compensate for the participants’
lack of expertise by measuring participants’ task-specific knowledge 共e.g., so many courses in
accounting or years of work experience兲. They also use measures of the participants’ general
problem-solving ability, such as SAT or GMAT scores or responses to selected questions from
tests of that type 共e.g., Dearman and Shields 2005兲. For a nonaccounting study, see Burks et al.
共2008兲. These measures typically are used to identify potentially relevant differences among in-
experienced participants 共i.e., students兲. However, Dearman and Shields 共2005兲 use their problem-
solving ability measure as an independent variable to explain why some participants exhibit
nonfixated behavior while others did.
One topic related to the selection of participants in which BAR has shown less interest than
others of decision making-oriented research is gender differences. Non-BAR strongly suggests
that this may be an issue. These studies have reported significant gender-related differences in
areas such as risk taking 共e.g., Jacobsen et al. 2007; Huang and Kisgen 2008兲, competition 共e.g.,
Gupta et al. 2005兲, and negotiation behavior 共e.g., Bowles et al. 2007兲. All these areas can be
important in BAR.
Those BAR studies reporting the presence 共or absence兲 of gender-related differences in ob-
served behavior have utilized these data in one of two ways. One uses the participant’s gender as
an independent variable 共e.g., Johnson et al. 1998兲. These studies investigate the conditions under
which the participant’s gender could affect behavior. If gender differences exist, randomization
may obscure their effect共s兲. Other studies check for gender differences to be sure that they do not
confound the experiment’s results 共e.g., Booker et al. 2007; Fleischman et al. 2007兲. Future BAR
may show greater awareness of the issue since SSRN in June 2009 established an ARN for
“Demographics, Gender, and Diversity Accounting Abstracts.”
Because of the limited research, it is an open question whether gender is as relevant an issue
when professional participants are used as it is in other studies. Do their professional training and
experiences override any gender issues? Two studies suggest that the differences may persist. Chin
and Chi 共2008兲, using archival data from Taiwanese audits, found that female auditors are more
risk-averse and more ethical in evaluating clients’ accruals. A survey of U.S., German, Italian, and
Thai fund managers 共Beckmann and Menkhoff 2008兲 found what they describe as the “expected
gender differences”: female respondents are more risk-averse and exhibit greater aversion to
competition.
Noneconomic Dimensions Affecting the Individual
In what could be labeled “post-modern” BAR, a line of research focuses on the appropriate-
ness of two assumptions in the traditional economic model. One is that self-interest is the sole

2
In an interesting twist, Allee et al. 共2007兲 used archival financial accounting data to provide convergent validity for BAR
hypotheses.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 7

motivator of choice; the other is the use of monetary outcomes as the sole basis for measuring the
utility of an outcome. While it is possible to integrate these arguments into the utility function
共e.g., Birnberg and Snodgrass 1988; Luft 1997; Casadesus-Masanell 2004兲, BAR tends to view
these dimensions as if they are constraints on the individual’s wealth-maximizing behavior.
Typically, BAR studies of this type bring together literature from psychology and experimen-
tal economics. They stress that rather than behave in a self-interested manner, individuals conform
to certain social norms such as fairness, equity, trust, honesty, or a willingness to cooperate. For a
discussion of these issues, see Camerer 共2001兲, Rabin 共1993, 1998兲, Fehr and Gaechter 共2000兲,
Fehr and Schmidt 共1999兲, Moser 共1998兲, Evans et al. 共2001兲, Evans et al. 共2005兲, and Dawes and
Thaler 共1988兲. Another dimension related to fairness and equity but not explicitly discussed in
BAR is egalitarianism 共Dawes et al. 2007兲. Overall, these studies are important for BAR for two
reasons. First, they show how little it takes for the participants to exhibit non-self-interested
behavior. Second, they show the importance of the individual’s perception of equal/fair treatment
relative to his or her peers and how they respond to a lack of perceived equity/fairness. Trust is of
interest to behavioral researchers of all types 共Rousseau et al. 1998; Sapienza et al. 2007兲. In BAR,
Rose 共2007兲 examined how management’s financial reporting behavior affected the investors’
willingness to trust them. Evans et al. 共2001兲 focus on the individual in a management accounting
environment and show that individuals will behave honestly in a setting where their dishonest
behavior would not be detected, thereby violating the self-interest assumption. As a possible
explanation of this type of behavior, Rutledge and Karim 共1999兲 found that those participants who
did not exploit their asymmetric information in a principal-agent setting scored higher on ethical
development than those who did. Their research and many other papers suggest that non-totally
self-interested behavior is the norm or “default” behavior for many individuals and in many
settings, rather than the self-interested behavior postulated in traditional economic theory. A pos-
sible explanation for this behavior is their perception of whether they were treated fairly 共e.g.,
Greenberg 1990; Hannan 2005兲.
These findings can lead to interesting research on the individual’s response to their absence of
fairness. Remindful of Lucy van Pelt and Charlie Brown’s ongoing “relationship” over his kicking
the football, Bohnet and Zeckhauser 共2004兲 report that decision makers exhibit an aversion to
betrayal and take actions to avoid it. Wang 共2007兲 examines the symmetry between the punish-
ment for dishonesty and the reward for honesty. She finds that honesty is rewarded more gener-
ously than dishonesty is punished. Issues of this type can be related to resource allocation in
managerial accounting and client behavior in auditing. In both cases, the research question would
involve identifying which behaviors lead to trust 共or distrust兲 between the parties. What causes an
auditor to trust one client more than another? What causes a superior 共manager or auditor兲 to trust
a particular subordinate?
Any trust-oriented research raises 共at least兲 two questions related to experimental design. One
is the importance of the experiment’s context 共degree of realism兲 and the choice of participants
共students or professionals兲 used in the study. The other is the importance of the presence or
absence of the interaction with a real person when the participant is told of the existence of
another participant. The latter issue is discussed under strategic choice situations.
Culture and Its Impact on Decision Makers
BAR studies dealing with social norms and potentially differing values across cultures ask
whether differences in culture result in different decisions/behaviors. For the most part, these
studies have utilized the framework of Hofstede 共1980兲. However, it is important to be aware that
some issues have been raised about the appropriateness of his categories 共e.g., Baskerville 2003;
McSweeney 2002兲. Because of the readily apparent cultural differences, the greatest portion of
this research has compared Asian and North American workers 共e.g., Birnberg and Snodgrass

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
8 Birnberg

1988; Chow et al. 1999兲. Thus far, the studies are inconclusive. While some of the studies have
found differences consistent with their predictions 共e.g., Kachelmeier and Shehata 1997兲, others
have not 共Birnberg et al. 2008兲. In an interesting archival financial accounting study related to
BAR, Doupnik 共2008兲 finds inter-country differences in earnings management after allowing for
differences for legal regimes.
The potential role of national cultures is becoming more important as BAR internationalizes
and research findings reported by researchers from many different countries appear in journals and
SSRN. This raises the following question. Are research findings from one country universally
applicable or should we be concerned and replicate them before we accept their universality?
As management systems and styles “internationalize” in large, industrialized economies, it
may mitigate concerns over cross-cultural differences. However, this homogeneity may not be
present in small-scale economies. In contrast to results reported in some BAR, Henrich and the
Cross Cultural Ultimatum Game Research Group conducted an extensive study across 15 small-
scale economies. Their study is important because they examine behavior among economies
where the variation in economic development is far greater than those typically studied by BAR.
Using the dictator game and a social dilemma game, as well as the ultimatum game, they report
that the “textbook economic model” failed to predict the observed behavior. Their results are
reported in various forms 共Henrich et al. 2005, 2001兲, as well as in Henrich’s 共2007兲 plenary
address at the AAA’s 2007 annual meeting. They conclude behavior in the experiments is gener-
ally consistent with economic patterns of everyday life in these societies. Henrich et al. 共2001,
73–74兲 report that, “The higher the degree of market integration 关in their society兴 and the higher
the payoffs to cooperation 关in their society兴, the greater the level of cooperation in experimental
games.”

Summary
While the methods used to study individual behavior have not changed significantly since
Birnberg and Shields 共1989兲, BAR has paralleled the trend found in experimental economics. A
significant portion of BAR now focuses on factors that influence decision makers in directions at
odds with the self-interest and wealth-maximizing assumptions. These noneconomic dimensions
include trusting behavior, cooperation, and the expectation of a fair share of any rewards. In
certain settings this can lead to greater monetary returns to the decision maker. However, they also
can expose the decision maker to greater risk. Other characteristics of the “work environment,”
such as the national/local culture, also can affect the expectations and behavior of the decision
maker. It has been suggested that certain of the cultural differences observed in individuals may be
based on different market conditions among countries.

Individual Choice Studies


There are a variety of reasons for the popularity of individual-focused research in BAR. The
first is simplicity. Considering the individual investor, auditor, etc., in isolation lends simplicity to
both the study’s research model and its design. It also simplifies the analysis and interpretation of
the results. The second is parsimony. It takes the fewest number of participants to achieve the
desired number of observations per cell. This is especially important when the participants are
professionals. The third reflects the models generated in the disciplines on which BAR has drawn
most heavily 共economics and psychology兲. Both contain a significant literature relating to how the
individual makes a decision. Sociology and organization theory consider the “group” to be the
smallest unit and have been drawn on by BAR to a significantly lesser extent.
Individual choice studies in BAR can be divided into two types, depending on the type of
variable investigated. One group of studies is interested in better understanding the impact of

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 9

elements of the setting within which the individual acts on the individual. The other is concerned
with the appropriateness of rational wealth-maximizing characterization of the decision maker.

Factors Related to the Task Setting


Four elements of the task setting are of particular interest in individual BAR. These are
incentives, participation, accountability, and systems interface. The first two are the focus of a
significant portion of BAR; the latter two, much less.
Incentives. Chow 共1983兲 initiated experimental research on the role of incentives in BAR.
This line of BAR literature typically uses the principal-agent model to generate hypotheses. For a
survey of the economic models of incentives, see Prendergast 共1999兲. In general, the studies report
that incentives matter and the nature of the incentive system impacts an agent’s behavior 共e.g.,
Bonner et al. 2000; Towry 2003; Sprinkle et al. 2008兲.
Participation. Participation is, essentially, concerned with the honesty of communication
within the organizational hierarchy. Early BAR investigated how accurately the workers/agents
would communicate their private information. Would they use it to create slack? Generally, the
answer was yes 共e.g., Young 1985; Shields and Shields 1988兲.3 However, as discussed subse-
quently, later research recognized the strategic nature of the interaction between the subordinate
and the superior and modeled participation as a negotiation process.
Accountability. Given the function of accounting, it is surprising that the formal develop-
ment of accountability was in psychology 共see Lerner et al. 1998 for a review兲 despite the obvious
link to management accounting research; that is, the effect of evaluation on individual behavior
共e.g., Argyris 1952; Prakash and Rappaport 1977兲. The notion of evaluation in BAR is not limited
to management accounting. When the superior in an audit team examines the work of a subordi-
nate or a client examines the work of a tax professional, an “evaluation” is taking place. The
difference between the evaluation literature and BAR on accountability is reflected in the breadth
of the questions they ask. The evaluation literature focuses on how the accounting system 共e.g., the
performance indicator兲 affects the extent and direction of the effort provided by the “workers”
共Prakash and Rappaport 1977兲. Accountability BAR not only asks for what the worker feels
accountable, but also asks to whom the “worker” feels accountable when facing conflicting de-
mands 共e.g., Johnson and Kaplan 1991; Messier and Quilliam 1992兲, or how elements present in
the accountability setting 共e.g., a need to justify one’s actions兲 affect the worker’s behavior 共Ah-
rens 1996兲.
Miller et al. 共2006兲 recognized that there is an element of mutual accountability in the evalu-
ation process. The superior likely has a prior relationship with the subordinate and in many
instances must “justify” any evaluation he/she makes. Their study focuses on the reviewer in an
audit setting. While the study only examines one party to the dyad, their findings suggest that
factors such as familiarity between the two parties can affect the reviewer’s assessment. There
may be limitations on the ability to perform these experiments with professional participants in
dyads because of the potential impact on the participants’ post-experimental relations.
Systems interface. Information systems in BAR essentially are viewed as decision aids. They
are discussed under various labels, such as decision support systems 共DSS兲 and knowledge based
systems 共KBS兲. The DSS typically is used in the management information systems literature to
describe an information system intended to support a specific decision and is closest to the term
decision aid 共DA兲, which typically is used in auditing to describe what may or may not be a
computerized calculating system. In contrast, the KBS refers to a database collected for a specific

3
Those familiar with the dictator game discussed below will recognize that Young’s 共1985兲 task is essentially the use of
a dictator game to simulate participation.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
10 Birnberg

area of inquiry 共e.g., XBRL兲.


The simpler of the two is the DSS. Two broad questions are researched under DSS. How well
are the systems utilized by those for whom they are intended? And, what characteristics of the
DSS facilitate or inhibit their utilization? Specific issues researched under the former include not
only whether the DSS improves decisions, but whether the potential users utilize them and
whether the system can be used to facilitate learning. They differ from the individual choice BAR
studies discussed earlier 共i.e., that examined how the individual responds to specific outputs of the
system兲. Those studies typically are linked to cognitive issues and the use of accounting data 共e.g.,
Lipe and Salterio 2000; Dearman and Shields 2005兲. The papers discussed in this section are
concerned with the utilization of a DSS as a DA designed to assist an individual perform a specific
task. In general, they report that the DSS is not always utilized 共e.g., Whitecotton 1996; Eining et
al. 1997兲.
Whitecotton 共1996兲 found that auditors’ reliance on the DA was inversely related to their
confidence in their own judgment. Obviously, this raises two questions. Is the auditor’s confidence
appropriate? And, how do those using the DA perform relative to the best auditors? Rose and
Wolfe 共2000兲 shed some light on the second question. Using student participants and a tax calcu-
lation task, they report participants who performed the calculation using “pencil and paper” rather
than the DA outperformed the best DA-assisted group by 22 percent, but required 112 percent
more effort 共Rose and Wolfe 2000, 297; also see Glover et al. 1997; Borthick et al. 2006兲. It is
important to learn whether the results can be replicated with professionals because it is likely that
their judgment is superior to that of the students.
Arnold et al. 共2006兲 studied the type of data from the KBS used by 共relative兲 novices 共senior/
staff auditors兲 and 共relative兲 experts 共partner/manager兲. The two groups differed on several dimen-
sions. Novices chose feedforward explanations, while the experts chose feedback. Arnold et al.
共2006兲 report that the greater the experts’ reliance on feedback explanations from the KBS, the
greater their adherence to the KBS’ recommendation.
There also are interactive systems intended to facilitate access to larger databases. These DSS
are intended to improve the quality of decision making or assist in training. The issues considered
revolve around the usefulness of the database. In BAR, the issue typically can be framed in terms
of the behavioral characteristics of the user and the usefulness to the user of the DSS. The XBRL
is an example of such a system. It is intended to enhance the user’s ability to obtain and under-
stand financial data about the firm. Hodge et al. 共2004兲 found that nonprofessional users of
financial statements were better able to ascertain the impact of differing reporting methods for
stock options between firms using the XBRL than without it. However, like Rose and Wolfe
共2000兲, they reported that many of their participants did not utilize XBRL. Other BAR has as its
purpose examining the use of DSS as a tool for training/educating novices.
Alternative modes of communicating information, such as graphs, frequently are used in
reports. For example, nonnumerical formats are regularly used in corporations’ annual reports,
internal reports, and our research. This issue initially was asked by MIS researchers in the 1970s
共Dickson et al. 1977兲 and subsequently extended 共e.g., Vessey 1994兲. Despite the extensive use of
pie charts and graphs in internal and external reports, there is little research in BAR on this topic
共for an exception, see Amer 2005兲. In marketing, MacKay and Villarreal 共2007兲 found that the
recipient’s ability to take advantage of the simpler nature of nonnumerical data is likely to vary
among individuals. An interesting example of earlier research in this area, using faces to commu-
nicate financial data, was reported by Moriarity 共1979兲.

Noneconomic Dimensions Affecting the Individual


The above dimensions of the task are essentially elements of the task setting in which the
individual makes a decision. They typically are set by the organization or environment within

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 11

which the decision maker is operating. The decision maker also brings certain characteristics such
as trust and fairness to the setting. These characteristics may be 共relatively兲 stable for any decision
maker 共e.g., desire to be treated fairly兲, or they may vary with the situation 共e.g., the decision
maker’s mood兲. In this section, these characteristics as they relate to individual choice are dis-
cussed.
Ethics. Closely related to the study of norms is the study of ethical behavior. The former
often is researched in the context of what others expect the actor to do, while ethical behavior
typically refers to the actor’s behavior. Noreen 共1988兲 offers a theoretical link between ethics and
agency theory. He argues that parties to the contract could be expected to follow social norms.
Early BAR on ethics focused on the participants’ moral development 共e.g., Ponemon 1990兲. These
studies are concerned with two issues. How developed is the moral reasoning of particular
individuals/groups? And, how does a given level of ethical development affect participants’ on-
the-job behavior? These two questions can easily be adapted for BAR in any of the accounting
sub-areas. The broader issue is how significant the ethical issue is in that sub-area. Auditing
researchers have led the way in considering the role of ethics in BAR. For reviews, see Louwers
et al. 共1997兲 and Jones et al. 共2003兲.
Like the cross-culture research described earlier, the ethics-based research has been charac-
terized by issues over how to measure the level of ethical development/behavior of the partici-
pants. This is not surprising since, like culture, the level of an individual’s ethical development is
not observable 共as distinct from actions兲. For a discussion of the different approaches, see Cohen
et al. 共1996兲.
In a post-Enron world, BAR in both auditing and management may find the issue of increased
importance. The problem facing the researcher is likely to be one of access. To minimize the
degree of intrusiveness and obtain responses, this research typically relies on surveys or cases to
elicit responses. There also appears to be a reluctance to publish these papers in the mainstream
accounting journals. A significant number of BAR studies have been published in The Journal of
Business Ethics 共e.g., Arnold et al. 2007; Emerson et al. 2007兲.
Two tax-oriented ethics studies suggest possible studies for management accounting behav-
ioral researchers. Fleischman et al. 共2007兲 demonstrate the linkage across the various aspects of
individual-focused research. The paper examines the evaluation by managers in a case concerning
the ethical behavior of a spouse in the context of a tax setting 共innocent spouse rule兲. The paper
explores the potential existence of the innocent spouse rule as a norm and the extent to which
research in ethics by behavioral scientists can explain it. Similar studies might be conducted in
management accounting. They could relate the participant’s response to the firing of an innocent
manager and, for example, the participant’s predicted subsequent job behavior. This behavior
relates to the issue of perceived fairness discussed earlier. In the area of financial accounting, Rose
共2007兲 related how what could be labeled 共un兲ethical reporting by management leads to 共dis兲trust
on the part of investors.
Cruz et al. 共2000兲 report that tax professionals’ willingness to resist the client’s desire for
aggressive tax reporting is positively correlated with professionals’ score on the Multidimensional
Ethics Scale. This raises the question of how a subordinate might respond to a superior’s efforts
for a more favorable set of budget estimates. Would a measure of ethical development predict the
likelihood of cooperation? In an experiment in financial reporting, Vance et al. 共2008兲 hypoth-
esized and found that the better the superior-subordinate relationship, the less likely the subordi-
nate was to resist the superior’s request for aggressive financial reporting.
Two sets of BAR studies have extended early BAR on ethics in interesting ways. They
examine the impact of the individual’s environment on the individual’s ethical behavior. Booth
and Schulz 共2004兲 examine the impact of the organization’s ethical climate on the individual’s
behavior. In a laboratory study, they find that holding the participant’s level of ethical development

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
12 Birnberg

constant, the behavior of the participant moves in the direction of the organization’s ethical
climate. There is no reason to believe that similar results would not be found in the effect of the
permissiveness of audit firms on auditor behavior.
Spicer et al. 共2004兲 and Bailey and Spicer 共2007兲 linked cross-cultural research and ethics.
Earlier studies had reported ethical differences among auditors in different countries 共e.g., Patel et
al. 2003; Arnold et al. 2007兲. Spicer et al. 共2004兲 and Bailey and Spicer 共2007兲 researched the
ethical norms of a culture on individuals raised in a different culture. In their studies, they studied
U.S. expatriates in Russia involved in the Russian business community. They report convergence
in ethical attitudes and intended behaviors between U.S. expatriate and Russian respondents to
their ethics survey. The U.S. expatriates in their study responded more like their Russian counter-
parts than U.S. nationals in the U.S. The respondents also expressed similar attitudes toward
organizational practices that violated the ethical standards or “hyper-norms.” The U.S. expatriate
respondents who were highly integrated into the Russian community expressed ethical attitudes
similar to those of Russian respondents under conditions of “local 共Russian兲 norms.” In both cases,
the ethical attitudes of Russians and Americans converge despite the differences that might have
been expected to arise due to their respective national identities.
Mood. Recently, psychologists, experimental economists, and accountants have begun to
examine the role of the decision maker’s emotional state 共affect兲 on the decision process. These
studies could be important if different mood states affect the decision maker’s perceptions and
decisions. While mood could affect strategic interactions, the research undertaken in BAR thus far
has focused on the individual decision maker.
The rationale underlying studies of this type is that mood affects the nature of the prior
experiences retrieved from memory. Positive mood states lead to retrieving positive outcomes in
comparable situations and vice versa. Wright and Bower 共1992兲, in a BAR-related study, reported
the effect of decision makers’ emotional state 共happy, neutral, or sad兲 on their perception of the
degree of riskiness of a decision and probability of success. As they conjectured, the subjective
probability estimate is influenced by the decision maker’s mood. “Happy” decision makers give
higher probabilities for the outcome of positive events and lower probabilities for the outcome of
negative events. They report the opposite results for “sad” decision makers.
In an accounting context, Moreno et al. 共2002兲 and Kida et al. 共2001兲 report similar results.
Consistent with these results, Chung et al. 共2007兲 studied auditors making inventory valuation
decisions and find that mood state affects the degree of conservatism in the auditor’s inventory
valuation. Auditors in a positive mood are less conservative than those in a negative mood.
Moreno and Bhattacharjee 共2008兲, in a single-party study 共the other party did not actually exist兲,
report that knowledge of the other party’s emotional state affects bargaining behavior. For a
discussion of the literature arguing that emotion can enhance the individual’s ability to make
rational choices, see Ackert et al. 共2003兲.
Psychologists and experimental economists have studied other emotional states that could be
of interest to accountants. Lerner and Keltner 共2000, 2001兲 report that fearful participants make
more pessimistic estimates and more risk-averse choices, while anger leads participants to make
more optimistic risk estimates and risk-seeking choices. Interestingly, the responses of angry
participants more closely resembled those of happy participants than those of fearful participants.
For reviews, see Lerner et al. 共2004兲 and Pham 共2007兲.
An interesting issue raised by these studies is whether the effect of these emotions is to make
people overly optimistic/pessimistic. We cannot conclude one way or the other without having
some baseline measure of the probability. What should the individuals believe the probability to
be? Since the participants disagree, we can assume that their emotional state has led at least one
of the groups to be incorrect, but that does not preclude the possibility that they both may be in
error. Ideally, further research will be undertaken in this area where there is a known “correct”

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 13

answer. A topic that conceivably could be related to the issue of optimism/pessimism is the effect
of regret in decision making. It has been shown to have an impact in many nonbusiness decision
settings 共e.g., Gilbert et al. 2004兲.
A paper by Libby et al. 共2008兲 suggests that optimism/pessimism is not always the “irrational”
result of the decision maker’s emotional state. They report that in some circumstances optimism/
pessimism may be the result of the incentives. If analysts desire good relations with management,
they report that, all else being held constant, the optimism/pessimism of sell-side analysts is a
deliberate act and not based on an emotion or trait.
Two recent studies suggest the possibility of yet another emotion that could be affecting
worker behavior—guilt/guilt aversion. These studies also illustrate how labels potentially can
serve to separate like ideas. Schnedler and Vadovic 共2007兲 hypothesize and find that guilt aversion
motivated participants to exert effort beyond the minimum required by the control system. One
might conjecture that this merely renames the concept embodied in “gift exchange” 共e.g., Hannan
2005兲. Staffiero 共2006兲 used guilt to describe the behavior of individual members of Japanese
work groups. The workers felt guilt when they made insufficient contributions to their work group.
In contrast, Birnberg and Snodgrass 共1988兲 offer a more positive explanation of this behavior,
suggesting that the outcomes to other members of the group may have a positive utility to an
individual member. Failure to achieve the group’s goal results in lowered utility because of the
loss to others as well as to oneself.
Fairness. While the perception of fairness has primarily been researched in strategic settings,
the perceived fairness of the accounting system affects the behavior of the individual in individual
choice settings as well. Libby 共2001兲 and Hufnagel and Birnberg 共1994兲 found that the participants
were sensitive to the perceived unfairness of the accounting system 共procedural fairness兲 even
when they were not adversely affected by the rule or system.

Physiological Measures and BAR


Behavioral accounting researchers have tried a variety of methods to understand decision
processes. The methods utilized are relatively non-intrusive, but provide greater insight than
observing an outcome/response in an experimental setting. These approaches include think-aloud
protocols 共e.g., Bedard and Biggs 1991兲 and data boards 共e.g., Shields 1980兲. These approaches
yielded insights into “cognitive flow” or the decision process being followed. However, both of
these methods directly involve the participant and are limited to reporting the decision maker’s
conscious behavior. The methods discussed in this section measure the same behaviors discussed
earlier, but use methods intended to measure physiological changes.
Hunton and McEwen 共1997兲 utilized an eye movement retinal imaging computer to study the
information search strategy of financial analysts. Unlike protocol analysis that relies on self-
reporting and data boards that report only choices, they were able to track the search strategies of
the analysts in a less obtrusive but more detailed manner. They were able to observe data scanned
but not reported 共protocols兲 or chosen 共data boards兲 by the participants. Consistent with data board
research, they found that the more accurate analysts used a directed rather than a sequential search
strategy. Their search appeared to be motivated by hypotheses generated by the process.4
In finance, Lo and Repin 共2002兲 used more traditional methods 共electro-dermal and pulse rate
measures兲 to measure the emotional state 共level of excitement兲 of ten stock traders while they were
actually trading. Lo and Repin 共2002兲 found significant differences between periods when signifi-
cant market events were and were not taking place. They argue this suggests that emotion is a

4
For a discussion of the use of eye movements in marketing research where they have been used more often, see Zaltman
共1997兲.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
14 Birnberg

relevant component of the traders’ decisions. Their data suggest that the response varies with
experience, but the sample is too small to draw any statistically significant conclusions.

Neuroeconomics and Neuroaccounting


Recently researchers studying decision making have taken a new approach. Working with
neuroscientists, they have gone one step deeper inside the “black box” that is the decision maker.
Using various devices, they observe the patterns of brain activation as individuals make choices
共e.g., McCabe et al. 2001; Camerer et al. 2005; Knudsen et al. 2007兲. Given the neuroscientists’
knowledge of the function of the brain centers, conclusions can be drawn about what underlies the
observed behavior. By moving one step closer to the decision maker’s cognitive activity, the role
of the stimulus and the response changes in an interesting way. The decision, typically considered
the response in BAR studies, now is the stimulus and the brain center activation is the response.
This is in contrast to traditional research in BAR where researchers observed behavior and inferred
the underlying cognitive processes or extracted them from protocols.
Thus far, little research of this type has been undertaken by behavioral accounting researchers
except for John Dickhaut 共e.g., Dickhaut et al. 2003; Smith and Dickhaut 2005; Rustichini et al.
2005; Dickhaut 2009兲. Dickhaut and his colleagues have papers 共Dickhaut 2009; Dickhaut et al.
2009a, 2009b兲 using neuroscience to study the evolution of recordkeeping 共i.e., accounting兲.
However, none of these papers provide the type of systematic review of the possible link between
neuroscience and BAR that can be found for finance in Sapra and Zak 共2008兲, who offer neuro-
science explanations for observed behaviors in financial decision making where data from neuro-
science and neuroeconomics are available.
While potentially quite insightful, there are at least three reasons why research of this type
will progress more slowly than other types of BAR. First, it requires cooperation with a researcher
possessing access to machines to perform the scans and skilled in reading brain scans. Second, it
would appear that research of this type is quite expensive. Third, explaining the findings to other
BAR researchers may be difficult. Moreover, the results may not eliminate the issue of “hard-
wired” versus “learned” behavior as the explanation for the response.
An example of neuroeconomic research’s potential relevance to BAR can be illustrated using
the findings of Luft 共1994兲 and Hannan et al. 共2005兲. Luft 共1994兲 found that participants in her
study preferred a bonus to a penalty pay scheme even though the payoffs from the two systems
were equivalent. Hannan et al. 共2005兲 found that the participants in the penalty condition exerted
more effort. Given that neuroscientists have shown that different brain centers are used to measure
pleasure 共reward兲 and pain 共penalty兲 共Delgado et al. 2000兲, this raises the question of whether the
preference for a bonus scheme reflects differences between the pleasure and pain brain centers
共“hardwired” neuroscience explanation兲 or whether it is the approval implied by the “reward” and
disapproval associated with a “penalty” 共a social psychology issue of intrinsic reward兲. Barnea et
al. 共2009兲, using Swedish data on twins to study investing behavior, suggest that there is both a
genetic and a learned component.
A series of neuroscience studies may provide some insight into what is happening. Using the
ultimatum game, Tabibnia et al. 共2008兲 report MRIs of the brain that suggest similar results to
those above for fair and unfair behavior. Their design utilized an individual choice study using
only participants who receive the “offer” 共ultimatum兲. First, the results suggest that the 共recipient兲
participants differ in what they believe to be a fair offer. Second, those who judge the offer to be
“unfair” show different patterns of brain activity than those who consider the offer to be “fair.”
Finally, participants who accept an unfair offer had different patterns in their MRIs than those who
reject unfair offers 共Tabibnia et al. 2008兲.
A study by Harbaugh et al. 共2007兲 that relates brain activity to altruism in decision makers
also illustrates the potential link of neuroscience to BAR. They studied the brain scans of 19

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 15

female students who were asked to make a decision allocating $100 between a food bank and
themselves. The brain scans of the “altruistic” 共gave more兲 and “selfish” 共gave less兲 participants
show that the altruistic participants exhibit greater activity in the part of the brain that reflects
pleasure than do the selfish participants. The altruistic participants show significant activity in that
part of the brain even when they were required to contribute a fixed portion of the $100 to the
charity. Studies of this type suggest that there is a physiological basis for the altruistic behavior
that is observed in the real world. It does not explain if the behavior is inherent and hardwired
共Hsu et al. 2008兲 or related to interacting with people and learned 共Andreoni 1990兲. The authors
suggest that they believe their results also would apply to male participants had they been included
in the study.
Zak and his colleagues introduced a line of neuroeconomic research that approaches the
“black box” of human cognitive processes in a different way. They argued that the observed
behavior, in this case trust, is based on the brain’s response to a particular hormone. Trusting
participants exhibit higher levels of the relevant hormone than nontrusting 共i.e., economically
rational兲 participants. This work is summarized in Zak 共2008兲. Kuhnen and Chiao 共2009兲 show
that there also appears to be a genetic basis for the differences in the amount of dopamine and
serotonin. In their study these differences, like those reported by Zak 共2008兲, are associated with
different patterns of behavior.

Summary: Individual Choice Studies


Overall, research focused on the individual’s decision-making behavior has played an impor-
tant role in BAR historically. The predominance of individual-focused research, particularly
among North American and many Australian researchers, is easily observed by examining a recent
issue of BRIA 共2007兲. It contained 13 papers. All of these papers could be classified as focused on
the individual even though they may describe in the scenario the existence of another/other
hypothetical person共s兲 or have a scripted confederate role-play the “other person.” Equally impor-
tant is the diversity in topics/areas in which the research is located. Three were related to auditing.
Four dealt with aspects of management accounting. Three were related to financial reporting/
decision making. There was one in tax ethics, one in cross-cultural ethics, and one related to
education. While this admittedly is a convenience sample, the results are similar to Shields 共2007兲.
They likely are representative of current BAR in North America. A very different view of BAR in
Europe would result from examining an issue共s兲 of AOS or other European-based accounting
journals.
This emphasis on individual-focused research is likely to continue to be true of BAR in North
America for several reasons. Many BAR questions focus on the behavior of individuals acting
alone. For example, some of the studies involve one individual’s processing data provided by
another individual or a system 共e.g., Fedor and Ramsey 2007兲. Others continue to be concerned
with the cognitive processes of individuals 共e.g., Joe 2003兲. Still others involve norms, ethics, and
culture, which typically have been studied by examining the behavior of the individual in isola-
tion. Finally, the individual also may be the easiest approach for researchers.
Individual choice studies do not exist in isolation from the other categories of BAR discussed
in this paper. As the research on strategic choice and group-focused behavior shows, understand-
ing the behavior of individuals often is the basis for hypotheses about behavior in dyads and
groups. Behavior such as honesty 共Evans et al. 2001; Cohen et al. 2007兲, that has been exhibited
in studies in which the individual does not actually interact with another participant, can lead to
predictions of behavior in dyads and groups that differ from those of classical economics. This is
particularly true because many of the individually focused studies are studies isolating one mem-
ber of a network of individuals. This is readily apparent in the next section in the discussion of
participation.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
16 Birnberg

There also are limitations in studying the individual in isolation. In part, this results from the
movement in organizations to make groups and teams the decision-making unit. In addition, a
certain amount of the richness found in the decision-making situation may be lost when BAR
isolates the individual from his or her environment.

Strategic Choice Studies


Studies that explicitly consider the participants’ strategic behavior are relatively new in BAR,
though strategic behavior often was implicit and important in earlier BAR. How managers behave
in a participative management setting is an example of a strategic setting. Moving from an
individual choice study where the actor’s behavior is “inward facing” to one where another actor’s
behavior explicitly must be considered introduces the strategic dimension to BAR. In contrast to
the individual choice studies, in the strategic behavior studies the decision maker must consider
the choices made 共or to be made兲 by an actual rather than a hypothetical fellow participant. For
example, in a management accounting study, the “strategy” to which the participant responds
could be the choice of budget level set by another participant acting as “management.” While an
individual choice study informs us how the manager/agent responds to a given budget level, we do
not learn which budget level the owner/principal would choose to offer to motivate the manager/
agent. In an individual choice study, the researcher may set the independent variable 共e.g., the
budget兲 at levels different from those a manager actually would choose.
A significant amount of experimental economics research uses experimental dyads 共see Roth
1995兲. In BAR, strategic choice studies recognize the limitations in studying the individual in
isolation from the environment and the importance in many settings of the behavior of the “other”
party on the individual. Some argue that it is important actually to have the “other party” exist
whenever the instructions indicate he/she does. Experimental economists argue that it is required
for one of two reasons. The first is “maintaining the integrity of the participant pool.” Experimen-
tal economists often utilize the same pool of participants in different studies. In some studies, the
participant’s experience in a prior study even is a criterion for selection. They argue it is important
the participants believe what they are told. If the post experimental debriefing informs them that
something was not really the case, they may speculate in future studies about the true nature of the
study. The other reason relates to the richness of the experimental setting. Unless the experimenter
has insight into how the other party will behave from prior field or laboratory research, including
the actual behavior of a participant will increase both the potential insights from and the validity
of the study. See Calegari et al. 共1998兲 for an example of this issue.5
Negotiation Studies
The negotiation process is ubiquitous in the business setting. For a review, see Tsay and
Bazerman 共2009兲. Audit firms negotiate with clients over changes in financial statements and
accounting methods 共McCracken et al. 2010兲, firms negotiate with suppliers when they establish
operationally intimate relationships 共JIT兲, and sub-units within the organization negotiate transfer
prices and/or quantities. While the surface characteristics of the situations are different, many of
the behaviors may be the same 共e.g., the strategies adopted by the parties兲. They may differ on
information asymmetry, division of payoffs, and relative power. The degree of information asym-
metry would be expected to affect negotiation, as could the incentives of the parties. For example,
in budgeting negotiations the parties typically are playing a zero-sum game. The slack absorbed by
the worker reduces the manager’s/principal’s profit by a like amount. In other cases, such as the

5
For a discussion of this literature from an auditing perspective but germane to all BAR, see Hooks and Schultz 共1996兲
and the symposium in Auditing 共e.g., Dopuch 1992 and Gibbins 1992兲. For the contrary view from psychology, see
Kelman 共1967兲.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 17

audit or transfer price settings, the negotiation game being played need not be a zero-sum game.
Rather, a small concession by one party may be significant to the other. Such an asymmetry in
payoffs should affect the negotiation process. Negotiation studies also can be characterized based
on the relative power of the participants: those where the parties have equal power and those
where one party has an advantage.
The significance of the strategic interaction is of particular importance for BAR because of
the importance of performance as a “response.” An example of how individual choice literature
and strategic choice settings are related can be found in Fisher et al.’s 共2000兲 study of participation
utilizing interacting dyads. In the framework utilized in this paper, this represents a paradigm shift.
Early BAR into participative budgeting focused on how the “worker” would behave. Would the
workers take advantage of their private information to create slack? Young 共1985兲 even had his
participants meet with a “supervisor” played by the experimenter or a colleague. However, the
“supervisor” did nothing more than accept the worker-participant’s budget. Thus, Young’s 共1985兲
study essentially is an individual choice study. While social pressure was present 共the design
forced the worker-participant to face a supervisor兲, it omitted any negotiation over the acceptabil-
ity of the worker’s proposed budget. The explicit power in the situation was vested with the
worker. In reality, the budget-setting process is quite different. In the natural setting, the supervisor
also has significant power. Thus, while Young 共1985兲 reported how the worker would act in
isolation, important aspects of participation are better captured as a dyad that permits strategic
interaction.
A second area of negotiation studies where the use of dyads is present is in the transfer price
literature. Like the participation studies, they are outcome-oriented. In an early study, DeJong et
al. 共1989兲 test the efficacy of various transfer pricing rules. Haka et al. 共2000兲 vary the precision
of the accounting data the manager possesses. The participants receiving the less precise informa-
tion negotiated strategically. They tried to achieve the best price at the risk of failing to reach an
agreement. In contrast, the participants with more precise data used the negotiation process to
communicate information to the other party about his or her position in an attempt to reach a more
informed decision. Chalos and Haka 共1990兲 and Ghosh 共2000兲 also studied the negotiation process
in the transfer price setting in laboratory experiments. Ghosh 共2000兲 observed that when the
incentive system is consistent with the sourcing of the input, the systems are perceived as fairer
and the participants behaved in a less exploitive manner. Also see Luft and Libby 共1997兲.
How humans negotiate and what motivates them to behave in a particular way is a question
of interest to all BAR. Findings in one area have implications for the others. Calegari et al. 共1998兲
report two interesting results concerning dyads using an auditing-based task. One relates to the
outcome of the negotiation process, the other to method. In their study, M.B.A. students, partici-
pating in the experiments as “auditors” and “clients,” exhibited two types of behavior: competitive
pairs and cooperative pairs. The competitive pairs behave as Calegari et al.’s 共1998兲 economic-
based hypotheses predict. However, the cooperative pairs exhibit what Calegari et al. 共1998兲
describe as signaling and cooperative behavior. What causes the pairs to behave differently is an
unanswered question that should interest BAR.
Calegari et al. 共1998兲 also reported an interesting methodological result. The outcomes from
a human-computer dyad were different from those of the human-human pairs. Obviously, the
computer was not programmed to respond to cues/signals, such as willingness to cooperate, that
the human partner might send. This reinforces the concern about the limits in utilizing the indi-
vidual choice style of research when the “other party” has an opportunity to act/interact strategi-
cally. This is especially true where the set of actions includes choices that could facilitate reaching
a noncompetitive, but mutually beneficial, conclusion.
There are, however, settings when studying dyads in a laboratory may not be practical or even
feasible. This would be especially true in cases such as Calegari et al. 共1998兲, where students may

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
18 Birnberg

not be suitable surrogates for professionals. This raises the issue of external validity. Researchers
have tried to resolve this problem in an audit setting by studying the negotiation process using
professionals as participants in individual choice studies that “simulate” interacting dyads. For
example, Favere-Marchesi 共2006兲 studied the initial negotiation postures of auditors and clients
over a proposed change in the financials, giving the same case study separately to each type of
participant. They conclude that ex ante the clients have a better understanding of the auditors’
initial position than the auditors do of the clients’. In a related study, Tan and Trotman 共2007兲
proposed and tested a model of when in the negotiation process auditors should make concessions
to clients. Their experiment uses financial officers as clients and a computer simulation as the
auditor who negotiates with the client 共via email兲. They report the clients’ responses and the
clients’ strategies in responding to the simulated auditor. However, their findings should be viewed
in light of Calegari et al. 共1998兲. How this initial difference and differing strategies would play out
during negotiations between financial officers and actual auditors remains an open question.
Because of the potential problems involved in using actual auditors and their clients, it is unlikely
to be studied in an experimental setting using professionals as participants in both roles. We may
need to rely on archival research to understand the behavior of these dyads 共e.g., Nelson et al.
2002兲.
Settings with explicitly unequal power. Other papers have utilized dyads in negotiation/
bargaining studies where the parties possess unequal power. These studies usually investigate the
presence or absence of the norm of fairness in economic man rather than negotiation in a specific
setting. They typically utilize either the ultimatum or the dictator game 共Roth 1995兲. In the dictator
game, one person 共the dictator兲 is given an endowment to allocate between self and another party
共the recipient兲. The recipient must accept the dictator’s allocation. These studies utilize actual
rather than simulated recipients. Because the recipient is passive in the experimental setting, the
use of a dyad would appear to be intended to meet the criterion of not misleading the participants.6
In contrast, in the ultimatum game, the first party’s 共the “proposer”兲 situation is identical to that of
the dictator except that the recipient now may accept or reject the proposer’s offer. If accepted, the
proposer’s offer determines each party’s payoff. However, if rejected, both parties receive nothing.
The results of studies using both games tend to support a norm of fair treatment expected by
the responders and recognized by the dictator/proposer 共Roth et al. 1991; Berg et al. 1995兲. In both
the dictator and ultimatum games, the first party makes an offer approaching, on average, 40
percent of the endowment 共Roth 1995兲. This result appears to reflect the recognition by many of
the participants of a norm that sets the “fair” allocation of the endowment.
Cheap talk research in dyads. The typical “cheap talk” study also reflects a setting where
the strategic interaction is germane to the study 共e.g., Kachelmeier et al. 1994; Rankin et al. 2003兲.
How will the party receiving the nonbinding message react to it? Obviously, such a study could be
done using the individual receiving the message as the focus. However, such a study would lose
the behavior of the participant who is allowed to make the cheap talk commitment. That individu-
al’s behavior also is of interest to the researcher. Thus, it is preferable for the study to use a dyad
共potential sender and receiver兲 rather than only a receiver. In general, research has found that the
cheap talk often is viewed by the recipient as if it is a binding commitment 共e.g., Kachelmeier et
al. 1994; Zhang 2008兲. Cheap talk studies can be conducted in any setting in accounting where the
context permits one party to communicate with and make a nonbinding pre-commitment to an-
other party that, if true, should affect the other party’s behavior.

6
There are, of course, designs where the recipient-participant could be needed later for another experiment. For example,
the recipient-participant in the early rounds could, in the later rounds or in another experiment, play the role of the
dictator. The researcher could study the interaction between the amount offered to a participant and the amount subse-
quently offered by that participant when acting as the dictator.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 19

Effect of non-negotiating third party. The work of Fehr and Gaechter 共2000兲 and Zhang
共2008兲 provide insight into why it is beneficial for the researcher to include all the potential parties
in a study. Fehr and Gaechter 共2000兲 report that a third party, who only observes unfair behavior,
is willing to incur a cost to punish the unfair participant. Zhang 共2008兲, in a BAR study, provides
an interesting twist on the strategic interaction present in dyads. The dyad about which she
hypothesizes consists of two managers 共agents兲 who report to the same owner 共principal兲. She
examined the truthfulness and whistle-blowing behavior of two agents. Each agent’s cost is com-
mon knowledge to the two agents, but asymmetrical information to the principal. Essentially, her
findings show that the strategic behavior of the members of the dyad 共the agents兲 depends on the
endogenous behavior 共fairness兲 of the third party 共the principal兲. The actual presence of the third
party in the study had two benefits. First, it enhances the internal validity of the study. Second, it
ensures that the principal’s behavior in the experiment actually reflects how the principal would
act. In this case, the principal offers a lower wage because of concerns over being cheated by the
agents. This insight, in turn, can serve as a basis for future BAR on the principal’s behavior in this
setting.
Reputation. We all utilize information on another’s past behavior 共i.e., reputation兲 in making
choices. Similarly, managers must rely on the reputation of other managers in making investment
decisions, and investors, analysts, and auditors rely on managers’ reputations in their interactions
with firms. However, there is limited research on the role of reputation in the willingness of one
party to trust another.7 This reflects the design of experiments. Most studies, such as those de-
scribed in the previous sections, use a “turnpike” approach. The participants are anonymously
paired and typically do not “play” the same participant more than once. This is intended to
eliminate reputation as a factor in decision making and as a potential confound. Thus, the question
of the reputation of individual players must be set aside.
But what is known is that when players interact over time, expectations and reputations are
formed and, moreover, the quality of decision making may improve relative to the turnpike design
共Schwartz and Young 2002兲. Duffy et al. 共2009兲 provide further insight into reputations. Partici-
pants may not always recognize the value of acquiring information about the other participant’s
behavior, a form of reputation. They reported that participants who initially received costless
feedback about the behavior of others utilized the feedback/reputation-related information. How-
ever, those participants who did not receive feedback information until later in the experiment did
not utilize the information to the same degree. In addition, they report that when a nominal cost is
attached to the feedback, participants did not buy the information even though it was quite
profitable to do so.
Note that in the studies discussed above, reputation is very stylized: it takes the form of very
specific information. This encapsulates the idea of reputation in the laboratory. However, in the
“real world,” the information that goes into forming a reputation may be subjective and imprecise.
Given the role that reputation can play in business settings, there is room for additional research
in this area.

Summary: Strategic Choice Studies


The study of dyads is at the intersection of individual and group BAR. It offers valuable
insights into the individual’s strategic behavior and is important for three reasons. One is that
strategic behavior is integral to many business activities. A second is that participants act differ-
ently when the other party is present rather than hypothetical 共e.g., Calegari et al. 1998兲. Finally,

7
Archival markets research has concerned itself with audit firm reputation, particularly in the wake of Arthur Andersen
共e.g., Barton 2005兲. There has been very limited BAR in this area 共Mayhew 2001, Mayhew et al. 2001兲. BAR has
operationalized the audit firm as an individual in experimental markets studies.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
20 Birnberg

and perhaps most importantly, the use of dyads permits the researcher to study both sides of the
strategic interaction and do so over a series of iterations between members of the dyad. The dyad
may be composed of peers as in Zhang 共2008兲 and Towry 共2003兲, or be hierarchical as in the
studies of budget negotiation 共Fisher et al. 2000兲 and the dictator and ultimatum games.
BAR research undertaken thus far suggests that the presence of a “real person” with whom the
participant interacts affects their behavior 共Calegari et al. 1998兲. BAR using dyads could be useful
in developing a better understanding of how managers and workers, as well as auditors and tax
professionals/payers, behave in various settings, in addition to insights into the negation process.
It also could reveal how “soft behavioral constraints” such as norms can affect behavior.
The nature of the interaction can vary, as can the mechanism used to achieve it. As even the
ultimatum game shows, both parties possess some power 共i.e., the ability to affect the behavior of
the other兲, albeit in some cases a very “soft” power. The study of how they use this power and how
the parties interact 共their strategies兲 is what makes the study of dyads interesting. It is important to
note that the results discussed above and elsewhere often run counter to the simplistic notion of the
self-interested, wealth-maximizing “economic person.”
Because dyads can be viewed as a subset of group behavior, studying dyads yields potentially
valuable insights into group behavior. However, there are obvious limitations. The greater level of
complexity facing the individual members of a group increases with the number of members
interacting. Thus, many of the laboratory studies reported below under group-focused BAR limit
the strategic choices available to the interacting parties. As useful as data gleaned from the study
of dyads may be, to better understand the group phenomenon in question researchers have turned
to alternative research methods relying on naturally occurring events 共fieldwork, archival data,
surveys, and interviews兲.
The ability to undertake research on dyads and observe the strategic interaction of the parties
may not be as easy as the BAR focusing on the individual. Dyad research at least doubles the
number of participants required with a comparable increase in the cost of the experiment. It also
can require a high degree of coordination. The participants must be available at the same time and,
typically, in the same place. This suggests that research of this type is likely to take place in a
laboratory or through fieldwork. The former is likely to mean student participants; the latter,
professionals performing their job in their natural environment. This would appear to limit the
amount of work of this sort that will be undertaken using nonstudent participants.

GROUPS
The label “group” in this context is used to include a variety of organizational structures.
Group is defined as any collection of individuals greater than two and typically no more than four
in laboratory studies. Rarely is it more than five members. This definition is admittedly arbitrary,
but consistent with the literature in the area. The above definition does not specify a particular
organizational structure共s兲 for a group. Thus, group as defined for this section includes not only
peer groups, but also teams and hierarchical groups.
Psychology research on group decision making initially focused on the quality and nature of
the individual versus group decisions. Which makes the better decision? Which makes the riskier
decisions? For a review, see Sutton and Hayne 共1997兲 and Daroca 共1984兲. Sociology was inter-
ested in the development of networks 共e.g., Homans 1951兲 and the affect of context variables on
group behavior 共e.g., Dalton 1959兲. For a review of sociology based studies, see Miller 共2007兲.
More recent studies have focused on the nature of the group processes. How does the composition
of the group 共e.g., temporary or permanent兲 affect its decision? What is the effect of changes in
group membership? How does the decision rule used by/imposed on the group affect their deci-
sion?

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 21

BAR on groups has addressed five broad categories: 共1兲 individual versus group performance,
共2兲 group decision processes, 共3兲 the role of technical and accounting systems in group decisions,
共4兲 the role of incentives, and 共5兲 the role of a group’s characteristics in its performance. Many
studies have asked questions that relate to more than one of the above categories. BAR group
research has utilized the full range of research methods including experiments 共e.g., Young et al.
1993兲, surveys 共e.g., Chalos and Poon 2000兲, protocols 共e.g., Bedard et al. 1998兲, video 共Walker
and Aritz 2006兲, and field research 共e.g., Anderson et al. 2002兲.

Participants
The type of participants used in group research has varied depending primarily on the sub-
category of BAR being studied. As is described below, auditing studies have used auditors as
participants whenever possible. Recently, studies have again begun to use students. This reflects
both the declining availability of auditors as participants and the belief that student participants
possess the appropriate knowledge, skill, and experience for many group tasks.
In contrast, the study of groups in other areas, particularly management accounting, has used
a more diverse set of participants. Laboratory studies typically have used students, albeit often
with significant work experience 共e.g., Daroca 1984; Rowe 2004兲. Managerial accounting re-
searchers have studied “real people” in field studies 共e.g., Anderson et al. 2002兲, and surveys of
managers reporting on “on the job” experiences 共e.g., Chalos and Poon 2000兲.

Group Decisions and Processes


It is interesting to note that the much of the early research on groups in BAR was in auditing
共Schultz and Reckers 1981; Reckers and Schultz 1982; Trotman et al. 1983兲. This likely reflected
the overall level of BAR interest in auditing during this period, as well as the absence of teamwork
in U.S. firms at that time. The findings of the auditing BAR studies generally are consistent with
earlier non-BAR group research. For example, Schultz and Reckers 共1981兲 report that decision-
making groups exhibited higher confidence and less variability than individuals. In a topic more
closely related to accounting than generic group research, Reckers and Schultz 共1982兲 report that
groups adhere to the accounting rules more closely than individuals. Indeed, because groups 共audit
teams兲 are the way audits are performed, the use of groups in auditing has been a continuing area
of BAR in auditing 共e.g., Solomon 1987; Reckers and Schultz 1993兲.
In management accounting, Daroca 共1984兲 studied participation in a group setting. He re-
ported that, as Becker and Green 共1962兲 conjectured, participation could result in group polariza-
tion against management, leading to negative rather than positive “gains” from participation.
These findings, like those of Zhang 共2008兲 and Greenberg 共1990兲, indicate that group involvement
may have negative outcomes for the organization if the leader’s style is perceived negatively by
the group.
Unlike the typical generic group study that focused solely on the group’s output/decision,
Bedard et al. 共1998兲 studied group processes as well as the efficacy of groups versus individuals.
They utilized protocols developed from audio tapes to examine communication among group
members and identify what type of interactions characterized successful and unsuccessful groups.
Because their sample was of necessity small, the findings must be viewed tentatively. However,
they raised an important issue by delving into what makes groups effective. Accordingly, they
studied process as well as outcomes. Bedard et al. 共1998兲 also investigated how the voting rule,
formal or informal, affects group behavior. Given the range of possible rules 共e.g., unanimity,
majority rule, and leader with a veto兲, it is reasonable to expect that the voting rule could affect the
group’s behavior and output/decision 共Birnberg et al. 1970兲. This issue is relevant to any group
decision-making setting within accounting. Given the size of Bedard et al.’s 共1998兲 sample, it is
hard to draw a conclusion.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
22 Birnberg

Chalos and Poon 共2000兲 used the group setting to study participation. They used a survey of
and interviews with 177 managers comprising 55 budget teams in a single firm to collect data on
the effect of group process on perceived quality of group decisions. They studied how the presence
of participation in the group’s capital budgeting process affects information sharing, budget em-
phasis, and self-reported measures of performance. They report that participation positively affects
the perception of the amount of performance information available, amount of information shar-
ing, and the reported importance of the budget process. However, it is central to note that the
researchers did not observe the groups in action.
Role of Decision Support Systems
Just as an individual’s decision making can be affected by the use of a decision support
system 共DSS兲, group decision making can be altered by a DSS. Murthy and Kerr 共2003兲 and Kerr
and Murthy 共2004兲 investigated the impact of different types of computer-mediated communica-
tion 共CMC兲 in different task settings on the quality of the group’s decision. Typically, the condi-
tions compared are face-to-face communication and computer-based system共s兲. The findings indi-
cate that face-to-face groups outperformed CMC groups when problem solving was the measure
of performance 共see also Rowe 2004兲. Interestingly, both CMC and face-to-face were equally
effective in generating ideas, though performance appears to be sensitive to the task setting and the
type of CMC. Kerr and Murthy 共2004兲 report that a bulletin board form of CMC outperforms
“chat rooms” and face-to-face communication in a decision setting that requires the participants to
exchange uniquely held information to reach a successful conclusion.
One caveat in evaluating the above findings is the use of student participants. Ho 共1999兲 used
audit partners, managers, and seniors to study the role of computerized decision support system
relative to face-to-face communication in a going-concern evaluation. Her study reports that
groups of both types considered evidence that individuals did not. She reports when comparing the
two groups that CMC groups had greater agreement on the going-concern assessment than did
face-to-face groups and had greater satisfaction with their evaluation. A possible explanation is
that the “impersonal” CMC setting may neutralize the ability of an influential/powerful individu-
al共s兲 in the group to exert undue influence in the group’s decision.
Carpenter 共2007兲 also used auditors in a group research study examining the recommendation
of SAS No. 99, which requires the use of groups formally in the audit process through “brain-
storming” sessions. Because the brainstorming literature in psychology using students had not
uniformly reported the synergistic behavior expected from group discussion 共Dennis and Valacich
1993兲, she studied the process in an audit setting using auditors as participants. She hypothesized
that brainstorming groups would perform better than individuals or nominal groups in part because
the group members were professionals “doing their job” in the experiment rather than student
participants performing a mundane task. Her results support the benefits of brainstorming. Hoff-
man and Zimbelman 共2009兲 extended Carpenter 共2007兲. They used brainstorming to improve the
audit program. In their study, a panel of experts brainstorm potential modifications in the fraud
detection program for a case study. They report that auditors subsequently given the case and the
modified program performed better than those who were not.
Based on the findings of Ho 共1999兲, Carpenter 共2007兲, and Hoffman and Zimbelman 共2009兲,
it would appear that the findings of generic group research may not always apply to BAR. The
good news for BAR is that it opens a wide variety of questions. The bad news is the ability to
secure access to professionals functioning in a group setting. In its early stages, research on
computer-aided group decision making may need to rely on field and archival data.
Role of Incentive Systems
As in other areas, the role of incentive systems has been very important in group research.
Management accounting group research recognizes the conflict between group incentives and

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 23

individual incentives. When the contribution of the individual is identifiable, an individual-based


incentive system prevents the free rider problem, where the individual makes a minimal contri-
bution to the group effort and secures a disproportionately large reward. However, when the only
observable measure is the group outcome, the manager is limited on what he/she can base the
payoff 共e.g., Drake et al. 1999兲.
Researchers have attempted to ascertain ways in which free rider behavior in groups might be
mitigated. Towry 共2003兲 argues that information on peers’ performance that is unknown to the
principal/manager is observable by group members. Thus, the group members are capable of
mutual monitoring. She reports that the greater the members’ group identity, the more effective
their ability to monitor each other’s behavior 共mutual monitoring兲 and the greater degree of
coordination they can achieve.
Rowe 共2004兲, in a study that examined both systems and incentives, takes a slightly different
approach to resolving the free rider problem. Rather than using monitoring, he used the “infor-
mation system” to inform the group’s members that free riding in his task actually was sub-
optimal behavior for both the free rider and the group. Rowe 共2004兲 modeled the free rider
problem as a public goods dilemma. Each member of a four-person group decided how much of
their endowment to contribute anonymously to a common pool. The amount in the pool was
tripled and divided equally among the group’s four members without regard to how much each
had contributed to the pool. Obviously, the self-interested strategy is to contribute nothing and
share whatever is in the tripled common pool. If all four members of the group follow a free rider
strategy, they would be no worse off than they were at the beginning of the experiment. At the
other extreme, if all members of the group contributed their entire endowment to the pool, every-
one would be significantly better off. Rowe 共2004兲 found that the group members contribute 共and
therefore receive兲 more when the information system informs an intact group of the benefits of
contributing. This occurs even though the information system did not provide any new information
and there is no communication among group members.

The Impact of Extra-Group Factors


While all of the above studies were laboratory-based, BAR also has examined the behavior of
real groups. This permits the researcher to observe the effect of the setting in which the decision
takes place. Rowe et al. 共2008兲 and Anderson et al. 共2002兲 used two different research methods to
study the decision processes of groups in their natural setting. Both papers study 共among other
things兲 group conflict, the sharing of horizontal asymmetric information, and the potential role of
consultants. Rowe et al. 共2008兲 report the results of a longitudinal, participant-observer field study
of a particular cross-functional group within a division of a firm. The group was formed by
management to try to reduce costs. Initially, each group member behaved in a self-interested
fashion to retain slack and benefit their particular function in the organization. The outcome of the
study shows that consultants, by redesigning the information system, were able to mitigate self-
interested behavior 共conflict兲 and replace it with more group-oriented behaviors. Rowe 共2004兲8
tested this finding in a laboratory setting.
Post-decision, Anderson et al. 共2002兲 used a survey supplemented with interviews of group
members within a single firm. They examined the effects of a large number of variables 共e.g.,
conflict resolution, group size, presence of consultants, importance of decision兲 on the complexity
and speed of adoption of the ABC system by a group. One of the most significant findings is that
complexity of the ABC system increased with group size. They did not include any data on the
group’s subsequent performance.

8
Despite the dates of publication, the field research was conducted before the experimental study.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
24 Birnberg

Summary
Group research is increasing in BAR. This reflects the increasing use of groups in practice
across areas of accounting. Moreover, because so much of the research is concerned with how
groups function, it would appear that BAR in one area of accounting is germane to others. In
general, research suggests that, consistent with non-BAR research, group members have greater
confidence in their decisions than individuals, there is less variability among the decisions by
groups than among individuals, and groups reach more conservative policy decisions. Because the
typical group study does not have a correct decision, the quality of their performance is not always
ascertainable. However, Bedard et al. 共1998兲 reported, in a study that did have a correct answer,
that groups perform better than individuals, but still may consider the proper action and reject it.
Greater insight into group decision processes is needed if BAR is to understand why groups
and individuals make different decisions. This would permit BAR to make positive recommenda-
tions about how groups should function rather than solely descriptive statements. Bedard et al.
共1998兲 showed one way this could be done: using audio 共or video兲 tape of the committee’s
deliberation. One question that emerged from Bedard et al. 共1998兲 is the critical nature of the
voting rule adopted by the group. Others 共Anderson et al. 2002兲 related selected group character-
istic 共e.g., group size and use of consultants兲 to the group’s recommendation. Group size led to
more complex systems, and the use of consultants facilitated reaching simpler decisions. Interest-
ingly, the papers in this area have not examined the role of factors such as the members’ ex ante
willingness to trust or cooperate on the group’s behavior and whether the group’s members are
volunteers or were assigned to the activity. The latter could be significant in the behavior of
real-world group membership.
While the initial research involved face-to-face groups, recent studies 共e.g., Kerr and Murthy
2004兲 have examined the role of information technology on group interaction. In general, CMC
resulted in more confidence and satisfaction than face-to-face communication. One can only
conjecture why this is true. A possible study would be to insert the same influential or forceful
person in each type of group 共CMC and face-to-face兲 and test to see if CMC moderates his/her
influence. It also is possible that utilizing CMC gives the group members a greater feeling of
involvement.
It would appear that group research can be conducted by and likely requires the use of a
variety of methods—laboratory, fieldwork, survey, and even protocol analysis. It also is reasonable
to assume that archival data may be useful for certain questions. Group research, like individual
research, has certain limitations. Using participants 共students or professionals兲 with no knowledge
of their history with the other members could affect the results. However, finding existing groups
that can be observed in the field means a loss of control, in addition to the expenditure of time on
the part of the researcher共s兲. Taking the same group members into a laboratory setting may not be
feasible. Creating ad hoc groups in the laboratory using student participants may miss some
important aspects of the group behavior and necessarily results in a less rich environment. Finally,
every “observation” typically requires at least four participants. This can restrict the number of
observations reported in a group study or create a need for a very large number of participants. All
of these considerations argue for the use of multiple methods to achieve convergent validity.
One type of group that has not received attention in BAR are groups that have an ongoing
existence within an organization. This would include committees and some teams.9 In these
groups, the members likely have a “history,” both good and bad, with each other. As we saw with

9
Team is used here in the sports sense of the word: a group where each member has a specific responsibility. The
governance literature that has typically been archival empirical can be done as a BAR study of the board and/or the audit
committee 共see Cohen et al. 2008b兲.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 25

dyads, familiarity affected interpersonal behavior 共Favere-Marchesi 2006兲. Many committees that
are intended to serve a particular function have continuing membership and have evolved rules or
group norms that the members follow in transacting the committee’s business. This type of group
presents a variety of issues, such as voting rules and coalition formation, that may have evolved
over time. For a discussion of this issue see Birnberg 共2004兲.
As was the case with the individual-focused BAR, group research has a linkage to other BAR.
In this instance, it is organization-focused research. The linkage, however, is less obvious. Earlier
organization-focused BAR was more macro in character. The typical variables used in contingency
research are related to the organization’s characteristics 共e.g., size, task uncertainty, and/or com-
plexity兲. More recent studies are organization process-oriented. This research is more concerned
with how a given organization decides rather than a single characteristic that varies across orga-
nizations. Thus, the elements of the network whose output is being studied are important. Group
characteristics can be part of the characteristics of the network that are studied.

ORGANIZATION FOCUS
This research originated with Hopwood 共1972兲, who argued that when BAR observes prob-
lems with the organization’s control system, the focus of BAR should be the “fit” between the
system and the organization rather than tinkering with the design of the system. He repeated this
concern in his invited presidential address at the AAA 2006 Annual Meeting 共Hopwood 2007兲. As
noted in the initial description of BAR focusing on organization, exactly which papers fall in this
category is not always obvious. Those BAR studies that present some difficulties in classification
examine how particular characteristics of the organization’s environment affect its accounting/
reporting systems, or they examine how those systems affect individuals or groups within the
organization 共e.g., contingency research兲. However, this problem of classification should be of
little concern to researchers. For an overview of the methods used, as well as research findings in
this focus, see Covaleski et al. 共1996兲, Anderson and Widener 共2007兲, and Ahrens and Chapman
共2007a, 2007b兲.
BAR in this category has utilized a variety of methods including field studies, surveys, and
archival studies. Field studies typically involve studying a single organization 共e.g., Hopwood
1972兲 or multiple units within an organization 共e.g., Otley 1980兲. Data typically are collected after
the event being researched has taken place via interviews, surveys, and observation of the orga-
nization’s activities. In these studies, researcher-collected data may be supplemented by archival
data from the unit being observed 共e.g., Anderson et al. 2002兲. Surveys in organization-focused
research often combine elements of field research and survey research. Interviews may precede the
survey and be used to help design it, or the interviews may follow the survey and be used to
clarify/amplify its findings. Archival BAR utilizes data collected by the organization共s兲 to examine
the effect of changes in systems or differences between systems. Ittner 共2007兲 discusses the
strengths and weaknesses of this approach. A few studies in BAR 共or cited in BAR兲 involve
real-time data collection utilizing participant observers, where the researcher is a part of the
activity 共e.g., Rowe et al. 2008兲.
As the various surveys of this literature indicate, the vast majority of these studies in BAR
involve management accounting in for-profit organizations 共Dillard and Becker 1997; Merchant
and Van der Stede 2006兲. However, a few studies cover a variety of not-for-profit organizations
共e.g., Covaleski and Dirsmith 1983兲, audit firms 共e.g., Dirsmith and Covaleski 1985兲, and govern-
mental units 共e.g., Boland and Pondy 1983; Ansari and Euske 1987兲. In financial accounting and
auditing, research on organizations typically is related to fraud. For example, Cohen et al. 共2008a兲
used archival news clippings to study the role of managers in firms where fraud was present.
Merchant and Van der Stede 共2006兲 reviewed the extent of field research in BAR. They
utilized what by their own admission was a restrictive definition of field research. To be classified

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
26 Birnberg

as a field study, Merchant and Van der Stede 共2006兲 required that there be intensive collection of
data in the field and extensive reporting of data in the paper. They reported that only one paper in
BRIA during the period they surveyed 共1981–2004兲 met their criteria, and the leading North
American journals 共TAR, JAR, CAR, JAE, Auditing, JMAR, and BRIA兲 published a total of 23 共8
percent兲 of the field studies. Ten of these were in JMAR and seven were in CAR 共Merchant and
Van der Stede 2006, Table 1兲. This finding is not surprising. Though there are a number of active
field researchers in North America, an examination of Merchant and Van der Stede’s 共2006兲 data
reveals a significant portion of the organization-focused BAR is performed by European and
Australian-New Zealand researchers, and it is published in European-based accounting journals.
AOS and Management Accounting Research 共U.K.兲 together published 215 of the 318 共68 percent兲
field studies 共Merchant and Van der Stede 2006, Table 1兲. Obviously, these data underestimate the
level of field research in North American BAR because many of the papers published in European
journals were by North American-based researchers.
What distinguishes the organization-focused research from the research on individuals and
groups is the relative insignificance of the person共s兲 in the papers. As the breadth of vision
expands, the ability to focus on the more micro aspects decreases. The study by Anderson et al.
共2002兲 illustrates this issue. They count the number of persons composing a team and ask how
group size affects the organization’s ability to achieve a desirable outcome, but do not examine the
behavior of the individuals who composed the team.
The types of issues investigated in organization-oriented BAR are quite varied. A simple
summary includes the following:
• The effect of the “task” on the appropriate accounting/reporting system 共i.e., contingency
research兲.
• The effect of task and goal uncertainty on the nature of accounting.
• The effect of internal and external forces on innovations/changes in the accounting/
reporting system.
• The importance of accounting as compared to other metrics in the organization.
• The role of various nontask characteristics of the organization on the organization’s ac-
counting and/or strategy.
Contingency Studies
Contingency research has a long history in BAR. The earliest studies drew on the work of
Burns and Stalker 共1961兲 and typically revolved around task characteristics such as task uncer-
tainty 共e.g., Hirst 1983; Gordon and Narayanan 1984兲. This line of research raises the issue of the
“fit” between the task’s/organization’s characteristics and the appropriate accounting system.
Firms that adopt the appropriate accounting system given the organization’s characteristics, all
else equal, should perform better than those that do not have the appropriate fit between charac-
teristics and system. Despite the intuitive appeal of this line of research, it has not been as popular
as one might expect because of the difficulty in finding appropriate data and research design
共Otley 1980兲. Fisher 共1995, 1998兲 and Chenhall 共2003兲 reviewed the literature in this area. From
their reviews it is apparent that organizations, like individuals, are diverse and differ on a variety
of dimensions. Each of these dimensions becomes a basis for research on differences in account-
ing information and control systems.
A few studies examine the effect of individuals within the organization’s hierarchy on the
form of control exercised. Hopwood 共1972, 1974兲 examines the role of management style and
other organization variables on the way the data of the accounting system are utilized. While
Hopwood’s research would appear to reflect a study of the individual, he reports that the nature of
the organization’s task affected the management style, rather than the selection of a management
style being a “free choice” 共see also Otley 1978兲.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 27

Closely related to these “traditional” contingency studies is the work of Simons 共1987, 1990兲.
Simons studied the fit between the strategy of the organization and the form of its control system.
He argues that there is a need for a fit between strategy and control system. Innovative strategies
require controls that permit the manager to make critical decisions 共i.e., innovate兲 if they are to
operate effectively. In a recent study on this issue using a variety of data collection methods,
Kober et al. 共2003兲 argue, unlike Simons, that the link between strategy and control systems over
time works in both directions. Not only does the organization’s strategy affect the form of the
control system, but the control system may alert management to the need for a change in strategy.
This idea is consistent with Hedberg and Jonsson’s 共1978兲 conception of the role of the informa-
tion system.
The research of Simons 共1987兲 and Kober et al. 共2003兲 illustrates an important method issue
in organization-focused research; that is, a cross-sectional versus longitudinal approach to organi-
zational research. Simons’ 共1987兲 work is static. He examines multiple organizations at a given
moment in time using cross-sectional analysis. Ideally, the firms in this type of study are assumed
to be in equilibrium, so there already is an appropriate fit between strategy and control systems. In
contrast, Kober et al. 共2003兲 did a longitudinal study of a single organization. This permitted an
examination of changes in the nature of the organization’s control system over time. Note that the
answer to the contingency question of fit between strategy and control system does not change.
However, the longitudinal study provides a richer picture of how the fit is achieved, maintained,
and ultimately may again change when the firm’s environment changes. In a study similar to
Kober et al. 共2003兲, Cardinal et al. 共2004兲 describe the evolution of an organization’s control
system over a longer period. Hopwood 共1987兲 used historical research methods to obtain insight
into how Josiah Wedgwood adapted the activities of Wedgwood Potteries to fit changing economic
conditions. For a discussion of the use of historical accounting research, see Luft 共2007兲.
A set of studies that relate incentives to the nature of the task would appear to potentially
relate to both contingency research in organizations and individual-focused BAR. These studies
examine at the organization level the same issue as Chow 共1983兲 and others studied at the
individual decision level. The original behavioral paper on this question by Ouchi and Maguire
共1975兲 clearly was an attempt to examine the fit between incentive schemes used in particular task
settings and the prescriptions of agency theory. For example, do firms in which the outcome of the
workers’ task is measurable, but effort is not, reward the worker on performance as agency theory
suggests? Their findings and later work, such as Eisenhardt 共1989兲, support the broad outlines of
agency theory. A more organization-focused study examines the new 共nonfinancial兲 measures
developed by the organization to increase productivity. Banker et al. 共2000b兲 took advantage of a
naturally occurring experiment and used archival data from a hotel firm to assess the efficacy of a
new set of procedures intended to enhance profitability by increasing customer satisfaction and
their return visits. Banker et al. 共2000a兲 used archival data to examine the effectiveness of an
organization’s decision to alter its incentive scheme. Both of these studies are concerned with the
organization’s policies and the policies’ effects on the organization’s performance rather than the
policies’ effects on the individuals who compose the organization. This approach is similar to
studying the behavior of markets as opposed to the actions of the individuals participating in the
market.
Burchell et al. 共1980兲 extended the nature of the contingencies analyzed. They argued that the
role of the accounting system varied not only based on the characteristics of the task, but also with
the extent of agreement present among the parties on their goals. Burchell et al. 共1980兲 argue
against the role of accounting as a neutral technical system and a source of objective data for
decision making. Rather, they argue both the knowledge of the task and agreement over goals in
many cases are problematic. Thus, the outputs of the accounting system may be seen differently by
the parties involved and can serve a variety of purposes depending on the situation. For example,

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
28 Birnberg

they argue that the accounting data may be the inputs into “argument machines” 共high knowledge
and low goal agreement兲 or “learning machines” 共low knowledge and high goal agreement兲.
Different settings place different stresses on the accounting system. Miller and O’Leary 共1987兲
discuss how these forces can work.

System Innovation
Periodically, accountants have studied how innovations in the firm’s accounting system occur.
There are two broad types of studies. One attempts to ascertain the characteristics of firms adopt-
ing the accounting innovation. Others are concerned with ascertaining the characteristics of a
successful innovation.
Few studies attempt to understand why innovations such as ABC or balanced scorecard are
adopted by some organizations and not others 共e.g., Chenhall 2003兲. Given the range of organi-
zations required to study the diffusion of an innovation, it is not surprising that the data are
collected through the use of surveys. Gosselin 共1997兲 studies the effect of the organization’s
strategy and structure on the adoption of ABC. Obviously, the economic environment also is
relevant. It is only conjecture, but one might argue that the advent of the recent changes in
management and accounting 共e.g., ABC, value chain analysis, balanced scorecard兲 resulted from
the presence of external pressure on the organization in the form of significantly increased 共for-
eign兲 competition. How desirable would organizations have found the adoption of ABC in the
absence of competition from Japan? Would organizations have adopted it if their profits had been
high?
It also is important to study why innovations such as ABC and Balanced Scorecard do not
succeed. Brunsson 共1990兲 suggests one explanation. He argues that there is a difference between
choice of a system 共decision making兲 and gaining approval from the appropriate members of
management to implement that system. The latter, Brunsson 共1990兲 argues, is a political decision
reflecting the diverse goals and views of the organization’s members. His argument is in the spirit
of Burchell et al. 共1980兲 and argues that decisions reflect diverse goals and power.

Role of Accounting in the Organization


One of the most interesting lines of organization-focused research discusses the relative im-
portance of accounting data in the organization. As accountants, we may believe that accounting
data are the basis for decision making in organizations. But there is evidence that accounting is not
always the primary basis for decision making. Lawrenson 共1992兲 researched decision making in
British Rail when engineers occupied the dominant role in the organization. Technical 共engineer-
ing兲 data were of primary importance and were supplemented by accounting data. This changed
when the Thatcher government proposed privatizing British Rail. It then was important that the
organization be able to show a profit. Accounting data became of paramount importance in deci-
sion making 共Dent 1991兲, and engineering data were relegated to a secondary role.
Researchers have studied why accounting achieved a particular role in the organization look-
ing at other factors. It would appear that the social environment within which accounting exists
can affect its role in the organization 共Bougen 1989兲. Ansari and Euske 共1987兲 reported that
intra-organization conflict affected the extent to which a new system was implemented by repair
depots. While the home office 共DOD兲 thought it was implemented, the repair depot continued to
manage its activities with the old system. Berry et al. 共1985兲 report a similar finding within
England’s National Coal Board. At the operating level, the accounting system focused on costs and
output. At the district and central management level, the accounting system was much more
sophisticated and focused on planning and control. The two systems appear to have coexisted
because they met the differing needs of the different user groups. Overall, it would appear that
forces within the organization influence the preference for and use of accounting data.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 29

As noted earlier, the boundaries for organization-focused research are not without contro-
versy. Others 共e.g., Dillard and Becker 1997兲 have a broader view of what would be included.
Many of the studies they would include in this section relate to how accounting serves broader
societal issues. The emphasis of these studies, however, is better understood in the context of
accounting and its societal context. Thus, I have chosen to consider their focus, which is on
accounting and the external environment, in the next section.

Summary
Organization-focused BAR asks how the characteristics of the organization affect the account-
ing system. These characteristics have been very broadly defined. At their simplest, the character-
istics refer to the nature of the firm. A more complex view relates to how accounting and/or
organizational innovations impact the nature of the organization’s adoption of a particular account-
ing system. At the other extreme are those studies that are intended to understand the relative role
of accounting in the organization. Is it the metric used in making decisions in the organization or
only one of the metrics used? What factors influence the answer to the previous question? All in
all, it reflects a broad literature that is most concerned with management accounting and for-profit
organizations.
The research reviewed in this section continues to reflect the richness of methods and disci-
plines utilized in BAR. The methods used reflect the same wide array found in the other research
field: field surveys combined with interviews, archival data, and naturally occurring experiments.
The data are both qualitative and quantitative and the research may investigate anywhere from a
single unit within a firm to multiple organizations.
The most striking aspect of the organization-focused BAR is the disciplines on which the
researchers draw. They tend to be a different set from those found in the other sections reviewed.
Individual, dyad, and group studies draw primarily from psychology and economics and, to a
much lesser degree, on sociology for both theories and method. In organization-focused BAR, it is
exactly the opposite. Organization-focused BAR draws more on organization theory and sociol-
ogy. Data often are qualitative, leading the researchers to rely heavily on their interpretation of the
data and to “draw conclusions” rather than present the results of statistical tests with their apparent
objectivity.
It would be a mistake for researchers who do not do organization-focused BAR to ignore it.
Not only are the findings relevant to those who wish to understand how accounting functions
within organizations, but this research often serves as the basis for more controlled studies that are
narrower in their focus. Conclusions drawn from small samples or from qualitative data may
provide laboratory researchers with an issue that merits further inquiry. In this regard, see Rowe
共2004兲 and Rowe et al. 共2008兲, in which case the latter informed and motivated the former. There
also has been a link between organizational culture and individual 共ethical兲 behavior 共Windsor and
Ashkanasy 1996兲. Indeed, the role of an organization’s culture could affect and/or reinforce other
aspects of individual behavior 共e.g., trust, honesty兲.

ENVIRONMENTAL CONDITIONS
The BAR included in this focus is concerned with the interaction between society and ac-
counting and vice versa. BAR of this type examines how the environment 共i.e., the context兲 in
which the organization exists affects accounting and how the resulting accounting affects the
members of the organization. In that sense, it is a study of the context within which accounting
comes into being and how the environment affects the development of the resulting systems. It

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
30 Birnberg

also studies how accounting can affect the environment in which the organization carries on its
activities.10
Research in this area draws on a different set of nonaccounting research than the studies
discussed in the previous sections, including a variety of sociological and philosophical theories
and theorists 共see Cooper and Hopper 2007兲. The research may be perceived as more diverse than
other foci, but this reflects the difference in theory base rather than issues. The general topics
include the following:
• The way environment and/or environmental events have influenced accounting in the firm.
• The manner in which accounting has been used to “control” or manipulate firm members
or society.
• The impact of the firm’s accounting on the firm’s external environment.
• The simulation of macro policies that do not involve accounting choices, including studies
of market mechanisms.
The first two topics are the most closely related conceptually. The environmental forces affect
the organization’s environment and the nature of the accounting that develops within the organi-
zation. The particular accounting system that results from the environmental forces may have as
its intent specific effects on the behavior of the organization’s members. The latter two research
strands are quite distinct. The third type of research includes both examples of how accounting is
used to affect the organization’s external environment as well as unintended effects of accounting
on the environment. For a review of this research, see Baxter and Chua 共2003兲 and Cooper and
Hopper 共2007兲. Simulation through BAR studies of proposed policy changes examines the manner
in which accounting policy can affect policy issues or is linked to such issues.
An element of the environment, national culture, by definition is a part of this focus. Nearly
all BAR on the effects of national culture are related to the behavior of individuals 共research
discussed under the individual focus兲. An exception is Fligstein 共1998兲. He discusses the role of
national culture on the importance of quantification. He contrasts France and the U.S., where the
latter’s culture stresses measurement to a far greater extent than the former’s culture does. His
arguments suggest the preference for rule-based versus principle-based accounting is rooted in the
national culture of the two countries. It might also explain the different foci of North American
and European BAR.

Power and Conflict


A central theme of much of BAR in this area is the attempt by individuals or groups to exert
power over others and the resulting conflict. The argument, as advanced by Cooper and Hopper
共2007兲 in their review of the critical theory literature, is that one cannot fully understand the
accounting system within an organization without examining the social, political, and economic
context that produced it. Moreover, once in place, they argue that the system has implications
beyond its mechanistic function. It affects the behaviors within the organization beyond those
usually ascribed to the system.
A significant portion of contemporary BAR in this area draws on the work of Foucault 共1977兲
on power and control. To those researchers, accounting is not 共only兲 a means of measuring and
reporting outcomes, but more importantly is a means of exerting control over other “units” 共e.g.,
Miller and O’Leary 1987兲. Thus, it is concerned with the process through which the change occurs

10
For purposes of classification, the question is the relative degree to which the research is concerned with the external
environment and the role of accounting in the organization. Dent’s 共1991兲 paper easily could have been considered in
this section. Its inclusion in the organizations section reflected its relationship to the other papers 共e.g., Lawrenson 1992兲
discussed there.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 31

and the relative power of the parties who cause it to occur. This, in turn, means that the studies
tend to reflect events occurring within a single “organization.” The organization typically is a firm,
such as Caterpillar 共Miller and O’Leary 1997兲. However, others have studied broader organiza-
tions such as the 共English兲 National Coal Board 共Berry et al. 1985兲, not-for-profits 共Covaleski and
Dirsmith 1983兲, and standard-setting bodies 共Durocher et al. 2007兲.
This research has been more active in the study of public management compared to other
areas of BAR discussed here. See Cooper and Hopper 共2007, 222–228兲 for a review. The central
theme to these papers, regardless of their particular context, is the need to understand accounting
systems as the product of forces both internal and external to the organization. They reject the
notion that changes in systems always result from rational decision making. This can be inter-
preted by extending the approach of Burchell et al. 共1980兲 to an inter-organization context. When
groups in the environment have different goals, accounting is not perceived as objective by the
parties. Rather, they are selective in the accounting data they use to defend/justify their position or
attack their opponent’s position. While this process often is explicit and observable 共e.g., changes
in specific sections of the tax code兲, more typically it is not. To understand why an accounting/
measurement system is in place and how it came to be, we must analyze the events that preceded
its adoption. While the questions, indeed, are behavioral, they may lead us to using historical
research techniques 共see Luft 2007 or Jones and Mellett 2007兲. An example of this approach is
Jeacle and Walsh 共2002兲, who studied the evolution of credit analysis and the shift in responsi-
bility 共“power”兲 that accompanied the changes.
Another interesting example of this research is Durocher et al. 共2007兲. They review the
literature discussing various groups’ attempts to influence the financial accounting standard-setting
process. They develop an explanatory theory to describe whether a user group attempts to influ-
ence the process. It reflects the fact that, in studying the behavior in the “environment,” many of
the same issues discussed under other foci of analysis in this paper also exist in this context. For
example, Durocher et al. 共2007兲 utilize models similar to those found in BAR relating to the
motivation of the individual to explain the behavior of the groups involved. It also utilizes themes
of power and legitimacy found in this paper under organizations.
While much of this literature, almost by definition, is a series of unrelated case studies, there
is a degree of overlap. This overlap, in turn, provides the basis for more general conclusions. For
example, in the final phase of their study of U.K. health service, Jones and Mellett 共2007兲 discuss
the same “environmental force” 共Thatcher’s desire to privatize governmental services兲 as Dent
共1991兲 does for British Rail.

How Accounting Affects the Organization’s Environment


There is much less BAR in this area. One type could be labeled an “unintended consequence”
of the accounting system. These are changes that occur externally to the organization as the result
of an organization’s accounting innovation, but were not the intent of the innovation. An example
of such a consequence is the evolution of standard sizes in clothing. Jeacle 共2003a兲 argues that the
change occurred as the result of standardizing production and the adoption of standard costs in the
clothing industry. She also described the role of standardization on other industries 共Jeacle 2003b,
2005兲. While many other examples such as this may exist, relatively few have been researched.
Organizations also consciously use accounting to influence their environments. One example
is Chwastiak 共2001兲. She summarizes an interesting series of papers describing how measurement
systems were used by the Department of Defense 共DOD兲 to legitimize the Vietnam war effort and
the related expenditures. She argues that, because of pressure from external groups, accounting
systems were used to provide an aura of efficiency that was not truly present. Her central argument
across these papers is that the DOD responded to the external pressure groups by selecting an
accounting system that provided the appearance of efficiency and effectiveness that, in reality,

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
32 Birnberg

was not present. The intent was to provide a rationale for expanded military budgets and is
consistent with what Fligstein 共1998兲 would have labeled the U.S. national culture.
It is not unusual for organizations to use press releases and other disclosures to present
information supporting a particular point of view. Tinker and Neimark 共1987兲, for example, show
how an organization, General Motors, used accounting 共their annual reports from 1917 to 1976兲 to
present their position on the social issues of gender and class.

Simulation of Policies and Market Mechanisms


BAR on the effect of new accounting policies was discussed under individuals because the
research question concerned how the individual would respond to an accounting change, be it
internal 共e.g., the balanced scorecard兲 or external 共e.g., partial consolidations兲. These studies
clearly are accounting research.
Behavioral research conducted by accounting researchers concerned with economic policy
and market mechanisms clearly lies within the domain of behavioral research and experimental
economics. The issue is whether being performed by accounting researchers qualifies them as
behavioral accounting research. This line of study is included, albeit briefly, to indicate the scope
of the behavioral research being done by accountants.
While this research primarily deals with generic topics, it is possible for studies using experi-
mental markets to examine issues clearly with the BAR domain. A study of this type can examine
the role of auditing and auditors in the behavior of the market for assets. While this research
typically has been done using archival markets data, experimental markets provide the researcher
with the opportunity to observe conditions that currently do not exist, may not exist to a sufficient
degree, or where the required data are not available. An example of this is Ackert et al. 共2001b兲,
who studied the effect of uncertain litigation cost on seller behavior.
The research done on market mechanisms 共i.e., various types of auctions兲 is most relevant to
the study of markets in experimental economics. For a discussion of the nature of this research, see
Sunder 共1995兲. The papers are unlikely to appear in accounting journals. Their findings may have
relevance to markets within the organization. However, thus far there is no research in this area.
Other research has focused on how particular characteristics of a market such as information
共Ganguly et al. 1994兲, the use of “circuit breakers” in a market 共Ackert et al. 2001a兲, and bubbles
共Ackert et al. 2003兲 affect the outcomes. Dopuch et al. 共1989兲 discuss the role of experimental
markets in auditing research.

Summary
Perhaps the most significant contribution of the BAR reviewed in this section is not the
specific findings. Rather, it is its central theme: accounting does not exist in a vacuum, and factors
and forces external to the organization affect the accounting system and, in turn, the members of
that organization. It is equally, though less obviously, the case that changes in the organization’s
accounting system can impact the external environment. As the result, this research can differ
significantly from BAR discussed in the three previous sections. This BAR often involves a
longitudinal 共historical兲 study of an organization and examines the role that relative power plays in
the development of the organization.
Environment-focused research draws on a different research knowledge base than that which
predominates in the other areas. It draws much more heavily on sociology, anthropology, and
critical theory research in other research domains. This tends to give the impression that this BAR
is distinct from and unrelated to the other research foci. In reality, it can be viewed as providing
the context within which the other research, particularly organization-focused research, must be

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 33

viewed. This does not mean that it is an input to all BAR. However, just as organization-focused
research may inform group- and individual-focused BAR, this research may inform organization-
and group-focused research.
The use of experimental markets to study macro behavior of investors under particular con-
ditions appears to be an emerging area 共see Moser 1998兲. As such, it represents the obverse to the
individual-focused research on investor behavior. This research may alter the balance of financial
accounting BAR from the behavior of the individual to the manner in which the sum of these
behaviors work their way through the market mechanism just as archival financial accounting
moved the focus of accounting research from the individual effects to market effects.

CONCLUSION
Each section of this paper ended with a summary intended to reflect the role and direction of
research in that area. Thus, the conclusions that can be drawn reflect the overall trends in these
diverse areas. In general, it would appear in the 20 or so years since Birnberg and Shields 共1989兲,
BAR has continued to flourish. All of the foci used to organize the research in that paper have
continued to develop.

REFERENCES
Ackert, L., B. Church, and R. Deaves. 2003. Emotion and financial markets. Federal Reserve Bank of Atlanta
Economic Review 88 共2兲: 1–9.
——–, ——–, and N. Jayaraman. 200la. An experimental study of circuit breakers: The effects of mandated
closures and temporary halts on market behavior. Journal of Financial Markets: 4 共2兲: 185–208.
——–, ——–, and P. Zhang. 2001b. Uncertain litigation cost and seller behavior: Evidence from an audit
game. Advances in Accounting 18: 77–104.
Ahrens, T. 1996. Styles of accountability. Accounting, Organizations and Society 21 共2兲: 139–173.
——–, and C. Chapman. 2007a. Theorizing practice in management accounting research. In Handbook of
Management Accounting Research, Vol. 1, edited by Chapman, C., A. Hopwood, and M. Shields,
99–112. Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.†
——–, and ——–. 2007b. Management accounting in practice. Accounting, Organizations and Society 32
共1/2兲: 1–26.†
Allee, K., N. Bhattacharya, E. Black, and T. Christensen. 2007. Pro forma disclosure and investor sophisti-
cation: External validation of experimental evidence using archival data. Accounting, Organizations
and Society 32 共3兲: 201–220.
Amer, T. S. 2005. Bias due to visual illusion in the graphical presentation of information. Journal of
Information Systems 19 共1兲: 1–18.
Anderson, S., J. Hesford, and S. M. Young. 2002. Factors influencing the performance of activity based
costing teams: A field study of ABC model development time in the automobile industry. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 27 共3兲: 195–212.
——–, and S. Weidner. 2007. Doing quantitative field research in management accounting. In Handbook of
Management Accounting Research, Vol. 1, edited by Chapman, C., A. Hopwood, and M. Shields,
319–342. Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.†
Andreoni, J. 1990. Impure altruism and donations to public goods: A theory of warm glow giving. The
Economic Journal 100 共June兲: 464–177.
Ansari, S., and K. Euske. 1987. Rational, rationalizing, and reifying uses of accounting data in organizations.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 12 共6兲: 549–570.
Appiah, K. A. 2007. The new philosophy. The New York Times Magazine, Section 6 共December 9兲: 34, 36.
Argyris, C. 1952. The Impact of Budgets on People. New York, NY: The Controllership Foundation.
Arnold, V., D. Clark, N. Collier, S. Leech, and S. Sutton. 2006. The differential effect of knowledge-based
system explanations in novice and expert judgment decisions. Management Information Systems Quar-
terly 30 共1兲: 79–97.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
34 Birnberg

Arnold, D., R. Bernardi, P. Neidermeyer, and J. Schmee. 2007. The effect of country and culture on percep-
tions of appropriate ethical actions prescribed by codes of conduct: A Western European perspective
among accountants. Journal of Business Ethics 70 共4兲: 327–340.
Bailey, W., and A. Spicer. 2007. When does national identity matter? Convergence and divergence in
international business ethics. Academy of Management Journal 50 共6兲: 1462–1480.
Banker, R., S. Lee, and G. Potter. 2000a. A field study on the impact of a performance-based incentive plan.
Journal of Accounting and Economics 30: 315–350.
——–, G. Porter, and D. Srinivasan. 2000b. An empirical investigation of an incentive plan that includes
nonfinancial measures. The Accounting Review 75 共1兲: 65–92.
Barberis, N., and R. Thaler. 2003. A survey of behavioral finance. In Handbook of Economics of Finance,
edited by Constantinides, G., M. Harris, and R. M. Stultz, 1053–1123. Amsterdam, The Netherlands:
Elsevier.†
Barnea, A., H. Conqvist, and S. Siegel. 2009. Nature or nurture: What determines investor behavior? Avail-
able at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1467088&CFID⫽15357410&CFTO KEN
⫽39092446.†
Barton, J. 2005. Who cares about auditor reputation? Contemporary Accounting Research 22 共3兲: 549–586.
Baskerville, W. 2003. Hofstede never studied culture. Accounting, Organizations and Society 28 共1兲: 1–14.
Baxter, A., and W. F. Chua. 2003. Alternative management accounting research—Whence and wither. Ac-
counting, Organizations and Society 28 共2/3兲: 97–126.†
Becker, S., and D. Green. 1962. Budgeting and employee behavior. The Journal of Business 35: 392–402.
Beckmann, D., and L. Menkhoff. 2008. Will women be women? Analyzing the gender differences among
financial experts. Kyklos 61 共3兲: 364–384.
Bedard, J. C., and S. Biggs. 1991. Pattern recognition, hypothesis generation, and auditor performance in an
analytical task. The Accounting Review 66 共33兲: 622–642.
——–, ——–, and J. Maroney. 1998. Source of process gain and loss from group interactions in performance
of analytical procedures. Behavioral Research in Accounting 10 共Supplement兲: 207–233.
Berg, J., J. Dickhaut, and K. McCabe. 1995. Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic
Behavior 10: 122–142.
Berry, A., T. Capps, P. Ferguson, T. Hopper, and A. Lowe. 1985. Management accounting in an area of the
NCB: Rationales of accounting practices in a public enterprise. Accounting, Organizations and Society
10 共1兲: 3–28.
Birnberg, J. 2004. Expanding our horizon: Management accounting in the next decade. Advances in Man-
agement Accounting 13: 1–27.
——–, V. Hoffman, and S. Yuen. 2008. The accountability demand for information in China and the U.S.—A
research note. Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 共1兲: 20–32.
——–, L. Pondy, and C. L. Davis. 1970. An experimental study in decision-making. Management Science 16
共6兲: B356–B372.
——–, and J. Shields. 1989. Thirty years of behavioral research. Behavioral Research in Accounting 1:
23–74.
——–, and C. Snodgrass. 1988. Culture and control: A field study. Accounting, Organizations and Society 13
共5兲: 447–464.
Bohnet, I., and R. Zeckhauser. 2004. Trust, risk, and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization
55 共4兲: 467–484.
Boland, R., and L. Pondy. 1983. Accounting in organizations: A union of the rational and natural perspec-
tives. Accounting, Organizations and Society 8 共2/3兲: 223–234.
Bonner, S., G. Sprinkle, and S. M. Young. 2000. A review of the effects of financial incentives on perfor-
mance in laboratory tasks: Implications for management accounting. Journal of Management Account-
ing Research 12: 19–64.†
Booker, A., A. Drake, and D. Heitiger. 2007. New product development: How cost information affects
designer focus and behavior in a multiple objective setting. Behavioral Research in Accounting 19:
19–41.
Booth, P., and A. Schulz. 2004. The impact of an ethical environment on managers’ project evaluation

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 35

judgments under agency problem conditions. Accounting, Organizations and Society 29 共7/8兲: 473–
488.
Borthick, A. F., M. Curtis, and R. Sriram. 2006. Accelerating the acquisition of knowledge structure to
improve performance in internal control reviews. Accounting, Organizations and Society 31 共4/5兲:
323–342.
Bougen, P. 1989. The emergence, roles and consequences of an accounting-industrial relations interaction.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 14 共2兲: 203–234.
Bowles, H., L. Babcock, and L. Lai. 2007. Social incentives for gender differences in the propensity to
initiate negotiations: Sometimes it does hurt to ask. Organizational Performance and Human Decision
Processes 103 共1兲: 84–103.
Brunsson, N. 1990. Deciding for responsibility and legitimation: Alternative interpretations of organizational
decision-making. Accounting, Organizations and Society 15 共1/2兲: 47–59.
Burchell, S., C. Clubb, A. Hopwood, J. Hughes, and J. Nahapiet. 1980. The roles of accounting in organi-
zations and society. Accounting, Organizations and Society 5 共1兲: 5–28.
Burks, S., J. Carpenter, L. Geotte, and A. Rustichini. 2008. Cognitive skills explain economic preferences,
strategic behavior, and job attachment. IZA Discussion Paper No. 3609. Available at: http://
papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1184016&CFID⫽15357410&CFTO KEN⫽39092446.
Burns, T., and G. Stalker. 1961. The Management of Innovation. London, U.K.: Tavistock.
Calegari, M., J. Schatzberg, and G. Sevcik. 1998. Experimental evidence of differential auditor pricing and
reporting strategies. The Accounting Review 73 共2兲: 255–275.
Callahan, C., E. A. Gabriel, and E. Sainty. 2006. A review and classification of experimental economics
research in accounting. Journal of Accounting Literature 25: 59–126.†
Camerer, C. 2001. Behavioral Economics. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.†
——–, G. Lowenstein, and D. Prelec. 2005. Neuroeconomics: How neuroscience can inform economics.
Journal of Economic Literature 43 共1兲: 9–64.
Cardinal, L., S. Sitkin, and C. Long. 2004. Balancing and rebalancing in the creation and evolution of
organizational control. Organization Science 15 共4兲: 411–431.
Carpenter, T. 2007. Audit team brainstorming, fraud risk identification, and fraud risk assessment: Implica-
tions of SAS 99. The Accounting Review 82 共5兲: 1119–1140.
Casadesus-Masanell, R. 2004. Trust in agency. Journal of Economics & Management Strategy 13 共3兲:
375–404.
Chalos, P., and M. Poon. 2000. Participation and performance in capital budgeting teams. Behavioral Re-
search in Accounting 12: 199–230.
——–, and S. Haka. 1990. Transfer pricing under bilateral bargaining. The Accounting Review 65: 624–641.
Chenhall, R. 2003. Management control system design within an organizational context: Findings from
contingency-based research and directions for the future. Accounting, Organizations and Society 28
共2/3兲: 127–168.†
Chin, C-L., and H-Y. Chi. 2008. Gender differences in audit quality. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/
papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1149405.
Chow, C. 1983. The effects of job standard tightness and incentives on performance: An exploration of
linkages. The Accounting Review 58 共October兲: 667–685.
——–, M. Shields, and A. Wu. 1999. The importance of national culture in the design of and preference for
management controls for multinational operations. Accounting, Organizations and Society 16 共1兲:
47–60.†
Chung, J., J. Cohen, and G. Monroe. 2008. The effect of moods on auditors’ inventory valuation. Auditing:
A Journal of Practice & Theory 27 共2兲: 137–160.
Chwastiak, M. 2001. Taming the untamable: Planning, programming, and budgeting and the normalization of
war. Accounting, Organizations and Society 26: 501–526.†
Clinton, B., and J. Hunton. 2001. Linking participative budgeting congruence to organizational performance.
Behavioral Research in Accounting 13: 127–141.
Cohen, J., Y. Ding, C. Lesage, and H. Stolowy. 2008a. The role of managers’ behavior in corporate fraud.
Working paper, HEC Paris.
——–, L. Gaynor, G. Krishnamoorthy, and A. Wright. 2008b. Academic research on communication among

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
36 Birnberg

external auditors, the audit committee and the board: Implications and recommendations for practice.
Current Issues in Auditing 2 共1兲: A1–A8.
——–, L. Pant, and D. Sharp. 1996. Measuring the ethical awareness and ethical orientation of Canadian
auditors. Behavioral Research in Accounting 8 共Supplement兲: 98–119.†
——–, L. Holder-Webb, D. Sharp, and L. Pant. 2007. The effects of perceived fairness on opportunistic
behavior. Contemporary Accounting Research 24 共4兲: 1119–1153.
Coletti, A., K. Sedatole, and K. Towry. 2005. The effect of control systems on trust and cooperation in
collaborative environments. The Accounting Review 80: 477–500.
Cooper, D., and T. Hopper. 2007. Critical theorizing in management accounting research. In Handbook of
Management Accounting Research, Vol. 1, edited by Chapman, C., A. Hopwood, and M. Shields,
207–246. Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.†
Covaleski, M., and M. Dirsmith. 1983. Budgeting as a means for control and loose coupling. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 8 共4兲: 323–340.
——–, ——–, and S. Samuel. 1996. Managerial accounting research: The contributions of organizational and
sociological theories. Journal of Management Accounting Research 8: 1–35.†
Cruz, C., W. Shafer, and J. Strawser. 2000. A multidimensional analysis of tax practitioners’ ethical judg-
ments. Journal of Business Ethics 24 共3兲: 223–244.
Dalton, M. 1959. Men Who Manage: Fusion of Theory and Practice in Management. New York, NY: Wiley.
Daroca, F. 1984. Informational influences on group decision making in a participative context. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 9 共1兲: 13–32.
Dawes, C., J. Fowler, T. Johnson, R. McElreath, and O. Smirov. 2007. Egalitarian motives in humans. Nature
446 共April兲: 794–796.
Dawes, R., and R. Thaler. 1988. Anomalies: Cooperation. The Journal of Economic Perspectives 2 共3兲:
187–197.
Dearman, D., and M. Shields. 2005. Avoiding accounting fixation: Determinants of cognitive adaptation to
differences in accounting method. Contemporary Accounting Research 22: 351–384.
DeJong, D., R. Forsythe, J-O. Kim, and W. Uecker. 1989. A laboratory investigation of alternative transfer
pricing mechanisms. Accounting, Organizations and Society 14 共1/2兲: 41–64.
Delgado, M., L. Nystrom, C. Fissell, D. Noll, and J. Fiez. 2000. Tracking hemodynamic responses to reward
and punishment in the striatum. Journal of Neurophysiology 84: 3072–3072.
Dennis, A., and J. Valacich. 1993. Computer brainstorms: Two heads are better than one. The Journal of
Applied Psychology 78 共4兲: 531–537.
Dent, J. 1991. Accounting and organizational cultures: A field study of the emergence of a new organizational
reality. Accounting, Organizations and Society 16: 705–732.
DeZoort, F. T., D. Hermanson, and R. Houston. 2008. Audit committee members support of proposed
adjustments: Pre-SOX versus post-SOX. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory 27 共1兲: 85–104.
Dickhaut, J. 2009. The brain as the original accounting institution. The Accounting Review 84: 1703–1712.
——–, S. Basu, K. McCabe, and G. Waymire. 2009a. Neuroaccounting, part I: The primate brain and
reciprocal exchange. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstractid⫽1336526.
——–, ——–, ——–, and ——–. 2009b. Neuroaccounting, part II: Consilience between accounting principals
and the primate brain. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1336517&rec
⫽1&srcabs⫽1336526.
——–, K. McCabe, J. Nagode, A. Rustichini, K. Smith, and J. Pardo. 2003. The impact of the certainty
context on the process of choice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 100: 3536–3541.
Dickson, G., J. Senn, and N. Chervaney. 1977. Research in management information systems: The Minnesota
experiments. Management Science 23 共9兲: 913–934.†
Dillard, J., and D. Becker. 1997. Organizational sociology and accounting research or understanding account-
ing in organizations using sociology. In Behavioral Accounting Research: Foundations and Frontiers,
edited by Arnold, V., and S. Sutton, 247–274. Sarasota, FL: AAA.
Dirsmith, M., and M. Covaleski. 1985. Practice management issues in public accounting firms. Journal of
Accounting, Auditing and Finance 9: 5–21.†
Dopuch, N. 1992. Another view on the use of deception in auditing experiments. Auditing: A Journal of
Practice and Theory 11 共Fall兲: 100–112.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 37

——–, R. King, and D. Wallin. 1989. The use of experimental markets in auditing research: Some initial
findings. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 8 共Supplement兲: 98–127.
Doupnik, T. 2008. Influence of culture on earnings management: A note. Abacus 44 共3兲: 317–340.
Drake, A., S. Haka, and S. Ravenscroft. 1999. Cost system and incentive structure effects on innovation,
efficiency, and profitability in teams. The Accounting Review 74: 323–345.
Duffy, J., H. Xie, and Y-J. Lee. 2009. Social norms, information, and trust among strangers: Theory and
evidence. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1430506&CFID
⫽15357410&CFTOKEN⫽39092446.
Durocher, S., A. Fortin, and L. Cote. 2007. User participation in the standard setting process: A theory-
building study. Accounting, Organizations and Society 32 共1/2兲: 1–28.
Eining, M., D. Jones, and J. Loebbecke. 1997. Reliance on decision aids: An examination of management
fraud. Auditing: A Journal of Practice and Theory 16 共2兲: 1–19†
Eisenhardt, K. 1989. Agency theory: An assessment and review. Academy of Management Review 14:
57–74.†
Emerson, T., S. Conroy, and C. Stanley. 2007. Ethical attitudes of accountants: Recent evidence from a
practitioners’ survey. Journal of Business Ethics 71 共1兲: 73–87.
Evans, J. H., R. L. Hannan, R. Krishnan, and D. Moser. 2001. Honesty in managerial reporting. The Ac-
counting Review 76 共3兲: 537–559.
——–, D. Moser, and C. Kim. 2005. Economic and equity effects on reporting decisions. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 30 共7/8兲: 609–625.
Favere-Marchesi, M. 2006. Audit review: The impact of discussion timing and familiarity. Behavioral Re-
search in Accounting 18: 53–65.
Fedor, D., and R. Ramsey. 2007. Effects of supervisor power on preparers’ responses to audit review: A field
study. Behavioral Research in Accounting 19: 91–107.
Fehr, E., and S. Gaechter. 2000. Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. The American
Economic Review 90 共4兲: 845–859.
——–, and A. Leibbrandt. 2008. Cooperativeness and impatience in the tragedy of the commons. IZA
Discussion Paper No. 3625. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id
⫽1214914.
——–, and K. Schmidt. 1999. A theory of fairness, competition and cooperation. The Quarterly Journal of
Economics 114: 817–868.
Fisher, J. 1995. Contingency-based research on management control systems: Categorization by level of
complexity. Journal of Accounting Literature 14: 25–53.†
——–. 1998. Contingency theory, management control systems and firm outcomes: Past results and future
directions. Behavioral Research in Accounting 10 共Supplement兲: 47–64.†
——–, J. Fredrickson, and S. Peffer. 2000. Budgeting: An experimental investigation of the effects of nego-
tiation. The Accounting Review 75 共1兲: 93–114.
Fleischman, G., S. Valentine, and D. Finn. 2007. Ethical reasoning and equitable relief. Behavioral Research
in Accounting 19: 107–132.
Fligstein, N. 1998. The politics of quantification. Accounting, Organizations and Society 23 共3兲: 325–331.
Foucault, M. 1977. Discipline and Punish: The Birth of the Prison. Middlesex, U.K.: Peregrine Books.
Ganguly, A., J. Kagel, and D. Moser. 1994. The effect of biases in probability judgments on market prices.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 19 共8兲: 675–700.
Ghosh, D. 2000. Complementary arrangements of organizational factors and outcomes of negotiated transfer
price. Accounting, Organizations and Society 25: 661–682.
Gibbins, M. 1992. Deception: A tricky issue for behavioral research in accounting and auditing. Auditing: A
Journal of Practice & Theory 11 共Fall兲: 112–126.
Gilad, D., and D. Kliger. 2008. Priming the risk attitudes of professionals in financial decision making.
Review of Finance 12 共3兲: 567–586.
Gilbert, D., C. Morewedge, J. Risen, and T. Wilson. 2004. Looking forward to looking backward: The
misprediction of regret. Psychological Science 15 共5兲: 346–350.
Glover, S., D. Prawitt, and B. Spilker. 1997. The influence of decision aids on user behavior: Implications for
knowledge acquisition and inappropriate reliance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
38 Birnberg

cesses 72 共2兲: 232–253.


Gordon, L., and V. Narayanan. 1984. Management accounting systems, perceived environmental uncertainty
and organizational structure: An empirical study. Accounting, Organizations and Society 9 共1兲: 33–47.
Gosselin, M. 1997. The effect of strategy and organizational structure on the adoption of activity based
costing. Accounting, Organizations and Society 22 共2兲: 105–122.
Greenberg, J. 1990. Employee theft as a reaction to underpayment inequity: The hidden cost of paycuts. The
Journal of Applied Psychology 75 共5兲: 561–568.
Gupta, N., A. Poulsen, and M-C. Villeval. 2005. Male and female competitive behavior: Experimental
evidence. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽851227.
Haka, S., J. Luft, and B. Ballou. 2000. Second order uncertainty in accounting information and bilateral
bargaining costs. Journal of Management Accounting Research 12: 115–139.
Hannan, R. L. 2005. The combined effect of wages and firm profit on employee effort. The Accounting
Review 80 共1兲: 167–189.
——–, V. Hoffman, and D. Moser. 2005. Bonus versus penalty—Does contract frame affect employee effort?
In Experimental Business Research: Volume II: Economic and Managerial Perspectives, edited by
Rapoport, A., and R. Zwick. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Springer.
Harbaugh, W., U. Mayr, and D. Burghart. 2007. Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal
motives for charitable donations. Science 316 共5831兲: 1622–1625.
Hedberg, B., and S. Jonsson. 1978. Designing semi-confusing information systems for organizations in
changing environments. Accounting, Organizations and Society 3 共1兲: 47–68.
Henrich, J. 2007. Culture and the nature of human sociality. Address to AAA Annual Meeting in Chicago
共August兲.
——–, R. Boyd, S. Bowles, C. Camerer, E. Fehr, H. Gintis, and R. McElreath. 2001. In search of homoeco-
nomicus: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. American Economic Review 91 共2兲:
73–78.
——–, ——–, ——–, ——–, ——–, ——–, ——–, M. Alvard, A. Barr, J. Ensminger, N. Henrich, K. Hill, F.
Gil-White, M. Gurven, F. Marlowe, J. Patton, and D. Tracer. 2005. “Economic man” in cross-cultural
perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28:
795–855.†
Hirst, D. E., and P. Hopkins. 1998. Comprehensive income reporting and analysts’ valuation judgments.
Journal of Accounting Research 36 共Supplement兲: 47–75.
Hirst, M. 1983. Reliance on accounting measures, task uncertainty, and dysfunctional behavior: Some exten-
sions. Journal of Accounting Research 21 共2兲: 596–605.
Ho, J. 1999. Technology and group decision processes in going-concern judgments. Group Decision and
Negotiation 8: 33–49.
Hodge, F. D., J. J. Kennedy, and L. A. Maines. 2004. Does search-facilitating technology improve the
transparency of financial reporting? The Accounting Review 79 共3兲: 687–703.
Hoffman, V., and M. Zimbelman. 2009. Do strategic reasoning and brainstorming help auditors change their
standard audit procedures in response to fraud risk? The Accounting Review 84 共3兲: 811–838.†
Hofstede, G. 1980. Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work Attitudes. Beverly Hills, CA:
Russell Sage.
Homans, G. 1951. The Human Group. London, U.K.: Rutledge.
Hooks, K., and J. Schultz, Jr. 1996. Ethics and accounting research: The issue of deception. Behavioral
Research in Accounting 8 共Supplement兲: 25–47.
Hopwood, A. 1972. An empirical study of the role of accounting data in performance evaluation. Journal of
Accounting Research 10 共Supplement兲: 156–182.
——–. 1974. Leadership climate and the use of accounting data in performance evaluation. The Accounting
Review 49 共July兲: 485–495.
——–. 1976. Accounting and Human Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
——–. 1987. The archeology of accounting systems. Accounting, Organizations and Society 12 共3兲: 207–
234.
——–. 2007. Whither accounting research? The Accounting Review 82 共5兲: 1365–1374.
Hsu, M., C. Anen, and S. Quartz. 2008. The right and the good: Distributive justice and neural encoding of

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 39

equity and efficiency. Science 320 共5879兲: 1092–1095.


Huang, J., and D. Kisgen. 2008. Gender and corporate finance. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/
papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1263990.
Hufnagel, E., and J.Birnberg.1994. Perceived chargeback system fairness: A laboratory study. Accounting,
Management and Information Technology 4 共1兲: 1–22.
Hunton, J., and R. McEwen. 1997. An assessment of the relation between analysts’ earnings forecast accu-
racy, motivational incentives, and cognitive information search strategy. The Accounting Review 72
共4兲: 407–515.
——–, and J. Rose. 2010. The effects of anonymous whistle-blowing and perceived reputation threats on
investigations of whistle-blowing allegations by audit committee members. Journal of Management
Studies 共forthcoming兲.
Ittner, C. 2007. Archival research and management accounting. Presented at Doctoral Consortium at Man-
agement Accounting Section Mid-Year Meeting 共January兲.
Jacobsen, B., J. Lee, and W. Marquering. 2007. Are men more optimistic? Available at: http://
papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1030478.
Jeacle, I. 2003a. Accounting and the construction of the standard body. Accounting, Organizations and
Society 28 共4兲: 257–277.
——–. 2003b. Accounting and the construction of the standard house. Accounting, Auditing & Accountability
Journal 16 共4兲: 582–605.
——–. 2005. Accounting and the construction of taste: Standard labour costs and the Georgian cabinet-
maker. Abacus 41 共2兲: 117–137.
——–, and E. Walsh. 2002. From moral evaluation to rationalization: Accounting and the shifting technolo-
gies of credit. Accounting, Organizations and Society 27 共8兲: 737–762.
Joe, J. 2003. Why press coverage of a client influences the audit opinion. Journal of Accounting Research 41
共Spring兲: 109–133.
Johnson, E., S. Kaplan, and P. Reckers. 1998. An examination of potential gender-based differences in audit
managers’ performance evaluation judgments. Behavioral Research in Accounting 10: 47–75.
Johnson, V., and S. Kaplan. 1991. An experimental study of the effects of accountability on auditor decisions.
Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 共Supplement兲: 96–107.
Jones, J., D. Massey, and L. Thorne. 2003. Auditors’ ethical reasoning: Insights from past research and
implications for the future. Journal of Accounting Literature 22: 45–103.†
Jones, M., and H. Mellett. 2007. Determinants of changes in accounting practices: Accounting and the U.K.
health services. Critical Perspectives on Accounting 18 共1兲: 91–121.
Kachelmeier, S., and M. Shehata. 1997. Internal auditing and voluntary cooperation in firms: A cross-cultural
experiment. The Accounting Review 72 共3兲: 407–431.
——–, R. Smith, and W. Yancy. 1994. Budgets as a credible threat: An experimental study of cheap talk and
forward induction. Journal of Management Accounting Research 6: 144–174.
Kahneman, D., and A. Tversky. 1979. Prospect theory: An analysis of decision making under risk. Econo-
metrica 47 共2兲: 263–291.
Kelman, H. 1967. Human use of human participants: The problem of deception in social psychological
experiments. Psychological Bulletin 67 共1兲: 1–11.
Kennedy, S. J. 1993. Debiasing audit judgment with accountability: A framework and experimental results.
Journal of Accounting Research 31 共2兲: 231–245.
Kerr, D., and U. Murthy. 2004. Divergent and convergent idea generation in teams: A comparison of
computer-mediated and face-to-face communication. Group Decision and Negotiation 13 共4兲: 381–
394.
Kida, T., K. Moreno, and J. Smith. 2001. The influence of affect on managers’ capital-budgeting decisions.
Contemporary Accounting Research 18 共3兲: 477–494.
Knobe, J. 2003. Intentional action in folk psychology: An experimental investigation. Philosophical Psychol-
ogy 16 共2兲: 309–324.
Knudsen, E., J. Heckman, J. Cameron, and J. Shonkoff. 2007. Economic, neurobiological and behavioral
perspectives on building America’s future workforce. IZA Discussion Paper No. 2190. Available at:
http://ftp.iza.org/dp2190.pdf.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
40 Birnberg

Kober, R., J. Ng, and B. Paul. 2003. Change in strategy and MCS: A match over time. Advances in Account-
ing 20: 199–232.
Koonce, L., and M. Mercer. 2005. Using psychological theories in archival financial accounting research.
Journal of Accounting Literature 24: 175–214.†
Kuhnen, C., and J. Chiao. 2009. The genetic determinants of risk taking. PLoS ONE 4: 1–4.
La Porta, R., F. Lopez-de-Silanes, A. Schleifer, and R. Vishney. 1997. Trust in large organizations. The
American Economic Review 87 共2兲: 333–338.
Lawrenson, D. 1992. Britain’s railways: The predominance of engineering over accountancy during the
inter-war period. Critical Perspectives on Accounting 3 共1兲: 45–60.
Lerner, J., J. Goldberg, and P. Tetlock. 1998. Sober second thoughts: The effects of accountability, anger and
authoritarianism on attributions of accountability. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24:
563–574. †
——–, and D. Keltner. 2000. Beyond valence: Towards a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment.
Cognition and Emotion 14 共4兲: 473–493.†
——–, and ——–. 2001. Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 共1兲: 146–
159.
——–, D. Small, and G. Lowenstein. 2004. Research report: Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover effects
of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science 15 共5兲: 337–341.†
Libby, R., R. Bloomfield, and M. Nelson. 2002. Experimental research in financial accounting. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 27 共6兲: 775–810.†
——–, J. Hunton, H-T. Tan, and N. Seybert. 2008. Relationship incentives and the optimistic/pessimistic
pattern in analysts’ forecasts. Journal of Accounting Research 46 共1兲: 173–198.
——–, and H-T. Tan. 1999. Analysts’ reaction to negative earnings surprises. Journal of Accounting Research
37 共2兲: 415–436.
——–, and K. Trotman. 1993. The review process as a control for differential recall of evidence in auditor
judgments. Accounting, Organizations and Society 18 共6兲: 559–574.
Libby, T. 2001. Referent cognition and budgetary fairness: A research note. Journal of Management Account-
ing Research 13: 91–105.
Lipe, M., and S. Salterio. 2000. The balanced scorecard: Judgment effects of common and unique perfor-
mance measures. The Accounting Review 75 共3兲: 283–298.
Lo, A., and D. Repin. 2002. The psychophysiology of real-time financial risk processing. Journal of Cogni-
tive Neuroscience 14 共3兲: 323–339.
Louwers, T., L. Ponemon, and R. Radke. 1997. Examining accountants’ ethical behavior: A review and
implications for future research. In Behavioral Accounting Research: Foundations and Frontiers,
edited by Arnold, V., and S. Sutton, 188–221. Sarasota, FL: American Accounting Association.†
Luft, J. 1994. Bonus and penalty incentives contract choice by employees. Journal of Accounting and
Economics 18: 181–206.
——–. 1997. Fairness, ethics and the effect of management accounting on transaction costs. Journal of
Management Accounting Research 9: 200–216.
——–. 2007. Historical theorizing in management accounting research. In Handbook of Management Ac-
counting Research, Vol. 1, edited by Chapman, C. S., A. G. Hopwood, and M. D. Shields, 269–284.
Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.
——–, and R. Libby. 1997. Profit comparisons, market prices, and management’s judgments about negotiated
transfer prices. The Accounting Review 72 共1兲: 217–229.
MacKay, D., and A. Villarreal. 1987. Performance differences in the use of graphic and tabular displays of
multivariate data. Decision Sciences 18 共4兲: 535–548.
Mayhew, B. 2001. Auditor reputation building. Journal of Accounting Research 39 共3兲: 599–611.
——–, J. Schatzberg, and G. Sevcek. 2001. The effect of accounting uncertainty and auditor reputation on
auditor objectivity. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 20 共2兲: 49–70.
McCabe, K., D. Houser, L. Ryan, V. Smith, and T. Trouard. 2001. A functional imaging study of cooperation
in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences of the U.S.A. 98
共20兲: 11832–11835.
McCaffery, E., and J. Slemrod. 2006. Behavioral Public Finance. New York, NY: Russell Sage Press.†

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 41

McCracken, S., S. Salterio, and R. Schmidt. 2010. What happens when managers plan negotiations instead of
partners? Behavioral Research in Accounting 共forthcoming兲.
McSweeney, B. 2002. Hofstede’s model of national cultural differences and their consequences: A triumph of
faith—A failure of analysis. Human Relations 55 共1兲: 89–118.
Merchant, K., and W. Van der Stede. 2006. Field-based research in accounting: Accomplishments and pros-
pects. Behavioral Research in Accounting 18: 117–134.†
Messier, W., and W. Quilliam. 1992. The effect of accountability on judgment: Development of hypotheses
for auditing. Auditing: A Journal of Practice & Theory 11 共Supplement兲: 123–138.
Miller, C., D. Fedor, and R. Ramsey. 2006. Effects of discussion of audit reviews on auditors’ motivation and
performance. Behavioral Research in Accounting 18: 135–146.
Miller, P. 2007. Management accounting and sociology. In Handbook of Management Accounting Research,
Vol. 1, edited by Chapman, C., A. Hopwood, and M. Shields, 285–298. Oxford, U.K.: Elsevier.†t
——–, and T. O’Leary. 1987. Accounting and the construction of the governable person. Accounting, Orga-
nizations and Society 12 共3兲: 235–266.
——–, and T. O’Leary. 1997. Capital budgeting practices and complementarity relations in transition to
modem manufacture: A field-based study. Journal of Accounting Research 35 共Autumn兲: 257–271.
Moreno, K., and S. Bhattacharjee. 2008. The interplay of an opponent’s emotions and the management
control system during transfer price negotiation. Presented at the American Accounting Association
Accounting, Behavior, and Organizations Section Conference 共October兲.
——–, T. Kida, and J. Smith. 2002. The impact of affective reactions on risky decision making in accounting
contexts. Journal of Accounting Research 40 共5兲: 1331–1349.
Moriarity, S. 1979. Communicating financial information through multi-dimensional graphics. Journal of
Accounting Research 17 共1兲: 205–224.
Moser, D. 1998. Using an experimental economics approach in accounting research. Behavioral Research in
Accounting 10 共Supplement兲: 94–110.†
Murthy, U., and D. Kerr. 2003. Decision making performance of interacting groups: An experimental inves-
tigation of the effects of task type and communication. Information & Management 40 共5兲: 351–360.
Nelson, M., J. Elliott, and R. Tarpley. 2002. Evidence from auditors about managers’ and auditors’ earnings
management decisions. The Accounting Review 11 共1兲: 175–202.
Noreen, E. 1988. The ethics of economics: A new perspective on agency theory. Accounting, Organizations
and Society 13 共4兲: 359–369.
Otley, D. 1978. Budget use and managerial performance. Journal of Accounting Research 16 共Spring兲:
122–149.
——–. 1980. The contingency theory of management accounting: Achievement and prognosis. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 5 共4兲: 413–438.
Ouchi, W., and M. A. Maguire. 1975. Organizational control: Two functions. Administrative Science Quar-
terly 20: 559–569.
Patel, C., G. Harrison, and J. McKinnon. 2003. Cultural influences on judgments of professional accountants
in auditor-client conflict resolution. Journal of International Financial Management & Accounting 13
共1兲: l–31.
Pham, M. 2007. Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review
of General Psychology 11 共2兲: 155–178.†
Ponemon, L. 1990. Ethical judgments in accounting: A cognitive-developmental perspective. Critical Per-
spectives on Accounting 1: 191–215.
Prakash, P., and A. Rappaport. 1977. Information inductance and its significance for accounting. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 2 共1兲: 29–38.
Prendergast, C. 1999. The provision of incentives in firms. Journal of Economic Literature 37 共March兲:
7–63.†
Rabin, M. 1993. Incorporating fairness into game theory. The American Economic Review 83: 1281–1302.
——–. 1998. Psychology and economics. Journal of Economic Literature 36: 11–46.
Rankin, F., S. Schwartz, and R. Young. 2003. Management control using nonbinding budgetary announce-
ments. Journal of Management Accounting Research 15: 75–93.
Reckers, P., and J. Schultz. 1982. Individual versus group-assisted audit evaluations. Auditing: A Journal of

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
42 Birnberg

Practice & Theory 2: 64–74.


——–, and ——–. 1993. The effect of fraud signals, evidence order, and group-assisted counsel on indepen-
dent auditor judgment. Behavioral Research in Accounting 5: 124–144.
Rose, J. 2007. Attention to evidence of aggressive financial reporting and intentional misstatement judg-
ments: Effects of experience and trust. Behavioral Research in Accounting 19: 215–230.
——–, and C. Wolfe. 2000. The effects of information system design alternatives on the acquisition of tax
knowledge from a computerized decision aid. Accounting, Organizations and Society 25 共3兲: 285–305.
Roth, A. 1995. Bargaining experiments. In Handbook of Experimental Economics, edited by Kagel, J. H., and
A. E. Roth, 253–348. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press,†
——–, V. Prasnikar, M. Okuno-Fujiwara, and S. Zamir. 1991. Bargaining and market behavior in Jerusalem,
Ljubljana, Pittsburgh and Tokyo: An experimental study. The American Economic Review 72: 362–
372.
Rousseau, D., S. Sitkin, R. Burt, and C. Camerer. 1998. Not so different after all: A cross-disciplinary view
of trust. The Academy of Management Review 23: 393–404.†
Rowe, C. 2004. The effect of accounting reporting structure and team structure on performance in cross-
functional teams. The Accounting Review 79 共4兲: 1153–1180.
——–, J. Birnberg, and M. Shields. 2008. Effects of organizational process change on responsibility account-
ing and managers’ revelations of private information. Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 共2/3兲:
164–198.
Rustichini, A., J. Dickhaut, P. Ghirardato, K. Smith, and J. Pardo. 2005. A brain imaging study of the choice
procedure. Games and Economic Behavior 52 共2兲: 257–282.
Rutledge, R., and K. Karim. 1999. The influence of self-interest and ethical considerations on managers’
evaluation judgments. Accounting, Organizations and Society 24 共2兲: 173–184.
Sapienza, P., A. Toldra, and L. Zingales. 2007. Understanding trust. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/
papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1008943. †
Sapra, S., and P. Zak. 2008. Neurofinance: Bridging psychology, neurology, and investor behavior. Available
at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1323051.†
Schnedler, W., and R. Vadovic. 2007. Legitimacy of control. IZA Working Paper No. 3013. Available at:
http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1012803.
Schultz, J., and P. Reckers. 1981. The impact of group processing on selected audit disclosure decisions.
Journal of Accounting Research 19 共Autumn兲: 482–501.
Shields, M. 1980. Some effects on information load on search patterns used to analyze performance reports.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 5 共4兲: 429–442.
——–. 2007. Presentation at the Editors Panel, ABO Annual Research Conference, Philadelphia PA.
Shields, J., and M. Shields. 1988. Antecedents of participative budgeting. Accounting, Organizations and
Society 23 共1兲: 49–76.†
Simons, R. 1987. Accounting control systems and business strategy: An empirical analysis. Accounting,
Organizations and Society 12 共3兲: 557–574.
——–. 1990. The role of management control systems in creating competitive advantage: New perspectives.
Accounting, Organizations and Society 15 共1/2兲: 127–143.
Sloan, R. 1996. Do stock prices fully reflect information in accruals and cash flows about future earnings?
The Accounting Review 71 共3兲: 281–315.
Smith, K., and J. Dickhaut. 2005. Economics and emotion: Institutions matter. Games and Economic Behav-
ior 52 共2兲: 315–335. †
Solomon, I. 1987. Multi-auditor judgment/decision making research. Journal of Accounting Literature 6:
l–25.
Spicer, A., T. Dunfee, and W. Bailey. 2004. Does national context matter in ethical decision making? An
empirical test of integrative social contracts theory. Academy of Management Journal 47 共4兲: 610–
620.
Sprinkle, G., M. Williamson, and D. Upton. 2008. The effort and risk taking effects of budget-based con-
tracts. Accounting, Organizations and Society 33 共4/5兲 436–452.
Staffiero, G. 2006. Peer pressure and inequity aversion in the Japanese firm. Working paper No. 645, Uni-
versity of Navarra.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
A Proposed Framework for Behavioral Accounting Research 43

Sunder, S. 1995. Experiments in asset markets: A survey. In Handbook of Experimental Economics, edited by
Kagel, J. H., and A. E. Roth, 445–500. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, †
Sunstein, C. 2000. Behavioral Law and Economics. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press,†
Sutton, S., and S. Hayne. 1997. Judgment and decision making, part iii: Group process. In Behavioral
Accounting Research: Foundations and Frontiers, edited by Arnold, V., and S. Sutton, 134–163.
Sarasota, FL: American Accounting Association.†
Schwartz, S., and R. Young. 2002. A laboratory investigation of verification and reputation formation in a
repeated joint investment setting. Contemporary Accounting Research 19 共2兲: 311–342.
Tabibnia, G., A. Satpute, and M. Lieberman. 2008. The sunny side of fairness: Preference for fairness
activates reward circuitry 共and disregarding unfairness activates self–control circuitry兲. Psychological
Science 19: 339–347.
Tan, H-T., and K. Trotman. 2007. The effect of auditors’ concession timing on financial officers’ negotiation
judgment. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽960907.
Tan, H-T., and S-T. Tan. 2009. Investors’ reactions to management disclosure corrections: Does presentation
format matter? Contemporary Accounting Research 26 共2兲: 605–626.
Thaler, R. 1993. Advances in Behavioral Finance. New York, NY: Russell Sage.
Tinker, T., and M. Neimark. 1987. The role of annual reports in gender and class contradiction at General
Motors: 1917–1976. Accounting, Organizations and Society 12 共1兲: 77–88.
Towry, K. 2003. Control in a teamwork environment—The impact of social ties on the effectiveness of
mutual monitoring contracts. The Accounting Review 78 共4兲: 1069–1095.
Trotman, K., P. Yetton, and I. Zimmer. 1983. Individual and group judgments of internal control systems.
Journal of Accounting Research 21 共1兲: 286–292.
Tsay, C-J., and M. Bazerman, M. 2009. A decision-making perspective to negotiation: A review of the past
and a look into the future. Negotiation Journal 25 共4兲: 467–480.
Udel, P., G. Lowenstein, N. Schwarz, and P. Smith. 2005. Misimagining the unimaginable: The happiness gap
and healthcare efficiency. Health Psychology 24 共4兲: S537–S562.
Vance, T., S. J. Kennedy, and A. Webb. 2008. Creating bias in accounting estimates: The effects of manager-
subordinate relationship quality and ethicality concerns. Available at: http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/
papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1082884.
Vessey, I. 1994. The effects of information presentation on decision making: A cost-benefit analysis. Infor-
mation & Management 27 共2兲: 103–119.
Walker, R., and J. Aritz. 2006. A videotaped interaction analysis of member contribution in multicultural
groups. International Journal of Knowledge, Culture and Change Management 6 共4兲: 141–150.
Wang, C. 2007. Punishing deception and rewarding honesty. ICAM 2007 Meeting Paper. Available at:
http://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id⫽1111631.
Whitecotton, S. 1996. Effects of experience and confidence on decision aid reliance: A causal model. Behav-
ioral Research in Accounting 8: 194–216.
Windsor, C., and N. Ashkanasy. 1996. Auditor independence decision making: The role of organizational
culture perceptions. Behavioral Research in Accounting 8 共Supplement兲: 80–97.
Wright, W., and G. Bower. 1992. Mood effects on subjective probability assessment. Organizational Behav-
ior and Human Decision Processes 52 共2兲: 276–291.
Young, S. M. 1985. Participative budgeting: The effects of risk aversion and asymmetric information on
budgetary slack. Journal of Accounting Research 23 共Autumn兲: 829–842.
——–, J. Fisher, and T. Lindquist. 1993. The effect of intergroup competition and intragroup cooperation on
slack and output in a manufacturing setting. The Accounting Review 68 共July兲: 466–481.
Zak, P. 2008. The neurobiology of trust. Scientific American 共June兲: 88–97.†
Zaltman, G. 1997. Rethinking market research: Putting people back in. Journal of Marketing Research 34
共4兲: 424–437.
Zhang, Y. 2008. The effects of perceived fairness and communication on honesty and collusion in a multi-
agent setting. The Accounting Review 83 共4兲: 1125–1146.

Behavioral Research In Accounting Volume 23, Number 1, 2011


American Accounting Association
Copyright of Behavioral Research in Accounting is the property of American Accounting Association and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's
express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

You might also like