You are on page 1of 11

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/8217896

Emotion Regulation in Children With Anxiety Disorders

Article  in  Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology · January 2005


DOI: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3304_10 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS
299 7,529

2 authors, including:

Janice Zeman
College of William and Mary
81 PUBLICATIONS   5,262 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Peers in Networks View project

Parent socialization of emotion View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Janice Zeman on 21 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology Copyright © 2004 by
2004, Vol. 33, No. 4, 750–759 Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Emotion Regulation in Children With Anxiety Disorders


Cynthia Suveg and Janice Zeman
Department of Psychology, University of Maine

This study examined emotion management skills in addition to the role of emotional
intensity and self-efficacy in emotion regulation in 26 children with anxiety disorders
(ADs) ages 8 to 12 years and their counterparts without any form of psychopathology.
Children completed the Children’s Emotion Management Scales (CEMS) and Emo-
tion Regulation Interview (ERI), and mothers reported on their children’s emotion
regulation using the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC). Results indicated that chil-
dren who met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (4th ed., Amer-
ican Psychiatric Association, 1994) criteria for an anxiety disorder had difficulty
managing worried, sad, and anger experiences, potentially due to their report of ex-
periencing emotions with high intensity and having little confidence in their ability to
regulate this arousal. These findings indicate that emotion regulation needs to be con-
sidered centrally in research with anxious populations.

A considerable body of research has identified Although the literature examining emotion regula-
emotional competence, the ability to act efficaciously tion in children with psychopathology is scant, a few
in emotionally arousing situations, as a crucial com- findings are beginning to emerge (e.g., Casey, 1996;
ponent in children’s adaptive social functioning and Suveg, Zeman, & Stegall, 2001; Southam-Gerow &
psychological adjustment (Cicchetti, Ackerman, & Kendall, 2000; Zeman, Shipman, & Suveg, 2002). For
Izard, 1995; Eisenberg & Fabes, 1992; Eisenberg, example, using self-report questionnaires and peer rat-
Fabes, & Losoya, 1997; Hubbard & Coie, 1994; ings, Zeman et al. examined the predictive relation be-
Saarni, 1999). From a functionalist theoretical per- tween anger and sadness regulation and internalizing
spective, one skill posited to underlie emotionally and externalizing symptoms in a community sample of
competent functioning is the ability to regulate emo- elementary school age children. Results of regression
tion in flexible and adaptive ways in response to the analyses indicated that the inability to identify emo-
demands of the social context (Campos, Mumme, tional states, the inhibition of anger, the dysregulated
Kermoian, & Campos, 1994). expression of anger and sadness, and maladaptive an-
Despite the growing body of empirical research that ger coping were significant predictors of internalizing
has investigated emotion-regulation skills in normative symptoms. In contrast, dysregulated sadness expres-
populations (for a review, see Saarni, 1999), relatively sion and maladaptive coping with anger were related to
little research has examined these abilities in atypical externalizing symptoms. Suveg et al. assessed fourth-
populations. In their review of emotion understanding and fifth-grade children’s self-report of anxious symp-
and regulation, Southam-Gerow and Kendall (2002) toms and methods of emotion regulation across fear-,
highlighted that only a few studies have examined sadness-, and anger-invoking situations. Results re-
emotion understanding and regulation in children with vealed that children who reported experiencing anx-
psychopathology. This is surprising given that aspects ious symptoms relied mostly on inhibited and dys-
of emotion regulation have been posited to play a role regulated methods to manage their scared, sad, and
in most forms of psychopathology (Casey, 1996; angry feelings to the neglect of adaptive regulation
Cicchetti et al., 1995; Cole, Michel, & O’Donnell-Teti, strategies. Furthermore, Southam-Gerow and Kendall
1994). found that children with anxiety disorders (ADs) dem-
onstrated less understanding of emotional dissem-
Cynthia Suveg is now at the Department of Psychology, Temple
blance and fluctuating emotional experiences com-
University. pared to youth with no anxiety disorders.
This article is part of the first author’s doctoral dissertation. We Two additional components of emotion regulation
would like to thank Philip Kendall for his helpful comments on an that have been investigated, albeit mostly with norma-
earlier draft of the article. tive populations, are intensity of emotional experience
Requests for reprints should be sent to Cynthia Suveg, Child
and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders Clinic, Temple University,
and self-efficacy. Although an optimal level of arousal is
Weiss Hall, 1701 N. 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 19122. E-mail: believed to facilitate performance in a situation, re-
csuveg@temple.edu search suggests that high arousal may lead to over- or

750
EMOTION REGULATION IN ANXIOUS CHILDREN

under-control of emotional experience, both of which ations or emotions along more global dimensions (e.g.,
have been inversely associated with social and emo- tripartite model of anxiety and depression; Chorpita,
tional competence (Bradley, 1990; Cole et al., 1994; 2002; Chorpita, Albano, & Barlow, 1998; Lonigan,
Eisenberg, Cumberland, & Spinrad, 1998). Although Hooe, David, & Kistner, 1999). Child sex was included
both positive and negative emotional overarousal is as- as a variable because the emotional development litera-
sociated with less adaptive responding, negative emo- ture suggests that children’s emotion-regulation deci-
tions tend to produce greater levels of arousal than posi- sions vary according to the child’s sex (Brody & Hall,
tive emotions (Bradley, 2000; Cacioppo, Berntson, 2000; Saarni, 1999; Zeman & Garber, 1996). Given
Larsen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000). The preponderance of that children develop stylized ways of managing emo-
negative emotional experience that is a central compo- tional experiences in middle childhood, children in the
nent of childhood anxiety (Blumberg & Izard, 1986; third through fifth grades were selected (Cole &
Laurent et al., 1999) and the physiological hyperarousal Kaslow, 1988). The vignettes developed for this study
that is a distinguishing feature of anxiety (Clark & Wat- included the mother as the audience figure, because
son, 1991) place anxious children at significant risk for children of this age group report being most comfort-
emotion-regulation difficulties that extend beyond the able expressing emotions to their mothers compared to
ability to manage anxious experiences. fathers and peers (Saarni, 1999; Zeman & Garber,
Self-efficacy is another factor that is likely to affect 1996).
children’s emotion regulation in that children who Based on the literature, it was hypothesized that, in
have a greater sense of self-efficacy are likely to perse- contrast to their peers without an anxiety disorder
vere (Bradley, 2000). When applied to emotion regula- (NADs), children with an AD would (a) display more
tion in particular, children with low self-efficacy may maladaptive patterns (i.e., more inhibition and
not be likely to try different strategies to manage emo- dysregulated expression and less adaptive coping) of
tionally arousing situations. Research has found trait worry, sadness, and anger; (b) report experiencing
anxiety in children to be associated with low levels higher levels of emotional intensity; and (c) perceive
of self-efficacy (Muris, 2002). Furthermore, research themselves as less efficacious in emotionally arousing
with anxious children indicates that they tend to with- situations. Lastly, it was hypothesized that emotional
draw from or avoid emotionally arousing situations, intensity would be negatively related to children’s re-
suggesting that they may have low self-efficacy in ported emotion-regulation decisions.
those situations (Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, & Ryan, 1996;
Suveg et al., 2001).
The aforementioned literature suggests that chil-
Method
dren with ADs may be a particularly useful population
in which to study certain facets of emotion regulation
Participants
(for further discussion of emotion regulation and child-
hood anxiety, see Thompson, 2001). Indeed, examina- Participants were 52 Caucasian children between
tion of emotion-regulation skills in anxious children the ages of 8 and 12 years and their biological mothers
may help identify emotion-related processes that con- who were primarily of middle-class socioeconomic
tribute to the ontology or maintenance of childhood status (SES). The AD group included 12 boys (M age =
anxiety. Thus, the goal of this study was to examine 10 years, 1 month, SD = 10 months) and 14 girls (M
methods of emotion regulation (i.e., dysregulated ex- age = 10 years, 10 months, SD = 10 months) who were
pression, inhibition, coping) and components thereof recruited from public elementary schools. Psychologi-
(i.e., self-efficacy, degrees of intensity) in children cal diagnoses were made using mother and child report
with ADs. To address limitations of previous studies, of symptoms on the Anxiety Disorder Interview
we used rigorous diagnostic procedures, investigated Schedule for Children–Fourth Edition (ADIS–IV), re-
specific emotions (i.e., worry, sadness, anger) instead sulting in the following principal diagnoses: separation
of studying global negative emotional states, obtained anxiety disorder (4 girls, 5 boys), social phobia (6 girls,
mother report of children’s emotion regulation, consid- 1 boy), generalized anxiety disorder (3 girls, 5 boys),
ered the influence of maternal psychopathology on and specific phobia (1 girl, 1 boy). Given the high
maternal report of children’s behavior given that this comorbidity among the anxiety disorders (Albano,
variable may influence parents’ reports of children’s Chorpita, & Barlow, 1996; Brady & Kendall, 1992),
behavior (cf. Richters, 1992; Schaughency & Lahey, children with multiple anxiety disorders were included.
1985), and established that participants had the requi- Eight boys and 10 girls in the AD group had a comorbid
site verbal ability to respond to the study questions. anxiety diagnosis, and 3 boys and 2 girls had a comorbid
Consistent with the functionalist perspective, the in- externalizing disorder (i.e., oppositional defiant disor-
dividual emotions of worry, sadness, and anger were der, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder).
chosen for study because much of the research with The NAD group included 12 boys (M age = 10
anxious children has examined anxiety-provoking situ- years, 4 months, SD = 12 months) and 14 girls (M age =

751
SUVEG AND ZEMAN

10 years, 9 months, SD = 8 months). Children in the 3-point scale and summed to yield a total depression
control group scored within the normative range on score. The psychometric properties of the CDI have
both the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI) and been established (Carey, Gresham, Ruggiero, Faul-
Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale (RCMAS) stich, & Enyart, 1987; Kovacs, 1985). A 2 (group) × 2
and did not meet diagnostic criteria for a psychological (sex) ANOVA indicated children in the AD group (M =
disorder based on the ADIS–IV. Analyses of variance 15.21, SD = 8.50) scored significantly higher on the
(ANOVAs) examining group and sex effects on age re- CDI than did children in the NAD group (M = 6.36, SD
vealed a significant sex main effect indicating that girls = 5.20), F(1, 45) = 18.71, p < .001, η2 = .29. A main ef-
(M = 10 years, 10 months, SD = 9 months) were signifi- fect for sex emerged such that girls (M = 12.57, SD =
cantly older than boys (M = 10 years, 3 months, SD = 1 9.79) endorsed more depressive symptoms than did
year, 0 months), F(1, 48) = 5.79, p < .05, η2 = .11. boys (M = 8.19, SD = 4.76), F(1, 45) = 4.81, p < .05, η2
= .10. There were no interaction effects.

Measures Emotion Regulation


Psychopathology Children’s Emotion Management Scales (CEMS):
Anger and sadness. The CEMS (Zeman, Shipman,
ADIS–IV. The ADIS–IV (Silverman & Albano, & Penza-Clyve, 2001) assesses self-report of chil-
1996) consists of child and parent semistructured inter- dren’s sadness (11 items) and anger (12 items) man-
views that assess a broad range of anxiety, mood, and agement. Using a Likert scale of 1 (hardly ever), 2
behavior disorders according to the Diagnostic and (sometimes), or 3 (often), children respond to items
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th ed. that comprise three subscales: (a) Inhibition, suppres-
[DSM–IV], American Psychiatric Association, 1994) sion of emotional expression (e.g., “I get sad inside but
criteria. Children were assigned a diagnosis if either I don’t show it”); (b) Dysregulated Expression, chil-
the child or the parent indicated that the symptoms dren’s culturally inappropriate emotional expression
were causing significant interference in functioning (e.g., “I say mean things to others when I am mad”);
and the clinician subsequently assigned a severity rat- and (c) Emotion Regulation Coping, children’s adap-
ing of 4 or greater (as suggested in the clinician’s man- tive methods of emotion management (e.g., “When I
ual of the ADIS–IV, Child and Parent Versions; Albano am feeling sad, I do something totally different until I
& Silverman, 1996). The psychometric properties of calm down”). A Worry Management scale that has a
the ADIS–IV indicate acceptable reliability and valid- similar three-factor structure was developed for use in
ity (Silverman & Albano, 1996; Silverman & Nelles, this study. Preliminary validity has been established in
1988; Silverman, Saavedra, & Pina, 2001; Wood, other research (Stegall, 2003) such that a total worry
Piacentini, Bergman, McCracken, & Barrios, 2002). score (Emotion Regulation Coping scale was inversely
An advanced graduate student blind to diagnostic scored) was found to be positively related with parent
status rated approximately one third of randomly se- and child report of internalizing and externalizing
lected audiotaped interviews to establish diagnostic re- symptomatology. Examination of the psychometric
liability. The kappa value for principal diagnosis properties of the CEMS indicate coefficient alphas that
was .89. Disagreements on diagnosis were resolved range from .62 to .77 and test–retest reliability ranging
through discussion. from .61 to .80 for the individual scales (Zeman et al.,
2001).
RCMAS. The RCMAS (Reynolds & Richmond,
1997) is a 37-item questionnaire designed to assess Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC). The ERC
symptoms of anxiety in children and adolescents. The (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) consists of 24 items that
psychometric properties of the RCMAS have been ex- assess parents’ perceptions of their child’s typical
tensively studied with adequate internal consistency methods of managing emotional experiences on a scale
established (Lonigan, Carey, & Finch, 1994; Pela & of 1 (never) to 4 (always). The ERC yields two sub-
Reynolds, 1982). A 2 (group) × 2 (sex) ANOVA indi- scales: (a) Lability/Negativity, which assesses inflexi-
cated that children in the AD group (M = 19.71, SD = bility, lability, and dysregulated negative affect (e.g.,
4.72) scored significantly higher on the RCMAS than “Exhibits wide mood swings”) and (b) Emotion Regu-
did children in the NAD group (M = 8.96, SD = 6.07), lation, which measures appropriate emotional expres-
F(1, 45) = 46.47, p < .0001, η2 = .51, with no signifi- sion, empathy, and emotional self-awareness (e.g.,
cant sex differences or interaction effects. “Can modulate excitement in emotionally arousing sit-
uations”). Reliability coefficients are high for the over-
CDI. The CDI (Kovacs, 1992) is a 26-item ques- all scale (.89) and for the two subscales (Lability/
tionnaire that assesses depressive symptomatology in Negativity = .96, Regulation = .83; Shields & Cicchet-
children over the past 2 weeks. Items are scored on a ti, 1997). Validity has been established through posi-

752
EMOTION REGULATION IN ANXIOUS CHILDREN

tive correlations with observers’ ratings of children’s validity (Deonandan, Campbell, Ostbye, Tummon, &
regulatory abilities and the proportion of expressed Robertson, 2000).
positive and negative affect (Shields & Cicchetti,
1997). Discriminant validity demonstrates that the Intellectual functioning. Children and their moth-
ERC can reliably be differentiated from other emo- ers were administered the Vocabulary subtest of the
tion-related constructs (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition
(Wechsler, 1991) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence
Emotion Regulation Interview (ERI). The ERI Scale–Third Edition (Wechsler, 1997), respectively.
was designed for use in this study to assess children’s These measures were included to determine that partici-
report of their emotional intensity and self-efficacy and pants had sufficient verbal ability to understand the
was modeled after previous research (e.g., Zeman & questionnaire items and to ensure that there were no
Garber, 1996). Children were read six vignettes that group differences in verbal ability that could potentially
were designed through pilot testing to elicit worry, sad- influence the validity of the findings. The Vocabulary
ness, and anger in the presence of their mother. Follow- subtest has high reliability, provides the best measure of
ing each vignette, children were asked three questions general intelligence of the scale, and has the highest cor-
that assessed their (a) decisions regarding emotional relation with the Full Scale IQ score of all subtests
expression (i.e., “Would you show how worried, sad, (Wechsler, 1991, 1997). All children in this study, ex-
or mad you feel to your mother?”), (b) emotional inten- cept for 1 anxious boy and 1 anxious girl, had at least an
sity (i.e., “How worried, sad, or mad would you feel in average-level score on this subtest (i.e., scaled score ≥
this situation?”), and (c) self-efficacy (i.e., “How much 7). The low score for these children was attributed to per-
would you be able to make yourself feel better in this formance anxiety rather than below-average intelli-
situation?”). Children responded to the questions using gence. All mothers had at least an average-level score.
a 4-point Likert scale. Their decision to show their There were no significant group or sex main effects
emotion was rated from 1 (definitely would) to 4 (defi- or interactions on the descriptive variables of maternal
nitely would not), and their perceived self-efficacy was age, SES, or children’s and mothers’ verbal abilities.
rated from 1 (a little) to 4 (a lot). For the intensity ques- See Table 1 for means and standard deviations for de-
tion, children were shown a picture of a thermometer scriptive variables.
and asked to indicate how worried, sad, or mad they
would feel (1 = not at all, 10 = very). Refer to the Ap- Maternal psychopathology. The Symptom Check-
pendix for the vignettes. list–90–Revised (SCL–90–R; Derogatis, 1994) is a 90-
item questionnaire that assesses symptoms of adult
psychopathology using a 5-point Likert response scale
Descriptive Variables
ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Adequate re-
SES. Mothers’ reported on their own and their liability and validity have been established (Derogatis,
spouse’s (when applicable) level of education and cur- 1994). Research has suggested that parental emotional
rent occupation. Responses were coded using the Hol- distress may impact parents’reports of child behavior in
lingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status (Hol- terms of overall adjustment and symptoms of
lingshead, 1975) that yields five social status psychopathology (cf. Richters, 1992; Schaughency &
categories. Higher numbers indicate higher level of Lahey, 1985). Thus, the SCL–90–R was administered to
SES. The Hollingshead has been shown to have good determine whether mothers of children with an AD dif-

Table 1. Variables as a Function of Diagnostic Group and Sex


Anxiety Disorder Group Non-Anxiety-Disorder Group

Girls Boys Girls Boys

Variable M SD Range M SD Range M SD Range M SD Range

Child vocabulary 10.29 3.67 5–18 11.33 3.55 3–16 12.07 2.53 7–16 12.33 3.42 7–17
scorea
Mother vocabulary 10.92 3.45 6–17 11.27 3.44 7–17 11.14 3.51 5–16 11.25 1.54 9–14
scoreb
Family incomec 3.18 1.25 1–5 3.80 1.55 1–5 3.83 .94 2–5 3.64 .92 2–5
SCL–90–Rd 65.59 66.16 2–240 43.92 40.06 7–134 31.02 27.70 6–112 26.42 20.50 4–82

Note: SCL–90–R = Symptom Checklist–90–Revised. AD = anxiety disorder; NAD = no anxiety disorder.


aAssessed with the Vocabulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition; n = 26 for both groups. bAssessed with the Vocab-

ulary subtest of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Third Edition; n = 23 for the AD group and n = 26 for the NAD group. cIndicating that, on
average, families in this study were of middle socioeconomic status; n = 21 for the AD group and n = 23 for the NAD group. n = 26 for both
groups.

753
SUVEG AND ZEMAN

fered from mothers of children without an AD with re- Table 2. Dysregulation Scores by Group and Emotion Type
spect to their level of psychological distress. Results of a as Assessed by the Children’s Emotion Management Scales
one-way ANOVA revealed that mothers of children with Anxious Nonanxious
an AD (M = 55.59, SD = 55.71) reported a significantly
greater number of total symptoms of psychopathology Emotion M SD M SD
than mothers of children without an AD (M = 28.89, SD Worry 1.60 .50 1.44 .46
= 24.28), F(1, 50) = 5.02, p < .05, η2 = .09. Sadness 1.67 .57 1.45 .39
Anger 1.70 .46 1.44 .44

Procedure Note: Higher scores indicate more dysregulated expression of emo-


tion. n = 26 for each group across all emotions.

The study implemented a multiple gating procedure


to ensure that all children who participated were placed lection was conducted either at the participant’s home
into the correct diagnostic group (i.e., AD, NAD). (AD = 15, NAD = 12) or in the research laboratory (AD
= 11, NAD = 14), depending on the family’s prefer-
Gate A ence. Maternal and child interviews were conducted in
random order, and questionnaires were placed in pack-
Within the public school system, children and ets in random order. Children and their mothers com-
mothers or guardians were invited to participate in the pleted their questionnaires autonomously and received
first stage of this project. A total of 210 children pro- assistance by a graduate student as needed.
vided verbal assent and had parental consent to partici-
pate. All of these children participated in a 30-min ses-
sion in their classrooms where they completed the Results
RCMAS and CDI in a group format. Two research as-
sistants attended each school screening. Children with Data Analytic Strategy
a T score ≥ 60 on the RCMAS were considered for in-
clusion in the AD group and those who scored within Repeated-measures ANOVAs were conducted to
the normative range were considered for inclusion in examine group, sex, and emotion differences on the
the NAD group. Given the high correlation between CEMS and ERI with ANOVAs utilized to examine
anxiety and depressive disorders (Brady & Kendall, group and sex differences on the ERC. All F tests re-
1992; Lonigan et al., 1994), children who scored in the ported represent Wilks’s lambda values. Bonferroni
clinical range (7 girls, 1 boy) on the CDI in addition to corrections were applied when conducting all subse-
the RCMAS were considered for inclusion in the study. quent within-subjects contrasts to limit the experi-
However, children who scored in the clinical range on ment-wise error rate to p < .05 (e.g., three tests compar-
the CDI but in the normative range on the RCMAS ing the different emotions; .05/3 = .016). Measures of
were excluded from the study (2 girls, 1 boy). effect size (i.e., eta-squared) were obtained for all anal-
yses where appropriate and interpreted according to
Gate B criteria suggested by Cohen (1988): (a) .01 to .05 =
small effect, (b) .06 to .13 = medium effect, and (c) .14
A research assistant who did not subsequently col- or larger = large effect.
lect Gate C data determined which children met these
initial screening requirements based on the RCMAS
and CDI results and called their mothers to solicit par- Emotion Regulation Strategies1
ticipation in Gate C. Given that the RCMAS and CDI Child-reported emotion regulation. Using the
are measures of distress and not necessarily indicative Children’s CEMS, a significant group main effect
of diagnostic status, an additional screening was con- emerged for the Dysregulated scale, F(1, 48) = 5.35, p
ducted over the phone to exclude children with a false < .05, η2 = .10, such that children with an AD reported
positive score on the anxiety and depression measures. more dysregulated expression across all emotions than
Families of children who met criteria to move on to did children in the NAD group. See Table 2 for means
Gate C but declined (n = 5) were offered help in secur- and standard deviations.
ing follow-up services if they so wished. Children who A significant Emotion × Group interaction emerged
did not meet the additional screening criteria (n = 8) for the Inhibition scale, F(2, 47) = 1.30, p < .05, η2 =
were not included in the study. .12. When the interaction was broken down, however,

Gate C
1To determine that results on the CEMS, ERC, and ERI were not
Four graduate students who were blind to group sta- driven by the comorbid externalizing disorders, analyses were rerun
tus and trained to administer the diagnostic interview without these participants’ data included and the results remained
and questionnaires collected all data. The 2-hr data col- statistically significant.

754
EMOTION REGULATION IN ANXIOUS CHILDREN

the only finding to approach significance (p < .08) re- scenarios. A significant Group × Emotion interaction
vealed that children with an AD reported more inhibi- emerged, F(2, 44) = 3.13, p < .05, η2 = .13, which was ex-
tion of worry than did children with no AD. plicated by examining group differences within emo-
Analyses examining the Emotion Regulation Cop- tion type. The results indicated that children with an AD
ing scale revealed significant main effects for group, reported experiencing worry and anger more intensely
F(1, 48) = 23.08, p < .0001, η2 = .33, and sex, F(1, 48) = than control children with no differences for sadness.
17.53, p < .0001, η2 = .27. Children with an AD and girls See Table 4 for means and standard deviations.
reported less adaptive coping than did children without The relation between emotional intensity and chil-
an AD and boys. A significant main effect was also dren’s reported emotion-regulation decisions was ex-
found for Emotion, F(2, 47) = 4.11, p < .05, η2 = .15. amined by correlating the responses to the questions
Subsequent within-subjects contrasts indicated that all from the ERI assessing intensity (i.e., “How worried,
children indicated more adaptive coping with sadness sad, or mad would you feel if this really happened to
(M = 2.25, SD = .42) than with worry (M = 2.12, SD = you?”) and regulation decisions (i.e., “Would you show
.36). Anger was not significantly different than either. how worried, sad, or mad you feel to your mother?”).
See Table 3 for means and standard deviations. Analyses were conducted separately by group status
only as there were no specific hypotheses by sex. For
Mother report of child emotion regulation. the AD group, significant correlations were found for
For analyses of the ERC data, the SCL–90–R total score the sadness and anger scenarios, whereas for the NAD
was entered as a covariate because correlational analy- group, all three scenarios yielded significant correla-
ses revealed significant relations between the tions (see Table 5). To examine whether the strength of
SCL–90–R Total score and the Negativity/Lability relation differed among the anxious and control
scale (r = .40, p < .01) and the Regulation scale (r = –.42, groups, Fisher’s r to z transformations were first calcu-
p < .01). A significant group main effect for the lated followed by two sample Z tests of correlation.
Negativity/Lability subscale emerged, F(1, 46) = 14.90, None of the transformations for any of the emotions
p < .0001, η2 = .25, such that mothers of ADs perceived were significant.
their children as more inflexible, labile, and emotionally
negative (adjusted M = 2.18, SD = .51) than mothers of Emotional Self-Efficacy
control children (adjusted M = 1.65, SD = .31). A group
Global self-efficacy scores were computed by ob-
main effect approached significance for the Regulation
taining a mean score for the camp and sports scenarios
subscale, F(1, 46) = 3.56, p = .07, η2 = .07. Mothers of
separately by emotion type (i.e.,“How much do you
children with an AD rated their children significantly
lower on appropriate emotion expression and
self-awareness (adjusted M = 3.04, SD = .43) than did Table 4. Children’s Self-Report of Intensity by Emotion
mothers of children without an AD (adjusted M = 3.31, Type as Assessed by the ERI
SD = .27). No sex or interaction effects were found. Anxiousa Nonanxiousb
ERI Intensity
Question M SD M SD F Value η2
Emotional Intensity
Worry 6.33 1.73 4.85 1.99 8.90** .15
To examine the hypothesis that children with an AD Sadness 6.41 2.34 5.71 1.92 2.35 —
would report experiencing higher levels of emotional Anger 7.48 2.06 5.19 1.98 16.11*** .25
intensity than children without an AD in response to
Note: ERI = Emotion Regulation Interview. Maximum score =
emotionally arousing situations, global intensity scores
10.00 (1 = not at all, 10 = very). Dashes indicate that eta squared was
were computed for each of three emotions on the ERI by not computed as the differences between groups was not significant.
obtaining a mean intensity score for the camp and sports an = 23 for all emotions. bn = 26 for all emotions.

**p < .01. ***p < .0001.

Table 3. Coping Scores by Group, Sex, and Emotion Type


Table 5. Correlations Between Self-Reported Emotional
as Assessed by the Children’s Emotion Management Scales
Intensity Scores and Emotion Regulation Decisions by
Worry Sadness Anger Group
M SD M SD M SD Group Emotion r

Anxious 1.97a .29 2.08a .40 2.03 .44 Anxious Worry –.22
Nonanxious 2.26a .38 2.43a .35 2.39 .38 Sadness –.63**
Boys 2.26a .37 2.48a .41 1.99 .31 Anger –.43*
Girls 2.06a .32 2.32a .42 2.12 .49 Nonanxious Worry –.45*
Sadness –.55**
Note: Higher scores indicate more adaptive coping with emotion. n Anger –.59**
= 26 for each group across all emotions. Means in the same row that
share superscripts differ significantly at p < .05. *p < .05. **p < .01.

755
SUVEG AND ZEMAN

think you would be able to make yourself feel better in without an AD. This finding is consistent with previous
this situation?”). Analyses revealed a significant main research that found that children experiencing anxious
effect for group, F(1, 46) = 9.92, p < .01, η2 = .17. symptomatology reported using less constructive ways
Across all emotions, children with an AD perceived of managing their negative emotions than nonanxious
themselves as less efficacious than did children with- children (Suveg et al., 2001) and that poor coping with
out an AD. A significant main effect also emerged for anger predicted internalizing symptomatology (Zeman
emotion type, F(2, 46) = 3.55, p < .05, η2 = .13. Subse- et al., 2002). Convergent evidence for the finding that
quent within-subjects contrasts indicated that all chil- children with an AD experience difficulties with emo-
dren perceived themselves as significantly less effica- tion regulation comes from maternal report. Mothers of
cious in the worried scenario (M = 2.89, SD = .67) than children with an AD perceived their children as signifi-
in sadness (M = 3.09, SD = .65) scenarios. Anger was cantly more negative and labile than did mothers of chil-
not significantly different than either. No significant dren without an AD. Given these findings, deficits in
sex differences or interactions were found. See Table 6 emotion regulation should be considered a potential cor-
for means and standard deviations. relate of the difficulties that children with an AD experi-
ence in their psychological functioning (Strauss, Frame,
& Forehand, 1987).
Discussion
An interesting, albeit unexpected, finding was that
all children indicated more adaptive coping with sad-
The functionalist approach underscores the impor-
ness than with worry. Although the emotional develop-
tance of learning how to manage emotions in flexible
ment literature suggests that by the age of 3 years chil-
and adaptive ways in response to the demands of the
dren experience the full repertoire of emotions (Lewis,
social context (Campos et al., 1994). This ability is
2000), children’s ability to identify and label such
considered a central component of emotional compe-
emotions follows a developmental course that is highly
tence (Saarni, 1999). Not surprisingly, difficulties with
interconnected with their cognitive development. Be-
emotion regulation have been posited to play a role in
cause worry involves a certain cognitive sophistica-
most forms of psychopathology (Bradley, 1990, 2000;
tion, children become better able to identify and ar-
Casey, 1996; Cicchetti et al., 1995). The results from
ticulate their worries with increasing age (Muris,
this study provide strong support for this theoretical as-
Merckelbach, Meesters, & van den Brand, 2002). In
sertion with regard to childhood anxiety disorders.
addition, sadness expression is more readily observ-
Specifically, children with an AD indicated more
able (e.g., tears, facial expression) than worry expres-
dysregulated expression of worry, sadness, and anger
sion, and, thus, children may have received more direct
than did children without an AD. Interestingly, how-
socialization to help them learn how to cope with this
ever, no group differences emerged for inhibition of
experience. Consequently, it could be that children of
emotion. This pattern of findings initially appears
the age group used in this study (i.e., 8 to 12 years) are
counterintuitive as anxious children are typically char-
likely to have had less experience and practice in cop-
acterized by shy and withdrawn behaviors rather than
ing with worry than sadness.
externalizing types of behaviors (similar to those on
It is not clear why, in this study, both girls with and
the Dysregulation scale). It could be, however, that
without anxiety disorders reported less adaptive emo-
children with an AD strive to contain their negative
tion coping than did boys. One explanation could be that
emotions, but when the strain of inhibiting their emo-
this study used a forced-choice questionnaire, as op-
tions becomes too uncomfortable, these “bottled up”
posed to observational or open-ended methodologies
emotions are unleashed in dysregulated ways.
that may yield different results. It could also be that girls
Children with an AD also reported coping less adap-
were more willing than boys to admit to having difficul-
tively with worry, sadness, and anger than did children
ties coping with negative emotions partly because they
have received more encouragement to express vulnera-
Table 6. Children’s Self-Efficacy Scores by Emotion Type ble feelings than have boys.2 Lastly, from a functionalist
as Assessed on the Emotion Regulation Interview perspective, endorsement of certain coping items in the
CEMS (i.e., “When I am feeling sad, I control my crying
Anxiousa Nonanxiousb
and carrying on”) actually may reflect a nonadaptive re-
Emotion M SD M SD sponse for girls if their goal is to obtain support or assis-
Worry 2.72 .66c 3.06 .64c
tance from their social context.
Sadness 2.82 .66c 3.33 .55c The hypothesis that children with an AD would ex-
Anger 2.86 .73d 3.33 .66d perience their emotions more intensely than children
without an AD was supported for the worry and anger
Note: Higher scores indicate greater self-efficacy. Means in the
same column with the same superscripts differ significantly at p <
.05. 2We thank one of the reviewers for this explanation for the find-
an = 25 for all emotions. bn = 26 for all emotions. ing that all girls reported less adaptive coping than did boys.

756
EMOTION REGULATION IN ANXIOUS CHILDREN

scenarios and was in the expected direction for the sad- span. Third, only mothers participated. Given that fa-
ness scenario, although it did not reach significance. thers play a role in children’s emotional development
These findings nicely dovetail research indicating that (Goodman, Brogan, Lynch, & Fielding, 1993), it will
hyperarousal is a distinguishing feature of anxiety be important to include father data in future research.
(Clark & Watson, 1991), and, as such, children with an Fourth, much of the emotion-regulation data was based
AD may be overly sensitive to emotional cues in their on self-report with its inherent limitations. Finally, the
environment. sample size was small, which may have decreased the
It was further hypothesized that intensity of emo- power needed to detect sex differences.
tional experience would relate to the decision to ex- These findings provide evidence for the argument
press emotion. For the AD group, intensity of emo- that emotion regulation needs to be considered in re-
tional experience was positively related to the decision search with atypical populations. Children with an AD
to express sad and angry feelings to their mothers, and their mothers reported difficulties with adaptive
whereas for the NAD group, emotional intensity was emotion regulation, potentially due to their report of
positively correlated to the decision to display all three experiencing emotions intensely and having little con-
emotions. Indeed, this is consistent with normative re- fidence in their regulation ability. Further, children
search, which has found that children endorse high with an AD not only reported difficulty managing anx-
emotional intensity as an appropriate reason to express iety-related experiences, but also sadness- and an-
emotions (Saarni, 1999). ger-provoking experiences. From a functionalist per-
Interestingly, for the AD group, the relation be- spective, it would be important to determine how the
tween worry intensity and the decision to express the demands of the social context impact the emotion-reg-
emotion was nonsignificant. It could be that the rules ulation goals and strategies chosen by children with an
for expressing worry, or other anxiety-like emotions, AD. It may be that the expressive difficulties docu-
are more complicated in families with an anxious mented in this study actually reflect the adoption of
child. Indeed, there is a growing body of both theoreti- adaptive management strategies in response to a long
cal and empirical literature implicating familial pro- history of socialization pressures to express emotion in
cesses in the development and maintenance of child- nonnormative ways. Future research needs to examine
hood anxiety (e.g., Barrett, et al., 1996; Dadds & Roth, the role of emotion socialization in children with ADs.
2001; Gruner, Muris, & Merckelbach, 1999; Rapee,
1997, 2001; Woodruff-Borden, Morrow, Bourland, &
Cambron, 2002).
The hypothesis that children with an AD would per- References
ceive themselves as significantly less emotionally
self-efficacious than children without an AD was sup- Albano, A. M., Chorpita, B. F., & Barlow, D. H. (1996). Anxiety dis-
ported across the worry, sadness, and anger scenarios. orders. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley (Eds.), Child psycho-
pathology (pp. 196–241). New York: Guilford.
This finding provides a potential contributing explana- Albano, A. M., & Silverman, W. K. (1996). Clinician manual for the
tion for research that has found that anxious children Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM–IV–Child ver-
tend to withdraw from or avoid emotionally arousing sion. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
situations (Barrett et al., 1996; Suveg et al., 2001). American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statisti-
Children who have greater self-efficacy are likely to cal manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Au-
thor.
persevere in difficult situations and develop more Barrett, P. M., Rapee, R. M., Dadds, M. R., & Ryan, S. (1996). Fam-
adaptive coping skills (Bradley, 2000). Furthermore, ily enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive
regardless of diagnostic status, all children perceived children: Threat bias and the FEAR effect. Journal of Abnormal
themselves as significantly less efficacious in the Child Psychology, 24, 187–203.
worry versus the sadness and anger scenarios, which is Blumberg, S. H., & Izard, C. E. (1986). Discriminating patterns
of emotions in 10- and 11-year-old children’s anxiety and de-
consistent with the previous finding that children re- pression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
ported less adaptive coping with worry than sadness 852–857.
and anger. As was the case with coping, it could be that Bradley, S. (1990). Affect regulation and psychopathology: Bridging
children of this age group have had less opportunity to the mind–body gap. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 35,
practice managing worry versus sadness experiences. 540–547.
Bradley, S. (2000). Affect regulation and the development of psy-
Thus, their self-efficacy in worry situations is less de- chopathology. New York: Guilford.
veloped than in sadness and anger situations because Brady, E. U., & Kendall, P. C. (1992). Comorbidity of anxiety and
of their lack of experience, success, or both. depression in children and adolescents. Psychological Bulletin,
Although this study reveals a coherent set of find- 111, 244–255.
ings, there are some potential limitations. First, the Brody, L. R., & Hall, J. A. (2000). Gender and emotion. In M. Lewis
& J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 338–349).
sample was homogenous with respect to ethnicity (i.e., New York: Guilford.
Caucasian) and SES (i.e., middle class). Second, the Cacioppo, J. J., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., &
sample did not include youth across the developmental Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M.

757
SUVEG AND ZEMAN

Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions Hollingshead, A. B. (1975). Four factor index of social status. Un-
(pp. 173–191). New York: Guilford. published manuscript, Yale University.
Campos, J. J., Mumme, D. L., Kermoian, R., & Campos, R. G. Hubbard, J., & Coie, J. (1994). Emotional correlates of social com-
(1994). A functionalist perspective on the nature of emotion. petence in children’s peer relationships. Merrill–Palmer Quar-
Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, terly, 40, 1–20.
59, 284–305. Kovacs, M. (1985). The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI).
Carey, M. P., Gresham, F. M., Ruggiero, L., Faulstich, M. E., & Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 21, 995–998.
Enyart, P. (1987). Children’s Depression Inventory: Construct Kovacs, M. (1992). The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI)
and discriminant validity across clinical and nonreferred (con- manual. North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.
trol) populations. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol- Laurent, J., Catanzaro, S., Joiner, T., Rudolph, K., Potter, K., Lam-
ogy, 55, 755–761. bert, S., et al. (1999). A measure of positive and negative affect
Casey, R. J. (1996). Emotional competence in children with ex- for children: Scale development and preliminary validation.
ternalizing and internalizing disorders. In M. Lewis & M. W. Psychological Assessment, 11, 326–338.
Sullivan (Eds.), Emotional development in atypical children (pp. Lewis, M. (2000). The emergence of human emotions. In M. Lewis
161–184). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp.
Chorpita, B. F. (2002). The tripartite model and dimensions of anxi- 265–281). New York: Guilford.
ety and depression: An examination of structure in a large Lonigan, C. J., Carey, M. P., & Finch, A. J. (1994). Anxiety and de-
school sample. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, pression in children and adolescents: Negative affectivity and
177–190. the utility of self-reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Chorpita, B. F., Albano, A. M., & Barlow, D. H. (1998). The struc- Psychology, 62, 1000–1008.
ture of negative emotions in a clinical sample of children and Lonigan, C. J., Hooe, E. S., David, C. F., & Kistner, J. A. (1999). Pos-
adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107, 74–85. itive and negative affectivity in children: Confirmatory factor
Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., & Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and analysis of a two-factor model and its relation to symptoms of
emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Devel- anxiety and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
opment and Psychopathology, 7, 1–10. Psychology, 67, 374–386.
Clark, L., & Watson, D. (1991). Tripartite model of anxiety and de- Muris, P. (2002). Relationship between self-efficacy and symptoms
pression: Psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications. of anxiety disorders and depression in a normal adolescent sam-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 316–336. ple. Personality and Individual Differences, 32, 337–348.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci- Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Meesters, C., & van den Brand, K.
ences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, (2002). Cognitive development and worry in normal children.
Inc. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 775–787.
Cole, P. M., & Kaslow, N. (1988). Interactional and cognitive strate- Pela, O. A., & Reynolds, C. R. (1982). Cross-cultural application of
gies for affect regulation: Developmental perspective on child- the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety Scale: Normative and
hood depression. In L. B. Alloy (Ed.), Cognitive processes in reliability data for Nigerian primary school children. Psycho-
depression (pp. 310–343). New York: Guilford. logical Reports, 51, 1135–1138.
Cole, P. M., Michel, M., & O’Donnell-Teti, L. (1994). The develop- Rapee, R. M. (1997). The potential role of childrearing practices in
ment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical per- the development of anxiety and depression. Clinical Psychol-
spective. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child De- ogy Review, 17, 47–67.
velopment, 59, 73–100. Rapee, R. M. (2001). The development of generalised anxiety. In M.
Dadds, M. R., & Roth, J. H. (2001). Family processes in the develop- W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psycho-
ment of anxiety problems. In M. W. Vasey & M. R. Dadds pathology of anxiety (pp. 481–503). New York: Oxford Univer-
(Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of anxiety (pp. sity Press.
278–303). New York: Oxford University Press. Reynolds, C. R., & Richmond, B. (1997). Revised Children’s Mani-
Deonandan, R., Campbell, K., Ostbye, T., Tummon, I., & Robertson, fest Anxiety Scale. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological
J. (2000). A comparison of methods for measuring socio-eco- Services.
nomic status by occupation or postal area. Chronic Diseases in Richters, J. E. (1992). Depressed mothers as informants about their
Canada, 21, 1–7. children: A critical review of the evidence for distortion. Psy-
Derogatis, L. R. (1994). Symptom Checklist–90–R (SCL–90–R): Ad- chological Bulletin, 112, 485–499.
ministration, scoring, and procedures manual. Minneapolis, Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New
MN: National Computer Systems. York: Guilford.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., & Spinrad, T. (1998). Parental so- Schaughency, E. A., & Lahey, B. B. (1985). Mothers’ and fathers’
cialization of emotion. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 241–273. perceptions of child deviance: Roles of child behavior, parental
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1992). Emotion regulation and the de- depression, and marital satisfaction. Journal of Consulting and
velopment of social competence. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review Clinical Psychology, 53, 718–723.
of personality and social psychology: Vol. 14. Emotion and so- Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among
cial behavior (pp. 119–150). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. school-age children: The development and validation of a new
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Losoya, S. (1997). Emotional re- criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology, 33, 906–917.
sponding: Regulation, social correlates, and socialization. In Silverman, W. K., & Albano, A. M. (1996). Clinician manual for the
P. Salovey & D. J. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development Anxiety Disorder Interview Schedule for DSM–IV: Child and
and emotional intelligence: Educational implications (pp. Parent Schedule. San Antonio, TX: Graywind.
129–163). New York: Basic Books. Silverman, W. K., & Nelles, W. B. (1988). The Anxiety Disorders In-
Goodman, S., Brogan, D., Lynch, M., & Fielding, B. (1993). Social terview Schedule for Children. Journal of the American Acad-
and emotional competence in children of depressed mothers. emy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 772–778.
Child Development, 64, 516–531. Silverman, W., Saavedra, L., & Pina, A. (2001). Test–retest reliabil-
Gruner, K., Muris, P, & Merckelbach, H. (1999). The relationship ity of anxiety symptoms and diagnoses with the Anxiety Disor-
between anxious rearing behaviours and anxiety disorders ders Interview Schedule for DSM–IV: Child and Parent Ver-
symptomatology in children. Journal of Behaviour Therapy sions. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
and Experimental Psychiatry, 30, 27–35. Psychiatry, 40, 937–944.

758
EMOTION REGULATION IN ANXIOUS CHILDREN

Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2000). A preliminary that there are a lot of kids who are really good at soccer.
study of the emotion understanding of youth referred for treat- You are not sure if you are good enough to make the
ment of anxiety disorders. Journal of Clinical Child Psychol-
ogy, 29, 319–327.
team. This makes you feel WORRIED.
Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Kendall, P. C. (2002). Emotion regulation
Sports–Sad Scenario. You really want to be on the soc-
and understanding: Implications for child psychopathology and
therapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 22, 189–222. cer team so you decide to try out. The next day your
Stegall, S. (2003). Adolescent emotional development: Relations mother goes with you to check the bulletin board
among shame- and guilt-proneness, emotion regulation, and where the names of kids who made the team are listed.
psychopathology. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Univer- When you get there you find out that you didn’t make
sity of Maine, Orono.
the team but that your friends did. This makes you feel
Strauss, C. C., Frame, C. L., & Forehand, R. (1987). Psychosocial
impairment associated with anxiety in children. Journal of SAD.
Clinical Child Psychology, 16, 235–239.
Sports–Mad Scenario. You really want to be on the
Suveg, C., Zeman, J., & Stegall, S. (2001, August). Deficits in emo-
tional competence: Predictions to anxious symptomatology in soccer team so you decide to try out. Your mother goes
childhood. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Ameri- with you to the try-outs. During the try-outs you prac-
can Psychological Association, San Francisco, CA. tice kicking the ball back and forth with another child
Thompson, R. A. (2001). Childhood anxiety disorders from the per- who purposely kicks the ball away from you so that
spective of emotion regulation and attachment. In M. W. Vasey
you cannot kick the ball back. This makes you feel
& M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The developmental psychopathology of
anxiety (pp. 160–182). New York: Oxford University Press. MAD.
Wechsler, D. (1991). WISC–III administration and scoring manual.
Camp–Worried Scenario. You take your child to meet
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
Wechsler, D. (1997). WAIS–III administration and scoring manual. the bus on the first day of summer camp. Your child is
San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. planning to share a seat on the bus and a cabin at camp
Wood, J. J., Piacentini, J. C., Bergman, R. L., McCracken, J., & Bar- with his/her best friend who is supposed to meet
rios, V. (2002). Concurrent validity of the anxiety disorders sec- him/her there. When you arrive at the bus stop, your
tion of the Anxiety Disorders Interview Section for DSM– IV:
child finds out that his/her best friend is unable to go to
Child and Parent Versions. Journal of Clinical Child and Ado-
lescent Psychology, 31, 335–342. camp. Your child doesn’t know ANY of the other chil-
Woodruff-Borden, J., Morrow, C., Bourland, S., & Cambron, S. dren who are going but they all seem to know each
(2002). The behavior of anxious parents: Examining mecha- other. This makes your child feel WORRIED.
nisms of transmission of anxiety from parent to child. Journal
of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 364–374. Camp–Sad Scenario. You take your child to meet the
Zeman, J., & Garber, J. (1996). Display rules for anger, sadness, and bus on the first day of summer camp. When you arrive,
pain: It depends on who is watching. Child Development, 67, your child finds out that some of his/her friends are go-
957–973.
ing but that his/her best friend got sick at the last min-
Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Development
and initial validation of the Children’s Sadness Management ute and can’t go. This makes your child feel SAD.
Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25, 540–547.
Camp–Mad Scenario. You take your child to meet the
Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness reg-
ulation: Predictions to internalizing and externalizing symp- bus on the first day of summer camp. Your child is glad
toms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psy- that he/she gets to be third in line because he/she will
chology, 31, 393–398. get a good seat on the bus. All of a sudden, another
child purposely pushes him/her out of line, which
causes your child to have to move to the end of the line.
Appendix This makes your child feel MAD.

Sports–Worried Scenario. You really want to be on the


soccer team so you decide to try out. Your mother goes Received November 18, 2003
with you to the try-outs. During the try-outs, you think Accepted June 2, 2004

759

View publication stats

You might also like