You are on page 1of 12

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL ANTHROPOLOGY 78:473-484 (1989)

Ethnoarchaeology Among the Efe Pygmies, Zaire: Spatial


Organization of Campsites
JOHN W.FISHER, JR., AND HELEN C. STRICKLAND
Department ofdnthropology, Smithsonian Institution, National Museum o f
Natural History, Washington, DC 20560

KEY WORDS Efe (Pygmies),Hunter-gatherers, Spatial analysis

ABSTRACT This paper examines the physical structure of present-day Efe


Pygmy campsites through study of the relationships between behavior pat-
terns and their material residues. Campsites consist of five main components:
the perimeter, huts, fireplaces, trash heaps, and the central open area enclosed
within the arrangement of huts. Most campsite activities are performed within
the perimeter, and exterior fireplaces associated with huts are the focus of a
variety of daily activities. Refuse is regularly discarded onto trash heaps. Hut
size does not correlate with either the number of occupants or their stature.
Site population can be estimated using a count of huts, although reoccupation
of a recently abandoned camp or a family’s moving from one hut to a new one
during a single occupation can inflate the count of huts relative to the number
of families that lived at the camp at any one time. The spatial organization of
campsites is influenced by environment, physical factors (such as human body
size), and sociocultural phenomena.

Investigating the physical structure of hab- 1980, 1983, 1987; Brooks and Yellen, 1987;
itation sites is an indispensable part of ar- Gould, 1980; Hayden, 1979; O’Connell, 1977,
chaeological inquiry into the lifeways of 1987; Yellen, 1976,1977).
prehistoric hunter-gatherers. Through care- The fruits of ethnoarchaeological research
ful excavation and detailed analysis of the range from documenting the nature of camp-
distribution of artifacts, fireplaces, and other site activities and the resultant material
material remains at ancient habitation sites, record (e.g., preparing food, making im-
archaeologists seek to reconstruct hunter- plements, camp maintenance) to identifying
gatherer behavior throughout the evolution- and elucidating underlying variables that af-
ary history of humankind (Bunn et al., 1980; fect site structure (e.g., environment, subsis-
Cahen et al., 1979; Carr, 1984; Davis, 1983; tence organization). Thus, the usefulness of
Isaac, 1982; Kroll and Isaac, 1984; Leakey, ethnoarchaeology begins with providing eth-
1971; Leroi-Gourhan and BrCzillon, 1972;Re- nographic analogies for particular questions,
her, 1983; Stevenson, 1985; White, 1982). but goes well beyond this in contributing to
Many prehistorians have come to recog- the development and refinement of models,
nize, in recent years, that archaeological in- method, and theory for archaeological re-
quiry aimed at illuminating ancient hunter- search (see, for example, Binford, 1987). Eth-
gatherer societies can benefit richly from the noarchaeological studies have, in some cases,
study of living hunter-gatherers. A major fo- modified or dispelled conventional archaeo-
cus of such ethnoarchaeological research con- logical beliefs and assumptions, and have
cerns the spatial organization, or structure, raised issues that require additional re-
of residential campsites. Hunter-gatherer so- search (O’Connell, 1987).
cieties that have been studied in depth in- This paper presents a preliminary over-
clude the Nunamiut Eskimo of northern view of ethnoarchaeological research con-
Alaska, the !Kung San and other inhabitants ducted from June 1984 through June 1985
of the Kalahari Desert, and various groups
of Australian Aborigines (Binford, 1978, Received May 22,1987; revision accepted May 19,1988.

0 1989 ALAN R. LISS, INC.


474 J.W. FISHER, JR.,AND H.C. STRICKLAND

among Efe Pygmies of the Ituri Forest, Zaire cates approximately the distance from the
(see Fisher, 1987, for a fuller account). The nearest Lese village; the only significant ex-
Efe are of interest for various reasons. The ception is the camp located 25 km from the
number of studied hunter-gatherer groups is road, which lies only 100 m from a Lese
small, and a need exists for a larger sample village.
representing a variety of environments and Four of the camps were inhabited when
adaptations, in order to study the range of mapped; the remainder had been abandoned
variability in adaptive strategies. We need to from a few days to several months earlier.
understand the causes and consequences of Following camp abandonment, vegetation re-
differences between groups living in dissimi- generates in cleared areas, fallen leaves and
lar environments. The tropical rain forest of rainwashed sediments accumulate, trash
the Efe differs greatly from the environment heaps become difficult to discern, and hut
of the Australian Aborigines, the !Kung, and frames and the leaf cover gradually soak up
the Nunamiut; and consequently the Efe, water, rot, and cave in. These processes con-
possessing an adaptive strategy that differs spire to progressively diminish the amount
from the aforementioned groups, add valu- and quality of available data. Thus, hut
able dimensions to the growing body of re- height, for example, could be measured on 95
search. In addition, the Efe have close social of the 201 dwellings, and measurements from
and economic ties with neighboring horticul- which to calculate floor area could be made
turalists, and thus are of special interest to at 115 dwellings. At older sites trash heaps
archaeologists studying situations where could not be distinguished and mapped reli-
prehistoric hunter-gatherers and horticultur- ably, and organic materials had decomposed
alists shared the landscape. and otherwise disappeared. We considered it
important, however, to maintain a balance
MATERIALS AND METHODS between the inhabited and very recently
We mapped 30 Efe campsites, which collec- abandoned camps on the one hand-with
tively contain 201 dwellings. Twenty-eight of their high-quality data-and on the other
the mapped camps are of four Efe bands, and hand the older camps-mapping of which
the remaining two camps each represents yet would allow us to study these degenerative
a different band. Campsites were mapped to processes. When mapping, one or more in-
show the camp perimeter and the location of formants who had lived at the camp usually
huts, fireplaces, trash heaps, food remains, accompanied us, and from them we obtained
implements, clothing, and any other cultural the identity of hut occupants and other rele-
materials a t the site. Most campsites were vant information.
surveyed using an Ushikata Surveying Com- Hut height is measured from the highest
pass (containing a transiting telescope) and a point inside the hut to the ground directly
leveling rod, and the remainder were sur- below that point. Hut floor area (of circular
veyed using a Brunton Pocket Transit and oval huts) is calculated by entering hut
mounted on a tripod and a 50-m measuring length and hut width into the following for-
tape. Certain measurements, such as the lo- mula: pibut length) (hut width)/4. This for-
cation of fireplaces within huts and the dis- mula assumes that the floor area of the huts
tance from hut doorway to the associated has a perfectly symmetrical shape, which is
exterior fire, were made using a pocket tape rarely or never true. However, to measure
measure. Generally, we drafted the maps the bulges and indentations in the floor plan
back at our field station (using a protractor of each hut would have been prohibitively
and an engineer’s scale) within a day or two time-consuming, and the procedure adopted
after each camp survey, and made a follow- here gives results of acceptable accuracy. The
up trip to the camp to field-checkthe maps. floor area of the few rectangular huts is cal-
The sample of mapped camps emphasizes culated by multiplying length by width. Efe
camps located near the road on which the stature measurements come from anthropo-
field station (and many Lese villages) is lo- metry data collected systematically by our-
cated. The frequency distribution of the dis- selves and other project members.
tance from road to camps is as follows: 0-1 We conducted a total of 47.5 hours (discon-
km, 21 camps; 1-2 km, 3 camps; 2-3 km, 2 tinuous) of formal observations on Efe behav-
camps; 9-10 km, 1camp; 12-13 km, 1 camp; ior a t inhabited camps of three bands. The
13-14 km, 1camp; 24-25 km, 1camp, Gen- purpose of these observations was to docu-
erally, the distance from the road also indi- ment both the activities carried out at camp-
SPATIAL ORGANIZATION OF EFE CAMPSITES 475
sites and their material byproducts. The huts (Fig. 1). A sixth component occurs at
observations were made by two observers sit- some sites-one or more shelters that lack
uated separately within the camp, so that all walls.
the people in public areas could be seen si- The camp perimeter at a n inhabited or a
multaneously. Usually the duration of obser- recently abandoned camp is usually well de-
vations was less than 3 hours, to avoid fined. During initial preparation of a camp-
burdening the observers or the subjects. An- site, the inhabitants clear away the
cillary records for each observation episode undergrowth, leaving along the camp edge
include the weather, time of day, number of a n abrupt border of uncleared vegetation.
people present, and arrivals and departures. Camp size ran es from about 44 to 532 m2
The public activities of all the people at camp B
(X = 242.7 m , S.D. = 139.4 m2, N = 19)
were recorded, describing the type of activity, (Table 1). Camp population, in our sample,
where it was performed and for how long, ranges from 2 (the initial population of site I-
tools or implements used, and material by- NP-3 until others moved in) to about 33. Bai-
products generated and their disposition. ley (198545) observed a maximum camp pop-
These formal observations on the spatial ulation of 50. The camp perimeter is roughly
organization of campsites and on activity elliptical.
patterns within camps are augmented by in- Efe build their huts near the site perime-
formal observations made on numerous vis- ter. Huts consist of a sturdy frame of saplings
its to camps during our year of fieldwork. covered with broad leaves. The huts are
dome-shaped, and the floor outline ranges
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION from nearly circular to elliptical. Occasion-
Campsite components ally, Efe build a rectangular hut with a flat,
Efe campsites contain five primary physi- sloping roof. On rare occasions they construct
cal components: the edge (perimeter) of the a small house with wattle-and-daub walls,
camp, huts, fireplaces, trash heaps, and the imitating on a small scale a Lese house. A
central open area within the arrangement of hut usually is inhabited by a nuclear family.

Fig. 1. Map of an Efe campsite, showing the struc-


tural components.
TABLE 1. Data on Efe campsites
Mean distance (m)
Site Site area (m2) No. between each hut Duplicate Area (m2)of Approximate length of
Site area (m2) Population per person of huts and closest neighbor' hut(s) trash heaps occupation (weekd2
A-NB-1 123.69 24 5.15 7 3.84 Yes - -
A-NB-2 148.63 16 9.29 6 3.64 Yes - 11
A-NB-3 336.80 33 10.21 11 4.64 Yes - 3
A-NB-4 284.31 30 9.48 9 3.30 No 82.5 3-4
A-NE-1 - - - 9 3.90 - 97.4 ?, 13
A-NE-2 180.33 16 11.27 5 8.89 Yes 35.6 2
A-NE-3 92.16 - - 6 2.77 - - -
A-NE-4 - - - - - - - -
A-NE-5 226.56 21 10.79 6 5.40 Yes 76.5 4,y
A-NE-6 - 18 - 6 4.25 No - -
A-NG-1 476.00 21 22.67 10 5.39 Yes 215.0 Many4
A-NG-2 104.57 13 8.04 5 3.28 No - 12
A-NG-3 126.63 20 6.33 - - Yes - 12
A-NG-4 200.40 15 13.36 7 4.62 Yes - 16
A-NG-5 - 19 - 7 5.10 No - 3
A-NG-7 205.53 - - - - - - -
A-NP-1 - 14 - 6 6.82 Yes 113.2 11
A-NP-3 312.45 14 22.32 4 9.09 Yes 71.7 ?, 3
A-NP-5 440.33 23 19.14 8 5.68 No 75.5 4
A-NP-6 214.67 14 15.33 6 4.35 Yes 19.3 2
A-NP-7 401.05 21 19.10 6 6.44 Yes 34.5 1,4 1
I-NP-2 - 18 - 4 4.28 No 25.3 9b
I-NP-3 - 6 - 3 8.97 No 30.2 106
A-NR-1 43.87 10 4.39 3 3.08 No 4.6 3
A-NR-2 160.53 23 6.98 8 3.64 No 28.2 3,3,4
I-NR-2 - 17 - - - - - ?, 1
A-NT-1 531.73 30 17.72 16 3.65 Yes 98.4 16
A-NT-2 - - - 6 - - - -
A-BK-1 - - - 6 6.60 - - -
A-KE-1 - - - 4 3.40 - - -
'Measured from hut center. Includes lean-tos used for sleeping.
'Multiple entries for individual sites indicate reoccupation.
'Question mark (?I indicates occuuation of unknown leneth.
-
4Site A-NG-1 was occupied at least three times.
'This figure gives length of occupation at the time of mapping (shortly before we left the field). The inhabitants still occupied this camp when our fieldwork ended; thus, this figure
does not indicate the full length of occupation
'See footnote 5 .
SPATIAL ORGANIZATION OF EFE CAMPSITES 477
Efe spend much of the daytime outside and have little open ground except around the
use the hut to escape strong sunlight or rain, fires, whereas more spacious camps offer con-
and at night to sleep in. The hut doorway siderable room within the central open area.
faces in toward the center of the camp, or
occasionally somewhat sideways toward a Campsite activities
neighbor. Behaviors carried out at Efe campsites in-
Efe burn fires both within the dwelling and clude preparing and eating food, making and
outside near the doorway. Efe fires consist maintaining implements, camp mainte-
simply of several logs pushed together, upon nance, child care, self care, and leisure
which cooking pots are placed. Most huts (relaxing, smoking, playing musical instru-
have a single interior fire, but a few have ments, and playing games). Bailey (1985)and
two, or even three. The interior fire provides Peacock (1985) analyze in detail the time al-
warmth at night, and a place to cook during location of Efe men and women.
the day if rain or strong sunlight drives the Efe perform the majority of campsite activ-
people inside. ities within the camp perimeter. The only
All camps have trash heaps. Trash heaps habitual exceptions include playing games
lie along the camp perimeter, beside and be- and preparing poison-tipped arrows. Some
hind huts. Some camps have, in addition, a camps have a nearby playground, a clearing
trash heap in the central open area; such with a vine swing, where children, and occa-
trash heaps form around the base of a tree. sionally adults, play. Children also play
Trash heap formation begins with prepara- within the camp perimeter. When men apply
tion of the campsite, as cleared forest vege- poison to arrows, which they do by painting
tation is piled along the edge of the camp. the brown, liquid poison on the tips of bun-
While they occupy the camp, the inhabitants dles of arrows and placing the tips over a bed
discard debris onto the trash heaps: ashes of coals, they often select a location outside
from fireplaces, debris from food preparation of the camp perimeter to build a fire and
and consumption (such as vegetable peels, carry this out, for reasons of safety.
corn cobs, palm oil nuts, and animal bones), Efe usually sit next to the exterior fires
debris from tool manufacture and mainte-.. associated with the huts when they carry out
nance (such as wood shavings from carving campsite activities. Thus, these fires are the
arrow shafts), broken or worn-out imple- focus around which a great variety of activi-
ments (such as baskets, pottery sherds, and ties take place, including food preparation
broken bottles), and leaf bedding. (such as peeling vegetables, cooking), eating,
The longer that a site is occupied, the making or maintaining implements (such as
greater the amount of trash that is discarded, men carving arrow shafts, and sharpening
and generally the more extensive the area metal-tipped arrows), and socializing. Efe
encompassed by trash (Table 1). The average often intersperse work with relaxation.
length of campsite occupation, in our sample, Activities requiring considerable space,
is about 6 weeks. Sites occupied about 2 such as pounding rice with a large, wooden
weeks or less contain an average of 5.4m2 of mortar and pestle, are sometimes carried out
trash per hut (3 sites), whereas sites occupied away from the fire. Gutting and butchering
for about 4 or more weeks contain an average prey, if not accomplished during the hunt in
of 11.2 m2 of trash per hut (eight sites). the forest, are done on trash heaps or else-
Within both of these categories of sites, the where on the camp periphery.
area (m2) of trash per hut varies consider- Camp maintenance practices have critical
ably. This variability occurs, at least in part, archaeological significance. Although debris
because trash heaps grow vertically as well from camp activities (such as vegetable peels
as horizontally. The amount of lateral growth or wood shavings) often falls to the ground
probably is influenced and constrained by where the activity takes place, most such
the amount of space available beside and debris subsequently will be cleaned up and
behind huts to discard rubbish. Conse- discarded onto trash heaps.
quently, the height (vertical thickness) of Efe trash disposal practices have the fol-
trash heaps varies between sites. lowing archaeological consequences. Trash
The central open area consists of the camp heaps at Efe camps are generalized in con-
space (including where exterior fires are tent; that is, they contain a variety of differ-
placed) enclosed within the arrangement of ent kinds of rubbish, and are not segregated
huts. Some camps are relatively compact and t o receive specialized debris. Size sorting of
478 J.W. FISHER, JR.,AND H.C. STRICKLAND

debris can occur, wherein small items are height yields a coefficient of determination
sometimes overlooked during cleanup and do of 0.031 [N = 651). This is, perhaps, surpris-
not make it to the trash heap. These mate- ing, in that the method of hut construction
rials are vulnerable to scuffing about and allows flexibility in height, and one might
trampling by humans, dogs, and chickens. expect the woman building it to adjust the
The size of items in trash heaps ranges from height to her stature or that of her spouse. In
small to large. At abandoned camps, the ar- practice, however, a woman might build one
tifacts and movable debris (such as vegetable hut 14 cm shorter than her standing height,
peels) that lie near fireplaces and elsewhere and another one that exceeds her stature by
in the central open area generally had been 20 cm. There appear to be two physical limi-
deposited during the last days or hours of tations to the height of a hut: it must be
camp occupation. This comes about because taller than the occupants’ sitting height, and
the inhabitants, with foreknowledge that a it cannot be so tall so as to extend beyond the
move is imminent, apparently do not bother woman’s reach as she puts the leaf cover on
cleaning up the debris from their final activ- the frame (in practice, hut height does not
ities. Hence, the debris and artifacts found in approach the vertical limits of the woman’s
the central open area of abandoned camps reach). Within these bounds, Efe apparently
document activities performed during the feel considerable flexibility regarding the
terminal period of camp occupation. acceptable height of their hut. Although the
relationship of hut height to the stature of
Domestic space: Within and outside of the occupants might seem to have little direct
dwellings archaeological relevance, this relationship
Several aspects of site structure have im- gives insights into the design and use of do-
portance for archaeologists. Two of these en- mestic space (see Oliver, 1987), which are
tail how Efe create and use the domestic topics of considerable relevance to
space of their dwellings. Our sample of huts archaeologists.
shows considerable variation in both floor The distance from the doorway of a hut to
x
area (range = 1.3-13.6 m2, = 5.1 m2, S.D. the associated exterior fire is important both
= 2.0 m , N = 115) and maximum height behaviorally and with respect to site struc-
(range = 90-172 cm, X = 137.9 cm, S.D. = ture. The behavioral significance lies in the
15.3 cm, N=95). The number of people that importance of the exterior fire as the focus of
live in a hut ranges from one to eight. The most campsite activities. At Efe camps, this
floor area of a hut correlates very weakly distance ranges from 0 to 325 cm (X = 139.5
with the number of occupants (regression cm, S.D. = 67.4 cm, N = 89). This spacing
analysis of hut floor area to number of hut seems to be governed largely by physical fac-
occupants yields a coefficient of determina- tors of body size: it allows a person to keep
tion of 0.061 [N = 1021). This contrasts with within convenient distance of the hut while
expectations raised by Binford‘s (1983:173) maintaining sflicient space t o work and
assertion, based on observations among move around the fire without blocking the
Bushmen of southern Africa, that dwelling hut doorway (see Binford, 1983:173).Women
size varies with the number of occupants. It sometimes sit leaning against the outside of
seems that Efe have very flexible perceptions their hut while working at their fire. The
regarding a person’s space requirements. availability of space, and individual prefer-
Another causal factor could be that mobility ences, might also influence significantly the
between huts is fairly high, particularly distance from hut doorway t o fire. Fifty-eight
among children. This can create problems percent of exterior fires lie between 50 and
when counting the number of hut occupants; 150 cm from the hut doorway, suggesting
but more significantly, it speaks of a dy- that within these limits lies the optimum
namic relationship between dwelling size and spacing.
the number of occupants, and of a perception Archaeologists have long been concerned
of spatial requirements that is flexible. with devising methods for estimating the
The maximum height of huts has no corre- population of prehistoric communities using
lation with the stature of the adult occupants the size of dwelling or of sites (e.g., Kolb,
(regressionanalysis of the stature of the adult 1985; LeBlanc, 1971; Naroll, 1962; Wiessner,
female occupants to hut height yields a coef- 1974). Among the Efe, the number of huts at
ficient of determination of 0.005 [N = 631, a campsite has the potential to provide a
stature of the adult male occupant to hut reasonably precise estimate of group size. On
SPATIAL ORGANIZATION OF EFE CAMPSITES 479

average, a hut is occupied by three people (X


= 3.18, S.D. = 1.39, N [huts] = 145, 95%
confidence interval = 3.18 f 0.229). To esti-
mate the number of site inhabitants, one
multiplies the confidence interval by the
number of huts. For example, we would ex-
10
-0 pect a site with six huts to have between 17.7
m and 20.5 occupants. A potential difficulty, to
Fig. 2. Map of a campsite (A-NE-2)built immediately be discussed shortly, has to do with how a
after a disagreement within the group. (The pair of struc- simple count of huts can yield an overesti-
tures at each end were each used by a single household.) mate of the number of households that had
lived at the site.
The distance from a hut to its closest neigh-

3 bor varies greatly; in some cases two huts


almost touch one another, while other huts
are separated from their closest neighbor by
nearly 20 m. The average distance from a

0 hut to its closest neighbor, measured from


hut center, is slightly under 5 m. The dis-
tance that people use to separate their huts,
and the use of subtle barriers, such as inter-
vening trees or the direction the doorway
Fig. 3. Three separate huts, within a larger site (A- faces, are strongly influenced by kinship ties
NB-4) that the inhabitants joined together into a single
structure.

Fig. 4. Dwellings at four campsites occupied by the


same band (lean-tos not shown). One hut in each camp
0
- m
10

family is consistently slightly isolated from the other


huts. Clockwise from upper left: A-NP-3,A-NP-5,A-NP-
(drawn with a thick line) that was inhabited by the same 7, and A-NP-6.
480 J.W. FISHER, JR.,AND H.C. STRICKLAND

0
- m
3

Fig. 5. A campsite (A-NP-5) inhabited by two bands thicknesses). Each band maintains its dwellings in a
(the huts of each band are drawn with lines of different separate cluster.

and interpersonal relationships. Four exam- camp layout. The three huts in Figure 3
ples will illustrate this point. (which are part of a larger camp) were occu-
The camp shown in Figure 2, whose linear pied by a middle-aged man and his wife, this
shape is abnormal for Efe camps, illustrates man’s widowed sister, and a third widow.
the effect on camp layout of intracamp quar- This man was a source of support for these
relling. The death of a young girl from this women. After he died, the three women
band, in the previous camp, had engendered joined together their three separate huts into
bitterness and tension within the group. a single, large structure by building walls
These feelings are reflected through the use between the open spaces separating the huts.
of forest vegetation and long distances to iso- Shortly afterwards, the inhabitants aban-
late each household (the pair of structures in doned this camp.
the small clearings at the two ends of the site The four sites shown in Figure 4 were in-
were each used by a single household). Ten- habited, in different parts of the forest, by
sion is exemplified, in addition, by the phys- the same band. The hut drawn with a heavy
ical separation between two particular line in each camp was occupied by the same
households: that of a middle-aged man and family, and in all these camps (and others)
his wife at one end of the camp and their son this hut is somewhat isolated. This isolation
and his wife at the opposite end. Normally, seems to be a result of individual preference.
the huts of a father and his adult son often This family gets along well with the rest of
will be situated side by side. (Likewise,adult the group, but the wife is a quiet, reserved
brothers living in the same camp often live woman, and probably favors distancing her-
in adjacent, closely spaced huts.) self slightly from her neighbors.
Distress brought about by the death of a The final example concerns a camp that
person can have quite the opposite effect on was occupied during the flying termite sea-
SPATIAL ORGANIZATION OF EFE CAMPSITES 481
son by two bands that normally lived sepa- range means that they reuse formerly used
rately (Fig. 5). Even though they are all areas fairly regularly. When the hunting and
friends, each band maintains its huts in a honey seasons come to an end, they have a
separate cluster that emphasizes their sepa- predetermined area to move back to-the vi-
rate identity. cinity of the Lese village, where their re-
Each of the above examples has important cently abandoned camps await. Reinhabited
archaeological implications regarding where, camps apparently possess desirable ecologi-
within the campsite, material remains will cal or other factors that make them espe-
be deposited during the course of occupation. cially attractive; these features probably
The location of dwellings determines, to a include proximity to drinking water, a suit-
large extent, the placement of fires and trash able opening or clearing in the forest, a set-
heaps. Thus, the factors that affect decisions ting that provides adequate runoff of rainfall
as to where the inhabitants situate their and that is out of reach of flooding rivers,
dwellings have important consequences on and proximity to Lese villages and gardens,
the distribution of other material remains at honey, or other resources (see Bailey, 1985;
the camp. Peacock, 1985).
Campsite reoccupation EXTERNAL COMPARISONS
Efe often reoccupy a recently abandoned Comparisons with other present-day hunt-
camp (Table 1).When a band does so, some er-gatherers disclose differences and similar-
huts from the first occupation will be rein- ities in campsite structure and activity pat-
habited, but often, some families construct a terns, and help to identify factors that
new hut. Archaeologists would be hard account for the manner in which hunter-
pressed to separate these different occupa- gatherers organize and use space at their
tion episodes on stratigraphic evidence. Even habitation sites.
during the course of a single occupation it is Compared to the !Kung of southern Africa
not unusual for at least one household to and Australian Aborigines of the Western
abandon their first hut and build a second and Central deserts, Efe live in compact
one. These behaviors could lead archaeolo- campsites (see Gould and Yellen, 1987; 0’-
gists to overestimate the number of house- Connell, 1987; Yellen, 1977).Habitation sites
holds that had lived at the site, if that of the Alyawara of Australia, for example,
estimate is based on a count of huts. Fifty- range in size from IO,OOO m2 to over ~OO,OOO
nine percent of the 22 campsites we mapped m2 (O’Connell, 1987). Among the Efe, envi-
for which we have complete information on ronment has a significant influence on camp
the identity of the hut occupants contained size (area), in particular the trees and under-
one or more of these “duplicate” huts (Table brush of the forest. Small camps facilitate
1).At sites containing several duplicate huts, traffic within camps (which is considerable)
the density of huts increases and the average as well as oral and visual communication
distance between huts decreases, relative to between households. If an Efe camp were as
conditions prior to adding duplicate huts. large as even a small Alyawara camp, and
That Efe often reoccupy recently aban- individual households were to space them-
doned camps appears to be a consequence of selves evenly near the perimeter, each house-
four factors: high residential mobility, the hold would live out of sight and sound of its
identification of each band with a particular neighbors. In the deserts of Australia and
segment of the forest, their close ties with southern Africa, vegetation presents less of a
the Lese, and the ecological setting of indi- barrier.
vidual sites. For about 7 months of the year Fear of natural predators has been identi-
Efe campsites are situated close to Lese vil- fied by Gould and Yellen (1987) as an impor-
lages and gardens (usually less than an tant factor regulating the distance between
hour’s walk away) and during the other households. They point out that in Australia,
months, the hunting and honey seasons, they which lacks predators dangerous to humans,
move into the forest away from the villages. the distance between households is much
During the latter period they move camp greater than is the case among the !Kung,
about every 2 weeks, but while living near whose environment contains several species
villages the frequency of camp moves de- of predators. Efe do not, to our knowledge,
creases (Bailey, 1985). When living away fear predators. However, the dense forest
from the villages the limitations on their vegetation discussed above is more impor-
482 J.W. FISHER, JR., AND H.C. STRICKLAND

tant than the absence of fear of predators in cently abandoned camps, even reinhabiting
determining site size. former dwellings.
We have stressed the importance of inter- Camp maintenance and redepositing of re-
personal relationships and kinship ties in in- fuse from its initial location onto trash heaps
fluencing the distance between dwellings and or other secondary locations occurs widely
other aspects of campsite layout among the (Binford, 1983,1987; Hitchcock, 1987; O’Con-
Efe. The same has been documented among nell, 1987). The performance of site mainte-
Mbuti Pygmies elsewhere in the Ituri Forest nance has been linked to length of occupation
(Putnam, 1948; Turnbull, 1961, 1965). The (Hitchcock, 1987; O’Connell, 1987:lOO). We
placement of huts at !Kung camps generally suggest that site size also is relevant; if Efe
corresponds to extended families (Yellen, failed to carry out camp maintenance, even
197799; see also Gould and Yellen, 1987). at sites of short occupation, refuse would
Efe, !Kung, and Australian Aborigines are quickly accumulate in the relatively small
similar in that they carry out a wide variety central open area and become a nuisance.
of different tasks in the household area. Refuse disposal at Alyawara campsites cre-
Among the Efe and !Kung, the exterior fire ates sue sorting of debris: smaller items tend
associated with the hut is the primary focus to remain at the activity areas and larger
of these activities (see Brooks and Yellen, items are redeposited (O’Connell, 1987).Sim-
1987; Yellen, 19771, and among the Alyawara ilarly, at Efe sites the items most likely to be
this focus encompasses a cleared area, a main missed or overlooked during cleanup are
shelter, other structures, and fireplaces (0’- small.
Connell, 1987).
Differences arise in that the !Kung and
CONCLUSIONS
Australian Aborigines use space outside of
the camp perimeter t o a greater extent than By way of concluding, we want t o reflect
do the Efe. The nature of the vegetation prob- briefly on what, if anything, prehistorians
ably exerts a strong influence on the extent can learn from present-day hunter-gatherer
to which people use space outside of the camp. campsites that might help t o disentangle the
The Efe must clear underbrush and saplings archaeological record. Hunter-gatherer soci-
to prepare their campsite. This results in a eties can differ profoundly in the spatial or-
clearly demarcated camp perimeter, and the ganization of their settlements, and
uncleared space outside of the perimeter explaining these dissimilarities challenges
would be an uncomfortable and impractical archaeologists’ abilities to trace out compli-
place to carry out campsite activities. In con- cated networks of causal factors. Compari-
trast, the campsites of the !Kung and Austra- sons can profitably be made of the spatial
lian Aborigines, “lacked delimited organization at campsites of hunter-gather-
boundaries or other defining features” (Gould ers adapted to disparate environments, in
and Yellen, 1987:82), and desert vegetation order to explore the relationships between
presents fewer obstacles to impede activities settlement organization (including underly-
(see O’Connell, 1987). ing behavior patterns such as the location
A noteworthy dissimilarity between Efe where daily activities are performed, group
and !Kung campsites has to do with the pres- size, mobility, and subsistence practices) and
ence of fires inside of dwellings. Virtually all factors such as climate, landscape, and food
Efe dwellings contain one (or more) fires, but resources.
among the !Kung the opposite is true (see Comparisons can be made at two scales-
Yellen, 1977:campsitemaps). !Kung carry out macro and micro. Macrolevel comparisons
few activities within huts, and even sleep are concerned with overall settlement size;
outside except during rainstorms (Yellen, the microlevel entails examining the use of
1976:64). Efe use their huts during the day space within campsites: the distance separat-
to escape rain and strong sun, and to sleep in ing dwellings, and the spatial relationships
at night. between huts, fireplaces, refuse, and other
The proclivity to reoccupy recently aban- site components. Differences that are appar-
doned camps differs between Efe and !Kung. ent at the macrolevel seem t o result, in large
!Kung avoid reoccupying abandoned dry-sea- part, from environmental dissimilarities. Mi-
son camps, though they do sometimes rein- crolevel attributes of camps are responsive to
habit rainy-season sites (Brooks and Yellen, environmental, physical (e.g., body size), and
1987:69, 881, whereas Efe often return t o re- sociocultural factors.
SPATIAL ORGANIZATION OF EFE CAMPSITES 483
Our experiences stimulate, in addition, re- LITERATURE CITED
flections of a more abstract nature. The life- Bailey RC (1985) The Socioecology of Efe Pygmy Men in
way of the Efe whom we studied is not the Ituri Forest, Zaire. Ph.D. Thesis, Harvard Univer-
immutable. Contact with the outside world, sity, Cambridge.
both direct and indirect, has brought impor- Binford LR (1978) Dimensional analysis of behavior and
tant changes in subsistence and technology site structure: Learning from an Eskimo hunting stand.
Am. Antiq. 43:330-361.
during the last few centuries and even gen- Binford LR (1980)Willow smoke and dogs' tails: Hunter-
erations. For example, cassava and certain gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site
other crops that are important foods of the formation. Am. Antiq. 45:4-20.
Lese and Efe were introduced from outside of Binford LR (1983) In Pursuit of the Past. London: Thames
Africa (Purseglove, 1976); and the Lese and and Hudson.
Efe first acquired implements of aluminum, Binford LR (1987) Researching ambiguity: Frames of
reference and site structure. In S Kent (ed.): Method
steel, and plastic even more recently. The and Theory for Activity Area Research An Ethnoar-
precise mix of economic, technological, and chaeological Approach. New York: Columbia, pp. 449-
material circumstances by which the Efe to- 512.
day live never existed in the past and will Brooks AS, and Yellen JE (1987) The preservation of
activity areas in the archaeological record Ethnoar-
continue to undergo transformation. Thus, chaeological and archaeological work in northwest
some archaeologists might argue that analo- Ngamiland, Botswana. In S Kent (ed.): Method and
gies from Efe to prehistoric hunter-gatherers Theory for Activity Area Research An Ethnoarchaeo-
would be misleading at best. We believe, logical Approach. New York: Columbia, pp. 63-106.
Bunn H, Harris JWK, Isaac G, Kaufulu Z, Kroll E,
however, that understanding the patterns of Schick K, Toth N, and Behrensmeyer AK (1980) Fx-
Efe behavior, site formation, and site struc- Jj50: An early Pleistocene site in northern Kenya.
ture can help to guide archaeological in- World Archaeol. 12: 109-139.
quiry. We do not expect to find identical Cahen D, Keeley LH, and Van Noten FL (1979) Stone
patterns at campsites from earlier times or tools, toolkits, and human behavior in prehistory. Curr.
Anthropol. 20:661-683.
in different parts of the world. We do hope to Carr C (1984) The nature of organization of intrasite
discern the underlying organization of the archaeological records and spatial analytic approaches
Efe system and relate this to its material to their investigation. In MB Schiffer (ed.): Advances
manifestations (see Binford, 1987). By learn- in Archaeological Method and Theory 7. Orlando: Ac-
ing to recognize systematic relationships be- ademic, pp. 103-222.
Davis LB (1983) Stone circles in the Montana Rockies:
tween behavior and its material mani- relict households and transitory communities. In LB
festations among modern hunter-gatherers, Davis (ed.): From Microcosm to Macrocosm: Advances
prehistorians can gain rewarding insights to in Tipi Ring Investigation and Interpretation. Lincoln:
help guide their search for answers to basic Plains Anthropol. Memoir. 19, pp. 235-278.
questions about the past. Fisher JW, Jr. (1987) Shadows in the Forest: Ethnoar-
chaeology Among the Efe Pygmies. Ph.D. Thesis, Uni-
versity of California, Berkeley.
Gould RA (1980) Living Archaeology. Cambridge: Cam-
bridge University Press.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Guuld RA, and Yellen JE (1987) Man the hunted Deter-
This research was funded by National Sci- minants of household spacing in desert and tropical
foraging societies. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 6:77-103.
ence Foundation grants BNS8306620 and Hayden B (1979) Paleolithic Reflections: Lithic Technol-
BNS8319629 to Irven DeVore and Peter Elli- ogy of the Australian Western Desert. Canberra: Aus-
son. We are grateful for cooperation and per- tralian Institute Aboriginal Studies.
mission from the Institut des Musees Hitchcock RK (1987) Sedentism and site structure: Or-
ganizational changes in Kalahari Basarwa residential
Nationaux du Zaire and the Commissaire locations. In S Kent (ed.): Method and Theory for Activ-
d'Etat des Culture et Artes, Zaire, and of ity Area Research An Ethnoarchaeological Approach.
CEPLANUT. We thank Gurcharan Khanna New York Columbia, pp. 374-423.
and Tom Pilgram for considerable assistance Isaac GL (1982)The earliest archaeological traces. In JD
in the analysis and interpretation of quanti- Clark (ed.): The Cambridge History of Africa, Vol. I,
From the Earliest Times to c. 500 B.C. Cambridge:
tative data, and Robert Bailey for valuable Cambridge University Press, pp. 157-247.
comments on a previous draft of this manu- Kolb CC (1985) Demographic estimates in archaeology:
script. Leslie B. Davis also read a previous Contributions from ethnoarchaeology on Mesoameri-
draft of this manuscript. We thank the many can peasants. Curr. Anthropol. 26:581-599.
people who gave us logistical support and Kroll EM, and Isaac GL (1984) Configurations of arti-
facts and bones at early Pleistocene sites in East Af-
warm hospitality during our fieldwork, and rica. In H Hietala (ed.): Intrasite Spatial Analysis in
we are especially grateful to the Lese and Archaeology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press,
Efe among whom we lived and worked. pp. 4-31.
484 J.W. FISHER, JR.,AND H.C. STRICKLAND

Leakey MD (1971) Olduvai Gorge, Volume 3, Excava- York Henry Holt, pp. 322-342.
tions in Beds I and 11, 1960-1963. Cambridge: Cam- Reher CA (1983) Analysis of spatial structure in stone
bridge University Press. circle sites. In LB Davis (ed.): From Microcosm to Ma-
LeBlanc S (1971) An addition to Naroll's suggested floor crocosm: Advances in Tipi Ring Investigation and
area and settlement population relationship. Am. An- Interpretation. Lincoln Plains Anthropol. Memoir vol.
tiq. 36: 210-211. 19, pp. 193-222.
Leroi-Gourhan A, and Brezillon M (1972) Fouilles de Stevenson MG (1985) The formation of artifact assem-
Pincevent, Essai d'Analyse Ethnographique d'un Hab- blages at workshophabitation sites: Models from Peace
itat Magdalenien. Paris: C.N.R.S. Point in northern Alberta. Am. Antiq. 50:63-81.
Naroll R-(1962) Floor area and settlement population. Turnbull CM (1961) The Forest People: A Study of the
Am. Antiq. 27:587589. Pygmies of the Congo. New York: Simon and Schuster.
O'Connell JF (1977) Room to move: Contemporary Alya- Turnbull CM (1965)Wayward Servants: The Two Worlds
wara settlement patterns. Mankind 11:119-131. of the African Pygmies. Garden City: Natural History.
O'Connell JF (1987) Alyawara site structure and its ar- White R (1982) Rethinking the Middlempper Paleolithic
chaeological implications. Am. Antiq. 5274-108. transition. Curr. Anthropol. 23:169-192.
Oliver P (1987) Dwellings: The House Across the World. Wiessner P (1974) A functional estimate of population
Austin: University of Texas Press. from floor area. Am. Antiq 39:343-350.
Peacock N R (1985) Time Allocation, Work and Fertility Yellen JE (1976) Settlement patterns of the !Kung: An
Among Efe Pygmy Women of Northeast Zaire. Ph.D. archaeological perspective. In RB Lee and I DeVore
Thesis, Harvard University, Cambridge. (eds.): Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers: Studies of the !Kung
Purseglove JW (1976) The origins and migrations of crops San and Their Neighbors. Cambridge: Harvard Uni-
in tropical Africa. In JR Harlan, J M J de Wet, and ABL versity, pp. 43-72.
Stemler (eds.): Origins of African Plant Domestication. Yellen JE (1977) Archaeological Approaches to the Pres-
The Hague: Mouton, pp. 291-309. ent: Models for Reconstructing the Past. New York:
Putnam P (1948) The Pygmies of the Ituri Forest. In CS Academic Press.
Coon (ed.): A Reader in General Anthropology. New

You might also like