You are on page 1of 11

Psychological

pricing

Psychological pricing (also price ending, charm pricing) is a pricing and marketing strategy
based on the theory that certain prices have a psychological impact. In this pricing method, retail
prices are often expressed as just-below numbers: numbers that are just a little less than a
round number, e.g. $19.99 or £2.98.[1] There is evidence that consumers tend to perceive just-
below prices (also referred to as "odd prices") as being lower than they actually are, tending to
round to the next lowest monetary unit.[2] Thus, prices such as $1.99 may to some degree be
associated with spending $1 rather than $2. The theory that drives this is that pricing practices
such as this cause greater demand than if consumers were perfectly rational. Psychological
pricing is one cause of price points.

Example of psychological pricing at a gas station


Overview

According to a 1997 study published in the Marketing Bulletin, approximately 60% of prices in
advertising material ended in the digit 9, 30% ended in the digit 5, 7% ended in the digit 0 and the
remaining seven digits combined accounted for only slightly over 3% of prices evaluated.[3] In
the UK, before the withdrawal of the halfpenny coin in 1969, prices often ended in 111⁄2d
(elevenpence halfpenny: just under a shilling, which was 12d); another example (before 1961)
was £1/19/11¾d. (one pound, nineteen shillings and elevenpence three farthings) which is one
farthing under £2. This is still seen today in gasoline (petrol) pricing ending in 9⁄10 of the local
currency's smallest denomination; for example in the US the price of a gallon of gasoline almost
always ends in US$0.009 (e.g. US$2.199).

Digit
Proportion in the 1997

ending Marketing Bulletin study

0 7.5%  

1 0.3%  

2 0.3%  

3 0.8%  

4 0.3%  

5 28.6%  

6 0.3%  

7 0.4%  

8 1.0%  

9 60.7%  

In a traditional cash transaction, fractional pricing imposes tangible costs on the vendor
(printing fractional prices), the cashier (producing awkward change) and the customer (stowing
the change). These factors have become less relevant with the increased use of checks, credit
and debit cards and other forms of currency-free exchange; also, the addition of sales tax makes
the pre-tax price less relevant to the amount of change (although in Europe the sales tax is
generally included in the shelf price).

The psychological pricing theory is based on one or more of the following hypotheses:

Thomas and Morwitz (2005) coined the term "left-digit effect" and suggested that this bias is
caused by the use of an anchoring heuristic in multi-digit comparisons.

Another rationale for just-below pricing is prospect theory. This theory holds that "consumers
facing uncertainty in decision making, base the value of an alternative on gains or losses
offered by the alternative, relative to some reference point, rather than on final absolute states
of wealth or welfare." The theory also incorporates evidence that small deviations from a
reference point tend to be over-valued. So, based on prospect theory, pricing something only a
few cents under a whole dollar could be beneficial. This theory works well because of how the
reference point is established by the consumer. The reference point for something that is
$19.98 would be $20. This leads the just-below price to be seen as involving a gain, thus
making it feel like a better deal.

Consumers ignore the least significant digits rather than do the proper rounding. Even though
the cents are seen and not totally ignored, they may subconsciously be partially ignored. Keith
Coulter, Associate Professor of Marketing at the Graduate School of Management, Clark
University, suggests that this effect may be enhanced when the cents are printed smaller (for
example, $1999).[4]

Fractional prices suggest to consumers that goods are marked at the lowest possible price.

When items are listed in a way that is segregated into price bands (such as an online real
estate search), price ending is used to keep an item in a lower band, to be seen by more
potential purchasers.

The theory of psychological pricing is controversial. Some studies show that buyers, even young
children, have a very sophisticated understanding of true cost and relative value and that, to the
limits of the accuracy of the test, they behave rationally. Other researchers claim that this
ignores the non-rational nature of the phenomenon and that acceptance of the theory requires
belief in a subconscious level of thought processes, a belief that economic models tend to deny
or ignore. Results from research using modern scanner data are mixed.

Now that many customers are used to just-below pricing, some restaurants and high-end
retailers psychologically-price in even numbers in an attempt to reinforce their brand image of
quality and sophistication.[5]
Theories

Kaushik Basu used game theory in 1997 to argue that rational consumers value their own time
and effort at calculation. Such consumers process the price from left to right and tend to
mentally replace the last two digits of the price with an estimate of the mean "cent component"
of all goods in the marketplace. In a sufficiently large marketplace, this implies that any
individual seller can charge the largest possible "cent component" (99¢) without significantly
affecting the average of cent components and without changing customer behavior.[6] Ruffle and
Shtudiner's (2006) laboratory test shows considerable support for Basu's 99-cent pricing
equilibrium, particularly when other sellers' prices are observable.[7]

The introduction of the euro in 2002, with its various exchange rates, distorted existing nominal
price patterns while at the same time retaining real prices. A European wide study (el Sehity,
Hoelzl and Kirchler, 2005) investigated consumer price digits before and after the euro
introduction for price adjustments. The research showed a clear trend towards psychological
pricing after the transition. Further, Benford's Law as a benchmark for the investigation of price
digits was successfully introduced into the context of pricing. The importance of this benchmark
for detecting irregularities in prices was demonstrated and with it a clear trend towards
psychological pricing after the nominal shock of the euro introduction.[8]

Another phenomenon noted by economists is that a price point for a product (such as $4.99)
remains stable for a long period of time, with companies slowly reducing the quantity of product
in the package until consumers begin to notice. At this time, the price will increase marginally (to
$5.05) and then within an exceptionally short time will increase to the next price point ($5.99, for
example).[9]

Several studies have shown that when prices are presented to a prospect in descending order
(versus ascending order) positive effects result, mainly a willingness to pay a higher price, higher
perceptions of value, and higher probability of purchase. The reason for this is that when
presented in the former, the higher price serves as a reference point, and the lower prices are
perceived favorably as a result.[10]

Psychological pricing in consumer behavior

Thomas and Morwitz (2005) suggested that this bias is a manifestation of the pervasive
anchoring heuristic in multi-digit comparisons. (The anchoring heuristic is one of the heuristics
identified by Nobel laureate Kahneman and his co-author Tversky.) Judgments of numerical
differences are anchored on leftmost digits, causing a bias in relative magnitude judgments.[11]
This hypothesis suggests that people perceive the difference between 1.99 and 3.00 to be closer
to 2 than to 1 because their judgments are anchored on the leftmost digit.

Stiving and Winer (1997) examined the left-digit effect using scanner panel models. They
proposed that 9-ending prices can influence consumer behavior through two distinct processes:
image effects and level effects. Image effect suggests that 99-ending prices are associated with
images of sales promotions. Level effect captures the magnitude underestimation caused by
anchoring on leftmost digits of prices. Their results suggest that both of these effects account
for the influence of 9-ending prices in grocery stores.[12] Manning and Sprott (2009)
demonstrated that left-digit anchoring can influence consumer choices using experimental
studies.[1]

Choi, Lee, and Ji (2012) examined the interactive effects of 9-ending prices and message
framing in advertisements. The researchers found that when pairing nine-ending prices with
positive messages, advertisements were much more positively received by consumers. This in
turn increased their likelihood of making a purchase decision.[13]

Psychological pricing in financial markets

Left-digit effect has also been shown to influence stock-market transactions. Bhattacharya,
Holden, and Jacobsen (2011) examined the left-digit effect in stock market transactions. They
found that there was excess buying at just-below prices ($1.99) versus round numbers ($2.00)
right above them. This discrepancy in buy-sell can lead to significant changes in 24-hour returns,
that can meaningfully impact markets.[14]

Psychological pricing in public policy

Research has also found psychological pricing relevant to the study of politics and public
policy.[15] For instance, a study of Danish municipal income taxes found evidence of "odd
taxation" as tax rates with a nine-ending were found to be over-represented compared to other
ending digits.[16] Further, it was found that citizens’ evaluations of public-school districts in a
Danish population changed noticeably based on the leftmost digit. In particular, the researchers
looked at minuscule changes in average grades that shifted the leftmost digit. Once this value
changed, citizens responded more drastically and as such their stance in terms of public policy
on the issue changed.[17]
MacKillop et al. (2014) looked at how the left-digit effect affects the relationship between price
hikes and smoking cessation. There was a very clearly demonstrated inverse relationship
between the price of cigarettes and individual's motivation to smoke. Researchers found that
price hikes that impacted the leftmost digit in the price (i.e. $4.99 vs. $5.00) were particularly
effective in causing change among adult smokers. These findings can be utilized by public
policy researchers and legislators to implement more effective cigarette tax policies.[18]

Regulation

According to Davidovich-Weisberg (2013), in Israel several high-profile regulatory commissions


have joined to ban retailers from charging prices ending in 99. These regulatory bodies have
claimed that this was an attempt to make prices look less expensive to customers. In addition,
due to the phasing out of certain denominations of coins in Israel, these quirky prices also made
little practical sense in terms of everyday shopping.[19]

Historical comments

Exactly how psychological pricing came into common use is not clear, though it is known the
practice arose during the late 19th century. One source speculates it originated in a newspaper
pricing competition. Melville E. Stone founded the Chicago Daily News in 1875, intending to price
it at one cent to compete with the nickel papers of the day. The story claims that pennies were
not common currency at the time, and so Stone colluded with advertisers to set whole dollar
prices a cent lower—thus guaranteeing that customers would receive ample pennies in
change.[20]

Others have suggested that fractional pricing was first adopted as a control on employee theft.
For cash transactions with a round price, there is a chance that a dishonest cashier will pocket
the bill rather than record the sale. For cash transactions with a just-below price, the cashier
must nearly always make change for the customer. This generally means opening the cash
register which creates a record of the sale in the register and reduces the risk of the cashier
stealing from the store owner.[21]

In the former Czechoslovakia, people called this pricing "baťovská cena" ("Baťa's price"), referring
to Tomáš Baťa, a Czech manufacturer of footwear. He began to widely use this practice in
1920.[22]
Price ending has also been used by retailers to highlight sale or clearance items for
administrative purposes. A retailer might end all regular prices in 95 and all sale prices in 50.
This makes it easy for a buyer to identify which items are discounted when looking at a report.

In its 2005 United Kingdom general election manifesto, the Official Monster Raving Loony Party
proposed the introduction of a 99-pence coin to "save on change".[23][24]

A recent trend in some monetary systems is to eliminate the smallest denomination coin
(typically 0.01 of the local currency). The total cost of purchased items is then rounded up or
down to, for example, the nearest 0.05. This may have an effect on future just-below pricing so
as to maximize the rounding advantage for vendors by favoring 98 and 99 endings (rounded up)
over 96 and 97 ending (rounded down) especially at small retail outlets where single-item
purchases are more common. Australia is a good example of this practice where 5 cents has
been the smallest denomination coin since 1992, but pricing at .98 or .99 on items under several
hundred dollars is still almost universally applied (e.g.: $1.99 – $299.99) while goods on sale
often price at .94 and its variations. It is also the case in Finland and the Netherlands, the first
two countries using the euro currency to eliminate the 1 and 2 cent coins.

See also

Infomercial

Pricing

Price point

Marketing mix

Mental accounting

Microeconomics

Take a penny, leave a penny

Numerical cognition

References

1. Manning, Kenneth (2009). "Price Endings, Left‐Digit Effects, and Choice" (http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.1
086/597215?origin=JSTOR-pdf) . Journal of Consumer Research. 36: 328–335.
2. Bizer, George Y.; Schindler, Robert M. (2005). "Direct evidence of ending-digit drop-off in price information
processing". Psychology and Marketing. 22 (10): 771–783. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.585.1030 (https://citeseerx.i
st.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.585.1030) . doi:10.1002/mar.20084 (https://doi.org/10.100
2%2Fmar.20084) .

3. The Widespread Use Of Odd Pricing In The Retail Sector (http://marketing-bulletin.massey.ac.nz/V8/MB_


V8_N1_Holdershaw.pdf) , Marketing Bulletin, 1997, 8, Research Note 1, J Holdershaw, P Gendall and R
Garland. ISSN 1176-645X (https://www.worldcat.org/search?fq=x0:jrnl&q=n2:1176-645X)

4. "Keith Coulter" (https://web.archive.org/web/20160309162422/http://www.clarku.edu/gsom/document


s/ConnectWinter2012.pdf) (PDF). Connect: News from the Graduate School of Management at Clark
University. Clark University. Winter 2012. p. 5. Archived from the original (http://www.clarku.edu/gsom/do
cuments/ConnectWinter2012.pdf) (PDF) on 9 March 2016. Retrieved 31 January 2019.

5. Schindler, Robert M.; Parsa, H. G.; Naipaul, Sandra (2011). "Hospitality Managers' Price-Ending Beliefs A
Survey and Applications" (https://semanticscholar.org/paper/db0c35c19dd52de8793925807a37df4d6d5
47119) . Cornell Hospitality Quarterly. 52 (4): 421–428. doi:10.1177/1938965511421168 (https://doi.or
g/10.1177%2F1938965511421168) .

6. Basu, Kaushik (1997). "Why are so many goods priced to end in nine? And why this practice hurts the
producers". Economics Letters. 54: 41–44. doi:10.1016/S0165-1765(97)00009-8 (https://doi.org/10.101
6%2FS0165-1765%2897%2900009-8) .

7. Ruffle, B. J.; Shtudiner, Z. (2006). "99: Are Retailers Best Responding to Rational Consumers?".
Experimental Evidence. Managerial and Decision Economics. 27 (6): 459–475. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.641.98
(https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.641.98) . doi:10.1002/mde.1282 (https://d
oi.org/10.1002%2Fmde.1282) .

8. El Sehity, T.; Hoelzl, E.; Kirchler, E. (2005). "Price developments after a nominal shock: Benford's Law and
psychological pricing after the euro introduction". International Journal of Research in Marketing. 22 (4):
471–480. doi:10.1016/j.ijresmar.2005.09.002 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.ijresmar.2005.09.002) .

9. Choice magazine, January 2009

10. Bennett, P., Brennan, M., Kearns, Z. (2003). Psychological aspects of price: An empirical test of order and
range effects. Marketing Bulletin 2003; (14), Research note 1. pp. 1-2. PDF (http://marketing-bulletin.mas
sey.ac.nz/V14/MB_V14_N1_Bennett.pdf) ISSN 1176-645X (https://www.worldcat.org/search?fq=x0:jrnl
&q=n2:1176-645X)

11. Thomas, Manoj; Morwitz, Vicki (June 2005). "Penny Wise and Pound Foolish: The Left‐Digit Effect in Price
Cognition". Journal of Consumer Research. 32 (1): 55–64. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.519.6272 (https://citeseerx.is
t.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.519.6272) . doi:10.1086/429600 (https://doi.org/10.1086%2F4
29600) .

12. Mark Stiving, Russell S. Winer, An Empirical Analysis of Price Endings with Scanner Data, Journal of
Consumer Research, Volume 24, Issue 1, June 1997, Pages 57–67, https://doi.org/10.1086/209493
13. Choi, J., Lee, K., Ji, Y. (2012). What type of framing message is more appropriate with nine-ending
pricing? Marketing Letters, 23(3), 603-314. doi:10.1007/s11002-012-9164-7

14. Bhattacharya, Utpal and Holden, Craig W. and Jacobsen, Stacey E., Penny Wise, Dollar Foolish: Buy-Sell
Imbalances On and Around Round Numbers (30 March 2011). Management Science 15, 413-431, 2012..
Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=1569922

15. Ashworth, J.; Heyndels, B.; Smolders, C. (2003). "Psychological taxing in Flemish municipalities". Journal
of Economic Psychology. 24 (6): 741–762. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2003.06.002 (https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.j
oep.2003.06.002) .

16. Olsen, A. L. (2013). "The politics of digits: evidence of odd taxation". Public Choice. 154 (1–2): 59–73.
doi:10.1007/s11127-011-9807-x (https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs11127-011-9807-x) .

17. Olsen, A. L. (2013). Leftmost-digit-bias in an enumerated public sector? An experiment on citizens'


judgment of performance information. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(3), 365–371.

18. MacKillop, James; Amlung, Michael T.; Blackburn, Ashley; Murphy, James G.; Carrigan, Maureen;
Carpenter, Matthew J.; Chaloupka, Frank (13 July 2013). "Left-digit price effects on smoking cessation
motivation" (https://tobaccocontrol.bmj.com/content/23/6/501) . BMJ Journals – Tobacco Control.
doi:10.1136/tobaccocontrol-2012-050943 (https://doi.org/10.1136%2Ftobaccocontrol-2012-050943) .

19. Davidovich-Weisberg, Gabriela (10 April 2018). "Israel to Abolish Deceptive Pricing Ending in .99 Shekels"
(https://www.haaretz.com/israel-news/business/.premium-israeli-shoppers-no-more-nis-9-99-1.527619
4?v=AF032C1F4FF8D8AF03C0442E3782F2AB.) . Haaretz.com.

20. Adams, Cecil (21 February 1992). "Why do prices end in .99?" (http://www.straightdope.com/classics/a3_
166.html) . The Straight Dope.

21. Landsburg, Steven E. (2012). The Armchair Economist: Economics & Everyday Life (https://books.google.
com/books/about/The_Armchair_Economist_revised_and_updat.html?id=pWS5zrF_Fz0C) (Rev. ed.).
New York: Free Press. ISBN 9781451651737.

22. Social responsibility of the Bata Company (http://www.romanakladatelstvi.cz/clanky/clanek_2.pdf) , in


Czech

23. "The Official Monster Raving Loony Party" (http://www.loonyparty.com/index.php?page=2005-general-ele


ction-manifesto) . The Official Monster Raving Loony Party. Retrieved 7 August 2016.

24. Sonne, Paul; MacDonald, Alistair (6 May 2010). "In the Longest-Running Joke in Politics, Life Imitates
Farce" (https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB10001424052748703322204575225864160523470) . Wall
Street Journal.

Bibliography
Boyes, W. J.; Lynch, A. K.; Mounts, S. (2007). "Why odd pricing?". Journal of Applied Social
Psychology. 37 (5): 1130–1140. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00203.x (https://doi.org/10.11
11%2Fj.1559-1816.2007.00203.x) .
External links

Wikimedia Commons has media related to Psychological pricing.

The Left-Digit Effect in Price Cognition (https://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2005/06/0506


07030351.htm)

Heuristics in Numerical Cognition (https://www.wsj.com/articles/SB120407749294595441)

99-cent pricing hooks shoppers (http://www.post-gazette.com/pg/06351/746545-28.stm)

Nine-ending Price and Consumer Behavior: An Evaluation in a New Context (https://web.archive.


org/web/20110304193342/http://nicolas.gueguen.free.fr/Articles/J-Applied-Sc2005.pdf)
(PDF)

"Get smart for just $9.99" (http://www.dangoldstein.com/dsn/archives/2005/03/get_smart_fo


r_j.html)

"Perfect Pricing: The psychology of online prices" (http://www.slideshare.net/grahamjones/pe


rfect-pricing/)

Retrieved from
"https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?
title=Psychological_pricing&oldid=1045001828"


Last edited 11 days ago by ClueBot NG

Content is available under CC BY-SA 3.0 unless


otherwise noted.

You might also like