You are on page 1of 3

SAMPLE TEACHING STATEMENT

My goal in teaching is to foster the acquisition of a base of concepts and learning skills to facilitate
further learning and thinking. In all of my courses I guide students in the evaluation of evidence, critical
thinking, argument development, verbal and written expression, and the application of general principles
to novel settings. These competencies will equip students for a broad range of demands in future
academic and occupational settings. Of course, I also expect my students to attain a high level of
scholarship in specific content areas, especially in graduate courses in which course content is directly
relevant to students’ careers. To promote the development of general thinking and learning skills, as well
as facilitate an understanding of course content, all of my courses are structured around three common
themes: an emphasis on psychological research, the development of writing skills and the generation of
enthusiasm for scientific inquiry.

Exposure to research literature is an integral part of my course plans, in part because comprehension of
course material is enhanced by an understanding of the methods used to discover that information.
Moreover, an emphasis on the discovery of knowledge fosters an appreciation of research design and
methodology, and more generally, of the importance of critical evaluation. These skills make students
thoughtful consumers of scientific ideas presented in both academic and popular media sources. As I
indicate in my research statement, I have investigated this issue in a collaborative research project on
science learning in undergraduate students. To familiarize students with research, my lectures always
cover the methodology by which principles are discovered. I also introduce research by including primary
sources in the required course reading material, and organizing occasional in-class activities around one
or two such sources. For instance, in my undergraduate Cognitive Development course, we spend one
class period reading, summarizing and critiquing an empirical article published in Nature on numerical
understanding in human infants. Similarly, in my undergraduate Memory and Testimony in Children
class, I introduce students to the scientific controversy over the relation between stress and memory by
assigning half of the students an empirical paper suggesting a positive relation between stress and
memory, and assigning the other half an article suggesting a negative association. During the next class
period, students are organized into small groups and asked to explain their assigned articles to each other,
analyze differences in methodology and results, explain the discrepancies between the two investigations,
and come up with a feasible model of the relation between stress and memory. These sorts of activities
not only provide students with exposure to the empirical literature in developmental and cognitive
psychology, but promote the development of skills in critical thinking and argument development.
Finally, I also provide first-hand research experience through frequent in-class demonstrations and
assignments outside the classroom. Indeed, in my course Memory and Testimony in Children, almost
every class period includes one or two simple “memory experiments” exploring topics in classical and
modern models of memory.

Writing proficiency is a critical component of a university education, and all of my courses (even my
Child Psychology class, which has well over 400 students) involve writing assignments and a variety of
examination question types. In Child Psychology, I typically assign two to four brief “application papers,”
in response to a question related to the course material (e.g., after reading a section in the text about
premature infants, indicate what you might tell a close cousin who had a baby two months prematurely
about her child’s chances for normal development). These papers are designed to prompt students to
apply course material to settings outside the class, and in doing so reinforce important concepts. Students
in my undergraduate Cognitive Development and Memory and Testimony classes, as well as those in my
graduate courses, write more extensive research papers. But not all of my students arrive in my classroom
equipped with excellent writing skills, so rather than using writing solely as a form of evaluation, I teach
students the process of writing. With all writing assignments, I encourage students to take advantage of
the writing centers on campus and to submit rough drafts so that I can provide constructive feedback, and
I sometimes give students the opportunity to rewrite their “final” papers for additional credit. In addition
to these more formal projects, I use briefer in-class writing assignments to help students synthesize and
critically evaluate information. For instance, to prepare my Cognitive Development students for a
discussion of several controversial theories of intelligence, I ask them to spend a few minutes writing
about their own “definitions” of intelligence. Similarly, following a discussion of the role of experience in
brain development, I asked students to write a response to a comment about whether the field of
Psychology will ever be reducible to biology alone.

My courses are also designed to generate enthusiasm for science and the process of scientific inquiry.
Meeting this objective is particularly complicated in my undergraduate and graduate Cognitive
Development courses and my graduate History and Systems of Psychology: Developmental Theory
course, which have been service courses taken primarily by students from outside the discipline to fulfill a
curriculum requirement. The challenge is to stimulate curiosity, as well as learning, by linking material to
students’ existing knowledge and experience. My approach is informed by research on memory and
cognition (including some of my own research), which shows that memory is enhanced when to-be-
remembered information is familiar and meaningful. I always supplement lectures with structured
discussions, demonstrations, or cooperative learning activities that encourage students to think actively
about the material and relate to it personally and practically. For example, in my undergraduate Child
Psychology and Cognitive Development courses, I frequently use demonstrations with real children (live
and videotaped) to illustrate principles of development and the methodology that documents them. In my
undergraduate classes, I also assign brief writing assignments to connect information presented in class to
students’ intuitive knowledge. For instance, students in Memory and Testimony and Children write a
brief essay on their earliest memory and analyze the characteristics of the remembered event. They then
shared their analyses during a class discussion on “infantile amnesia” and the emergence of
autobiographical memory. Assignments in my graduate courses are designed to be “useful” and to relate
the material to students work in their own disciplines. In my graduate course, History and Systems of
Psychology: Developmental Theory, students choose a content area related to Developmental Psychology
and analyze either the current or historical theoretical approaches to that content area. For their final
project in my seminar on Memory Development (Psychology 800), students write a paper describing a
mock empirical study on an issue in memory development and present the paper at a memory
development “conference” on one of the last two days of class. This assignment involves reviewing the
literature in an area of the student’s choice in order to develop a hypothesis, describing the method of an
empirical study designed to test the hypothesis, “creating” results that bear on the hypothesis, and
discussing the implications of the findings.

I have also pursued extensive informal teaching at KU in the context of independent study and practicum
courses. I have supervised seven graduate students in the research enterprise, and each semester I
supervise 4 to 10 undergraduate students in Psychology Independent Study (PSYC 480) or Practicum in
Research on Human Development (HDFL 689). For both of these courses, I arrange research and
scholarly activities to teach students about research design, experimental procedures, and statistical
techniques. I also assign book chapters and journal articles on memory development throughout the
semester, and organize weekly or bimonthly meetings in which we discuss this literature, as well as recent
research activities. These additional responsibilities ensure that being a member of my research team is an
educational experience, and help to get students “invested” in the work we are doing.

The teaching techniques I use to promote students’ understanding of the research literature, writing
proficiency, and general enthusiasm for scientific inquiry also provide data that enable me to evaluate
student learning and make adaptive changes to my course plans and instructional methods. In-class
discussions with students not only prompt students to think critically and relate to the course material, but
provide me with “on-line” data about the development of these general thinking skills and their
appreciation of important course concepts. In response to this immediate feedback, I may either move
through the material more rapidly or revisit particularly difficult points, and occasionally may modify my
plans for the next class. These discussions form the backbone of class sessions in my graduate courses,
but are also an important component of my undergraduate courses, even my very large Child Psychology
class. Indeed, to promote discussion in Child Psychology I give students the opportunity to earn extra
credit points for offering comments or questions during class and for posting them on a Blackboard
discussion forum. I typically begin the following class period by addressing some of the common themes
that emerged in this student dialogue. Of course, I cannot realistically count on all students to participate
in all class discussions, so I rely on my brief in-class writing assignments (e.g., following a unit on
experience and brain development comment on whether the field of psychology will ever be reducible to
biology alone) for more systematic assessments of how well my students are meeting course goals. These
quick measures of student learning have been especially useful in planning the activities for the following
class period.

Student performance on formal writing assignments and examinations also provides benchmarks for
examining student progress, both within and across semesters. For instance, because one of my major
goals is to teach students how to apply general principles to novel settings, many of my examination
questions ask them to apply concepts learned in class to new contexts. In some of my undergraduate
classes, however, I found that many students had difficulty with the applied questions. Thus, I now teach
students how to generalize their knowledge through in-class activities designed to explicitly model this
type of thinking, and I have subsequently observed dramatic improvement in student performance on
applied questions. My reliance on rubrics for grading papers, essays and other formal writing assignments
also enables me, as well as my students, to quickly identify areas in which they are excelling or having
difficulty. For example, in my first two semesters of teaching the undergraduate-level Cognitive
Development course, I noticed that many students had trouble selecting and locating relevant empirical
articles from scholarly sources for their research papers. As a result, I have made learning about library
research an explicit goal of this course. Each semester I invite a librarian to visit the class and instruct
students on using search databases to locate papers in scholarly journals, and I have broken the research
paper into two components. The first assignment is to find articles relevant to one of several topics in
cognitive development and students are graded on the relevance and appropriateness of the sources they
choose. Once their articles have been approved, they then write an analysis of the research in the context
of a mock advice column for a parents’ magazine. The quality of the papers students produce has
increased substantially following these modifications.

In addition to making changes in response to student progress towards course goals and student feedback,
I continually refine my teaching techniques by attending workshops offered by the Center for Teaching
Excellence and seeking informal consultation with experienced colleagues. As a result, I feel I am
improving and evolving as a teacher. Even after several years on the KU faculty, I think it is important
that I look for additional ways to grow as an instructor and to prevent my teaching from getting stale. I
will continue to look for opportunities to create new courses, and to fine-tune and update my current
courses by incorporating innovative teaching techniques and presenting students the most up-to-date
research and theories, as well as historically important work, in each area.

You might also like