You are on page 1of 7

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/274865971

Differential Profiles of Adaptive Behavior of Maltreated Children

Article  in  Psychological Trauma Theory Research Practice and Policy · September 2014


DOI: 10.1037/a0036718

CITATIONS READS
4 309

4 authors, including:

Ari Lowell
Memphis VA Medical Center
12 PUBLICATIONS   107 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Trauma Focused Equine Assisted (TFEA) Treatment for Veterans with PTSD View project

Veterans' Mental Health: Assessment and Treatment View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Ari Lowell on 18 October 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy © 2014 American Psychological Association
2014, Vol. 6, No. 5, 574 –579 1942-9681/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0036718

Differential Profiles of Adaptive Behavior of Maltreated Children

Kathleen D. Viezel and Ari Lowell Andrew S. Davis


Fairleigh Dickinson University Ball State University

Jenean Castillo
New York Medical College

Although it is well established that maltreatment is associated with a host of developmental, emotional,
and behavioral challenges, relatively little is known about the adaptive behavior of children who have
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

been abused or neglected. The present study examined 160 children in foster care; 42 were removed from
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

their families because of abuse and 118 were removed because of neglect. A demographically matched
comparison group was obtained from the standardization sample of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior
Scales and Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition. The 3 groups of children were compared
in order to examine potential differences in communication, daily living, and socialization skills. A
multivariate analysis of variance and follow-up descriptive discriminant analysis concluded that the 3
groups had significantly different mean scores on all 3 Vineland domains, and children who were
neglected had the most clinically significant delays in all areas. Clinical and practice implications are
discussed.

Keywords: abuse, neglect, adaptive behavior, Vineland, foster care

Child abuse and neglect continue to be a salient societal prob- behavior” is generally accepted to mean basic conceptual, social,
lem. According to data collected through the National Child Abuse and practical skills that people acquire throughout their lifetime
and Neglect Data System, 676,579 unique incidents of child abuse and use in everyday life. Although first used within the context of
and neglect were substantiated in the United States in 2011 (U.S. identifying criteria for an intellectual disability, the term’s use has
Department of Health & Human Services, 2012). The negative grown beyond that field. Broadly, “conceptual skills” include
consequences of child maltreatment are numerous, including im- language, number concepts, and related skills; “social skills” in-
paired developmental trajectory (Cahill, Kaminer, & Johnson, clude interpersonal skills, self-concept, and rule-following behav-
1999; Cicchetti & Toth, 1995; Schatz & Lounds, 2007); significant ior; and “practical skills” include skills such as self-care, handling
neurobiological changes (Heim, Shugart, Craighead, & Nemeroff, of household items, and ability to handle time management, simple
2010; Palaszynski & Nemeroff, 2009); cognitive and academic travel, and money (Tasse et al., 2012).
deficits (Crozier & Barth, 2005; Jaffee & Maikovich-Fong, 2011; The lack of knowledge of the adaptive behavior of maltreated
Kurtz, Gaudin, Wodarski, & Howing, 1993); and emotional, social children is concerning, as knowledge of an individual’s ability to
functioning, and behavioral problems (Lee & Hoaken, 2007; perform activities of daily living is important in designing appro-
Shonk & Cicchetti, 2001; Teisl & Cicchetti, 2008). A history of priate interventions. Deficits in adaptive functioning should be
childhood abuse and neglect has also been associated with a higher directly addressed in treatment plans, as children’s adaptive be-
prevalence of depression, anxiety, and relationship difficulties in havior skills may respond better to intervention than other char-
college students (Gauthier, Stollak, Messe, & Aronoff, 1996), as acteristics, such as cognitive ability (Atkinson, 1990). Addition-
well as numerous psychological and physical health problems in ally, comprehensive knowledge of a child’s skills and abilities can
adulthood (Chartier, Walker, & Naimark, 2007; Cromer & Sachs- also help determine which type of therapeutic interventions will be
Ericsson, 2006). feasible and effective. The present study evaluated the adaptive
Although psychological, social, and behavioral consequences of behavior of children who have been abused or neglected by ex-
child maltreatment are well documented, there is a paucity of amining scores on the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
literature on the adaptive behavior of these children. “Adaptive (Vineland; Sparrow, Balla, & Cicchetti, 1984) and Vineland Adap-
tive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II; Sparrow, Cic-
chetti, & Balla, 2005). Examining adaptive behavior using the
Vineland scales may be particularly useful, as it is sensitive to
This article was published Online First June 2, 2014. change in skill attainment resulting from intervention (Williams,
Kathleen D. Viezel and Ari Lowell, School of Psychology, Fairleigh
1996).
Dickinson University; Andrew S. Davis, Department of Educational Psy-
chology, Ball State University; Jenean Castillo, Westchester Institute for
Although often referenced as a single construct, the phrase
Human Development, New York Medical College. “child abuse and neglect” subsumes numerous maltreatment cat-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Kathleen egories, among which “abuse” and “neglect” are the broadest in
D. Viezel, School of Psychology, Fairleigh Dickinson University, 1000 scope. “Abuse” is generally defined in the United States as a
River Road, T-WH1-01, Teaneck, NJ 07666. E-mail: kmdavis@fdu.edu nonaccidental injury, whereas “neglect” refers to “the failure of a

574
ADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR 575

parent, guardian, or other caregiver to provide for a child’s basic uating an individual’s capacity to function in relation to society’s
needs” (Child Welfare Information Gateway, 2008, pp. 2–3). State expectations (Cabrera, Grimes-Gaa, & Thyer, 1999).
definitions vary in the extent to which sexual and emotional abuse There have been some studies examining the adaptive behavior
are distinguished from physical abuse, and neglect may include of maltreated children using the Vineland. For example, Clausen,
failure to provide for medical, education, or emotional needs Landsverk, Ganger, Chadwick, and Litrownik (1998) examined
(Child Welfare Information Gateway, 2008). Despite the inherent the adaptive behavior of 128 children in foster care, and found
difficulties associated with attempting to classify children as either deficits across all scales, particularly in the areas of communica-
having been abused or neglected, dividing maltreated children into tion and socialization. Similarly, Urquiza, Wirtz, Peterson, and
somewhat more heterogeneous groups has been beneficial in re- Singer (1994) found deficits across all domains of the Vineland in
search. Numerous studies have demonstrated significant differ- a population of 134 children who had been abused and/or ne-
ences between children broadly classified as physically abused, glected. Similar findings were reproduced more recently using the
sexually abused, neglected, and emotionally abused when using Vineland-II in a study showing an average developmental and
either legal definitions or the principle reason for a report in order chronological age discrepancy of over 5 years in a sample of 57
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to determine categorization, a common approach in differentiated maltreated children with reactive attachment disorder (Becker-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

maltreatment research (Zuravin, 1999). In addition to the cogni- Weidman, 2009).


tive, academic, and other deficits generally present across undif- The results of the current literature review revealed only one
ferentiated groups of maltreated children, reviews of studies in previous study that attempted to examine the differences in adap-
which children are divided by maltreatment type have consistently tive behavior between children who have been abused, children
revealed physically abused children to be comparably more ag- who have been neglected, and nonmaltreated children. Wodarski et
gressive and violent than other maltreated groups, sexually abused al. (1990) used the Scales of Independent Behavior from the
children to be more anxious and depressed, neglected children to Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery (Bruininks,
be more socially unskilled, and psychologically abused children to Woodcock, Weatherman, & Hill, 1984), which contained three
be more emotionally blunted and guarded (Rodriguez-Srednicki & scales the authors identified as measuring adaptive behavior: Mo-
Twaite, 2004; Trickett & McBride-Chang, 1995; Twaite & tor Development, Personal Living Skills, and Community Adap-
tation. No significant differences between the three groups were
Rodriguez-Srednicki, 2004; Veltman & Browne, 2001). Therefore,
found. It should be noted the current study uses a more compre-
it is reasonable to suspect there may be differences in various
hensive measure of adaptive functioning. Therefore, the present
domains of adaptive functioning between children who have ex-
investigation appears to be the first study to thoroughly examine
perienced different types of maltreatment.
the differences in a variety of adaptive skills between different
One particular finding of note among maltreatment studies that
classifications of maltreated children (such as abused vs. ne-
distinguish between maltreatment type is that among all groups,
glected) as well as to compare maltreated children with a demo-
the lowest scores for cognitive, academic, and social functioning
graphically matched comparison group on well-validated measures
have most often been found for neglected children (e.g., Ecken-
of adaptive functioning.
rode, Laird, & Doris, 1993; Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002; Wodarski,
Kurtz, Gaudin, & Howing, 1990). Garbarino and Collins (1999)
suggested that deficits relating to neglect occur because of the
Method
absence of critical parental and caregiver support during stages of
development. Others (e.g., Nightingale & Walker, 1991) have Participants
argued that the effects of maltreatment are potentially confounded Data from participants was collected via chart review of chil-
by socioeconomic status (SES), which is especially difficult to dren in foster care in the southeast area of New York State
separate from the effects of neglect (Hearn, 2011). However, following approval by the institutional review boards of the coop-
although correlations between poverty and neglect are generally erating facilities. Every child who enters family foster care in the
acknowledged, the suggestion of an equivalence of the two is county is referred for a developmental assessment to screen for
challenged by the notion that neglect has not been demonstrably developmental, educational, and mental health problems in order
improved by reduction of poverty, and a concern that neglect is to identify appropriate services, with the goal of stabilizing chil-
simply easier to detect among the poor, creating the impression of dren and helping them reach their maximum potential. Participants
greater prevalence (Crittenden, 1999; McSherry, 2007). Further- in the current study were assessed at the Westchester Institute for
more, in studies that controlled for SES, maltreated children con- Human Development (WIHD). WIHD is one of 67 University
tinued to display deficits (e.g., Kurtz et al., 1993; Shonk & Cic- Centers of Excellence in Developmental Disabilities, Education,
chetti, 2001). Research and Service. WIHD’s mission focuses on the develop-
The present study proposes to add to the current literature ment of children, the safety of vulnerable children, and the support
concerning abused and neglected children by comparing maltreat- of families and communities to promote health and well-being
ment groups using the Vineland and Vineland-II, which are well- among people of all ages with disabilities. All evaluations were
validated and reliable measures of adaptive behavior that provide completed between 2003 and 2009. Inclusionary criteria for the
domain scores for communication, daily living, and socialization present study included age of between 5 and 18 years at the time
functioning (Sparrow et al., 2005). Measures of adaptive behavior of the evaluation, an assessment of adaptive behavior skills as part
(such as the Vineland scales) have been demonstrated to be useful of the psychological evaluation, and a stated reason for foster care
as an important component of a general diagnostic assessment in placement (either abuse or neglect). Types of abuse include phys-
conjunction with other assessment tools, and as a means of eval- ical and sexual, and types of neglect include medical, educational,
576 VIEZEL, LOWELL, DAVIS, AND CASTILLO

and physical. Because the present study relied on archival data, it has known the child for several weeks fits the required criteria for
was not possible to further delineate groups by abuse or neglect serving as a reporter on the Vineland (Sparrow et al., 2005).
type; the most reliable information was the Department of Social The Communication domain assesses the individual’s expres-
Services determination of reason for removal (abuse or neglect). In sive language, receptive language, and written language skills. The
the event of multiple foster care placements over time, only the Daily Living Skills domain examines personal, community, and
initial evaluation was used, thereby maximizing the likelihood that domestic skill acquisition. The Socialization domain includes in-
the sample represents children who are entering replacement care terpersonal relationships, play/leisure skills, and coping skills.
of the first time. The total number of participants included in the Importantly, as with most measures of adaptive functioning, the
present study was 160 children, 42 of whom had been placed in Vineland scales assess typical performance, not maximal perfor-
foster care because of abuse, and 118 of whom had been placed for mance. Results are useful for diagnosis and special-education
reasons of neglect. The proportion of children who were neglected classification decisions, treatment planning, and progress monitor-
versus those who were abused is approximately equivalent to ing (Sparrow et al., 2005). The instrument has a long history of
nationally collected data (U.S. Department of Health & Human popular use (the Vineland Social Maturity Scale was developed in
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Services, 2012). Table 1 details the demographic information for 1935) and is technically sound, including demonstrating good
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

the participants. In addition, a sample of matched controls was reliability and validity (e.g., Stein, 2010; Widaman, 2010).
provided by NCS Pearson, Inc.1,2 from the standardization samples
of the Vineland and Vineland II. Participants were matched on sex, Data Analysis
ethnicity, and age. The demographic information for the compar-
ison group is also presented in Table 1. A one-way ANOVA and a chi-square analysis were used to
confirm there were no differences between the three groups (chil-
dren who have been abused, children who have been neglected,
Measures
and the comparison group) on age and gender. To examine poten-
The primary measure for the current study was the Vineland tial differences in adaptive behavior between the three groups, a
Adaptive Behavior Scales; depending on the date of the evaluation MANOVA was conducted in which group served as the indepen-
either the first (Vineland; Sparrow et al., 1984) or second dent variable and the three Vineland indexes (Communication,
(Vineland-II; Sparrow et al., 2005) edition was administered. The Daily Living Skills, and Socialization) were the dependent vari-
correlations between the two versions of the scale are high, with ables. Following a significant result, a descriptive discriminant
coefficients for the overall Adaptive Behavior Composite score analysis (DDA) was conducted to determine where the differences
ranging from .87 to .94, depending on the age of the subject lay.
(Sparrow et al., 2005). Both forms yield composite scores in the
area of Communication, Daily Living Skills, and Socialization. A Results
Motor Skills domain is also available; however, as it was designed
primarily for children 6 and younger, it was inappropriate for most A one-way ANOVA confirmed there were no differences be-
of the children in this sample and therefore excluded from the tween the three groups for age, F(2) ⫽ .671, p ⫽ .512. A chi-
present study. The Vineland is an individually administered mea- square analysis confirmed there were no differences by gender,
sure of adaptive behavior and is typically completed by an adult ␹2(2, N ⫽ 160) ⫽ .346, p ⫽ .841. Because of the small number of
who knows the child well. For the present study, the informant was some ethnicities, it was not possible to run an accurate chi-square
the child’s foster parent. It is WIHD’s policy to administer the analysis to determine if there was a difference between the groups
Vineland 4 to 6 weeks following the child’s placement. This time on ethnicity; an examination of Table 1 confirms group similarity
frame ensures the foster parent knows the child well enough to in this regard.
provide reliable information, while still allowing for early assess- A MANOVA revealed that Wilks’ lambda was significant
ment and intervention planning. Additionally, a foster parent who (Wilks’ ␭ ⫽ .778), F(3, 313) ⫽ 13.95 p ⬍ .000, which indicated
the three groups differed on at least one of the three Vineland
indices. For all three of the Vineland composites, the group of
neglected children had lower scores than both the group of abused
Table 1 children and the comparison group, and the group of abused
Demographic Information of the Sample children had lower scores than the comparison group. Table 2
displays the mean scores of all three groups across the three
Abused Neglected Comparison Vineland composites. DDA was used to ascertain which index
Demographic children children group
most contributed to this significant result. All of the indices of the
Age (mean years) 8.45 8.95 9.44 Vineland scales contributed to the between group difference iden-
Gender (% male) 47.6 42.4 43.8 tified by the MANOVA. The results of the DDA are displayed in
Race (% of sample)
Table 3.
African American 47.6 65.3 67.5
Hispanic 23.8 18.6 20
Caucasian 7.1 5.9 9.4 1
Asian 2.4 0 .6 Standardization data from the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
Multiracial 7.1 .8 N/A (Vineland). Copyright© 1984 NCS Pearson, Inc. Used with permission. All
Other/Unknown 11.9 9.3 2.5 rights reserved.
2
FSIQ 87.93 87.97 N/A Standardization data from the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,
Second Edition (Vineland II). Copyright© 2005 NCS Pearson, Inc. Used
Note. FSIQ ⫽ Full Scale IQ. with permission. All rights reserved.
ADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR 577

Table 2 not have been taught (or expected to complete) many self-care and
Means and Standard Deviations of Vineland Scores by Group domestic skills.
The current results suggest that children who have been ne-
Abused Neglected Comparison glected experience more significant delays in adaptive functioning
children children group
Composite M (SD) M (SD) M (SD) than children who have been abused. This is a particularly critical
finding, given that child neglect remains considerably less fre-
Communication 93.83 (15.42) 85.65 (16.57) 100.34 (15.90) quently studied and tends to engender far less attention, interven-
Daily living skills 94.69 (15.96) 84.69 (17.81) 99.07 (15.72)
Socialization 90.69 (12.91) 83.29 (12.93) 98.41 (15.41) tion, and supporting services than child physical and sexual abuse
(Barber & Delfabbro, 2009; McSherry, 2007; Wolock & Horowitz,
1984; Zuravin, 1999). Numerous reasons have been suggested for
this phenomenon, often termed the “neglect of neglect” (Wolock &
Horowitz, 1984), such as underestimations of risk and harm by
Discussion
social workers, difficulties defining and substantiating neglect, and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

The current study examined differences in the adaptive behavior lack of prioritization by social and government agencies (Dubow-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

of children who have been abused, children who have been ne- itz, 2007; McSherry, 2007, 2011). And yet the “neglect of neglect”
glected, and a demographically matched comparison group. A remains a matter of considerable concern, especially given that,
MANOVA, with a follow-up DDA, concluded the three groups according to the most recently available data, child neglect ac-
differed significantly across all Vineland domains (Communica- counted for more than 75% of all unique substantiated maltreat-
tion, Daily Living Skills, and Socialization). An examination of the ment cases in 2011, as well as about one third of all child
means (see Table 2) is helpful in determining the profile of maltreatment fatalities (U.S. Department of Health & Human
adaptive skill differences between the groups. Across all three Services, 2012).
adaptive domains, the comparison group displayed the highest There are some limitations to the present study. It must be
level of skill; the children who were abused had lower adaptive acknowledged that the classification of children as either being
behavior than the comparison group, but higher adaptive behavior “abused” or “neglected” can be somewhat nebulous. For example,
than neglected children; and the children who were neglected had different forms of abuse or neglect could introduce some possible
the lowest levels of adaptive behavior. As would be expected, the heterogeneity in the sample. Despite this concern, the nature of
comparison group demonstrated mean Daily Living, Communica- classifying reasons for removal from primary caregivers is, by
tion, and Socialization scores very close to the standard score mean nature, imperfect, and the present study provides some evidence
of 100. Although the children who were abused had somewhat for important differences between children who have been placed
lower scores, all three domain means remained in the adequate in foster care for different reasons, and highlights the unique needs
range, according to interpretation guidelines offered by the of children who have been neglected. Future research that focuses
Vineland. Conversely, children who have been neglected dis- on further defining groups of children in substitute care would be
played means in the moderately low range for all three adaptive helpful. A second limitation is the potential lack of generalizabil-
domains. Overall, the largest functional difference was seen with ity, as the sample was restricted to a southeastern county in New
children who have been neglected, as they displayed significantly York. However, the ethnic diversity of the sample could be con-
lower Daily Living, Communication, and Socialization scores than sidered a strength. Finally, for the children in this study, the
what would be expected given their chronological age. informant on the Vineland was a foster parent, which could war-
Findings from the current study suggest children who have rant a degree of caution in interpreting Vineland scores, as children
experienced neglect are at risk of demonstrating significant adap- who have recently transitioned to a new home may have some
tive skill deficits across multiple domains, more so than their peers difficulty across items (e.g., remembering address or participating
who have been abused (as well as demographically matched com- in long-term school or community activities; Urquiza et al., 1994).
parison children). In this study, the children who had been ne- However, as the informant was a foster parent for both the abused
glected demonstrated significant language skill deficits, including and neglected groups, this concern is somewhat minimized.
expressive communication, receptive understanding of language, Despite these potential limitations, the policy, practice, and
and written language. Additionally, findings indicate children who clinical implications of the results of the present investigation are
have been neglected may present with limited activities of daily significant. Because it may be more difficult to appreciate the
living, including delayed personal care skills, ability to complete significance of neglect relative to abuse (McSherry, 2007), case-
domestic tasks, and community living skills. Finally, results sug- workers and other child protective personnel should be trained in
gest neglected children likely have delayed attainment of social recognizing all forms of neglect, and should be careful not to
skills, including the ability to form interpersonal connections,
engage in appropriate play and leisure activities, and express
adequate coping strategies. This is consistent with previous re- Table 3
search that suggested youth who were neglected (rather than Structure Coefficients From the DDA
physically abused) experienced difficulties in social interaction,
Vineland indices structure coefficient
social withdrawal, and isolation (Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002; Wil-
liamson, Borduin, & Howe, 1991). A possible explanation for the Socialization .934
current findings is that children who have been neglected may not Communication .801
have been exposed to environments rich with opportunities to Daily living .783
engage in a variety of communication and play activities, and may Note. DDA ⫽ Descriptive Discriminant Analysis.
578 VIEZEL, LOWELL, DAVIS, AND CASTILLO

deprioritize suspected cases. Further, there is evidence that parents Dubowitz, H. (2007). Understanding and addressing the “neglect of ne-
who neglect their children are more likely to be impoverished, glect:” Digging into the molehill. Child Abuse & Neglect, 31, 603– 606.
have substance problems, or have inadequate support, suggesting doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.04.002
other social agencies (outside of child welfare) could have a role in Eckenrode, J., Laird, M., & Doris, J. (1993). School performance and
preventing cases of neglect (Mayer, Lavergne, Tourigny, & disciplinary problems among abused and neglected children. Develop-
Wright, 2007). It is also recommended that children entering mental Psychology, 29, 53– 62. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.29.1.53
Garbarino, J., & Collins, C. C. (1999). Child neglect: The family with a
substitute care following allegations of abuse or neglect participate
hole in the middle. In H. Dubowitz (Ed.), Neglected children: Research,
in a comprehensive evaluation of their needs, including an assess-
practice, and policy (pp. 1–23). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
ment of their adaptive functioning. Although children who have Gauthier, L., Stollak, G., Messe, L., & Aronoff, J. (1996). Recall of
survived physical or sexual abuse may typically get more support childhood abuse and neglect as differential predictors of current psycho-
than children who have been neglected (Barber & Delfabbro, logical functioning. Childhood Abuse & Neglect, 20, 549 –559. doi:
2009; McSherry, 2007), the authors of the present study urge that 10.1016/0145-2134(96)00043-9
children who are removed for reason of neglect also receive Hearn, J. (2011). Unmet needs in addressing child neglect: Should we go
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

comprehensive screenings and targeted interventions. Indeed, an back to the drawing board? Children and Youth Services Review, 33,
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

evaluation of their adaptive functioning seems particularly impor- 715–722. doi:10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.11.011


tant. Administration of a thorough instrument examining adaptive Heim, C., Shugart, M., Craighead, W., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2010). Neuro-
behavior, such as the Vineland, can assist in developing individ- biological and psychiatric consequences of child abuse and neglect.
ualized treatment plans. Developmental Psychobiology, 52, 671– 690. doi:10.1002/dev.20494
Hildyard, K. L., & Wolfe, D. A. (2002). Child neglect: Developmental
issues and outcomes. Child Abuse & Neglect, 26, 679 – 695. doi:10.1016/
References S0145-2134(02)00341-1
Atkinson, L. (1990). Standard errors of prediction for the Vineland Adap- Jaffee, S. R., & Maikovich-Fong, A. K. (2011). Effects of chronic mal-
tive Behavior Scales. Journal of School Psychology, 28, 355–359. doi: treatment and maltreatment timing on children’s behavior and cognitive
10.1016/0022-4405(90)90025-3 abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 184 –194.
Barber, J. G., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2009). The profile and progress of doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02304.x
neglected and abused children in long-term foster care. Child Abuse & Kurtz, P. D., Gaudin, J. M., Wodarski, J. S., & Howing, P. T. (1993).
Neglect, 33, 421– 428. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.03.013 Maltreatment and the school-aged child: School performance consequences.
Becker-Weidman, A. (2009). Effects of early maltreatment on develop- Child Abuse & Neglect, 17, 581–589. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(93)90080-O
ment: A descriptive study using the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales- Lee, V., & Hoaken, P. N. S. (2007). Cognition, emotion, and neurobiological
II. Child Welfare, 88, 137–161. development: Mediating the relation between maltreatment and aggression.
Bruininks, R. H., Woodcock, R. W., Weatherman, R. F., & Hill, B. K. Child Maltreatment, 12, 281–298. doi:10.1177/1077559507303778
(1984). Scales of independent behavior: Woodcock-Johnson Psycho- Mayer, M., Lavergne, C., Tourigny, M., & Wright, J. (2007). Character-
Educational Battery, Part IV. Allen, TX: DLM Teacher Resources. istics differentiating neglected children from other reported children.
Cabrera, P. M., Grimes-Gaa, L., & Thyer, B. A. (1999). Social work Journal of Family Violence, 22, 721–732. doi:10.1007/s10896-007-
assessment of adaptive functioning using the Vineland Adaptive Behav- 9120-0
ior Scales: Issues of reliability and validity. Journal of Human Behavior McSherry, D. (2007). Understanding and addressing the “neglect of ne-
in the Social Environment, 2, 33–50. doi:10.1300/J137v02n04_03 glect”: Why are we making a mole-hill out of a mountain? Child Abuse
Cahill, L. T., Kaminer, R. K., & Johnson, P. G. (1999). Developmental, & Neglect, 31, 607– 614. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.08.011
cognitive, and behavioral sequelae of child abuse. Child and Adolescent McSherry, D. (2011). Lest we forget: Remembering the consequences of
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 8, 827– 843. child neglect—A clarion call to “feisty advocates.” Child Care in Prac-
Chartier, M. J., Walker, J. R., & Naimark, B. (2007). Childhood abuse, adult tice, 17, 103–113. doi:10.1080/13575279.2010.541426
health, and health care utilization: Results from a community sample.
Nightingale, N. N., & Walker, E. F. (1991). The impact of social class and
American Journal of Epidemiology, 165, 1031–1038. doi:10.1093/aje/
parental maltreatment on the cognitive functioning of children. Journal
kwk113
of Family Violence, 6, 115–130. doi:10.1007/BF00978714
Child Welfare Information Gateway. (2008). What is child abuse and
Palaszynski, K. M., & Nemeroff, C. B. (2009). The medical consequences
neglect. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Ser-
of child abuse and neglect. Psychiatric Annals, 39, 1004 –1009. doi:
vices, Children’s Bureau.
10.3928/00485718-20091123-02
Cicchetti, D., & Toth, S. (1995). A developmental psychopathology per-
Rodriguez-Srednicki, O., & Twaite, J. (2004). Understanding and reporting
spective on child abuse and neglect. Journal of the American Academy
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 541–565. doi:10.1097/00004583- child abuse: Legal and psychological perspectives: Part one: Physical
199505000-00008 abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect. Journal of Psychiatry & Law, 32,
Clausen, J. M., Landsverk, J., Ganger, W., Chadwick, D., & Litrownik, A. 315–359.
(1998). Mental health problems of children in foster. Journal of Child Schatz, J. N., & Lounds, J. J. (2007). Child maltreatment: Precursors of
and Family Studies, 7, 283–296. doi:10.1023/A:1022989411119 developmental delays. In J. G. Borkowsky, J. R. Farris, L. Whitman,
Crittenden, P. M. (1999). Child neglect: Causes and contributors. In H. S. S. Carothers, K. Weed, & D. A. Keogh (Eds.), Risk and resilience:
Dubowitz (Ed.), Neglected children: Research, practice, and policy (pp. Adolescent mothers and their children grow up (pp. 125–150). Mahwah,
47– 68). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. NJ: Erlbaum.
Cromer, K. R., & Sachs-Ericsson, N. (2006). The association between Shonk, S. M., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). Maltreatment, competency deficits,
childhood abuse, PTSD, and the occurrence of adult health problems. and risk for academic and behavioral maladjustment. Developmental
Journal of Traumatic Stress, 19, 967–971. doi:10.1002/jts.20168 Psychology, 37, 3–17. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.1.3
Crozier, J., & Barth, R. (2005). Cognitive and academic functioning in Sparrow, S. S., Balla, D. A., & Cicchetti, D. V. (1984). Vineland Adaptive
maltreated children. Children & Schools, 27, 197–206. doi:10.1093/cs/ Behavior Scales: Interview edition, survey form manual. Circle Pines,
27.4.197 MN: American Guidance Service.
ADAPTIVE BEHAVIOR 579

Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). Vineland Adaptive Veltman, M. W., & Browne, K. D. (2001). Three decades of child maltreat-
Behavior Scales, 2nd ed.: Survey forms manual. Circle Pines, MN: ment research. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 2, 215–239. doi:10.1177/
American Guidance Service. 1524838001002003002
Stein, S. (2010). Review of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Widaman, K. F. (2010). Review of the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales,
ed. In R. A. Spies, K. F. Geisinger, & J. F. Carlson (Eds.), The eighteenth 2nd ed. In R. A. Spies, K. F. Geisinger, & J. F. Carlson (Eds.), The
mental measurements yearbook. Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute of Mental eighteenth mental measurements yearbook. Lincoln, NE: Buros Institute
Measurements. of Mental Measurements.
Tasse, M. J., Schalock, R. L., Balboni, G., Berson, H., Borthwick-Duffy, Williams, K. C. (1996). Piagetian principles: Simple and effective appli-
S. A., Spreat, S., . . . Zhang, D. (2012). The construct of adaptive cation. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 40, 110 –119. doi:
behavior: Its conceptualization, measurement, and use in the field of 10.1111/j.1365-2788.1996.tb00612.x
intellectual disability. American Journal on Intellectual and Develop- Williamson, J. M., Borduin, C. M., & Howe, B. A. (1991). The ecology of
mental Disabilities, 117, 291–303.
adolescent maltreatment: A multilevel examination of adolescent phys-
Teisl, M., & Cicchetti, D. (2008). Physical abuse, cognitive and emotional
ical abuse, sexual abuse, and neglect. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
processes, and aggressive/disruptive behavior problems. Social Devel-
Psychology, 59, 449 – 457. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.59.3.449
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

opment, 17, 1–23.


Wodarski, J. S., Kurtz, P., Gaudin, J., & Howing, P. (1990). Maltreatment
Trickett, P. K., & McBride-Chang, C. P. (1995). The developmental impact
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and the school-age child: Major academic, socioemotional, and adaptive


of different forms of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Review, 15,
311–337. doi:10.1006/drev.1995.1012 outcomes. Social Work, 35, 506 –513.
Twaite, J., & Rodriguez-Srednicki, O. (2004). Understanding and reporting Wolock, I., & Horowitz, B. (1984). Child maltreatment of a social prob-
child abuse: Legal and psychological perspectives: Part two: Emotional lem: The neglect of neglect. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54,
Abuse and Secondary Abuse. Journal of Psychiatry & Law, 32, 443– 530 –543. doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.1984.tb01524.x
481. Zuravin, S. J. (1999). Child neglect: A review of definitions and measure-
Urquiza, A. J., Wirtz, S., Peterson, M., & Singer, V. (1994). Screening and ment research. In H. Dubowitz (Ed.), Neglected children: Research,
evaluating abused and neglected children entering protective custody. practice, and policy (pp. 24 – 46). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Child Welfare, 73, 155–171.
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Chil-
dren and Families, Administration on Children, Youth and Families, Received July 29, 2013
Children’s Bureau. (2012). Child maltreatment 2011. Retrieved from Revision received January 25, 2014
http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/cb/stats_research/index.htm#can Accepted March 2, 2014 䡲

View publication stats

You might also like