You are on page 1of 8

MONTESSORI AND PLAY

Laura Flores Shaw, MS

Montessori Student, Age 10

KEY POINTS

 Though articles abound in the media extolling the virtues of play on children’s cognitive
development while demonizing work,1 these articles rarely define play, and they only seem to
define work as the didactic instruction found in conventional schools.

 Within the research literature, there is the overall construct of playful learning, which is broken
down into free play and guided play.2 The latter benefits children’s learning while the former
does not.6

 Pretend play does not foster creativity, better problem solving, and higher intelligence; nor does
it foster better social and emotional competencies.6

 Dr. Maria Montessori initially thought children needed toys, but the children showed her
otherwise, so she removed them from the environment.11

 An examination of the broad playful learning construct and Montessori education found that the
two have much in common: both have an overarching structure, free choice, peer interaction,
materials specific to the developmental stage, a lack of extrinsic rewards, and just plain fun.14
These elements are also present during pretend play.6

 The dichotomy of play versus work is false, as it fails to consider how the two actually overlap.16

Laura Flores Shaw holds bachelors and masters degrees in psychology and is currently working towards a doctorate
in education with a specialization in mind, brain, and teaching at Johns Hopkins University School of Education. She is
the founder of White Paper Press, and she writes and speaks internationally about education, brain development, and
parenting.

The author gratefully acknowledges the contributions of Kay Baker, Jacqueline Cossentino, Annette Haines, and
Mauricio Flores to this article.
found in conventional schools. But when we
really examine the constructs of play and work,
we see that there is far more to this picture than
“play is good” and “work is bad” for young
MONTESSORI AND children – especially within a Montessori
PLAY context. In fact, we find that the dichotomy of
Laura Flores Shaw, MS work and play as usually presented in the
mainstream media by well-meaning child
advocates is actually a false one.
Had…children chosen instead to play with
WHAT IS PLAY?
toys, a very different educational system
would have been developed. Play is a construct that is likely to mean
Angeline Lillard, PhD
Montessori: The Science Behind the Genius
different things to different people. For parents,
play may bring to mind children frolicking
through fields, hanging from trees, playing in
mud, and generally just doing whatever one
When I was the head of a Montessori school
wants. But for researchers who must actually
many years ago, a prospective parent who
define their constructs to clarify what they’re
came to tour the school greeted me by saying,
studying, play is more complex than that.
“I’m only here because my husband went to a
Montessori school as a child, and he insisted I Within the research literature, there is the

visit. But I don’t believe in ‘work’ for children. I overall construct of playful learning, which is

believe in ‘play.’” broken down into free play and guided play.2
Free play involves pretending (which is
Needless to say the meeting did not go well.
discussed in detail later), playing with objects
Her assumption that work is bad while play is and/or peers, rough-and-tumble play with very
good for children kept her from seeing what little adult control, and no extrinsic rewards.
was really happening in the classroom. But who Guided play, on the other hand, falls on a
can blame her? Articles abound in the media continuum and involves adult guidance that
extolling the virtues of play on children’s promotes academic knowledge through
1
cognitive development while demonizing work. activities that feel like play rather than the I-
What no one seems to notice, however, is that want-to-poke-my-own-eyes-out-because-I-
these articles rarely define play, and they only have-to-sit-still-and-listen-to-the-adult form of
seem to define work as the didactic instruction didactic instruction. This continuum is also
based on the amount of guidance a teacher around free play won’t provide children with
provides. For instance, some teachers may only opportunities to practice purposeful
provide guidance via specific materials in the sensorimotor skills that need to become
environment, while other teachers may provide automatic so that later deeper learning can
materials and still lead all playful activities. But occur.4 We can’t just tell children, “Play until
there is that wonderful middle ground, as you’re six, but then you need to get down to
Fisher, et al. (2011)2 state: business” when we haven’t provided
opportunities for them to gain the sensorimotor
Teachers play a unique role in
skills necessary for literacy and numeracy.
guided play experiences. They
Instead, we need to provide those opportunities
can sensitively guide learning,
for learning in a way that feels playful.
creating flexible, interest-driven
experiences that encourage Pretend Play
children’s autonomy/control over
A common concern parents have with respect
the process (p. 343).
to Montessori is that the classrooms lack a
Further support for this wonderful middle dress-up corner where the children can engage
ground on the guided play continuum comes in pretend play. Montessori children are also
from two meta-analyses conducted in 2011 encouraged to use the materials as presented
examining 164 studies of discovery learning. rather than pretending, for instance, that the
These analyses showed that unassisted Red Rods (which provide indirect preparation
discovery, as it occurs in free play (wherein the for mathematics and directly train visual
teacher provides no actual guidance in the discrimination of differences in length) are ski
learning process), doesn’t benefit students. poles. Again, this concern is understandable
However, guided discovery involving more due to the plethora of articles claiming that
teacher scaffolding and feedback (which can pretend play fosters creativity, better problem
come directly from the materials or other solving, and higher intelligence.5
students and not the teacher) does benefit
However, careful analyses of 40 years worth of
3
students.
research on pretend play and its purported
Overall, play is a broad construct, and what the benefits shows that “the evidence that pretend
research shows is that free play doesn’t benefit play enhances creativity is not convincing”
children’s learning. Does that mean children (p. 8).6 These analyses also showed that
should not be allowed time for free play? No. construction play (such as block building)
But it does mean that developing a “curriculum” correlated with better problem-solving while
pretend play did not (score one for the Pink pretending contributes to social and emotional
Tower!). In fact, when children assign meaning competencies. Overall, the studies show
to an object through pretend play, that meaning inconsistent correlations, which shows that a
can interfere with understanding the object’s causal link between pretend play and social and
true meaning and use, suggesting that pretend emotional competencies doesn’t exist.6 (Of
play doesn’t improve problem-solving skills.7 course, correlation is not causation; however, if
Meaning interference is also why we don’t want a number of studies consistently show
the children pretending the Red Rods are ski correlational relationships between two
poles as this can interfere with their ability to variables in the same direction, then one can
embody the concept of length. begin to make a case for causation – though
very, very cautiously.)
Finally, while there is a relationship between
pretend play and intelligence, the direction of MONTESSORI AND WORK
that relationship is unclear, so the claim that
Dr. Montessori was fully aware that
pretend play raises intelligence is
psychologists assumed that play was vital to
unsubstantiated at this point.6
young children’s development. In a lecture
But what about pretend play’s effect on social presented in London in 1946, she stated:
and emotional skills? Surely pretending
Psychologists have attached
increases these competencies as children
great importance to [play] and
engage in role-playing games.
make vague statements – that
It is true that researchers have claimed that children play at this age – that
both pretending alone and in a group contribute they develop their character
to social and emotional competencies because through play. They also say that
they allow children to play out their own social the individuality of the children is
and emotional issues, and they can practice revealed in their play (p.151).10
their negotiation skills.8 But as developmental
In fact, as Dr. Montessori explains in The
psychologist Jerome Kagan points out,
Secret of Childhood, toys were available to the
“scientists who study human nature…usually
children in the first Montessori school, but the
have a favored purpose in mind before they
children rarely chose to play with them:
begin their work” (p. 4), and for many, play
9

Since they never freely chose


may be a good and necessary purpose for
these toys, I realized that in the
children. When analyzing the actual studies,
life of a child play is perhaps
however, they don’t confirm the claims that
something of little importance their classrooms without a teacher. The first
which he undertakes for lack of incident occurred at the first Montessori school
something better to do. A child in Rome, while the second occurred at the
feels that he has something of Panama-Pacific International Exposition in San
greater moment to do than to be Francisco in 1915 where a glass classroom was
engaged in such trivial set up so people could watch the children work.
occupations. He regards play as During both of these lockout incidents, the
we would regard a game of children could have easily chosen to stay
chess or bridge. These are outside and play. Instead, they chose to find a
pleasant occupations for hours way into their classrooms so they could work.
of leisure, but they would And the teachers weren’t even present.12
become painful if we were
I have witnessed firsthand children’s preference
obliged to pursue them at great
for working in their classroom over free play. At
length. When we have some
the Montessori school I once administered, we
important business to do, bridge
decided one year to try running a summer camp
is forgotten. And since a child
for the primary (ages 3-6) students instead of
always has some important thing
summer school so we could give the teachers a
at hand, he is not particularly
long break. To maintain a Montessori
11
interested in playing (p. 122).
atmosphere during camp, the children could
Unlike other educational reformers who sought choose what they wanted to do, and the
to impose their views and ideologies upon activities included crafts, toys, and free play. A
children, Dr. Montessori developed her method couple of weeks into the summer, we started
through systematic observation of children. And hearing from the children and their parents that
so while she initially thought that children the children were bored, and they wanted to go
needed toys just as the psychologists did, the back to working with the materials in their
children showed her otherwise, so she removed classrooms. We never ran another camp.
them from the environment.
Ultimately, frolicking in mud and hanging from
In fact, there were two other significant trees may seem like the idyllic early childhood
incidents wherein students showed their experience, but that’s only because we adults
preference for Montessori’s purposeful tend to perceive “work” as a pejorative term as
materials over toys or free play. In both we spend our work days counting down to the
situations, the children had been locked out of weekend. Montessori children, however, don’t
because their work brings them higher affect, In her article “Work” Versus “Play” and Early
energy, and intrinsic motivation.13 Work feels Childhood Care, Goodman articulates how the
good. research literature generally distinguishes work
from play, but she also shows their great
In fact, an examination of the broad playful
overlap. First, play is considered to be fun,
learning construct and Montessori education
easy, and pleasurable, while work is unpleasant
found that the two have much in common: both
and effortful. However, play can also require
have an overarching structure, free choice, peer
tremendous effort (as when one plays hard)
interaction, materials specific to the
and work can feel quite pleasurable – especially
developmental stage, a lack of extrinsic
when in a state of flow, wherein you’re fully
rewards, and just plain fun.14 These elements
immersed in what you’re doing and time just
are also present during pretend play.6 This
seems to fly by.17
means that “work” in Montessori classrooms
feels like play to children. In fact, it might even Second, play is about freedom while work is
feel better than pretend play because the about constraint, obliging “us to discipline our
children actually get to use real tools and behavior, follow rules, do what the conventional
materials! standard demands” (p. 185).16 But Goodman
reminds us that children often create rules for
A FALSE DICHOTOMY
their own play activities, as they prefer a sense
When educators advocate for play in early
of order.
childhood education, they are fighting against
Third, play is about process while work is
the adult-centered, didactic instruction found in
focused on outcome. But this distinction is also
conventional schools – and with good reason.
erroneous. Play, like work, has an endpoint,
Forcing children to sit and listen to adults for
and children often evaluate their own products
long periods is not developmentally appropriate.
of play.
I’m not even sure it’s appropriate for adults.15
Finally, play is considered to be intrinsically
But this dichotomy of play and work fails to
motivated and self-chosen, whereas work is
consider how the two actually overlap. As
extrinsically motivated and imposed upon us by
education professor Joan Goodman states:
some outside authority. This means “the very
Absent clear criteria, play comes
same activity . . . can be play or work,”
to be defined by its opposite –
depending upon whether or not the person is
work – and the large overlap is
obliged to do it (p. 186). In a Montessori
lost” (p. 185).16
context, this means that all of the classroom
activities can be considered play since it is the work in the work place for that
children who choose what to do! matter (p. 188).16

Because work feels like play within a Hopefully, Montessori students’ experience of
Montessori context, those students learn that work as self-chosen and intrinsically motivated
“work” means something one wants do. And as will lead them to spend their adult years doing
Goodman states: work that is meaningful to them rather than
spending those years counting the days down
What seems to be the case,
to the weekend.
then, is that the criteria of play
that most distinguishes it from
work – its self-chosen
intrinsically motivated quality – is
also the quality that should
imbue work; work in school and

1. Kohn, D. (2015). Let the kids learn through play. math achievement from developmental number
The New York Times. sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research
http://www.nytimes.com/2015/05/17/opinion/sund & Practice, 22(1), 36-46. ; Jordan, N. C., Kaplan,
ay/let-the-kids-learn-through-play.html?_r=0; D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early
Hamilton, J. (2014). Scientists say child's play math matters: Kindergarten number competence
helps build a better brain. NPR. and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental
http://www.npr.org/sections/ed/2014/08/06/33636 Psychology, 45(3), 850-867. doi:
1277/scientists-say-childs-play-helps-build-a- 10.1037/a0014939; Seung-Hee, S., & Meisels, S.
better-brain J. (2006). The relationship of young children's
motor skills to later reading and math
achievement. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 52(4),
2. Fisher, K., Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M.,
755-778.
Singer, D. G., & Berk, L. (2011). Playing around
in school: Implications for learning and
educational policy. The Oxford handbook of the 5. Copple, C., & Bredekamp, S. (2009).
development of play, 341-362. Developmentally appropriate practice in early
childhood programs serving children from birth
through age 8. Washington, DC: National
3. Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., &
Association for the Education of Young Children;
Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based
Hurwitz, S. C. (2002). To be successful--Let them
instruction enhance learning? Journal of
play! Childhood Education, 79(2), 101-102. ;
Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1-18. doi:
Kaufman, B. K. (2012). The need for pretend play
10.1037/a0021017
in child development. Psychology Today.
https://www.psychologytoday.com/blog/beautiful-
4. Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The minds/201203/the-need-pretend-play-in-child-
new science of how we read. New York, NY: development
Viking; Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M.
N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting first‐ grade
6. Lillard, A. S., Lerner, M. D., Hopkins, E. J., Dore, 12. Lillard, A. S. (2005). Montessori: The science
R. A., Smith, E. D., & Palmquist, C. M. (2013). behind the genius. New York, NY: Oxford
The impact of pretend play on children's University Press.
development: A review of the evidence.
Psychological Bulletin, 139(1), 1-34. doi: 13. Csikszentmihalyi, M., & LeFevre, J. (1989).
10.1037/a0029321 Optimal experience in work and leisure. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(5), 815.
7. DeLoache, J. S. (2000). Dual representation and
young children's use of scale models. Child 14. Lillard, A. S. (2013). Playful learning and
Development, 71(2), 329-338. Montessori education. American Journal of Play,
5(2), 157-186.
8. Stagnitti, K., & Unsworth, C. (2000). The
importance of pretend play in child development: 15. Prince, M. (2004). Does active learning work? A
An occupational therapy perspective. The British review of the research. Journal of Engineering
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 63(3), 121- Education, 93, 223-232.
127.; Harris, P. L. (2000). The work of the
imagination. Oxford, England: Blackwell 16. Goodman, J. (1994). “Work” versus “play” and
Publishing. early childhood care. Child and Youth Care
Forum, 23(3), 177-196. doi:
9. Kagan, J. (1984). The nature of the child. New 10.1007/BF02209227
York, NY: Basic Books.
17. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The
10. Montessori, M. (1946/2012). Lecture 21: Work psychology of optimal experience (1st ed.). New
and play. In A. Haines (Ed.), The 1946 London York, NY: HarperPerennial.
lectures. Amsterdam, The Netherlands:
Montessori-Pierson Publishing Company.

11. Montessori, M. (1966). The secret of childhood


(S. J. M. Joseph Costelloe, Trans.). New York,
NY: Ballantine Books.

You might also like