You are on page 1of 7

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2005, 46, 485 –491

Development and Aging


Blackwell Publishing, Ltd.

Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and


socioeconomic status in 18-month-old children
EVA BERGLUND,1 MÅRTEN ERIKSSON2 and MONICA WESTERLUND1
1
Uppsala University Hospital, Sweden
2
University of Gävle, Sweden

Berglund, E., Eriksson, M. & Westerlund, M. (2005). Communicative skills in relation to gender, birth order, childcare and SES in 18-month-
old children. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 46, 485 – 491.
Variation in communicative skills, defined as gestures, vocabulary comprehension and vocabulary production, was examined as a function of
gender, birth order, childcare and socioeconomic status (SES) in 1,019 18-month-old children. The children were recruited at their regular
check-up at a number of randomly selected Child Health Care centers in a Swedish county. The participation rate was 88%. The children were
assessed by their mothers using a short version of the Swedish Early Communicative Development Inventories. The results demonstrate
significant effects of gender and birth order on vocabulary comprehension and vocabulary production. Girls scored higher than boys and first-
born children scored higher than later-born children. Type of childcare (family care, care at home and day-care centers) interacted with gender
and birth order on vocabulary production and indicated that family care is not as advantageous as care at home or at day-care centers. SES
had no effect on children’s communicative skills at this age.
Key words: Gender, birth order, childcare, SES, CDI.
Eva Berglund, Department of Nursing Research and Development, Uppsala University Hospital, S-751 85 Uppsala, Sweden . E-mail:
evaberglund@safe-mail.net

INTRODUCTION related to the year of publication, where the difference


between girls and boys was smaller in more recently published
It is important to identify factors that affect the acquisition studies. This review was limited to studies in Canada and the
of language. This study examines if and how gender, birth United States and did not include studies of children below
order, socioeconomic status (SES) and childcare have an the age of 3 years. In a large sample of over 1,800 US chil-
influence on early communicative skills, more specifically on dren aged 8–30 months Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, Thal
gestures, vocabulary comprehension and vocabulary produc- and Pethick (1994) reported a slight female advantage on the
tion. The influences of these four demographic factors have MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories
been the subject of numerous studies. The present study (MCDI), explaining about 1–2% of the variance. Galasworthy,
contributes to this knowledge in two ways. First, by studying Dionne, Dale and Plomin (2000) used the same instruments
all four demographic factors simultaneously in a descriptive in a sample of over 3,000 2-year-old twins born in England and
study, the relative contribution of each factor on children’s Wales. They found a female advantage, accounting for about
communicative skills can be depicted. Second, the influence 3% of the variance. Two studies on Swedish-speaking children
of these demographic factors is likely to vary between differ- (the Swedish version of the CDI instruments was used) did
ent cultures and societal organizations. Most studies have not find any differences between girls and boys for vocabulary
focused on English-speaking children, in particular in the comprehension or vocabulary production (Eriksson &
United States. The present study concerns Swedish-speaking Berglund, 1999; Berglund & Eriksson, 2000). However, in
children. Data were collected in a project on population Eriksson and Berglund (1999) a small female advantage was
screening of communicative skills in 18-month-old children observed at the first sub-scale (“first communicative gestures
in collaboration with Child Health Care (CHC) nurses. Com- sub-scale”) of the gesture scales. In sum, differences between
municative skills were assessed by means of parental reports. girls and boys in the first years in verbal ability might vary
as a function of different ages, times and cultures. When
differences are found, it is commonly in favor of the girls.
Gender
Studies on gender differences in verbal ability are inconclu-
sive. Hyde and Linn (1988) performed a meta-analysis of 165 Birth order
studies and concluded that the overall female advantage was In contrast to first-born infants, later-borns have to compete
negligible. In addition, the authors found a significant effect with their older siblings for their mother’s attention. This

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd. Published by Blackwell Publishing Ltd., 9600 Garsington
Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. ISSN 0036-5564.
486 E. Berglund et al. Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

situation is partly compensated for by the extra attention the pattern is usually reversed. Low SES is associated with poor
that older siblings give to the later-born infants. However, performance in school and later academic achievements,
although studies show that even toddlers adapt their speech whereas no such associations are found between birth order
when communicating with infants (Dunn & Kendrick, 1982), and school performance. Hoff-Ginsberg (1998) suggests
preschoolers’ speech to younger siblings also differs in many therefore that the relative influence from birth order and
aspects from the mother’s infant-directed speech. In parti- SES on children’s language changes during childhood.
cular, preschool children’s infant-directed speech contains Particularly, birth order effects will be attenuated as the
more directives and fewer questions than the infant-directed composition of the family might change and children will be
speech of mothers (Mannle, Barton & Tomasello, 1991). more involved with peers at childcare centers and at school.
Differences have also been found in maternal speech to first- In contrast, the effects of SES are likely to continue or even
borns and similarly aged later-borns. Jones and Adamson accelerate as the environmental factors associated with SES
(1987), for instance, found that mothers made fewer explicit (language styles, cultural experiences and resources) tend to
attempts to elicit language from later-born children around be more resistant to change. In a study of Swedish children
20 months of age than what mothers to first-born children Westerlund (1994) demonstrated that the effects of SES on
of similar age did. children’s language development increase during the child-
Qualitative differences have also been found in the speech hood years. The impact of social class differences also varies
of first- and later-borns. Later-born infants use social regula- among societies. It is often suggested that Sweden has a
tive speech more (Jones & Adamson, 1987), more frozen narrower range of class differences as compared with, for
phrases (Pine, 1995), more pronouns (Oshima-Takane, example, the US or the UK. Broberg, Wessles, Lamb and
Goodz & Derevensky, 1996; Pine, 1995) and display more Hwang (1997), for instance, were unable to show differences
advanced conversational skills (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998) than related to SES in cognitive skills, including verbal ability, for
did first-borns. The evidence of quantitative differences Swedish children 8 years of age.
between first- and later-born infants is equivocal or ambigu- In summary, there is some evidence that the SES of the
ous. Pine (1995) found that first-borns reached the 50-word parents influences toddlers’ language and that this effect
milestone earlier than second-borns, but found no difference will remain or even accelerate during childhood. However,
at the point of 100 words. Oshima-Takane et al. (1996) cultures differ with regard to socioeconomic structure, which
reported no differences in general language development probably accounts for why such effects are of varying import-
in their 20–22-month-old children. Hoff-Ginsberg (1998), ance in different societies.
however, reported that first-born children 18–29 months old
were more advanced lexically and grammatically than later-
born children. Fenson et al. (1994) noted small negative but Childcare
significant correlations from the CDI measures between birth Research on the effects of early childcare outside the home
order on the one hand and gestures, vocabulary production, is extensive. Many studies have focused particularly on the
MLU and word combinations on the other. effects of various forms of childcare on cognitive development
for disadvantaged children. Positive effects have generally
been confirmed (Burchinal, Lee & Ramey, 1989; Burchinal,
Socioeconomic status Roberts, Nabors & Bryant, 1996; Burchinal, Roberts, Riggin,
Several studies have shown a positive association between Zeisel, Neebe & Bryant, 2000; Caughy, DiPietro & Strobino,
the SES of the parent and the verbal abilities of the child, 1994).
especially regarding lexical development (e.g., Arriaga, Fenson, The National Institute of Child Health and Human
Cronan & Pethick, 1998; Bornstein, Haynes & Painter, 1998; Development Early Childcare Research Network reports a
Fenson et al., 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991, 1998). This associa- 10-site longitudinal study in naturalistic settings of childcare
tion is commonly understood as being mediated by the and cognitive and language development of children below
mother’s language. Mothers high in SES have been found to the age of 3 years in the United States (NICHHD, 2000).
produce more speech, use a richer vocabulary and pose a The study concluded that there was no indication that early
larger number of questions to their children as compared and extensive care is either deleterious or advantageous for
with mothers of lower SES. In the case of parent report a child’s cognitive and language development. This conclusion
measures it has also been suggested that parents from higher was true for both advantaged and disadvantaged children.
social class backgrounds give more accurate reports of Positive long-term effects on children’s cognitive abilities,
their children’s communicative skills. However, the evidence including language, in early childcare in day-care centers have
for this hypothesis is inconclusive (see Arriaga et al., 1998 been reported in two naturalistic Swedish studies (Andersson,
for a discussion). 1989; 1992; Broberg, Wessles, Lamb & Hwang, 1997).
In Hoff-Ginsberg’s (1998) study the effect of birth order Negative effects of early childcare may occur, especially
had a greater impact than SES on children’s language at when care is of poor quality. Two factors known to influence
around the age of 2 years. For children at school, however, the quality of childcare are the number of children per adult

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Scand J Psychol 46 (2005) Communicative skills 487

and staff education (Burchinal, Cryer, Clifford & Howes, production of 90 words. The test-retest reliability is 0.89 for both
2002; NICHHD, 1999). These two factors are highly cor- gestures and vocabulary comprehension, and 0.97 for vocabulary
production. The part-whole correlations of the SCS18 in relation to
related to the two main types of childcare in Sweden: family
the larger inventories (Swedish early communicative development
childcare, in which children stay in the home of a “day inventories, SECDI) are 0.91 for both vocabulary production and
mother,” and regular day-care centers, which are far more vocabulary comprehension and 0.74 for the full gestures inventory
common (see Andersson, 1989, for more details regarding (Eriksson, Westerlund & Berglund, 2002). The concurrent validity
childcare in Sweden). The women in charge of family child- of the vocabulary section of SECDI has been estimated to be in the
rage of 70 – 82% compared with notes from diaries of Swedish children
care are usually responsible for more children and have
16 months old and 71–95% of recorded speech samples of children
less formal training than the personnel at day-care centers. aged 16–30 months (Berglund & Eriksson, 2000).
Andersson (1989) reported tendencies that care at day-
care centers, in contrast to family childcare, is especially
beneficial for cognitive development. This tendency was Procedure
confirmed in Broberg et al. (1997), who noted that children The parent who accompanied the child to the CHC, usually the
in day-care centers were typically performing at the top, mother, was informed about the nature of the study and was
followed by children in home care and children in family asked to participate. Agreeing parents completed SCS18 at the
childcare. However, the authors were cautious in interpreting CHC together with background questions about birth order, child-
care situation and some general health questions. Information
these results because the sample size, especially for children about the mother’s occupation was collected retrospectively from
in family childcare, was small. the national birth register. (The register did not contain information
on the occupation of the father).

METHOD
Analyses
Participants/sample First, the Chi-square test was used to examine the relation between
The sample consists of children participating in a screening study the different demographic variables. Descriptive statistics on the
involving 37 randomly selected child healthcare centers in Uppsala dependent measures are given after correction for outliers. Observa-
County Council. In all, 1,019 children participated (524 boys and tions diverging more than 2.5 SD from the mean were considered as
495 girls), which corresponds to 88% of the feasible population. outliers and were given the score of 2.5 SD (Tabachnick & Fidell,
Disabled children with expected language disturbances and chil- 1996). In total, 3 outliers were given a new score in gestures, 30 in
dren from non-Swedish-speaking families were excluded (for an vocabulary comprehension and 10 in vocabulary production.
account of non-participation, see Eriksson, Westerlund & Ber- Next, one-way ANOVAs were performed to determine relations
glund, 2002; Westerlund, Eriksson & Berglund, in press). and effect sizes for each demographic variable on each communica-
The children’s ages ranged from 499 days to 664 days (M = tive skill. Stepwise regression analyses were computed to examine
18.38 months, SD = 0.67). Of these children, 38% were first-borns interactions among the demographic variables on gestures, vocabu-
and 56% had some kind of childcare outside the home. SES was lary comprehension and vocabulary production.
determined by means of the Swedish SEI system (Statistics Sweden,
1983). The children were grouped into five SES categories according
to their mother’s occupation as stated in the national birth-register RESULTS
(see Table 1 for descriptive data). Because many mothers did not
contribute information on their occupation in this register or could
not be classified according to occupation (e.g., students and stay-at-
Demographic variables
home mothers), analyses involving SES are based on 614 children The Chi-square test was used to study the simple relations
(296 girls and 318 boys). between the demographic variables. No significant associa-
tions were found (Table 2).
Material
The instrument used to assess communicative skills was the SCS18 Descriptive statistics of communicative skills
(Swedish Communicative Screening at 18 months), a parental
report in checklist format. This instrument assesses the use of 13 The communicative scores were used as outcome measures.
common gestures together with vocabulary comprehension and The descriptive data (M, SD, range) as well as skewness and

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics in the 18-month-old children

No. (%) Missing

Gender Boys 524 (51.4) Girls 495 (48.6)


Birth order First-born 384 (37.7) Later-born 635 (62.3)
SES Blue collar: Unskilled 92 (15.0) Skilled 187 (30.5) 407
White collar: Lower 112 (18.2) Intermediate 179 (29.2) Senior 44 (7.2)
Childcare Home care 443 (45.1) Family childcare 135 (13.7) Day-care center 405 (41.2) 38

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


488 E. Berglund et al. Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

Table 2. Relations among the demographic variables gender, birth Table 4. Simple relations among gender, birth order, SES and
order and SES (Chi-square tests) childcare and the communicative variables gestures, vocabulary
comprehension and vocabulary production (F and Eta2 values)
Birth order SES Childcare
F P DF Eta2
Gender 2.758 3.679 1.962
Df 1 4 2 Gestures
Birth order – 3.505 0.964 Gender 22.800 0.001 1016 0.022
Df 4 2 Birth order 0.856 n.s. 1016 (0.001)
SES – – 5.165 SES 0.872 n.s. (4,607) (0.006)
Df 8 Childcare 12.560 0.021 (2,977) 0.008
Vocabulary comprehension
Gender 22.719 0.001 1018 0.022
Birth order 4.985 0.026 1018 0.005
kurtosis for the three communicative measures are shown SES 0.313 n.s. (4,609) (0.002)
Childcare 1.898 n.s. (2, 980) (0.004)
in Table 3. The distributions for the communicative skills are
Vocabulary production
close to normal. Gestures and vocabulary comprehension Gender 47.148 0.001 1018 0.044
had a mild negative skew, whereas vocabulary production Birth order 17.705 0.001 1018 0.017
had a mild positive skew. SES 0.800 n.s. (4,609) (0.005)
Childcare 2.774 n.s. (2,980) (0.006)

Simple relations among demographic variables and


communicative skills into first-borns and later-borns. The children in family childcare
Table 4 displays the F and Eta2 values for each demographic had the lowest mean scores on all communicative measures;
variable from separate analyses of variance and the three the mean scores for the children in home care were always
communicative skills. Most demographic variables had in the middle and mean scores for children at day-care centers
their largest effect on the children’s vocabulary production. were highest on gestures, vocabulary comprehension and
Gender was the variable with the largest effect and was vocabulary production. We used this information to form a
significantly related to all three communicative measures, scale for the variable childcare. Family care was assigned the
explaining 4.4% of the variance (as measured by Eta2) on value 0, home care the value 1, and care at a day-care center
vocabulary production and 2.2% on both vocabulary com- the value 2. SES also formed a scale in which unskilled
prehension and gestures. Girls obtained higher scores than blue-collar workers were assigned the lowest value and senior
boys. Birth order was significantly related to vocabulary white-collar workers were assigned the highest value. SES was
comprehension (0.5%) and vocabulary production (1.7%). first included in all regressions, but as it yielded no significant
First-born scored higher than later-born children. Childcare main or interaction effects, it was eventually dropped from
was significantly related to the gesture score and explained the analyses to increase power.
0.8% of the variance. Post-hoc comparisons (Bonferroni) Step-wise regression analyses were calculated with gender,
revealed no significant differences between groups ( p > 0.05). birth order, childcare, and the full set of possible interactions
However, the mean score for each type of care was ordered in a single block on gestures, vocabulary comprehension and
in the same way for all the communicative skills. The mean vocabulary production respectively. A forward selection
was always highest for the children at the day-care centers, procedure was used in which the independent variable (or
followed by the children cared for in the home. The children interaction term) with the highest positive correlation with
in family care always had the lowest mean score. SES was the dependent variable was entered first, and so on. An
not significantly related to any of the communicative skills. independent variable was allowed to enter into the equation
if the probability of its associated F-value was 0.05 or less.
The main effects of gender and childcare on gestures from
Regression analyses the simple ANOVAs (Table 4) came out as an interaction
Interactions among gender, birth order, childcare and SES were effect between gender and childcare in the regression
explored by regression analyses. Birth order was dichotomized analysis (Table 5). The interaction revealed that both boys

Table 3. Descriptive statistics for the communicative skills gestures, vocabulary comprehension and vocabulary production

M SD Range Skewness Kurtosis

Gestures 10.22 1.80 6 –13 − 0.24 − 0.74


Vocabulary comprehension 68.62 15.57 28 – 90 − 0.82 − 0.08
Vocabulary production 30.29 21.26 0 – 84 0.76 − 0.31

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Scand J Psychol 46 (2005) Communicative skills 489

Table 5. Demographic variables (gender, birth order and SES) in relation to communicative skills (gestures, vocabulary comprehension and
vocabulary production) (adjusted R2 values)

R2 total β β t-value Model F

Gestures 0.027 28.468***


Gender 0.056 1.206
Birth order 0.028 0.895
Childcare − 0.043 − 1.022
Gender × Birth order 0.053 1.393
Gender × Childcare 0.168 5.336***
Birth order × Childcare 0.000 − 0.008
Gender × Birth order × Childcare 0.040 0.722
Vocabulary comprehension 0.025 13.568***
Gender 0.148 4.695***
Birth order − 0.064 − 2.028*
Childcare 0.058 1.827
Gender × Birth order 0.029 0.201
Gender × Childcare 0.079 1.694
Birth order × Childcare 0.079 1.826
Gender × Birth order × Childcare 0.096 1.836
Vocabulary production 0.064 23.517***
Gender 0.114 2.596**
Birth order − 0.206 − 4.952***
Childcare − 0.019 − 0.298
Gender × Birth order − 0.210 − 1.422
Gender × Childcare − 0.012 − 0.103
Birth order × Childcare 0.032 0.316
Gender × Birth order × Childcare 0.143 2.794**

* = 0.05; ** = 0.01; *** = 0.001.

Table 6. Mean gesture scores as a function of gender and childcare Table 7. Mean vocabulary production scores as a function of gender,
birth order and childcare
Childcare
Childcare
Family Day-care
childcare Home care center Family Day-care
childcare Home care center
Boys 9.8 9.9 10.1
Girls 10.2 10.4 10.7 Boys First-born 26.1 29.1 30.3
Later-born 22.1 24.5 24.5
Girls First-born 41.1 38.2 37.8
Later-born 26.7 28.5 37.5
and girls displayed a greater gesture repertoire at day-care
centers and a more restricted repertoire of gestures in family
childcare. This effect was more pronounced for girls than for
boys (Table 6). Table 5 shows further that the main effects DISCUSSION
of gender and birth order on vocabulary comprehension This paper has focused on the effects of demographic fac-
remained and no significant interaction was revealed. Girls tors on early communicative skills in a population study of
scored higher than boys and first-born scored higher than 18-month-old Swedish-speaking children. The results demon-
later-born children. Main effects of gender and birth order strate significant effects of gender and birth order on voca-
were also observed on vocabulary production, but there was bulary comprehension and production. Girls scored higher
also a three-way interaction involving gender, birth order than boys and first-borns scored higher than later-borns.
and childcare (Table 5). Again, girls scored higher than boys The effect of gender was larger than the effect of birth
and first-borns scored higher than later-borns. The inter- order, with the former explaining between 2.2% and 4.4%
action indicated that both first-born and later-born boys of the variance in vocabulary production (corresponding
scored highest if cared for at home or at day-care centers, that to a difference of about one word). The present gender
first-born girls scored highest in family childcare and that effect corroborates the findings of Fenson et al. (1994) and
later-born girls scored highest at day-care centers (Table 7). Galsworthy et al. (2000) for similarly aged children. The
These regressions explained between 2.5 and 6.4% of the size of the effect in our study was in the same magnitude
variance in communicative skills. but slightly larger for vocabulary production than what was

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


490 E. Berglund et al. Scand J Psychol 46 (2005)

found in these other studies. On the other hand, the present vocabulary production of less than 50 words (t(821) = 2.155,
finding challenges our previous claim of no difference in p < 0.05). However, this was not true for the group of
vocabulary production between Swedish-speaking boys and children with a vocabulary production over 50 words (t(189)
girls 16–28 months old (Berglund & Eriksson, 2000) and the = −0.236, p > 0.05). As indicated by the negative sign, the
meta-analysis of Hyde and Linn (1988). A reanalysis of the mean vocabulary production score for later-borns in this
data from Berglund and Eriksson (2000) revealed a slight group was even slightly higher, than for first-borns, although
gender difference favoring girls if power is increased by ana- insignificantly so. It can also be noted that the children
lyzing longitudinal data cross-sectionally and collapsed over in the study of Oshima-Takane et al. (1996), for which no
cohorts with age group as covariate (F(1, 802) = 8.059, p < general effect of birth order on language development could
0.01). The effect size in this sample amounted to 1%. Hence, be found, had a mean vocabulary production of over 80 words
a small female advantage could be detected when the variance at first assessment. Hence, it seems that the effect of birth
from age was controlled and the sample size was increased. order is limited to the onset of language production.
Hyde and Linn’s (1988) analysis incorporates studies of Effects of childcare appeared in the present study first
subjects from 3 years to adults and does not directly apply as a main effect in the simple analysis of variance for the
to children below the age of 3. By the age of 3 years, boys gesture data, but were later present as part of an interaction
might catch up with the girls. However, Hyde and Linn also effect with gender. The interaction between childcare and
reported a small female advantage for children aged 3–5 gender indicates that both boys and girls at day-care centers
years (as for the weighted overall mean across all ages), displayed more types of gestures than boys and girls in
corresponding to a Cohen’s d of 0.07. Nevertheless, Hyde family childcare, but that this effect was more pronounced
and Linn’s ( p. 62) general conclusion is that a gender differ- for girls than for boys. There was also an interaction regard-
ence of one tenth of a standard deviation is scarcely one that ing vocabulary production between gender, birth order and
deserves continued attention in theory, research, or textbooks. childcare, indicating that both first- and later-born boys in
In agreement with this statement, we see no need for differ- family childcare had a lower vocabulary production than
ent praxis for girls and boys, such as different age norms, boys in home and day care. In contrast, first-born girls in
due to a difference that is just statistically detectable. family childcare had the largest vocabulary production of all
Given that the difference between girls and boys in voca- groups of children. All but the last result indicates that
bulary production (as well as in general measures of verbal family care is not as advantageous as care at home or at a
ability) is so small that it cannot be directly observed, the day-care center for children’s communicative development.
prevalent awareness among people of this difference is The fact that only a small part (14%) of the study popula-
surprising. One hypothesis, pursued by Hyde and Linn among tion was cared for in family care probably contributes to the
others, is that studies on “normal populations” suppress a inconsistent result regarding this form of care. The organi-
real difference if more boys than girls are excluded for zation of family care in which a single person, typically with-
medical reasons or placement in special education. On the out special education, is in charge of many children is thus
other hand, such possible effects might be counterbalanced likely to display a high variation in care quality. The system
if the general belief of girls’ verbal superiority over boys also is much more vulnerable than care at day-care centers in
influences the standards for referral, leading to larger required which several specially trained persons are in charge. We
deviations for boys to be referred in the first place. However, note also that we did not have control over the length of
both speculations need to be substantiated by data. time children had spent outside of home care. Because the
The small gender effect in verbal ability might also be related social security system in Sweden generally allows a parent to
to parents’ expectation of a gender difference in one of two be home with his or her child for about 1 year, children’s
ways. The expectations might cause parents to stimulate the experience outside of home care was seldom longer than 6
language of girls more than that of boys, making the expecta- months. It might be argued that this is a rather short period
tion self-fulfilling, or parental reports might be especially that is probably without any significance; nevertheless, 6
sensitive to such expectations and over report female linguistic months is one-third of the life of these 18-month-old children.
performance. The latter argument is, however, unlikely as Moreover, this period is most important when it comes to
a female advantage is generally also found in formal testing language learning. In contrast, the two studies on the effect
and in natural observations. It should also be remembered of Swedish childcare on children’s cognitive development have
that expectations are closely dependent on the prevailing focused on long-term effects at 8 years (Broberg et al., 1997)
time and culture. or 13 years (Andersson, 1992). Yet, the results from these studies
The effect of birth order is consistent with other related regarding different forms of early childcare are very similar
studies (Pine, 1995; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1998; Fenson et al., 1994). to those of the present study. All three studies indicate that
We were also able to confirm Pine’s (1995) claim that first- children who attend family care score among the lowest in
borns reached the 50-word milestone before later-born cognitive development and children who attend day-care centers
children. Detailed analyses of the present data showed that from early on score at the highest levels. However, we would also
the effect of birth order was evident for the children with a like to stress that daycare policies are subject to different policies

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.


Scand J Psychol 46 (2005) Communicative skills 491

in different countries, policies that are likely to change over (1996). Quality of center childcare and infant cognitive and
time. Thus, we would recommend further studies of the day- language development. Child Development, 67, 606 – 620.
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Riggin, R. Jr, Zeisel, S. A., Neebe,
care situation and what affects its quality and its effects on
E. & Bryant, D. (2000). Relating quality of center-based child-
children, not the least regarding family care. care to early cognitive and language development longitudinally.
We were unable to demonstrate any effects of SES in this Child Development, 71, 339–357.
period and hence could not confirm the findings of several Caughy, M. O., DiPietro, J. & Strobino, D. M. (1994). Day care
US studies (Arriaga et al., 1998; Bornstein et al., 1998; participation as a protective factor in the cognitive development
of low-income children. Child Development, 65, 457– 471.
Fenson et al., 1994; Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991; 1998) and one
Dunn, J. & Kendrick, C. (1982). The speech of two- and three-year-
Swedish study (Andersson, 1992). One reason for this lack olds to infant siblings: “Baby talk” and the context of commu-
of confirmation might have been that the power in our nication. Journal of Child Language, 9, 579–582.
analyses was weaker than for the other demographic factors Eriksson, M. & Berglund, E. (1999). Swedish early communicative
because of incompleteness in the birth register. However, no development. First Language, 19, 55–90.
Eriksson, M., Westerlund, M. & Berglund, E. (2002). A screening
tendencies of SES were found and no changes were noted
version of the Swedish communicative development inventories
in the main effects of gender and birth order when analyses designed for use with 18-month-old children. Journal of Speech
were run with the smaller sample that included complete Language and Hearing Research, 45, 948–960.
SES information. We therefore conclude that SES is not Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D. &
associated with quantitative differences in early communica- Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative develop-
ment. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Develop-
tive skills in Swedish-speaking children 18 months of age.
ment, 59, 1–173.
Moreover, it is of no surprise that there are psychological Galsworthy, M., Dionne, G., Dale, P. & Plomin, R. (2000). Sex
differences associated with the political organization of differences in early verbal and non-verbal cognitive development.
different societies. That SES was an important predictor in Developmental Science, 3, 206–215.
Andersson’s (1992) follow-up of cognitive performance in Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social
classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62, 782–796.
13-year-old Swedish children underscores Hoff-Ginsberg
Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). The relation of birth order and socioeco-
(1998) and Westerlund’s (1994) suggestion that the import- nomic status to children’s language experience and language
ance of SES is likely to increase with increasing age during development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 603–629.
the childhood and adolescent periods. Hyde, J. & Linn, M. (1988). Gender differences in verbal ability:
A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 53–69.
Jones, C. P. & Adamson, L. B. (1987). Language use in mother-
The research reported in this paper has received grants from the
child and mother-child-sibling interactions. Child Development,
University of Gävle. We thank all the parents for participating
58, 356 –366.
in the study, and the nurses at the Child Health Clinics for their
Mannle, S., Barton, M. & Tomasello, M. (1991). Two-year-olds’
co-operation.
conversation with their mothers and preschool-aged siblings.
First Language, 12, 57–71.
NICHHD Early Childcare Research Network (1999). Child outcomes
REFERENCES when childcare centre classes meet recommended standards for
Andersson, B.-E. (1989). Effects of public day care: A longitudinal quality. American Journal of Public Health, 89, 1072–1077.
study. Child development, 60, 857–866. NICHHD Early Childcare Research Network (2000). The relation
Andersson, B.-E. (1992). Effects of day care on cognitive and of childcare to cognitive and language development. Child
socioemotional competence of thirteen-year-old Swedish school- Development, 71, 960–980.
children. Child Development, 63, 20–36. Oshima-Takane, Y., Goodz, E. & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). Birth order
Arriaga, R., Fenson, L., Cronan, T. & Pethick, S. (1998). Scores on effects on early language development: Do second-born children
the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of learn from overheard speech? Child Development, 67, 621–634.
children from low- and middle-income families. Applied Psy- Pine, J. M. (1995). Variation in vocabulary development as a func-
cholinguistics, 19, 209–223. tion of birth order. Child Development, 66, 272–281.
Berglund, E. & Eriksson, M. (2000). Communicative development Statistics Sweden (1983). SEI Swedish socioeconomic classification.
in Swedish children 16–28 months old: The Swedish early com- Stockholm, Sweden.
municative development inventory – words and sentences. Scan- Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics.
dinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, 133–144. New York: Harper Collins.
Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O. & Painter, K. M. (1998). Sources Westerlund, M. (1994). Barn med tal- och språkavvikelser. En
of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal prospektiv longitudinell epidemiologisk studie av en årskull
of Child Language, 25, 367–393. uppsalabarn vid 4, 7 och 9 års ålder. [Children with speech and
Broberg, A., Wessles, H., Lamb, M. & Hwang, C. P. (1997). Effects language deviations. A prospective longitudinal epidemiological
of day care on the development of cognitive abilities in 8-year-olds: study of a total age cohort living in Uppsala at 4, 7 and 9 years
A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33, 62–69. of age – English summary] Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis,
Burchinal, M., Cryer, D., Clifford, R. & Howes, C. (2002). Care- Uppsala Dissertations from the faculty of Medicine, 5.
giver training and classroom quality in childcare centers. Applied Westerlund, M., Eriksson, M. & Berglund, E. (in press). Can
Developmental Science, 6, 2–11. severely language delayed 3-year olds be identified at 18 months
Burchinal, M., Lee, M. & Ramey, C. (1989). Type of day care and of age? – Evaluation of a screening version of the MacArthur-
preschool intellectual development in disadvantaged children. Bates Communicative Development Inventories.
Child Development, 60, 128–137.
Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., Nabors, L. A. & Bryant, D. M. Received 21 April 2004, accepted 28 January 2005

© 2005 The Scandinavian Psychological Associations/Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

You might also like