You are on page 1of 9

ann. behav. med.

DOI 10.1007/s12160-017-9888-y

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Understanding the Dimensions of Anti-Vaccination Attitudes:


the Vaccination Attitudes Examination (VAX) Scale
Leslie R. Martin, PhD 1 & Keith J. Petrie, PhD 2

# The Society of Behavioral Medicine 2017

Abstract perceived sensitivity to medicines, and the tendency to obtain


Background Anti-vaccination attitudes are important predic- health information online.
tors of vaccination behavior. Existing measures of vaccination Conclusions The VAX scale provides an efficient method for
attitudes focus on specific age groups and/or particular vac- identifying those with vaccination resistance, and the four
cines; a more comprehensive measure would facilitate com- subscales enable a more nuanced understanding of the nature
parisons across studies. of those views. It should be noted, however, that the strong
Purpose The aim of this study was to develop a short measure correlations amongst the four subscales suggest that interven-
of general vaccination attitudes and establish its reliability and tions should target all four attitude areas, and it remains to be
validity. seen whether differential emphasis across the four areas is
Methods Two studies were conducted using the VAX scale. warranted.
For Study 1, participants were 409 individuals (53% female),
with a mean age of 34.5 years. For Study 2, participants were
92 individuals (67% female) with a mean age of 28.6. Keywords Vaccination behavior . Vaccination attitudes .
Participants answered paper-and-pencil questions about their Scale development . Vaccine . Intentions
attitudes toward vaccines, prior and expected-future vaccina-
tion behaviors, perceived sensitivity to medicines, online be-
havior, and basic demographic information. Exploratory and
confirmatory factor analyses were conducted with correlations
and t tests then used to assess the scale’s reliability and Despite the demonstrated effectiveness of vaccines for reduc-
validity. ing the mortality and morbidity of communicable diseases,
Results Four distinct but correlated vaccine attitudes were vaccination rates are on the decline in many areas of the world
identified: (1) mistrust of vaccine benefit, (2) worries about [1, 2]. This has led to a commensurate resurgence of diseases
unforeseen future effects, (3) concerns about commercial thought to be largely controlled or eradicated [3, 4]. Recently,
profiteering, and (4) preference for natural immunity. These refusal to vaccinate has been related to outbreaks of whooping
factors were significantly related to prior vaccination behav- cough, measles, and other vaccine preventable illnesses [5, 6].
ior, future intentions to obtain recommended vaccinations, Failure to vaccinate is an important health behavior, but it is
important to recognize that individuals may have arrived at
this decision through quite different pathways. In certain
* Leslie R. Martin cases, factors such as forgetfulness or not having enough time
Lmartin@Lasierra.edu to obtain the vaccination may be primary drivers of the behav-
ior. For other individuals, failure to vaccinate may reflect gen-
1
Department of Psychology, La Sierra University, 4500 Riverwalk eral concerns about unnatural medical interventions [7, 8], or
Parkway, Riverside, CA 92515, USA specific concerns such as the safety of vaccines, or mistrust of
2
Department of Psychological Medicine, University of Auckland, the motivations of pharmaceutical companies or others who
Auckland, New Zealand promote vaccines [9–11, 12].
ann. behav. med.

Table 1 Demographic
characteristics and descriptive Study 1 Study 2
information
Sex
Male 46.9% 32.6%
Female 53.1% 67.4%
Education
1–5 years 0.8% 0%
6–8 years 0.8% 21.7%
9–12 years 3.9% 71.7%
Some college 61.4% 4.3%
Bachelor’s degree 27.6% 2.2%
Master’s degree or more 5.5% 0%
Time spent viewing news per day
None 8.7% 10.9%
1 min–1 h 48.1% 44.5%
1–2 h 16.4% 35.2%
120+ min 26.8% 9.4%
Behaviors, intentions, and additional characteristics Study 1 Study 2
Yes No Yes No
Got flu shot this year 28.9% 71.1% 47.8% 52.2%
Will get flu shot next year 34.4% 65.6% 47.8% 52.2%
Child got flu shot this year 31.3% 68.7% 38.2% 61.8%
Child will get flu shot next year 37.5% 62.5% 41.3% 58.7%
Ever declined rec. vaccine, self 0.3% 69.7%
Ever declined rec. vaccine, child 21.8% 78.2% 19.5% 80.5%
Mean SD Mean SD
Age 34.5 11.2 28.6 8.2
Self-rated health 3.48 0.92 3.39 0.91
Preference for Online Health Info 3.13 1.24 3.20 1.24
Sensitivity to medicines 2.45 1.36 2.44 1.47
VAX 2.77 1.02 3.20 0.90
PACV 3.84 0.44
Likelihood of Ebola vaccine 4.80 1.53 4.91 1.60
Likelihood of Bird-flu vaccine 4.63 1.52 4.65 1.46

Understanding the attitudes that underlie vaccine refusal or Childhood Vaccines survey [14] and the most recent Vaccine
hesitancy is important for predicting vaccination behavior and Hesitancy Scale [15] also exclusively target parental attitudes
also for developing effective interventions or public health toward having children vaccinated against diseases.
campaigns. Identifying the most prominent concern driving Other vaccine scales are specific to particular vaccines. For
vaccine refusal can help target interventions, as individuals example, the HIV Vaccine Attitudes Scale [16] is directed spe-
who are concerned about vaccine safety, for example, are un- cifically at attitudes toward a HIV vaccine, and the Carolina
likely to be influenced by messages directed at changing be- HPV Immunization Attitudes and Beliefs Scale (CHIAS) [17]
liefs about the effectiveness of vaccines for reducing the like- is an HPV-specific instrument, although it has recently been
lihood of contracting a specific infectious disease. widened from its original parent-focus for use with young
Existing vaccine scales often focus on particular populations women [18]. Currently, no multifaceted tool exists for
or specific vaccines, and since most vaccinations occur during assessing general attitudes toward vaccinations as a health
childhood, it is not surprising that multiple scales focus on pa- intervention. And, although attitudes toward vaccination cer-
rental decisions about their children’s vaccinations. For example, tainly vary by vaccine type, there is reason to believe that
the Attitudes and Behaviors Regarding Vaccination Decisions negative attitudes may co-occur [19] and thus a single mea-
[13] focuses specifically on parental concerns about vaccinating sure may be the most efficient way to identify individuals with
children aged 1–6 years. Similarly, the Parent Attitudes about vaccine-related concerns.
ann. behav. med.

Thus, the aims of the present study were (1) to develop and private physician offices. The first focus group was comprised
validate a short measure of general vaccination attitudes, the of five individuals self-identified as favoring vaccinations
Vaccination Attitudes Examination (VAX) scale; (2) to test the (one male); the second and third groups were comprised of
usefulness of the VAX scale (or subscales) for predicting vac- five (one male) and four (all female) individuals who self-
cination behavior; and (3) to test the degree to which various identified as vaccine-hesitant. The only criterion for inclusion
facets of negative vaccination perceptions covary. A high de- in a focus group was willingness to spend the ~30 min re-
gree of covariation would suggest that interventions ought to quired for the group meeting, and groups were purposely
be broad in scope, targeting all potential concerns, whereas structured to be homogeneous and small in order to maximize
less covariation would suggest that interventions that are more likelihood of candid disclosure. This portion of the study was
specifically targeted at individual concerns might be more IRB-approved (#LSU-1505) and participants who gave in-
effective, as they could address existing worries without intro- formed consent at the physician’s office prior to the group
ducing new ones. These aims were addressed in two separate meeting. The first author led all focus groups, and discussions
studies. The first study utilized exploratory factor analysis to were audiotaped for later transcription.
reduce the number of items in the scale and to identify sub- There were five structured questions that were used for
scales; and established initial reliability and validity. The sec- each group. These questions were “When you hear the word
ond study employed confirmatory factor analysis and demon- ‘vaccination’ what comes to mind?”, “What specific benefits
strated the reliability and validity of the scale in a different do vaccinations have, in your view?”, “What specific con-
sample. cerns do you have about vaccinations?”, “What do you think
about the scientific evidence for the safety and effectiveness of
vaccinations?”, and “How do the important people in your life
Study 1 Method feel about vaccinations?”. Following transcription, statements
were organized into groups based on similarity by two inde-
Participants pendent raters; the emergent themes were then combined with
(1) information derived from a thorough review of the litera-
Participants were 409 individuals (53% female) with a mean ture (using the PsycINFO® and MEDLINE® databases, and
age of 34.5 years. Of these, most were community-living including examination of the reference lists of all identified
adults (87%) from the west coast of the USA and the remain- articles) on attitudes toward vaccinations and (2) an informal
ing 13% were students enrolled in an undergraduate psychol- evaluation of the content of anti-vaccination websites and
ogy class at a small, liberal arts university in the same geo- blogs to create an initial item-list which was then reviewed
graphic area. Three hundred and thirty-five participants had by both authors and two additional colleagues for clarity and
completed at least some college and 166 were parents. Basic inclusivity. Although one item was especially complex (“al-
demographics and other descriptive statistics are found in though most vaccines appear to be safe, there may be prob-
Table 1. lems that we haven’t yet discovered”), it was retained because
Nonstudent participants were recruited through in-person it was seen as a belief that a vaccine-positive person would
contacts in the proximity of grocery stores, pharmacies, gyms, feel comfortable endorsing. The final item list used for the
and yoga studios. Undergraduate students were enrolled exploratory factor analysis was comprised of 45 items. Items
through the psychology department’s research participation were presented in the form of statements, with responses on a
pool, and they received class credit for their participation. 6-point Likert-type scale ranging from “strongly agree” to
No compensation was given to nonstudent participants, but “strongly disagree.” Higher scores reflect stronger anti-
they were provided with information on how to obtain results vaccination attitudes, with 15 of the initial 45 items being
from the study upon its completion. reverse coded.
The study was approved by La Sierra University’s
Institutional Review Board (#LSU-1505), and all participants Validation Measures
gave informed consent prior to entering the study. All data
were then collected using an anonymous, self-report question- Vaccination Behavior and Intention
naire in which the Vaccination Attitude Examination (VAX)
scale items were listed first, followed by the validation items Prior vaccination behavior was assessed with three dichoto-
and demographics. mous (yes/no) items asking (1) whether the respondent had
received an influenza vaccination in the prior year, (2) whether
Vaccination Attitude Examination Scale Items her/his child(ren) had received an influenza vaccination in the
prior year, and (3) whether the respondent had ever refused or
To create the initial item pool, we first conducted three focus elected to forego a doctor-recommended vaccine for her/his
groups using individuals recruited from the waiting rooms of child. Vaccination intentions were also assessed with two
ann. behav. med.

Table 2 Study 1 VAX scale items factor loading matrix (EFA)

Items Mistrust of vaccine Worries about unforeseen Concerns about commercial Preference for natural
benefit future effects profiteering immunity

I feel safe after being vaccinated (–) .799 .212 .056 .113
I can rely on vaccines to stop serious .711 .120 .103 .082
infectious diseases (–)
I feel protected after getting vaccinated (–) .761 .219 .095 .158
Although most vaccines appear to be safe, .142 .809 .088 .226
there may be problems that we have
not yet discovered.
Vaccines can cause unforeseen problems in .227 .723 .015 .179
children.
I worry about the unknown effects of .139 .677 .009 .282
vaccines in the future.
Vaccines make a lot of money for pharmaceutical .202 .107 .602 .152
companies, but do not do much for regular
people.
Authorities promote vaccination for financial .251 .153 .631 .140
gain, not for people’s health.
Vaccination programs are a big con. .286 .099 .789 .132
Natural immunity lasts longer than a vaccination. .112 .140 .158 .652
Natural exposure to viruses and germs gives .081 .122 .125 .727
the safest protection.
Being exposed to diseases naturally is safer for .190 .165 .103 .608
the immune system than being exposed
through vaccination.

Factor loadings representative of a particular factor appear in boldface

dichotomous (yes/no) items asking (1) whether the respondent agreement with each of the items (e.g., “My body is very
expected to get an influenza vaccine in the coming year and sensitive to medicines”) on a 6-point Likert-type scale ranging
(2) whether the respondent expected that her/his child(ren) from strongly agree to strongly disagree. Higher perceived
would receive an influenza vaccine in the coming year. Each sensitivity to medicine scores have been associated with great-
of the child-related items included a “not applicable” response er information seeking about medication and higher symptom
option for nonparents. In addition, respondents were asked reporting [21].
about the likelihood (on a 6-point scale, from “very unlikely”
to “very likely”) of their getting vaccinated against (1) bird-flu
and (2) Ebola should there be an outbreak in their geographic Current Health
area and a vaccine available to protect against contracting the
disease. Perceived current level of health was assessed with a
single item drawn from the General Health Perceptions
Sensitivity to Medicines Scale [22]: “In general, would you say your health is…”
The item has five response options ranging from “poor”
The 5-item Perceived Sensitivity to Medicines Scale [20] was to “excellent,” and it has been shown to correlate well
used to assess perceptions of susceptibility to potential ad- with more lengthy and objective measures of health
verse effects of medication. Participants rated their level of status.

Table 3 Alpha reliabilities,


means, and standard deviations Study 1 Study 2
for VAX subscales
Mean SD Cronbach’s α Mean SD Cronbach’s α

Trust/mistrust of vaccine benefit 2.28 1.34 .92 2.41 1.19 .91


Worries over unforeseen future effects 3.28 1.50 .89 4.19 1.23 .77
Concerns about commercial profiteering 2.38 1.47 .93 2.83 1.07 .85
Preference for natural immunity 3.14 1.42 .86 3.38 1.19 .78
ann. behav. med.

Fig. 1 VAX scale scores (mean,


SE), vaccination behavior, and
intentions (Study 1)

Media the items that best reflected the underlying subscales, re-
gardless of the direction of their wording; thus, although
Two additional media-related items relevant to vaccination the initial 45-item pool contained 30 positively worded
attitudes were also included (1) amount of time spent per items, the final scale contains only three positively worded
day viewing the news, including television, online news pro- items. The four factors or subscales, each comprised of
grams, and news articles—this was a free-response question three items, are found in Table 3.
and responses were converted to minutes and then converted We next examined the associations amongst the VAX sub-
to an 8-point scale ranging from “0” to “120+” minutes—(2) scale scores. Although the four factors were distinct, they were
preference for getting health information online, scored on a highly correlated with one another (rs ranging from .61 to .78)
6-point Likert-type scale that ranged from “never” to indicating that within individuals, holding one negative view
“always.” of vaccines was associated with holding other negative per-
ceptions. Because of the high degree of inter-scale correlation,
Statistical Analyses we combined the subscales into a single VAX score for subse-
quent analyses.
All analyses were conducted using R 3.3.2 for Windows. We found that VAX scale scores were significantly higher in
An initial exploratory factor analysis (EFA) with diago- those individuals who had not had an influenza vaccine in the
nally weighted least squares (DWLS) estimation was con- prior year (MVAX = 2.92, SD = 1.30) compared to those who
ducted, followed by correlations and t tests to assess as- had (MVAX = 2.34, SD = 1.08), t(406) = −4.125, p < .001,
sociations between the VAX scale and the validation d = 0.48, 95% CI [−0.852, −0.302]. There were also signifi-
measures. cant differences in VAX score between those who intended not
to be vaccinated this year (MVAX = 3.08, SD = 1.33) vs. those
who said they would (M VA X = 2.14, SD = 0.90),
Study 1 Results t(406) = −7.476, p < .001, d = 0.83, 95% CI [−1.184,-
0.691]. Parents whose children had received an influenza vac-
The exploratory factor analysis was conducted using all 45 cination in the prior year had significantly lower VAX scores
items in the revised item pool. To determine the number of (MVAX = 2.03, SD = 0.84) than those whose children had not
factors to extract, parallel analysis [23] (appropriate for (MVAX = 3.43, SD = 1.41), t(156) = −7.170, p < .001, d = 1.21,
Likert-type data, using polychoric correlation matrices) 95% CI [−1.784, −1.014], and the same was true for those
[24] with 100 simulations was used. Four eigenvalues who said their child would receive a flu vaccine in the coming
exceeded those of their parallel factors’ average eigen- year (MVAX = 2.05, SD = 0.90) as compared with those who
values and thus four factors were retained. Items were then said their children would not (MVAX = 3.46, SD = 1.37),
reviewed and retained for a given factor if they loaded at t(157) = −7.322, p < .001, d = 1.22, 95% CI [−1.789, −1.029].
least .60 on that factor and not more than .30 on any addi- We next examined vaccine refusal behavior and found
tional factor(s). These items and their factor loadings are those participants who said that they had previously refused
found in Table 2. Each item loads strongly (at least .60) on a doctor’s recommended vaccine for their child also had sig-
its designated factor and has a low association (not more nificantly higher VAX scores (MVAX = 4.09, SD = 1.31) com-
than .30) with the others. The decision was made to retain pared with those who had never refused a recommended
ann. behav. med.

Table 4 Correlation matrix showing interrelationships between the VAX scale and relevant demographic and health-related variables

VAX Education Self-rated health Time viewing news Sensitivity to medicines Health info online

VAX –
Education –
Study 1 −.09
Study2 −.11
Self-rated health –
Study 1 .00 .03
Study 2 .00 −.03
Time viewing news –
Study 1 −.02 .05 −.02
Study 2 .06 −.08 .06
Sensitivity to medicines –
Study 1 .55** .05 −.17** −.05
Study 2 .37** −.01 −.24* .11
Health info online –
Study 1 .51** .06 −.06 .09 .41**
Study 2 .32** .07 .03 .10 .25*

*p < .05; **p < .01

vaccine for their child (M VA X = 2.49, SD = 1.15), wave of data was collected immediately following consent
t(164) = 7.363, p < .001, d = 1.30, 95% CI [1.175, 2.036] (IRB approval #LSU-1505), using an anonymous, self-
(see Fig. 1). Those with higher VAX scale scores also reported report questionnaire that included an identifying “code name”
being less likely to get vaccinated against Ebola r(408) = −.49, of the participant’s choosing. Participants also provided an
p < .001 and bird flu r(408) = −.53, p < .001 if outbreaks email address to which a link was sent 4 weeks after the initial
should occur and vaccines were available. contact. Participants who clicked on the link were directed to a
VAX scale scores were not related to education level (al- Google Form that contained the 12 VAX scale items only. Of
though it should be noted that the range on education was the initially recruited 92 participants, 63 completed both
somewhat restricted in this sample), self-rated health, or self- waves of data collection.
reported time per day spent viewing the news. However, VAX
scale scores did strongly correlate with a perceived sensitivity Vaccination Attitude Scale and Validation Measures
to medicines and also preferences for getting health informa-
tion online (see Table 4). The 12 items of the Vaccination Attitude Examination (VAX)
scale, previously described (Study 1), were administered to
evaluate general attitudes toward vaccinations. These items
Study 2 Method appeared first on the questionnaire, followed by validation
measures and demographic questions.
Participants The 8-item subscale assessing beliefs about the safety and
efficacy of childhood vaccinations from the Parent Attitudes
Participants were 92 individuals (67% female) with a mean about Childhood Vaccines (PACV) [14] was also included in
age of 28.6 years. Of these, all were community-living adults this study as a validation measure for the VAX scale. The
from the west coast of the USA. Only two participants had PACV was designed to identify vaccine-hesitant parents with
completed a bachelor’s degree; most had at least some high under-immunized young children. Seven of the items are
school (72%) but few had attended any college. Seventy-two scored on a 5-point, Likert scale with varying anchors; the
percent of participants were parents. Basic demographics and eighth is on a 3-point scale (“yes,” “no,” and “do not know”).
other descriptive statistics are found in Table 1. Additional validation measures included prior vaccination
Participants were recruited through in-person contacts in behavior, vaccination intentions, the Perceived Sensitivity to
the proximity of grocery stores and at public laundry facilities. Medicines Scale [20], the General Health Perceptions Scale
Because two waves of data collection were requested, partic- [22], and two media-related items (time per day viewing the
ipants were offered a single entry into a drawing for an Apple news and preference for getting health information online) as
iPod nano as a token of appreciation for their time. The first described for Study 1.
ann. behav. med.

Statistical Analyses their children would not (M VAX = 3.61, SD = 1.42),


t(64) = −7.298, p < .001, d = 1.17, 95% CI [−1.742, −1.009].
All analyses were conducted using R 3.3.2 for Windows; Regarding refusal of a doctor-recommended vaccination,
Lavaan was employed for the confirmatory factor analysis trends were consistent with Study 1 findings, but in this sample
(CFA). As before, the factor analysis was followed by corre- here were no statistically significant differences in VAX scores
lations and t tests to evaluate test-retest reliability and associ- between those who had previously refused a doctor’s recom-
ations between the VAX scale and the validation measures. mended vaccine for their child (MVAX = 3.51, SD = 0.70) and
those who had not (MVAX = 3.20, SD = 0.98), t(64) = 0.61,
p = .55, g = 0.43, 95% CI [−1.102, 0.655]. An additional anal-
Study 2 Results ysis, not included in Study 1, was a comparison of those who
had refused a doctor-recommended vaccination for themselves,
Using a diagonally weighted least squares (DWLS) algorithm, and this also showed no significant differences between those
a CFA was conducted on the 12 items previously selected as who previously had (MVAX = 3.43, SD = 0.74) and those who
comprising the VAX scale. The Normed Fit Index (NFI) indi- had not (MVAX = 3.18, SD = 1.10), t(90) = −0.66, p = .51,
cated good model fit (.97) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), g = 0.30, 95% CI [−1.032, 0.527] although, again, the trend
which has a greater penalty for complexity, indicated accept- was in the expected direction. It should be noted that fewer
able model fit (.94); thus, the expected four factors were individuals reported child-vaccine refusal in this study than in
retained. The means, standard deviations, and alpha reliabilities Study 1. Additionally, consistent with findings from Study 1,
for the VAX subscales in Study 2 (all of which were comprised those with higher VAX scale scores also reported being less
of the same items as described in Study 1) are found in Table 3. likely to get vaccinated against Ebola r(90) = −.53, p < .001
As in Study 1, we next examined the associations amongst and bird flu r(90) = −.48, p < .001 if outbreaks should occur and
the VAX subscale scores. As before, although the four factors vaccines were available.
were distinct, they were highly correlated with one another (rs VAX scale scores were not related to education level (although
ranging from .65 to .75), and so, we again combined the sub- it should be noted that the range on education was somewhat
scales into a single VAX score for subsequent analyses. restricted in this study, too, this time in the direction of lower
We next assessed test-retest reliability by correlating VAX education levels), self-rated health, or self-reported time per day
scale scores at the time of recruitment with scores approxi- spent viewing the news. However, VAX scale scores did strongly
mately 1 month later (30–38 days). Consistency over time was correlate with a perceived sensitivity to medicines and also pref-
high (r(61) = .84, p < .001), suggesting that this tool reliably erences for getting health information online (see Table 3).
measures the underlying construct of vaccination attitudes.
Next, convergent validity was evaluated by correlating the
VAX with the PACV safety-efficacy subscale. The more global Discussion
VAX measure correlated well (r(90) = .42, p < .001) with the
child-specific PACV subscale, providing further support for The goals of the study were to develop a short tool for
the validity of the VAX scale. assessing general vaccination attitudes, establish its reliability
Finally, we replicated the analyses relating VAX scores to the and validity, and evaluate its effectiveness for predicting vac-
behavioral and attitudinal validation measures. Results were cination behavior. The 12-item VAX scale is short, has high
consistent with findings from Study 1, with significantly higher internal consistency reliability, and comprises four distinct but
VAX scale scores observed for those who had not had an influ- correlated factors: mistrust of vaccine benefit, worries about
enza vaccine in the prior year (MVAX = 3.55, SD = 0.96) com- unforeseen future effects, concerns about commercial
pared to those who had (M VAX = 2.82, SD = 0.65), profiteering, and a preference for natural immunity.
t(90) = −2.98, p < .001, d = 0.89, 95% CI [−1.224, −0.236], Although different enough to warrant maintaining them as
and between those who did not intend to be vaccinated this year individual factors, these four factors reflect differences in con-
(MVAX = 3.61, SD = 0.92) and those who intended to be vacci- cerns about vaccination and perceived utility reflected by the
nated (MVAX = 2.75, SD = 0.64), t(90) = −3.70, p < .001, Necessity-Concerns Framework [25]. Scores on the VAX scale
d = 1.09, 95% CI [−0.395,-1.342]. Similar patterns were seen also correlate highly with perceived sensitivity to medicines,
for parents—those whose children had received an influenza another indicator of personal concern about how one’s body
vaccination in the prior year had significantly lower VAX scores might react to substances recommended by health care pro-
(MVAX = 2.14, SD = 0.87) than those whose children had not viders. Beliefs about the necessity of medicines and concerns
(MVAX = 3.45, SD = 1.02), t(64) = −7.205, p < .001, d = 1.38, about those medicines have been found in meta-analytic stud-
95% CI [−1.661, −0.919], and the same was true for those who ies to be relevant to medication adherence [26, 27], and ad-
said their child would receive a flu vaccine in the coming year herence to vaccination recommendations is likely no different.
(MVAX = 2.20, SD = 0.95) as compared with those who said Indeed, the VAX scale is significantly associated with previous
ann. behav. med.

vaccination behavior and refusal, as well as intentions to re- recommendations. Thus, although hesitancy is related to inten-
ceive future vaccines either for oneself or for one’s children. tions and behaviors in both groups, the link between the two
Future research with this measure should address the degree to appears stronger for the better-educated, making them a partic-
which these associations are robust relative to other factors ularly important group to target for intervention.
such as vaccine type, cost, and accessibility. In sum, the VAX scale is a short and simple tool that has
The scale is likely to be useful for identifying individuals demonstrated significant associations with vaccination behav-
who are unlikely to get vaccinated and for identifying their iors and intentions. As such, it provides an efficient method
strongest objections regarding vaccination. From a public for identifying those with vaccination resistance and, using
health perspective, the strong correlations amongst subscales subscale scores, enables a more nuanced understanding of
suggest that interventions and public health messages aimed at the nature of those views. It should be noted, however, that
changing attitudes toward vaccination should likely be broad the strong correlations amongst the four subscales indicate
in scope and cover the four main concerns identified in the that the most appropriate approach may be to calculate and
present scale. This does not mean, however, that there may be use the overall VAX scale score and that interventions should
no value in emphasizing area(s) of particular concern for in- target all four attitude areas, and it remains to be seen whether
dividuals. The VAX subscales now allow the possibility to differential emphasis on the four areas is warranted.
explore empirically whether an inclusive approach that places
special emphasis on one or more of the four dimensions might Compliance with Ethical Standards
be more effective than an approach that gives equal weight to
Ethical Adherence This study was approved by La Sierra University’s
each of the four dimensions.
Institutional Review Board (#LSU-1505), and all participants gave in-
The VAX scale is unrelated to level of schooling completed formed consent prior to entering the study. All data were then collected
both in the highly educated Study 1 sample and the less- using an anonymous, self-report questionnaire.
educated Study 2 sample, and is also uncorrelated with self-
reported health and time spent viewing news reports. It is, Conflicts of Interest Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and
Adherence to Ethical Standards Authors Leslie R. Martin and Keith J.
however, strongly and significantly correlated with the ten-
Petrie declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures, in-
dency to obtain health information online. This suggests an cluding the informed consent process, were conducted in accordance with
interesting possibility—that anti-vaccination attitudes are, at the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimen-
least in some cases, a facet of a more broad skepticism and tation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of
1975, as revised in 2000.
conspiratorial ideology [28, 29] fueled by websites and online
data that reflect misinformation about vaccines [30–32]. This
would also fit with prior findings that parents’ social networks
predict vaccination decisions and that the internet is a more References
important network source for nonvaccinators than for those
1. Palache A, Oriol-Mathieu V, Abelin A, Music T. Seasonal influenza
who vaccinate their children [33].
vaccine dose distribution in 157 countries (2004–2011). Vaccine.
The diversity of the samples in which this investigation was 2014;32:6369–6376.
conducted is a clear strength of the study. The first sample was 2. Pearce A, Law C, Elliman D, Cole TJ, Bedford H, Millennium
comprised of nearly equal numbers of women and men, a wide Cohort Study Child Health Group. Factors associated with uptake
age-range, and approximately 41% parents; the second sample of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine (MMR) and use of single
antigen vaccines in a contemporary UK cohort: prospective cohort
was two-thirds female, younger, and had a higher proportion study. BMJ. 2008;336:754–757.
(72%) of parents. In the first sample, participants were more 3. Jansen VA, Stollenwerk N, Jensen HJ, Ramsay ME, Edmunds WJ,
highly educated than average, which was initially viewed as a Rhodes CJ. Measles outbreaks in a population with declining vac-
strength because vaccine reluctance was expected to be higher cine uptake. Science. 2003;301:804.
4. Phadke VK, Bednarczyk RA, Salmon DA, Omer SB. Association
in the more educated and affluent. This overrepresentation of between vaccine refusal and vaccine-preventable diseases in the
highly educated individuals was viewed as optimizing the op- United States: a review of measles and pertussis. JAMA.
portunity for identifying the entire range of negative vaccine 2016;315:1149–1158.
attitudes in this more-likely resistant group. With the inclusion 5. Atwell JE, Salmon DA. Pertussis resurgence and vaccine uptake:
implications for reducing vaccine hesitancy. Pediatrics. 2014;134:
of the second sample, however, we found that the less-educated
602–604.
group actually expressed more hesitancy toward vaccines than 6. Cherry JD. Epidemic pertussis in 2012—the resurgence of a
did the better-educated group. And yet, with regard to reports of vaccine-preventable disease. N Engl J Med. 2012;367:785–787.
behaviors and intentions, this same group was more likely to 7. Petrie KJ, Sivertsen B, Hysing M, et al. Thoroughly modern
express that they had followed vaccination recommendations worries: the relationship of worries about modernity to reported
symptoms, health and medical care utilization. J Psychosom Res.
and would continue to do so. That is, although they appeared to 2001;51:395–401.
be quite concerned about various negative possibilities related 8. Petrie KJ, Wessely S. Modern worries, new technology, and med-
to vaccines, they were less likely to actively disregard medical icine. BMJ. 2002;324:690–691.
ann. behav. med.

9. Brown KF, Kroll JS, Hudson MJ, et al. Factors underlying parental 21. Faasse K, Grey A, Horne R, Petrie KJ. High perceived sensitivity to
decisions about combination childhood vaccinations including medicines is associated with higher medical care utilisation, in-
MMR: a systematic review. Vaccine. 2010;28:4235–4248. creased symptom reporting and greater information-seeking about
10. Garcini L, Galvan T, Barnack-Tavlaris J. The study of human pap- medication. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2015;24:592–599.
illomavirus (HPV) vaccine uptake from a parental perspective: a 22. Stewart AL, Hays RD, Ware JEJ. Health perceptions, energy/fatigue,
systematic review of observational studies in the United States. and health distress measure. In: Stewart EL, Ware JEJ, eds.
Vaccine. 2012;30:4588–4595. Measuring functioning and well-being: The medical outcomes study
11. Hofmann F, Ferracin C, Marsh G, Dumas R. Influenza vaccination approach. Durham, NC: Duke University Press; 1992:143–172.
of healthcare workers: a literature review of attitudes and beliefs. 23. Horn, JL. A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor
Infection. 2006;34:142–147. analysis. Psychometrika. 1965;30:179–185.
12. Yaqub O, Castle-Clarke S, Sevdalis N, Chataway J. Attitudes to 24. Weng, LJ, Cheng, ChP. Parallel analysis with unidimensional bina-
vaccination: a critical review. Soc Sci Med. 2014;112:1–11. ry data. Educ Psychol Meas. 2005;65:697–716.
13. Kennedy A, Basket M, Sheedy K. Vaccine attitudes, concerns, and 25. Horne R, Weinman J, Hankins M. The beliefs about medicines
information sources reported by parents of young children: results questionnaire: the development and evaluation of a new method
from the 2009 HealthStyles survey. Pediatrics. 2011;127(Suppl 1): for assessing the cognitive representation of medication. Psychol
S92–9. Health. 1999;14:1–24.
14. Opel DJ, Mangione-Smith R, Taylor JA, et al. Development of a 26. Foot H, La Caze A, Gujral G, Cottrell N. The necessity–concerns
survey to identify vaccine-hesitant parents: the parent attitudes framework predicts adherence to medication in multiple illness con-
about childhood vaccines survey. Hum Vaccines. 2011;7:419–425. ditions: a meta-analysis. Patient Educ Couns. 2016;99:706–717.
15. Larson HJ, Jarrett C, Schulz WS, et al. Measuring vaccine hesitan-
27. Horne R, Chapman SC, Parham R, Freemantle N, Forbes A, Cooper
cy: the development of a survey tool. Vaccine. 2015;33:4165–4175.
V. Understanding patients’ adherence-related beliefs about medicines
16. Lee S, Newman PA, Duan N, Cunningham WE. Development of an
prescribed for long-term conditions: a meta-analytic review of the
HIV vaccine attitudes scale to predict HIV vaccine acceptability
necessity-concerns framework. PloS one. 2013;8(12):e80633.
among vulnerable populations: LA VOICES. Vaccine. 2014;32:
5013–5018. 28. Lewandowsky S, Gignac GE, Oberauer K. The role of conspiracist
17. McRee AL, Brewer NT, Reiter PL, Gottlieb SL, Smith JS. The ideation and worldviews in predicting rejection of science. PLoS
carolina HPV immunization attitudes and beliefs scale (CHIAS): One. 2013;8(10):e75637.
scale development and associations with intentions to vaccinate. 29. Page LA, Petrie KJ, Wessely SC. Psychosocial responses to envi-
Sex Transm Dis. 2010;37:234–239. ronmental incidents: a review and a proposed typology. J
18. Dempsey AF, Fuhrel-Forbis A, Konrath S. Use of the carolina HPV Psychosom Res. 2006;60:413–422.
immunization attitudes and beliefs scale (CHIAS) in young adult 30. Eysenbach G, Powell J, Kuss O, Sa E. Empirical studies assessing
women. PloS one. 2014;9(6):e100193. the quality of health information for consumers on the world wide
19. Prislin R, Dyer JA, Blakely CH, Johnson CD. Immunization status web: a systematic review. JAMA. 2002;287:2691–2700.
and sociodemographic characteristics: the mediating role of beliefs, 31. Kata A. A postmodern pandora's box: anti-vaccination misinforma-
attitudes, and perceived control. Am J Public Health. 1998;88: tion on the internet. Vaccine. 2010;28:1709–1716.
1821–1826. 32. Wolfe RM, Sharp LK. Vaccination or immunization? The impact of
20. Horne R, Faasse K, Cooper V, et al. The perceived sensitivity to search terms on the internet. J Health Commun. 2005;10:537–551.
medicines (PSM) scale: an evaluation of validity and reliability. Br 33. Brunson EK. The impact of social networks on parents’ vaccination
J Health Psychol. 2013;18:18–30. decisions. Pediatrics. 2013;131:e1397–404.

You might also like