You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11 – 19

Factors associated with objective (actigraphic) and subjective sleep quality


in young adult women
Shelley S. Tworogera,T, Scott Davisb,c, Michael V. Vitiellod,
Martha J. Lentze, Anne McTiernanb,c,f
a
Channing Laboratory, Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA 02115, United States
b
Division of Public Health Sciences, The Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, Seattle, WA 98109, United States
c
Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health and Community Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, United States
d
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, United States
e
School of Nursing, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, United States
f
Department of Medicine, School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195, United States
Received 24 July 2004; accepted 18 March 2005

Abstract
Objective: The aim of this study was to describe factors medication use, employment, increased daylight hours, longer
associated with actigraphic and subjective sleep quality in young menstrual cycle length, and higher body mass index (BMI) were
women. Methods: Participants were 73 regularly menstruating associated with poorer actigraphic sleep measures. Employment,
women, 20 – 40 years old, who were not taking oral contra- age, and perceived stress were associated with subjective sleep
ceptives, pregnant, or shift workers. Women contributed an quality. Conclusion: Multiple factors were associated with sleep
average of 7 nights of actigraphy data during the luteal menstrual quality in these young women who were sleeping at home.
cycle phase, resulting in a total of 595 nights of data. Results: However, the associations differed for subjectively versus acti-
One night of actigraphy data was unreliable for measuring total graphically assessed sleep quality. Actigraphy is feasible for
sleep time, sleep onset, and time in bed (intraclass correlation measuring sleep, but multiple recording nights may be needed to
V.15) but was acceptable for measuring sleep efficiency and total obtain reliable estimates.
wake time (intraclass correlation [ICC]=.52). Going to bed late, D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Sleep; Actigraphy; Women; Employment; Day of week; Daylight hours; Menstrual cycle length; Alcohol consumption; Perceived stress

Introduction ments of sleep quality should be considered because the


two are only modestly correlated [4 –6], suggesting that
Chronically disturbed sleep is associated with a number each modality assesses different aspects of an individual’s
of deleterious effects, including reduced memory and sleep experience [5– 7].
learning ability, compromised immune function, and an Actigraphy is a validated, objective method to monitor
increased risk of cardiovascular disease [1–3]. Thus, it is sleep and other activity patterns (and their variability), over
important to understand factors that are associated with long periods in the home [8–10], and has been successfully
poor sleep quality in a natural environment. In assessing used to assess sleep quality among children and adolescents
such relationships, both subjective and objective assess- [9,10] and the elderly [11]. All three studies reported
considerable within-subject variability in sleep patterns
[9–11]. However, little actigraphically assessed sleep quality
Abbreviations: LH, Luteinizing hormone; CI, Confidence interval.
information is available for young adult, menstruating
T Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 617 525 2087; fax: +1 617 525 2008. women [12]. Two small actigraphy studies (nV 20)
E-mail address: nhsst@channing.harvard.edu (S.S. Tworoger). of young and middle-aged men and women reported

0022-3999/05/$ – see front matter D 2005 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.03.008
12 S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19

consistently high Pearson correlations between two consec- surge, before proceeding to the intervention phase. After the
utive nights for sleep efficiency ( Pz.70) and awakenings next two detectable LH surges, the participant was
z3 minutes ( Pz.69), but not for total sleep time ( P=.60 and scheduled for the two measurement periods in which the
P=.05, respectively; [13,14]). study intervention was applied and sleep assessed via
The purpose of this study was to examine objectively actigraphy (see below). Both measurement periods were 5
assessed sleep patterns across multiple nights (range=2 –10), nights long and started approximately 2 days after the LH
using actigraphy, in a large sample of 20- to 40-year-old surge. Half the women were randomly assigned to the
women. Women were assessed while sleeping at home, intervention and half to ambient exposure (no intervention)
where they were unrestricted in their behavior. Specifically, during the first measurement period; exposure status was
we assessed the within- and between-subject variability and switched at the second period.
the reliability of actigraphic sleep assessments. We also The intervention consisted of a continuous, 60-Hz
examined potential risk factors for poor sleep quality for magnetic field, 0.5 to 1.0 AT above the ambient levels, at
both actigraphic and subjective sleep assessments and the participant’s normal head location on the bed. The
determined whether patterns of association differed between exposure was administered by placing a common household
the two types of measures. Participants from this study were appliance underneath the bed, which was plugged into a
drawn from an intervention trial examining the effect of power strip that was either off or on. Participants were
magnetic field exposure on hormone levels and sleep blinded to exposure status.
patterns; the current study represents a cross-sectional
analysis of these data. Sleep data

Participants wore an actigraph (Actiwatch-16, Mini


Methods Mitter, Bend, OR) on their nondominant wrist [15] from
bedtime to rise time all nights of both measurement
Participants periods. Because the parent study protocol was compli-
cated, participants were not asked to wear the actigraph
Participants were identified from women in a randomized during the day to minimize the burden of participating in
crossover trial investigating the effect of a nighttime the additional sleep component. Written and oral instruc-
magnetic field on melatonin and reproductive hormone tions for using the actigraph were provided at each
levels in premenopausal women, specifically during the measurement period.
luteal phase of the menstrual cycle. This menstrual phase The actigraph is designed for long-term monitoring of
was chosen to due to the high concentrations of estrogens, gross motor activity in humans and has an accelerometer
the primary hormone of interest, during this part of the capable of sensing motion with a minimal resultant force of
cycle. Eligibility criteria included being 20 to 40 years old, 0.01g [16]. We used 1-min sampling intervals. All but
not taking oral contraceptives or other hormones in the past eight participants used the same actigraph at both measure-
6 months, having a body mass index (BMI) V30.0 kg/m2, ment periods.
having regular menstrual cycles, not pregnant or breast Actigraph data were downloaded onto a personal
feeding during the previous year, not a shift worker, and not computer and analyzed via a FORTRAN program using
taking melatonin supplements. Participants lived in the the method of Cole et al. [17]. Using polysomnographic
greater Seattle, WA area. validation data provided by Mini Mitter [16], the sleep/wake
Participants who contacted the study telephone line in status of each minute of the night was assessed as follows:
response to posted advertisements for the parent study,
which did not mention sleep in any way, completed an D ¼ 0:025ð0:04A2 þ 0:20A1 þ 1:0A0 þ 0:20Aþ1
initial screening interview and, if eligible, were scheduled þ 0:04Aþ2 Þ
for a home visit. Between March and September 2001, 85
women from the primary study were asked to participate in where A x is the number of detectable motions in that minute.
an additional component to assess sleep. We did not screen If D z1, then the participant was considered to be awake
participants for sleep disturbances. during minute A 0, otherwise, the participant was considered
At the first home visit, a technician obtained written to be asleep. To increase accuracy, we applied the five
consent, approved by the Institutional Review Board at the rescoring rules outlined by Cole et al. [17], which corrected
Fred Hutchinson Cancer Research Center, and taught for the problem that participants falling asleep after waking
participants to determine their ovulation date using a up during the night tend to stop moving a few minutes
menstruation calendar and a commercial ovulation kit before polysomnography indicates sleep onset. Initial sleep
(Assure LH Ovulation Predictor, Conception Technologies, onset was considered to be at the beginning of the first
San Diego, CA), which detects the luteinizing hormone 20-min interval in which no more than 1 min was scored
(LH) surge 24 to 48 h before ovulation. Participants tracked as wake; this criterion had the highest correlation with
one complete menstrual cycle, including detecting an LH polysomnography [17].
S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19 13

Other data collection because the intervention was not associated with sleep
patterns [23].
Information about age and self-reported menstrual Using linear regression, we assessed whether sleep
cycle length were collected during the screening inter- outcomes with an ICC N0.30 were associated with various
view. On the first day of each measurement period, a potential risk factors. This ICC was chosen because, based
technician measured height to the nearest 0.1 cm; weight on the Spearman–Brown formula, we needed five or less
to the nearest pound; administered a structured interview days of sleep assessment to achieve a reliability z.70; about
collecting demographic information, job status, exercise 83% of the participants had at least 5 nights of data. The risk
habits, and the Perceived Stress Scale [18]; and asked factors considered include (1) bedtime differed from usual
participants to complete a truncated version of the bedtime on the PSQI (within 1 h, z1 h later, and z1 h
Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI; [19]). Participants earlier); (2) rise time differed from usual rise time on the
were not asked to have their bed partners complete that PSQI (within 1 h, z1 h later, and z1 h earlier); (3) alcohol
portion of the PSQI due to the already large burden of servings (none, one, and two or more); (4) min/week of
the parent study. Usual bedtime and rise time were exercise during the previous month (linear); (5) took a
abstracted from the PSQI, and the global index assessing prescription or over-the-counter medication other than a
sleep quality was calculated. Participants also completed a sedative (no vs. yes); (6) job status (employed vs. unem-
nightly diary during both measurement periods, recording ployed); (7) perceived Stress Scale in quartiles (V12, 13 –18,
their bedtime, rise time, medication use, and alcohol 19 –24, and z25); (8) day of week (weeknight vs. weekend);
consumption. Hours of daylight were determined via (9) daylight hours (linear); (10) age (linear); (11) menstrual
sunrise/sunset tables calculated by the National Research cycle length (linear); and (12) BMI (linear). Categorical
Council (Herzberg Institute of Astrophysics, Victoria, exposures were included as indicator variables in the model.
BC). On the last night of the measurement period, All variables were included in one model, adjusting for
participants collected all urine excreted during the night intervention status (exposed, not-exposed), exposure order
after sleep onset plus the first morning void. To determine (exposed/not-exposed, not-exposed/exposed), measurement
whether the cycle was ovulatory, pregnanediol-3-glucur- period (1, 2), night (1, 2, 3, 4, 5), and external sleep
onide was measured in the urine by enzyme immunoassay disruptions (yes, no). For this last covariate, we used the
[20] at the University of Southern California under the question about botherQ sleep disturbances on the PSQI as a
direction of Dr. Frank Z. Stanczyk. surrogate for disruptions due to bed partners, children, and
pets. We also considered whether these associations differed
Statistical methods on weeknights versus weekend nights and was similar
between the intervention and nonintervention periods. We
We considered seven actigraphic sleep measures (total did not consider sedative use due to the small number of
sleep time, sleep efficiency, total wake time, minutes awake women (n=2) using them. For this analysis, we excluded six
after sleep onset, the number of awakenings, number of records with extreme bed (N03:00) or rise times (N14:00).
awakenings z3 minutes, and sleep onset latency) and three Using logistic regression, we determined whether self-
measures abstracted from the sleep diary (time in bed, reported poor sleep quality (global PSQI score N5) was
bedtime, and rise time). Sleep efficiency was calculated as associated with the following exposures measured before or
(total sleep time/time in bed) 100. The within- and at the same time as the PSQI: age (20 –24, 25 –29, 30 –34, and
between-subject standard deviations of each measure were 35 – 40), min/week of exercise during previous month (V139,
determined using a random effects model, controlling for 140 –249 250 –359, and z360), menstrual cycle length, BMI,
exposure status, exposure order, and measurement period as employment status, and Perceived Stress Scale, adjusting for
fixed effects [21]. We also considered whether the within- measurement period and external sleep disruptions. Age and
subject variance differed for participants who had different minutes of exercise were categorized for this analysis because
actigraphs at the two measurement periods. The intraclass their association with self-reported sleep quality was not
correlation (ICC) is an estimate of the reliability of a specific linear. Due to the correlated nature of the data, we used
sleep measure with 1 night of data collection and provides an generalized estimating equations with an independent work-
indication of within-subject stability across nights. We ing correlation matrix for all regression models [24]. All
calculated the ICC by dividing the between-subject variance statistical tests were two sided, and analyses were conducted
by the total variance [22]; we considered all nights, week- using Stata Version 8.0 (College Station, TX).
nights only (Sunday through Thursday), and weekend nights Of the 85 eligible women from the parent trial, 77 (91%)
only (Friday and Saturday). The Spearman–Brown formula consented to participate in the sleep component. Nineteen
[22] was used to estimate how many nights of aggregated participants completed one measurement period (16 became
sleep data were needed to obtain a representative sample of a ineligible or dropped the study and for 3 no actigraph was
participant’s sleep patterns; a reliability z.70 was used based available) and 58 completed both. We excluded measure-
on previous literature [9]. We used data from both the ment periods in which the participant was anovulatory
intervention and nonintervention measurement periods (pregnanediol-3-glucuronide b1.25 Ag/mg creatinine) or
14 S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19

Table 1 diary data. Some participants did not wear the actigraph for
Characteristics of 73 women who provided 595 nights of data collection
the entire night, put it on N30 min after bedtime, or took it
Mean (S.D.a) off N30 min before rise time, resulting in at least partially
Age (years) 30.6 (5.3) missing data for 82 (14%) of 595 nights. Removing the
Body mass index (kg/m2) 23.8 (3.7) actigraph early was associated with significantly worse
Menstrual cycle length (days) 28.8 (2.1)
sleep outcomes compared with nights that had complete
Daylight (h) 14.3 (1.1)
Exercise in past month (min/wk) 276.5 (199.0) data (data not shown).
Perceived Stress Scaleb 18.8 (8.1)
Usual bedtimec (min) 22:50 (56.6)
Usual rise timec (min) 06:53 (71.0) Results
Number of participants n (%)
Self-reported poor sleepd 22 (30.1) On average, participants were 31 years old and had a
Exposed to magnetic field during 36 (49.3) normal BMI (Table 1). The usual bed and rise times reported
first measurement period on the PSQI were about 22:50 and 06:53, respectively, and
Employed 65 (89.0) nearly a third of women had a global score N5 on the PSQI,
indicating poor sleep. Some type of nonsedative medication
Number of nights in which participants
Took a sedative 7 (1.2) was used on 21.5% of the nights; however, sedatives were
Took another medicatione 128 (21.5) used by only two participants on a total of 7 nights. Alcohol
Weekend day 173 (29.1) was consumed on about 27% of nights; on half of these
Consumed nights, one drink was consumed. Bed and rise times differed
One alcoholic drink 78 (13.1)
by over an hour from the usual times reported on the PSQI
Two or more alcoholic drinks 82 (13.8)
Bedtimec for over a third of the nights.
z1 h later than usual 176 (29.6) The mean total sleep time was almost 7 h per night, but
z1 h earlier than usual 52 (8.8) ranged from about 2.9 to 11.5 h (Table 2). Similarly, mean
Rise timec sleep efficiency was 88.4%, but varied from 62.6% to
z1 h later than usual 141 (23.7)
98.7%. The mean number of awakenings was 22.1 per night,
z1 h earlier than usual 70 (11.8)
a
with an average of 4.1 awakenings being z3 min long. The
Standard deviation.
b
Scale ranges from 0 to 56.
median sleep onset was 7 min. Bed and rise times spanned a
c
Usual bed and rise times were abstracted from the PSQI. wide range. For all sleep measures, the within-subject
d
Poor sleep was defined as having a global score N5 on the PSQI. standard deviation was nearly the same as or larger than
e
Any nonsedative prescription or over-the-counter medication. the between-subject standard deviation. In general, the
within-subject standard deviations were slightly lower when
menstruating (n=10), or if sleep data for 4 or 5 nights were excluding the eight participants with different actigraphs at
missing (n=6). No significant differences existed between their measurement periods (data not shown).
participants who contributed data for two (n=46) versus one The overall ICC was low for total sleep time ( ICC=.15),
measurement period (n=27). We had 119 measurement sleep onset ( ICC=.09), and time in bed ( ICC=.13); the
periods from 73 participants available for analysis, with an Spearman–Brown formula estimates that N5 nights of data
average of 7 nights of actigraphic data and 8 nights of sleep collection are needed to obtain a reliability z.70 (Table 3).

Table 2
Mean, median, between- and within-subject standard deviations, and range for sleep parameters measured via actigraphy or abstracted from the sleep diary
Standard deviation
na Mean Median Between subject Within subject Range
Total sleep time (min)b 513 419.5 419 27.2 64.5 175–689
Sleep efficiency (%)b 513 88.4 89.3 3.4 3.3 62.6–98.7
Total wake time (min)b 513 55.0 51 17.5 16.8 7–163
Wake after sleep onset (min)b 513 39.9 37 13.2 13.8 4–118
Awakeningsb 513 22.1 22 5.0 6.1 3–51
Awakenings z3 minutesb 513 4.1 4 2.0 2.2 0–15
Sleep onset (min)b 532 12.7 7 4.6 14.3 1–89
Time in bed (min)c 594 480.9 481 28.2 74.4 201–751
Bedtime (min)d 594 23:18 23:00 47.2 63.7 19:00–07:00
Rise time (min)d 594 07:18 07:07 50.5 71.5 03:57–16:00
a
Number of nights with nonmissing data.
b
Assessed via actigraphy.
c
Calculated using information from the sleep diary.
d
Obtained from the sleep diary.
S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19 15

Table 3
Intraclass correlation coefficientsa (ICC) for the reliability of sleep measures, for all nights and by day of weekb
All nights Weeknights Weekend nights
nc ICC Nightsd nc ICC Nightsd nc ICC Nightsd
Total sleep time (min) 513 0.15 N5 370 0.24 N5 143 0.13 N5
Sleep efficiency (%) 513 0.52 3 370 0.50 3 143 0.49 3
Total wake time (min) 513 0.52 3 370 0.52 3 143 0.39 4
Wake after sleep onset (min) 513 0.48 3 370 0.51 3 143 0.23 N5
Awakenings 513 0.40 4 370 0.40 4 143 0.29 N5
Awakenings z3 minutes 513 0.45 3 370 0.45 3 143 0.43 3
Sleep onset (min) 532 0.09 N5 383 0.08 N5 149 0.16 N5
Time in bed (min) 594 0.13 N5 422 0.20 N5 172 0.08 N5
Bedtime (min) 594 0.35 5 422 0.45 3 172 0.27 N5
Rise time (min) 594 0.33 5 422 0.55 2 172 0.25 N5
a
The intraclass correlation is equivalent to the reliability for 1 night of data collection.
b
Weeknights are Sunday through Thursday nights and weekend nights are Friday and Saturday nights.
c
Number of nights with nonmissing data.
d
Estimated number of nights of data collection needed to have a reliability of N.70, based on the Spearman–Brown formula.

All other sleep outcomes had an ICC z.30; sleep efficiency than usual was associated with increased total wake time,
and total wake time had the highest ICCs and needed an wake after sleep onset, and awakenings. Using any non-
estimated 3 to 4 days of data collection to obtain reliable sedative medication was associated with 1.9% lower sleep
estimates. The ICCs were substantially higher on week- efficiency [95% confidence interval (CI): 3.8, 0.2] and 11.3
nights versus weekend nights for all actigraphic sleep more min of total wake time (95% CI: 0.5, 22.1). Compared
measures, except sleep efficiency, awakenings z3 minutes, with employed women, unemployed women had 1.9% higher
and sleep onset. sleep efficiency (95% CI: 0.7, 3.2), 10.6 min less total wake
No difference in sleep efficiency was detected for nights in time (95% CI: 17.0, 4.2), and 1.2 fewer awakenings
which the participant’s bed or rise time was more than an hour z3 minutes (95% CI: 2.1, 0.3). On weekend nights,
different versus within an hour of usual (Table 4). However, participants had 1.7 more awakenings (95% CI: 0.0, 3.3)
going to bed z1 h later than usual was associated with than on weeknights. Increasing daylight hours, menstrual
decreased total wake time, wake after sleep onset, and cycle length, and BMI were associated with significantly
awakenings. Going to bed z1 h earlier or rising z1 h later lower sleep efficiency and more total wake time. Alcohol,

Table 4
Averagea difference (95% CIb) in selected sleep outcomes by various exposures that may affect sleep
Sleep efficiency Total wake time Wake after sleep onset Awakenings Awakenings z3
(%) (min) (min) minutes
Bedtime z1 h later than 0.5 (0.5, 1.5) 8.9TT(14.0, 3.7) 5.7** (9.9, 1.5) 3.7** (5.0, 2.3) 0.3 (1.0, 0.4)
usualc
Bedtime z1 h earlier than 0.3 (2.2, 1.5) 10.5** (1.5, 19.4) 8.6** (1.0, 16.3) 4.7** (1.3, 8.0) 1.1 (0.5, 2.8)
usualc
Rise time z1 h later than 0.1 (1.6, 1.4) 6.6T (0.6, 13.8) 4.3* (0.2, 8.8) 2.6** (0.3, 4.8) 0.4 (1.3, 1.2)
usualc
Rise time z1 h earlier than 0.8 (2.6, 1.1) 5.2 (13.9, 3.5) 4.7 (11.1, 1.7) 2.8* (5.8, 0.2) 0.2 (1.3, 1.0)
usualc
Used a medication vs. notd 1.9* (3.9, 0.2) 11.3** (0.5, 22.1) 7.0* (0.9, 14.9) 2.0 (1.1, 5.1) 0.7 (0.5, 2.0)
Unemployed vs. employed 1.9** (0.7, 3.2) 10.6** (17.0, 4.2) 6.2** (11.9, 0.6) 2.8** (5.6, 0.0) 1.2** (2.1, 0.3)
Weekend vs. weekday 0.1 (0.9, 1.1) 0.8 (4.0, 5.6) 2.5 (0.9, 6.0) 1.7** (0.0, 3.3) 0.3 (0.2, 0.9)
Daylight hourse 0.6** (1.2, 0.1) 3.4** (0.5, 6.3) 2.0* (0.3, 4.2) 0.7 (0.2, 1.5) 0.2 (0.2, 0.5)
Menstrual cycle lengthe 0.4** (0.6, 0.1) 1.7** (0.2, 3.2) 1.3** (0.0, 2.6) 0.4 (0.2, 1.1) 0.2 (0.05, 0.4)
(day)
Body mass indexe (kg/m2) 0.2* (0.4, 0.05) 1.0** (0.05, 2.0) 0.8** (0.0, 1.6) 0.1 (0.2, 0.4) 0.1 (0.05, 0.2)
a
All exposures were modeled simultaneously and were adjusted for intervention status, exposure order, measurement period, night, external sleep
disruptions, exercise in the previous month, age, alcohol servings, and perceived stress. Sleep measures were not associated with exercise, age, alcohol, or stress.
b
95% confidence interval.
c
Usual bed and rise times were abstracted from the PSQI; reference groups are nights in which bed or rise times are within 1 h of usual.
d
Any nonsedative prescription or over-the-counter medication.
e
Included as a linear variable, such that the coefficient is the change in the sleep parameter for a (1) 1-h increase in daylight, (2) 1-day increase in
menstrual cycle length, or (3) 1 kg/m2 increase in BMI.
T PV.10.
TT PV.05.
16 S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19

exercise over the previous month, perceived stress, and age there is a substantial proportion with at least some sleep
were not associated with actigraphic sleep measures (data not disturbances, indicating that it is important to study sleep in
shown). Associations were not substantially different on this population.
weekend versus weeknights (data not shown). Overall, the We observed a wide range of sleep patterns, although we
results were similar when only considering nonintervention excluded women who did shift work, had irregular
nights, although the association between medication use and menstrual cycles, or had recently had or breastfed a child.
sleep outcomes was weaker (data not shown). This suggests that the stringent eligibility criteria did not
On average, participants went to bed 14.7 min later (95% lead to the exclusion of all women with poor sleep patterns.
CI: 2.7, 32.0) and rose 48.3 min later (95% CI: 30.1, 66.5) We also found that the within- and between-subject
on weekend versus weeknights. Unemployed participants variabilities were similar and the ICCs were moderate to
had significantly later bedtimes and rise times than did the low, signifying that sleep measures in a natural environment
employed participants only on weeknights, and older age may not be stable across nights, necessitating multiple
was associated with earlier bedtimes on all nights and earlier nights of data collection. Pair-wise, consecutive-night
rise times on weekdays (data not shown). correlations for all sleep measures were similar to the ICCs
Self-reported poor sleep (global PSQI score N5) was not (data not shown). Studies in both children [9,10] and the
significantly associated with menstrual cycle length, BMI, elderly [11] also reported large within-subject variabilities
or exercise during the previous month (data not shown). and low ICCs for sleep in a natural environment. However,
However, 25- to 29- and 30- to 34-year-olds had a modestly two small studies collecting two consecutive nights of sleep
increased risk of reporting poor sleep compared with 20- to data reported higher correlations for sleep efficiency and
24-year olds [odds ratio (OR): 5.3, 95% CI: 0.8, 33.6 and awakenings z3 minutes than our study [13,14]. This
OR: 7.4, 95% CI: 1.2, 46.0, respectively]; the oldest women discrepancy could be explained by the healthy population
were not significantly different from the youngest. Unem- used or the inclusion of men in these latter studies.
ployed versus employed women had a modestly increased Furthermore, we found that, for most sleep measures, the
risk (OR: 5.1, 95% CI: 1.0, 25.7) of reporting poor sleep. ICC was lower on weekend nights than on weeknights,
Score on the Perceived Stress Scale was positively suggesting that sleep is more variable on weekends. In our
associated with risk of reporting poor sleep ( P trend=.04). study, sleep efficiency was the most stable sleep parameter
on both weekends and weeknights.
We also examined potential risk factors for poor sleep
Discussion and found multiple associations. Medication use, but not
alcohol consumption or exercise over the past month, was
This is the first large study to examine sleep quality and associated with worse sleep patterns. Taking a medication
the factors associated with it among young women using may reflect the presence of an underlying chronic disease,
both actigraphic and subjective assessment methods while such as asthma or gastrointestinal problems, that could
sleeping at home, where they were unrestricted in their affect sleep [29,30]. Although we did not find that alcohol
behavior. A unique aspect of this study is that women were was associated with sleep patterns, other studies suggest that
assessed during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle; it may initially improve sleep but then disrupt sleep during
sleep during this phase may be more variable than other the second half of the night [31,32]. We may not have
menstrual phases due to its hormonal complexity. Overall, observed an association with alcohol because we considered
actigraphy was an acceptable modality for measuring sleep sleep across the entire night. Other research suggests that
in this population, as over 90% of participants consented to exercise may moderately improve sleep [33,34], however,
participate. We observed a wide range of sleep patterns in our assessment of exercise may not have been sensitive
this young adult female sample, with low ICCs for total enough to observe an association because we only asked
sleep time, sleep onset, and time in bed, but higher ICCs for about exercise over the previous month.
sleep efficiency and total wake time. Unusual bed or rise Sleep hygiene recommendations suggest that it is
times, medication use, employment, day of week, daylight important to establish a routine bed and rise time [35]. In
hours, menstrual cycle length, and BMI were associated our study, nearly 40% of bedtimes and over a third of rise
with actigraphic sleep measures, while age, alcohol con- times were more than 1 h different than the usual reported
sumption, exercise over the previous month, and perceived times, suggesting that fluctuations in bed and rise times are
stress were not. Employment, age, and perceived stress were common. While such fluctuations were associated with total
associated with subjective sleep quality. wake time and awakenings, there were no associations with
About 30% of the participants had a score N5 on the sleep efficiency. This discrepancy is likely explained by the
PSQI, indicating a perception of poor sleep quality; only fact that total wake time and the number of awakenings are
two participants (2.4%) reported taking a sedative. This is not adjusted for the amount of time spent in bed.
consistent with surveys of sleep problems in this age group We also observed that an increasing number of daylight
among different populations [25–28]. Thus, although young hours was associated with lower sleep efficiency and more
people have fewer sleep problems than the elderly do [27], total wake time, despite the fact that data collection occurred
S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19 17

between March and September. To our knowledge, no other sleep data in a natural environment. Another unique aspect
studies have directly assessed the association between the of the study is that we did not place restrictions on
number of daylight hours and sleep quality. However, participants with respect to various parameters that may
several studies have suggested that sleep patterns and affect sleep, such as medication use and alcohol consump-
circadian rhythms can vary by season in men [36 –38]; tion. This allowed us to consider the variability of sleep
although one study of 190 men and women (ages 40 –59) patterns in a natural environment and look at multiple
found no effect of season on subjective sleep quality [39]. exposures that may affect sleep. In addition, measurement
These data suggest that studies measuring participants at periods were conducted during the same menstrual phase,
multiple time points should either ensure similar hours of thus eliminating the possibility that observed associations
daylight between measurement periods or adjust for this were due to differing sleep patterns across the menstrual
factor during the analytic phase. cycle [41– 43]. We also had an average of 7 nights of
In addition, we found that increased BMI and longer actigraphic sleep assessment from each woman, therefore,
menstrual cycle length were associated with slightly worse our data are relatively reliable with respect to the analyses
sleep efficiency, despite the limited range of these exposures exploring the association between sleep and other factors.
in our study. Obesity (BMI N30 kg/m2) is associated with Finally, our analytic technique accounted for the correlated
sleep apnea and other sleep disorders [40]. Our data suggest nature of the data, which has the benefit of providing valid
that even women who are overweight (BMI between 25 and standard error estimates.
30) may experience reductions in sleep quality. To our The present study, however, has several weaknesses.
knowledge, no other studies have examined menstrual cycle First, because the parent trial was not designed specifically
length and sleep; however, previous research suggests that to assess sleep patterns, we did not collect information on
sleep patterns change across the menstrual cycle [41– 43] some important exposures, such as caffeine consumption,
and by ovulation status [44]. pets in bed, the presence of small children in the home, or
As expected, we found that participants went to bed and bed partners. Although this does not affect the validity of the
got up later on weekends versus weeknights. This is reliability analysis, these exposures could potentially con-
consistent with a study of 266 healthy participants, aged found the observed associations between other exposures
20 to 50 years, which reported a 26-min later bedtime and a and sleep; we did adjust our analyses for external sleep
53-min later wake time on weekends [45]. We also found disruptions, primarily bed partner snoring, pets, and small
that the relationships of employment status and age with bed children. Second, we had some exclusion criteria (e.g., no
and rise times differed by the day of week. This suggests shift work) that may have excluded women with the worst
that the sleep experience on weekend days may differ from sleep. Thus, our results may not be generalizeable to all
that on weekdays; thus, sleep on both types of days should women in this age group. Third, although actigraphy is an
be collected. objective assessment of sleep, it overestimates total sleep
We also observed that patterns of association of time compared with polysomnography because it cannot
perceived stress, age, and employment status differed for detect wake time in which no movement occurs [17,48,49].
actigraphic versus subjective sleep assessments. For exam- This overestimation may be larger among those with poor
ple, perceived stress was not associated with actigraphic sleep quality [17,50,51], possibly causing an attenuation of
sleep measures, but was positively associated with poor the observed associations.
subjective sleep. This is consistent with two studies that Another limitation was that 14% of the nights had some
reported no difference in perceived stress between good and missing data, which may limit the generalizability of our
poor sleepers and that perceived stress was associated with results. We hypothesize that most missing data were due to
subjective, but not objective (actigraph), sleep [46,47]. the participant forgetting to put on the actigraph when going
Similarly, we found that unemployment was associated with to bed. However, for about 25% of the missing nights, the
better actigraphic sleep measures but worse subjective sleep participant took off the actigraph early, i.e., before getting
quality. Likewise, age was not associated with actigraphic out of bed. Sleep measures on these nights were signifi-
sleep measures, but some age groups (25–35 years) were cantly worse than nights with complete data, even when
more likely to report poor subjective sleep. This age group delineating all missing minutes as sleep. This suggests that
likely includes women with young children, as our study if a participant is sleeping poorly, she may remove the
protocol did not allow us to collect this information; actigraph in an effort to improve sleep. Thus, it is important
however, this potential relationship remains conjectural. to query participants about whether they removed the
Our results suggest that subjective sleep reports may partly actigraph during the night.
reflect an individual’s perspective or state of mind in In conclusion, we found that sleep measures during the
addition to some component of their objective sleep luteal phase of the menstrual cycle vary widely in women
patterns. Thus, it is important to assess both the subjective ages 20 to 40 years sleeping at home, where they were
and objective aspects of sleep, if possible. unrestricted in their behavior. Multiple nights of data
The current study has several strengths. We examined collection, including weekend and weeknights, may be
young women, a group for which there is little objective necessary to obtain reliable estimates of an individual’s
18 S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19

sleep patterns. Unusual bed or rise times, medication use, an evaluation of internight and intrasubject variability in healthy
persons aged 50 –98 years. Sleep 1993;16(2):146 – 50.
employment, day of week, daylight hours, menstrual cycle
[12] Sadeh A, Acebo C. The role of actigraphy in sleep medicine. Sleep
length, and BMI were associated with actigraphically Med Rev 2002;6(2):113 – 24.
assessed sleep in this population. Associations of per- [13] Jean-Louis G, von Gizycki H, ZiZi F, Spielman AJ, Fookson J,
ceived stress, employment, and age with objective and Taub H. Actigraphic determination of sleep/wake activity: an
subjective sleep patterns differed, suggesting that these two assessment of internight variability. Sleep Res 1995;24A:473.
modalities may assess different aspects of the sleep [14] Jean-Louis G, von Gizycki H, ZiZi F, Spielman AJ, Fookson J,
Taub H. Sleep/wake activity in normal individuals: a longitudinal
experience. Due to the preliminary nature of these actigraphic assessment. Sleep Res 1995;24A:475.
findings, these results need to be confirmed by other [15] Chung L, Kripke DF, Ancoli-Israel S, Mason WJ. Dominant versus
studies. Future studies also should examine the relation- nondominant wrist movements during sleep. Sleep Res 1995;24A:80.
ships between these factors and sleep quality during other [16] Actiwatch 16/actiwatch 64/actiwatch-l/actiwatch-score instruction
manual. Bend (OR)7 Mini Mitter, 2001. p. A1 – A9.
phases of the menstrual cycle and among young adult
[17] Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC. Automatic
women on oral contraceptives. sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep 1992;15(5):461 – 9.
[18] Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived
stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983;24(4):385 – 96.
Acknowledgments [19] Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The
Pittsburgh sleep quality index: a new instrument for psychiatric
practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989;28(2):193 – 213.
This research was performed at the Fred Hutchinson [20] Munro CJ, Stabenfeldt GH, Cragun JR, Addiego LA, Overstreet JW,
Cancer Research Center in Seattle, WA, and was supported Lasley BL. Relationship of serum estradiol and progesterone
by Research Grant RO1CA80346 from the National Cancer concentrations to the excretion profiles of their major urinary
Institute. Dr. Tworoger was supported, in part, by a National metabolites as measured by enzyme immunoassay and radioimmuno-
Institutes of Environmental Health Sciences Training Grant assay. Clin Chem 1991;37(6):838 – 44.
[21] Rosner B. Fundamentals of biostatistics. Belmont (CA)7 Duxbury
(T32EF07262) and by a National Cancer Institute Cancer Press, 1995. p. 323 – 8.
Epidemiology Training Grant (T32CA090001). Dr. Vitiello [22] Armstrong BK, White E, Saracci R. Principles of exposure measure-
was supported by an NIMH Independent Scientist Award ment in epidemiology. Monogr Epidemiol Biostat 1992;21:252 – 8.
(K02-MH01158). [23] Tworoger SS, Davis S, Emerson SS, Mirick DK, Lentz MJ,
McTiernan A. The effect of a nighttime magnetic field exposure on
sleep patterns in young women. Am J Epidemiol 2004;160(3):224 – 9.
[24] Zeger SL, Liang KY. Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and
References continuous outcomes. Biometrics 1986;42(1):121 – 30.
[25] Coren S. The prevalence of self-reported sleep disturbances in young
[1] Dotto L. Sleep stages, memory and learning. CMAJ 1996;154(8): adults. Int J Neurosci 1994;79(1–2):67 – 73.
1193 – 6. [26] Mniszek DH. Brighton sleep survey: a study of sleep in 20–45-year
[2] Ayas NT, White DP, Manson JE, Stampfer MJ, Speizer FE, Malhotra olds. J Int Med Res 1988;16(1):61 – 5.
A, Hu FB. A prospective study of sleep duration and coronary heart [27] Doi Y, Minowa M, Okawa M, Uchiyama M. Prevalence of sleep
disease in women. Arch Intern Med 2003;163(2):205 – 9. disturbance and hypnotic medication use in relation to sociodemo-
[3] Scott AJ, LaDou J. Shiftwork: effects on sleep and health with graphic factors in the general japanese adult population. J Epidemiol
recommendations for medical surveillance and screening. Occup Med 2000;10(2):79 – 86.
1990;5(2):273 – 99. [28] Janson C, Gislason T, DeBacker W, Plaschke P, Bjornsson E, Hetta J,
[4] Baker FC, Maloney S, Driver HS. A comparison of subjective Kristbjarnason H, Vermeire P, Boman G. Prevalence of sleep
estimates of sleep with objective polysomnographic data in healthy disturbances among young adults in three European countries. Sleep
men and women. J Psychosom Res 1999;47(4):335 – 41. 1995;18(7):589 – 97.
[5] Vitiello MV, Larsen LH, Drolet G, Madar EL, Moe KE. Gender [29] Orr WC. Gastrointestinal functioning during sleep: a new horizon in
differences in subjective–objective sleep relationships in non- sleep medicine. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(2):91 – 101.
complaining healthy older adults. Sleep 2002;25S:A61. [30] Bohadana AB, Hannhart B, Teculescu DB. Nocturnal worsening of
[6] Vitiello MV, Moe KE, Larsen LH, Prinz PN. Age-related sleep asthma and sleep-disordered breathing. J Asthma 2002;39(2):85 – 100.
change: relationships of objective and subjective measures of sleep in [31] Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness, sleep disorders and alcohol use
healthy older men and women. Sleep Res 1997;26:220. and abuse. Sleep Med Rev 2001;5(4):287 – 97.
[7] Coates TJ, Killen JD, George J, Marchini E, Silverman S, [32] Roehrs T, Roth T. Sleep, sleepiness, and alcohol use. Alcohol Res
Thoresen C. Estimating sleep parameters: a multitrait–multimethod Health 2001;25(2):101 – 9.
analysis. J Consult Clin Psychol 1982;50(3):345 – 52. [33] Youngstedt SD, O’Connor PJ, Dishman RK. The effects of acute
[8] Ancoli-Israel S, Cole R, Alessi C, Chambers M, Moorcroft W, Pollak exercise on sleep: a quantitative synthesis. Sleep 1997;20(3):203 – 14.
CP. The role of actigraphy in the study of sleep and circadian rhythms. [34] Driver HS, Taylor SR. Exercise and sleep. Sleep Med Rev
Sleep 2003;26(3):342 – 92. 2000;4(4):387 – 402.
[9] Acebo C, Sadeh A, Seifer R, Tzischinsky O, Wolfson AR, Hafer A, [35] Morin CM, Hauri PJ, Espie CA, Spielman AJ, Buysse DJ, Bootzin
Carskadon MA. Estimating sleep patterns with activity monitoring in RR. Nonpharmacologic treatment of chronic insomnia An American
children and adolescents: how many nights are necessary for reliable Academy of Sleep Medicine review. Sleep 1999;22(8):1134 – 56.
measures? Sleep 1999;22(1):95 – 103. [36] Kohsaka M, Fukuda N, Honma K, Honma S, Morita N. Seasonality in
[10] Sadeh A, Raviv A, Gruber R. Sleep patterns and sleep disruptions in human sleep. Experientia 1992;48(3):231 – 3.
school-age children. Dev Psychol 2000;36(3):291 – 301. [37] Honma K, Honma S, Kohsaka M, Fukuda N. Seasonal variation in the
[11] van Hilten JJ, Braat EA, van der Velde EA, Middelkoop HA, Kerkhof human circadian rhythm: dissociation between sleep and temperature
GA, Kamphuisen HA. Ambulatory activity monitoring during sleep: rhythm. Am J Physiol 1992;262(5 Pt 2):R885 – 91.
S.S. Tworoger et al. / Journal of Psychosomatic Research 59 (2005) 11–19 19

[38] Husby R, Lingjaerde O. Prevalence of reported sleeplessness in [45] Monk TH, Buysse DJ, Rose LR, Hall JA, Kupfer DJ. The sleep of
northern Norway in relation to sex, age and season. Acta Psychiatr healthy people—a diary study. Chronobiol Int 2000;17(1):49 – 60.
Scand 1990;81(6):542 – 7. [46] Friedman L, Brooks JO, Bliwise DL, Yesavage JA, Wicks DS.
[39] Gordo MA, Recio J, Sanchez-Barcelo EJ. Decreased sleep quality in Perceptions of life stress and chronic insomnia in older adults. Psychol
patients suffering from retinitis pigmentosa. J Sleep Res 2001;10(2): Aging 1995;10(3):352 – 7.
159 – 64. [47] Shaver JL, Johnston SK, Lentz MJ, Landis CA. Stress exposure,
[40] Khaodhiar L, McCowen KC, Blackburn GL. Obesity and its comorbid psychological distress, and physiological stress activation in midlife
conditions. Clin Cornerstone 1999;2(3):17 – 31. women with insomnia. Psychosom Med 2002;64(5):793 – 802.
[41] Parry BL, Mendelson WB, Duncan WC, Sack DA, Wehr TA. [48] Blood ML, Sack RL, Percy DC, Pen JC. A comparison of sleep
Longitudinal sleep eeg, temperature, and activity measurements detection by wrist actigraphy, behavioral response, and polysomnog-
across the menstrual cycle in patients with premenstrual depression raphy. Sleep 1997;20(6):388 – 95.
and in age-matched controls. Psychiatry Res 1989;30(3):285 – 303. [49] Babin L. Determining sleep–wake activity using actiwatch. Sleep
[42] Manber R, Bootzin RR. Sleep and the menstrual cycle. Health Psychol 1997;20:S178.
1997;16(3):209 – 14. [50] Hauri PJ, Wisbey J. Wrist actigraphy in insomnia. Sleep 1992;15(4):
[43] Driver HS, Dijk DJ, Werth E, Biedermann K, Borbely AA. Sleep and 293 – 301.
the sleep electroencephalogram across the menstrual cycle in young [51] Kushida CA, Chang A, Gadkary C, Guilleminault C, Carrillo O,
healthy women. J Clin Endocrinol Metab 1996;81(2):728 – 35. Dement WC. Comparison of actigraphic, polysomnographic, and
[44] Lee KA, McEnany G, Zaffke ME. REM sleep and mood state in subjective assessment of sleep parameters in sleep-disordered patients.
childbearing women: sleepy or weepy? Sleep 2000;23(7):877 – 85. Sleep Med 2001;2:389 – 96.

You might also like