You are on page 1of 14

Features

Discourse on Narrative Research


The Construction Zone: Literary Elements
in Narrative Research
Cathy A. Coulter and Mary Lee Smith

Narrative research has become part of the landscape of education To orient the reader, we organize the rest of the article as
inquiry, yet its theory and practice are still debated and evolving.This f­ ollows. First, we briefly review the literature that establishes a
article addresses the construction of narratives using literary ele- context for our work. Next, we describe the literary elements used
to construct narrative accounts, introduce the four texts that will
ments common to nonfiction and fiction writings. The authors dis-
be used as reference points, and provide examples of the literary
cuss these elements and use four narratives to illustrate them. They
elements. Finally, we examine the intersections of narrative con-
address how literary elements intersect with more familiar practices struction with conventional practices and epistemological ideas
of generating and analyzing evidence to reveal themes, and they relate regarding evidence and truth.
these intersections with wider issues about what can be known from
Literature on Narrative Inquiry
research and how it can be learned.
and Narrative Construction
From the wide range of narrative forms of inquiry, Polkinghorne
Keywords: narrative research; qualitative research; research (1995) designated two basic categories: analysis of narrative and
methodology narrative analysis. The former includes approaches close in form
and function to more general kinds of qualitative studies, in
which “narratives are analyzed into themes and categories”
(Clandinin & Murphy, 2007, p. 636). The latter includes “stud-

I
n the contested terrain of education research, narrative ies whose data consist of actions, events, and happenings, but
approaches have established themselves through specialized whose analysis produces stories” (Polkinghorne, 1995, p. 6).
publications, special interest groups in the American Narrative analysis studies rely on stories as a way of knowing.
Educational Research Association, and a committed community Stories emerge as data are collected and then are framed and
of scholars. Yet almost all of the theory and practice of narrative rendered through an analytical process that is artistic as well as
research are still debated, including matters of purpose, methods, rigorous (Clandinin & Connelly, 2000; Connelly & Clandinin,
ethics, and validity. As the discussions continue, narrative and 1990; Ecker, 1966; Eisner, 1981, 1998; Freeman, 2007). Barone
other arts-based forms of research continue to evolve (Barone, (2007) calls such studies narrative constructions because “this
2007; Clandinin & Murphy, 2007; Finley, 2005; Rosiek & recasting of data into a storied form is more accurately described
Atkinson, 2007). as an act of textual arrangement” (p. 456). This article focuses on
In this article we consider a single aspect1 of narrative research: Polkinghorne’s designation of narrative analysis (Barone’s “narra-
the construction of narratives as representations of research stud- tive construction”). When writing more generally about narrative
ies, using literary elements and devices common to nonfiction as a research process, we use the more general terms narrative
and fiction texts. These elements are familiar to specialists who research and narrative inquiry interchangeably.
theorize, teach, or practice writing but are unfamiliar to the vast Foremost in the extant literature on narrative construction is
majority of education researchers. Our purpose is to describe the idea that narrative research differs from traditional research
these literary elements to an audience of generalists and illustrate studies in matters of purpose. According to Barone, conventional
them by pointing out specific features of four chosen narratives. research strives to discover and verify knowledge about the real
For this audience,2 we explore the intersection of narrative con- state of the world. In contrast, narrative research strives to portray
struction with conventional trajectories of data generation and experience, to question common understandings, to offer “a
analysis, representation of the results of research, and application degree of interpretive space” (Barone, 2001c, p. 150). Narratives
of epistemological notions of warranted evidence. have the effect of evoking dissonance in the reader, enabling the

Educational Researcher,Vol. 38, No. 8, pp. 577–590


10.3102/0013189X09353787
© 2009 AERA. http://er.aera.net
november 2009 577
reader to look at educational phenomena with renewed interest Literary Elements and Four Illustrative Cases
and a more questioning stance. Narrative research, not unlike
Here, we address literary elements themselves and consider
good literature, “causes us to question our values, prompts new
them as research practices, while maintaining a critical eye on the
imaginings of the ideal and the possible. It can even stir action
relationships among researchers, authors, research participants,
against the conventional, the seemingly unquestionable, the tried
characters, events, scenes, and readers. To illustrate these ele-
and true” (Barone, 2001a, p. 736). Narratives also introduce the
ments we draw on four narratives.
centrality of emotions in lived experience (Denzin, 1992), which
are aesthetically posed to the reader through familiar story forms. Snow White, Revolutions, the American Dream,
Conventional researchers who accept the dichotomy of reason and Other Fairy Tales
and emotion often bridle at narratives’ explicit use of emotional
The first illustrative narrative is one of our own (Coulter, 2003).
descriptions and appeals. Narrative research brings the two parts
The reason we use it here is that, while the narrative was con-
of human experience together (cf. Novitz, 1997).3
structed, every choice was considered and reconsidered. For
This line of literature also states that conventional research
example, Coulter experimented with different tones, voices, and
appeals solely to the reader’s sense of logic or evidence (and to the
the like to see how each squared with the evidence and repre-
authority of the researcher) and thereby limits the readers’ range
sented the experience of the participants with fidelity and respect.
of interpretations (Barone, 2001c). In contrast, narrative research
As we reflected on this decision making and the effects of each
uses literary devices to allow readers to make sense of the study in
choice, we formulated general hypotheses about narrative con-
their own ways. Multiple interpretations by multiple readers are
struction. Thus we had more material to draw on to describe the
expected and promoted.
use and narrative effects of literary elements. In Coulter’s study
One of the debated aspects of narrative research is the ques-
of the experiences of high school students in a program for
tion of truth (Barone, 2001b; Doyle, 1997; Fenstermacher,
English language learners, she functions first as the teacher of
1997; Mayer, 2000; Phillips, 1994, 1997). How do we know
those students, then as the researcher of those experiences, and
that the researchers’ stories reflect what really happened? In the
finally as the author of the students’ stories. Eight people, includ-
main branch of the literature so far reviewed, the questions
ing Darek, Vicki, and David, function first as students, then as
about objectivity and truth of narrative research lie outside its
research participants, and finally as the characters in the nonfic-
province and purpose.” Indeed, the point of narrative research
tion novel that Coulter wrote. (All names in the study are pseud-
is to reveal the subjective experience of participants as they inter-
onyms.) Darek divides his time between a Northwestern city and
pret the events and conditions of their everyday lives (Miller,
his Cu’pik village. His mother is Cu’pik, and his father, now
2005). Barone (2007) wrote that questions of objectivity and
deceased, was African American. As Darek navigates life at
truth emerge from the dominance of the positivist paradigm as
“Northwest High School,” he is faced with questions of access
a “regulatory ideal,” which is an inappropriate paradigm for
and identity. Vicki is a second-generation immigrant whose
narrative research. According to Denzin (2000), “Narratives do
father is from Mexico and whose mother is from El Salvador. Her
not establish the truth of . . . events [or] reflect the truth of
story reflects the choices she had to make. David lives with his
experience. Narratives create the very events they reflect upon.
single mother, who is first-generation Filipino. His story portrays
In this sense, narratives are reflections on—not of—the world as
insight into low expectations and tracking in schools.
it is known” (quoted in Riessman, 2008, p. 188).
Richardson (1990) continues in this vein: “Unlike the logico-
This Boy’s Life
scientific mode, which looks for universal truth conditions, the
narrative mode is contextually embedded and looks for particular For our other three story selections we had less detail. We could
connections between events. The connections between the events not reproduce the acts of constructing the narratives except by
is the meaning” (emphasis in original, p. 13). The question of drawing on the texts themselves. This Boy’s Life (Wolff, 1989) is a
“did it really happen the way you describe it” is problematic. memoir that encompasses the author’s preadolescent and adoles-
Eyewitnesses to the same event have differing accounts depend- cent years and leaves off as he drops out of college to join the
ing on their perspectives. Throw in the filters of time and space, army. He begins the narrative as he and his mother drive west to
and “truth” becomes elusive indeed. The task of the narrative Utah, escaping the life of wealth and Protestantism of his father
researcher is not to describe the world as it is, because in the and older brother in Connecticut, looking for transformation
constructivist or postmodern paradigm, that one world does not and independence. He changes his name from Tobias (Toby) to
exist. Rather, narrative researchers strive to redescribe events ret- Jack and hopes to become a different boy. His mother hopes to
rospectively (Freeman, 2007) through the lens of collaborative strike it rich by mining uranium in Utah. “But we were late—
interpretations with participants. They recognize the difference months too late,” he writes. “Moab and the other mining towns
between the literal truth and the story truth (O’Brien, 1990). had been overrun. All the motels were full. . . . There were no
In this section we have discussed existing literature on narrative jobs, and people were getting ornery” (p. 6).
research as it bears on narrative construction and the literary ele- His mother finally settles for a job and a poor place to live. He
ments that we next describe. We have presented the literature as goes to Catholic school. An abusive man from her past tracks
grounding for our work. The presentation is incomplete for rea- them down. They escape by driving farther west. Later, she is
sons of space. However, we also argued that the dominant tradition courted and then marries a man named Dwight, and the narra-
in narrative research is itself partial and neglects what some have tive intensifies to reveal the depth of each character and the con-
called modernist or realist tales—an argument we return to later. flicts among them.

578 educational Researcher


Why did we choose this text? It does not match the conven- the rich material the author has made available in his other works
tional education research format of preserving observation notes and his prominence in teaching about the writing of research.
and interview transcripts contemporary with the acts recorded. In the preceding subsections, we have attempted to justify our
It  is a retrospective account, similar to historical research. It use of these four texts. They do not represent any universe of
potentially suffers from a self-interested reconstruction, but in texts, but they do represent authentic examples of the ideas we try
that respect it is the same as all memoirs. Its subject matter is not to describe in this article.4
focused on formal schooling, but nevertheless the author’s school We now turn to our discussion of literary elements as research
life constitutes much of the experience that he deems worth tools in narrative research. We first introduce each literary ele-
remembering and writing about. We chose this text because it is ment and describe how it is used in writing in the general sense.
iconic in courses and programs in writing for its vivid detail and We then discuss how the element can be used in research, draw-
consistent, strong voice, and because of Wolff ’s ability to con- ing from our four narratives for examples.
struct scenes—all of which is instructive for narrative researchers.
Examples of Literary Elements
Down by the River
Narratives rely on cultural expectations of readers about what
The third narrative, Down by the River (Bowden, 2004), we constitutes a story. Stories follow an arc from a beginning to an
include because of its complex structure and interrelationships of end, have characters and sites of action, and comprise events in
author and characters over time. The author investigates a mur- scenes arrayed across time in which the characters act or are acted
der he reads about in the newspaper. The victim is a man named upon by other characters and events. Without any of these under-
Bruno who lives in El Paso. A young man from across the river in lying elements, there is no story. The rendition of these elements
Juarez is arrested for the murder. The circumstances of the arrest, is the author’s construction, the author’s choice according to his
conviction, and overturning of the conviction leave open the or her purpose and craft.
question of whether larger forces were involved. Bowden finds In the next subsection we discuss the element known in fiction
that the incident is connected to international drug cartels and writing as point of view. It may be considered as including the next
the governments of both the United States and Mexico. five elements—person, omniscience, narrator reliability, narrative
Everywhere he turns in his own study, he finds that the connec- voice, and authorial distance (Burroway, 2003)—which we discuss
tive tissue that ties the various elements together makes the story in their own subsections in turn.
almost impossible to prove by the standards of research or jour-
Point of View
nalism. The literary forms he uses mimic the complexity and
ambiguity of the content and provoke complex and ambiguous Point of view is the lens through which a narrative is told. The
responses in the reader. reader sees and hears through the sensibilities and emotions of a
Why did we choose this text? Among the four, only this one focalized character. In narrative research, point of view consists of
typifies the postmodern turn in literary criticism (Barone, 2000). how the story presents the relationships between researcher and
A noted investigative journalist, nonfiction writer, and teacher of participants, and the relationships of the participants to each
literary nonfiction at the University of Arizona, Bowden writes other. The researchers are the writers, and the characters are
with variations in tone, voice, distance, and style and thus requires participants, people who have played a role in the lives of the
readers to make their own sense of his stories. By avoiding pat participants, or researchers.
answers, he forces readers to rethink their basic views of the world In both narrative theory and narrative research methodology,
and of the phenomena he describes. If there are conclusions or the relationships among writers, characters, and readers have
themes, they are of the reader’s making, not the author’s. We chose become contentious issues. The writer controls the telling. When
this particular text because of these qualities and because we could the writer decides which character has agency to “speak” the story
not find anything approaching its creative and skillful use of liter- or carry the action forward, she assumes a position of privilege.
ary elements in the education research literature. In narrative research, the writer who is also the researcher—as she
transforms research participants into characters and narrators in
Sneaky Kid and Its Aftermath
her story—faces the same issues and more, often juxtaposing her
The fourth narrative we use to show literary elements is Sneaky position of societal privilege against the marginalized position of
Kid and Its Aftermath (Wolcott, 2002). The initial section of this her participants. Barone and others argue that the researcher
book is a redescription of Wolcott’s ethnographic study of schools must attempt to counteract the inherent privilege of this relation-
and how they fail some students. He uses as his case a young man, ship from the very beginning, making literary decisions that
named Brad, who has taken up residence on Wolcott’s property. debunk it (Barone, 2000; Emihovich, 1995; Goodson, 1995;
Brad provides extensive material, and the resulting ethnography Nespor & Barber, 1995). Relevant to point of view, there are
is published in a scholarly journal. The narrative then is trans- literary decisions that researchers can make to mitigate the
formed into a story of the personal relationship between the researcher–participant relationship. These are discussed next, in
researcher and Brad. After the relationship ends, news comes that connection with the other literary elements.
Brad has had a psychotic break. He blames Wolcott and comes
Person
back to attack him and burn down his house. The next segment
of the narrative is the story of Brad’s trial and the censure directed Addressing the complex issue of point of view involves deciding
at Wolcott from his scholarly community and from the society in on person. Who tells the story? In some cases the author is the
general. We chose this particular nonfiction narrative because of narrator, but the author can also construct another, a character,

november 2009 579


as narrator. The narrator must stand somewhere in relation to the that the character would not be privy to. Darek can imagine how
actions taken by the characters in the story. What knowledge does Thai Boy feels about his decision to stay for a second senior year,
the narrator have available at the time of the narrating? The but he cannot know for sure. Neither can the reader. In the third
author can choose from first, second, or third person and can person, the narrator can get right into the head of every character
choose whether a character, a narrator as character, or the author so the reader can see what Thai Boy is thinking, even if Darek
herself tells the story. A story told in third person uses the third- cannot.
person pronouns—he, she, it, or they—and tells the story from a Given the concern about the inherent privileging in the
narrator’s perspective, whether or not that narrator also plays a researcher–participant relationship (Barone, 2000; Emihovich,
part in the action. A story told in first person uses the first-person 1995; Goodson, 1995; Nespor & Barber, 1995), both first- and
pronoun “I.” If that story is told by a character, it is filtered third-person renderings can be problematic. How can a researcher
through her unique perceptions. We later present excerpts of nar- get into the head of a participant and tell her story from her per-
rative constructions in the third and first persons. spective, using her voice, her words, her thoughts, as in first per-
The following excerpt represents a passage Coulter con- son? But then how can a researcher presume to get into everyone’s
structed to illustrate third-person storytelling: head, as in the third-person point of view? Richardson (1990)
described the dilemma in “occupying a godlike position” in nar-
I. rative research texts:

Darek stood up and started gathering dishes from the dinner Do researchers have the right to speak for others, distancing
table. He clanked the pots a little too loudly, and then tried to themselves from the text, acting as if their own subjectivity were
calm himself. It wasn’t Danny’s fault that he had failed two classes. not being inscribed in the text? And, conversely, how can the
Danny looked over at Darek. He knew that he had made Darek credibility of the writer’s claims to knowledge be decided? (p. 38)
angry. He hadn’t meant to. He just wanted his mother to know
Either way, the tendency is for the researcher to present her view
how well he did in school. The mother looked at her two sons.
Good-natured Danny, who tried so hard in school, and strong
as exclusive. The distortion and ethnocentrism that often go
Darek, who looked after Danny. Darek must not have done so along with such an exclusive rendering can be mitigated by vary-
well, she thought. It had been such a hard year for him. She ing other aspects of point of view: omniscience, narrator reliabil-
hoped he wouldn’t be too hard on himself. (Adapted from ity, tone, authorial distance, and the use of multiple narrators
Coulter, 2009, p. 15) with varying points of view.
The above excerpt is narrated in the third person, by a narrator Omniscience
positioned outside the story. Contrast the effect with that of the The term omniscience is used to refer to the amount of knowledge
following first-person point of view, in which one character the narrator (and particularly the author as narrator) assumes in
(Darek) tells his story from his own particular standpoint: the narrative. The omniscient narrator is an objective “all-
knower,” possessing all of the facts about characters and events
II. and having access to the heart and mind of every character, such
It was one of those drawn-out dusks when I told Thai Boy about as in Excerpt I, above, in which the narrator knew what Darek,
football. Maybe ten, maybe eleven o’clock. I had bounced the ball Danny, and their mother were thinking. Omniscience functions
and scored a perfect 3-pointer, a swish that didn’t even touch the to provide general descriptions and interpretations and thus has
rim, though technically it didn’t swish because there was no net a place, for example, at the beginning of the story, where the
on the Arctic Sun Trailer Park hoop. author establishes the initial state of affairs, and between scenes,
“I’m staying another year,” I told him as he was running for where the author indicates how much time has elapsed and how
the rebound. things have changed. Writing the scenes themselves calls for use
“And he . . . scores!” he said, firing a jump shot which banged of rich and thick description of the concrete particulars necessary
off the bare rim. The ball bounced right back into his hands. to establish the characters’ actions and intentions. Distanced tell-
“What?”
ing backs up and fills in between the focused showing (thick
“For football. Coach Kellogg asked me to stay for a second
senior year.”
description). Writing a passage such as “Seven years went by
He dribbled the ball criss-cross in front of him, then stopped between the time of Chansy’s death and Darek’s reflection on it”
and looked at me, ball hitched up on his hip. “You’re staying may convey the researcher’s omniscience. But it also functions to
another year?” (Coulter, 2009, p. 1) structure the plot line. Researchers cannot represent every single
occasion that happened in the participants’ lives but must choose
The decision regarding which person to use in a narrative con- which events to concentrate on and which to highlight; other-
struction—first, second, or third—is one of the first very impor- wise, the “map” of representation would be as extensive as the
tant choices the researcher makes. There are specific advantages “territory” of the life it represents (cf. Becker, 2007). Choices are
and disadvantages to the use of first versus third person. In gen- made based on the significance of the included event in the anal-
eral, a first-person construction lends closeness to the telling: The ysis of the evidence as a whole. This is how Geertz (1973) distin-
reader sees the story through the perspective of the character as guished thick description from simply detailed or rich description.
narrator—or as participant, in the case of a research narrative— The omniscient narrator has access to and can describe the
which can generate feelings of affinity. However, the researcher is inner thoughts and actions of every character. Thus the omni-
also limited by this decision, as she cannot include information scient third person serves the purpose of economy even as it gives

580 educational Researcher


the impression that the narrator knows equally and completely to the reader that the narrator’s perspective may or may not be
the thoughts of each character. This impression is problematic. entirely trusted. The reader is presented with the problem of
Researchers hope to construct adequate understanding of the assessing how much information the narrator has at a given point
lives of their research participants, but most acknowledge that in the story and what the interests and intentions of the narrator
their understanding, even after years of patient and thorough may be. The reader is thus led to see the narrator’s account as
work, falls short.5 partial or uncertain rather than definitive. In this way, narratives
Another aspect of point of view is limited omniscience, in lead readers to questions rather than to single answers. Varying
which the narrator has access to the thoughts and sensibilities of the degree of narrator reliability has the potential to introduce
one character but not other characters. This perspective lends the counternarratives and debunk the inherent tendency toward
reader a sense of closeness to the story because it is most similar dominating functions in literary elements when they are applied
to what we experience in life. In the following example, Coulter to narrative research.
(2005) used the limited omniscient point of view from the per- In the following excerpt from Coulter’s study, David describes
spective of 12-year-old Sahar: how he feels other students at his high school “made themselves
victims of racism.” Notice the detachment created by placing sev-
III. eral years between the narrated event and the occasion of narration:

At lunch Taylor Maloney and her groupies had thrown grapes at


IV.
her again. She had been walking to her usual spot at lunch recess:
the picnic table in the far corner, right under the mesquite tree. Some people will tell you exaggerated stories. Stories to show how
Suddenly she felt something wet hit her cheek. She looked down racist Northwest was. To me, you make your own fate. The kids
and saw a squished grape on the ground. She didn’t need to look that got into trouble got themselves into trouble. Like Eddie. I
up to see where it had come from. Taylor and her friends always mean, they thought Eddie was in a gang, but Eddie really was.
sat in the center of the square, on a short cement wall that had And he made the choice to kill a guy. He made the choice to go
been built around the biggest tree on campus. There they engaged to jail and sit there the rest of his life. Sure, I know there is racism
in various forms of social torture. The latest was “graping” the out there. But people make themselves victims of racism. When
least popular passers-by. people stereotyped me, I manipulated it to my own benefit. If
“What happened Sa-hair?” Taylor called out. Sahar kept on other people are stereotyped and then they become what people
walking. Taylor persisted. “Oh my God! There’s a grape in my think they are, really become it, that’s the choice they make. It’s
sa-hair!!” she said and everyone laughed. Sahar kept her eyes on not the racism that causes it. It’s a choice they make themselves.
the pavement beneath her feet. Taylor had a short attention span. The kids that got into trouble caused it themselves, and they just
Out of sight, out of mind. She finally made it to her place under want to blame racism for it. (Coulter, 2003, p. 176)
the mesquite tree. Wiping the rest of the grape off her cheek,
Sahar watched Taylor from afar. She was on to her next victim, This excerpt shows David’s perspective after several years and con-
poor little José Cardenes. He was new this year and didn’t speak
siderable reflection. But the data Coulter collected when David
much English. He ignored Taylor just like she had. Sahar won-
dered if it ever occurred to anyone that it was mostly minority
was in high school suggested that in addition to the stereotyping
students that were ostracized. Not that anyone would care. (p. 7) (which David did not think of as racist), he had, in fact, been an
object of racism. The conflicting evidence shows him to be an
The limited omniscient perspective allows the researcher to nar- unreliable narrator throughout his story. When the stories of other
rate the activities of all characters in third person, as in the omni- students in the study also reflect evidence of racism, the reader has
scient perspective. However, it also has similarities to first person. more information for interpreting David’s view. Rather than try
The story is rinsed in the sensibilities of a single character, whose to show that David is in a state of denial or is lying, however,
thoughts are accessible to the reader. In Excerpt III above, the Coulter uses the unreliable narrator to provoke the reader into
omniscient lens narrows in the line “Out of sight, out of mind” seeing the complexity of experiences at this school and struggling
and again in the line “Not that anyone would care.” These reflect to understand how students attempt to make sense of their own
Sahar’s thoughts, not those of the narrator. Limited omniscience experiences. Use of the unreliable narrator poses an ethical
centers on a single character, and the story is filtered through her dilemma when it is used for anyone other than the researcher
sensibilities. Thus the pitfalls of limited omniscience are similar herself. How can a researcher say that a participant’s views are
to those of first person: How can the researcher narrate through unreliable or misguided and the researcher’s interpretation cor-
the sensibilities of a participant whose experiences are vastly rect? Josselson (2007) reminds us that the “self is multiple and
different from his own? evolving. The aspects of persons we write about are contingent
To answer that question, we turn now to the next aspect of and selective” (p. 549). Use of the unreliable narrator allows the
point of view: narrator reliability. researcher to portray layers of self in a single narrative.
After writing a draft of her findings, Coulter presented rele-
Narrator Reliability
vant sections to each person she had rendered as a character and
If the researcher chooses to write the account in the first person, requested suggestions for revisions (all of which were honored).
through the eyes of a character, or from a limited omniscient David was aware of the content and interpretation and represen-
third-person perspective, he can draw upon the element of nar- tation that Coulter had made and was comfortable with the por-
rator reliability. Narrator reliability refers to the degree to which trayal. He stood firm in his point of view and expressed that
the reader can trust the narrator. The author can build in signals Coulter’s portrayal, given his revisions, was accurate. Researchers

november 2009 581


of various qualitative studies would no doubt point out the obscenity that had been written in the boys’ lavatory. . . . Well, he
complexities and inconsistencies in the varying accounts of was going to get them if he had to keep every single one of us here
David’s story. In this instance the researcher made no attempt to all night long. . . . The vice-principal was new and hard-nosed;
stamp a single definitive meaning on David’s experience, instead he meant what he said. I knew he wouldn’t let this drop, and
he would keep at it until he caught me. I got scared. Even more
employing the literary technique of unreliable narrator to show
than his anger, his righteousness scared me to the point where my
the inherent complexities in the situation. The ethics of manipu-
stomach cramped up. As the afternoon went on the cramp got
lating reliability in participants’ stories requires a deeper discus- worse and I had to go to the nurse’s office. That was where the
sion than is possible here.6 vice-principal finally came for me. . . . He kicked the cot where I
Other aspects of narrator reliability warrant discussion, such lay doubled up and sweating. “Get up,” he said. . . . I sat up part-
as how a researcher as narrator can establish trustworthiness. way, still miming incomprehension. (Wolff, 1989, p. 77)
Coulter varies person, omniscience, and reliability, but the narra-
tives of Wolff and Wolcott maintain the first-person point of The nurse intervenes, claiming Jack is genuinely sick. Wolff con-
view throughout. Both Wolff and Wolcott function as narrators. tinues: “I couldn’t bear for her to think that I was the kind of
When other characters speak, the reader knows that their words person who took advantage of other people’s kindness or wrote
are channeled through the narrator from a distance of some years. filth on bathroom walls. And at that moment I wasn’t” (p. 78).
The reader allows for ordinary lapses in human memory and Jack describes more of his high school experience:
understands that the characters’ words at the time of the event I brought home good grades at first. . . . They were a fraud. I
may not have been rendered exactly or completely. Nevertheless, copied other kids’ homework on the bus. . . . The report cards
the reconstructions are plausible, so the reader regards the author were made out, incredibly enough, in pencil, and I owned some
as narrator as trustworthy. pencils myself. (Wolff, 1989, p. 184)
However, Wolff and Wolcott establish trustworthiness in Later, Jack attempts to escape his impoverished home and abu-
quite different ways. Wolcott draws on three sources. The first is sive stepfather, Dwight, by applying to the East Coast prep
his status and authority as a professional anthropologist. The sec- schools that his father and brother attended. After an incident
ond is his device of interrupting the narrative to introduce discur- in which Dwight strikes him, Jack calls his brother, who ques-
sive arguments from the literature; such introjections are meant tions him about his prospects. Jack tells him about his good
to help readers make the meanings that the narrator wants them grades and invents other accomplishments: He was an Eagle
to make. The third is the research methods he used for the first Scout, he was a stalwart on the swim team, he played in the
part of the narrative to study Brad, the “sneaky kid.” He con- band, and so on. Based on this false account, his brother encour-
ducted and tape-recorded interviews and took notes from his ages him to apply to prep schools. In filling out the applications
extensive number of observations of Brad’s activities over a 2-year he has this thought: “I had to be that boy I’d described to my
period. The authoritativeness of his data generation, analysis, and brother” (Wolff, 1989, p. 209).
reporting was reinforced by the publication of his paper in a ref- To accomplish that, Jack steals school stationary, transcript
ereed work. By the standards of professional anthropology, and forms, and envelopes and forges letters of support from counsel-
even those of journalists and historians, such methods lend trust- ors and community members. In this spirit,
worthiness to a narrative. In contrast, Wolcott’s description of
events subsequent to the publication of his study was not based Now the words came as easily as if someone were breathing them
into my ear. I felt full of things that had to be said, full of stifled
on research in the traditional anthropological manner. In those
truth. It was the truth known only to me, but I believed in it more
sections of the book, he was a full participant in the events and
than I believed the facts arrayed against it. . . . These were ideas
recounted them as life story. His narrator is therefore less trust- about myself that I had held for dear life. (Wolff, 1989, p. 213)
worthy in those sections; this effect is purposeful on his part.
Because these narrated events sound less scientific and less dis- So much negative self-disclosure makes the account believable
tanced, the narrative becomes more personal and thus evokes the and reminds us of Josselson’s (2007) point that the self is multiple
readers’ emotions to a greater extent. and evolving. Tobias calls himself Jack for the time covered in the
Wolff ’s trustworthiness emerges from a different source: his narrative. He is struck with emotion when his brother calls him
harsh and unrelenting portrayal of the foibles of his younger self. Tobie. When he writes the narrative, he is again Tobias.
One expects self-presentation bias to be positive. But not so with Bowden’s Down by the River contrasts with the other three
Wolff. He describes Jack’s exploits: stealing money, stealing cars, narratives in point of view. The author switches back and forth
starting fights, cheating, destroying property, torturing animals, between first and third person, sometimes putting words and
and lying incessantly—the lies meant not only to avoid punish- thoughts in the mouths and minds of characters; he changes nar-
ment but to preserve a good image of himself in others’ eyes and rator distance from near to far, formal to informal. Here is an
his own. Wolff writes this story without angst or justification, in example of projecting into the thoughts of Patricia, Bruno’s fian-
cool, detached, detailed prose. In this example, Jack and his cée, on the evening of his murder:
friends have been acting up in the high school lavatory, and Jack
scratches “Fuck You” on the wall: VI.

Where is he? . . . [A]s the engine idles, Patricia can feel her irrita-
V.
tion rising. . . . She makes an effort for him. And she is tired of
During the first period after lunch the vice-principal visited every being late because of him. . . . Bruno can’t seem to ever be on
classroom and demanded the names of those responsible for the time. (Bowden, 2004, p. 13)

582 educational Researcher


Two pages later, Bowden shifts to the third person as he draws terms of effect on literary quality and interpretation of data) of
back for a more generalized view of what is just outside Patricia’s such decisions. We have explored elements of point of view includ-
immediate vision: “The city swallows dirt. . . . a few blocks away ing person, omniscience, and narrator reliability. We turn now to
the Rio Grande (the Rio Bravo to the Mexicans on the opposite a discussion of other literary elements that serve as tools in narra-
bank, a polluted snake that rises and falls. . . . ).” tive research: heteroglossia, authorial distance, and tone.
Still later in the narrative, Bowden uses the second person to
describe the life of the boy who was tried for Bruno’s murder: Narrative Voice
As research studies are transposed into narrative texts, the
VII. researcher can choose to vary the point of view from section to
section, portraying multiple voices. Bakhtin (1981) calls this
You are a natural entrepreneur so you leave school to get a part of approach heteroglossic. The term suggests that different characters
the action. You realize there is a greater market for your products
view the action differently or see different parts of the action
abroad than at home so you leave your nation and your culture
and become a pioneer of free trade and open markets. . . . You
portrayed. Such an approach suggests greater complexity of inter-
leap out from the curbside at intersections and busily begin wash- pretation: A polyvocal account—one in which several characters
ing the windshields of motorists. You allow the customers to set narrate their stories—varies the voice from section to section or
their own fees. . . . Your entertainment venture requires more chapter to chapter. Narratives and counternarratives are con-
preparation. You apply makeup to your face until you have the structed from the data and analyses such that the reader is free to
classic visage of a clown. Then when the light turns red you leap interpret the construction as rendered. In fiction, the writer has
out and begin to juggle. (Bowden, 2004, pp. 212–213) no obligation to characters to present them and their role in the
Although Bowden quotes and provides footnotes to articles from story with a sense of fairness or truth. The researcher, however,
newspapers and government reports, he also suggests that such has multiple interests and obligations.
sources are not to be believed. He uses El Financiero Internacional One of the ways that narrative researchers have tried to remove
and the Dallas Morning News as sources of statistics and news themselves from the omniscient point of view is through the use
reports on the Mexican economy but ends the paragraph with an of multiple narrators and perspectives. Coulter (2003) decided
ironic note: that the eight participants in her study should narrate their own
stories in first person from their own perspectives. She intended
VIII.
that the sum of the separate stories would represent the school as
a whole, creating a polyvocal, layered account. Choosing the
The minimum wage is shrinking minute by minute as 1995 rolls first-person point of view also gave Darek the agency to tell this
along [ending the year at] $2.59. President Bill Clinton declares story from his point of view. After writing Darek’s story, Coulter
as the year slides by, “The Mexican economy has turned the cor- confirmed with him that her representation of his story was con-
ner and the markets have taken notice.” President Ernesto Zedillo sistent with his view at the time. All of the people behind the
on a visit to Washington takes the opportunity to deny that he characters had a chance to review and alter their stories.
got $40 million from the Cali cartel for his 1994 campaign.
Narrative researchers approximate a heteroglossic account in
DEA, the source of the allegation, now denies the allegation.
(Bowden, 2004, p. 215)
a number of ways. One way is the form known as pastiche (Ely,
2007), which allows participants to express their points of view
The wealth of detail establishes credibility, as does his brief report in different forms, for example, in poems, songs, or the like, or
of his methods. Bowden did his research firsthand, interviewed in separate chapters or sections allotted to them. A second way is
extensively, and put himself in jeopardy to get multiple stories dialogue, a necessary part of all narrative texts. Through dia-
and a wealth of concrete details over a 7-year period. He has logue, the author presents different points of view as expressed
established narrator reliability and trustworthiness, yet his stance by different participants. Dialogue also presents opportunities to
in the narrative is anything but omniscient. show how various actors have made meaning from confronta-
A prevailing theme throughout Bowden’s book is that all tions with others (Becker, 2007). A third way is through the
sources of information contain distortions because of underlying actions of participants; for example, Darek’s friend Chansy
individual and institutional interests. At best, the sources should laughs at the security guard and Darek notes Chansy’s laughter.
be considered part of the truth: “Facts crumble at a touch” Darek and the reader can interpret Chansy as mocking, as an
(Bowden, 2004, p. 160). Here, Bowden addresses this theme and agent in defying authority. This approach makes possible addi-
demonstrates his stance toward the material: tional layers of interpretation on the part of both participants
and readers.
IX. Where does the writer place herself in the complex network of
readers and participants in a narrative? A person transforms her-
I am in a black hole. . . . Here is the problem: once you enter this self to researcher, then to writer. By using narrative rather than a
black hole and truly live in it and taste it, then you understand. . conventional research report, the researcher can become a charac-
. . And this understanding doesn’t matter at all. It becomes a curse
ter in the story. This provides many options for problematizing
and the curse never lifts. (Bowden, 2004, p. 8)
the researcher’s telling. For example, the researcher can take on
In summary, point of view can be used in research to varying effect. two perspectives, that of the writer and that of a character within
It is paramount that researchers be purposeful in the literary deci- the construction. When these two perspectives oppose each other,
sions they make and that they fully understand the implications (in there is room for readers to come to their own interpretations.
november 2009 583
Authorial Distance Authorial distance can be used in narrative research to accom-
plish various effects. Like other literary elements, it should be
Authorial distance refers to the degree of immediacy between the
used with purpose.
events portrayed and the time and place of the writing. Authors
choose less narrative distance when they want the reader to expe- Tone
rience the story almost as if he were living it along with the
Narrative researchers use tone to “match, emphasize, alter or con-
­narrator and perhaps identify with one or more characters. The
tradict the meaning of words. . . . As author, you manipulate
writer can choose temporal distance (as in presenting events as
intensity and value in your choice of language, sometimes match-
past or even future) or spatial distance (how far from events the
ing meaning, sometimes contradicting, sometimes overstating,
writer is “standing” as she tells the story). These kinds of decisions
sometimes understating, to indicate your attitude to the reader”
can suggest affiliations and relationships among author, narrator,
(Burroway, 2003, pp. 292–293). In the following excerpt, Vicki
and characters. Concrete detail, scene, and interior monologues
uses ironic humor to describe the location of the ESL classes—in
by characters can be used to achieve a feeling of closeness. Abstract
the darkest corners of the school—and recognizes the signifi-
nouns, summaries, and apparent objectivity can make the reader
cance of that placement:
feel detached from a character or event (Burroway, 2003). Both
effects can be achieved simultaneously by using a combination of
techniques. For example, Wolff speaks coolly and from a distance XI.
in his generalizations between scenes. But the scenes themselves, So this is me my first day of school. I’m carrying my schedule,
particularly those with dialogue, make the reader feel as if he looking for room G-13, thinking, where the hell is this place? I’m
hears and sees what the characters are doing and perceiving. walking down H, walking up and down Upper G, not finding it,
Because of the conventions of education research, researchers and then I notice the stairs on the far corner. I can’t help it. I start
tend to write as if they were viewing events from a great distance. to laugh. I mean could they have put ESL any further away from
For example, they use interview segments of disembodied voices, the office? So then I go down the stairs and I still can’t find it. So
out of context and depersonalized. In the past, researchers often I finally ask a security guard, and he’s like, “G-13? That’s the
used passive third person, as if to convey to the reader a sense mechanic shop!” And he points to another set of stairs, and a
door, about midway down and on the left. I laugh again. You
that no human being had a hand in the study. Choosing a dis-
know they put ESL in the dreariest, darkest place they could
tant position suggests an intention to portray oneself as objec-
find, hiding them like a pair of dirty socks. They say, “That’s your
tive, rational, disinterested, and thus scientific. But this is only classroom. Walk past the transmission, then turn left at the axle.
one of several choices that researchers can make. Choosing less DON’T TOUCH ANYTHING!” They put them where the
authorial distance brings readers directly to the scene, as they white kids go to trade school, you know? And I’m thinking,
“hear the characters’ heavy breathing and smell their emotional great, can their place at Northwest be made any clearer? (Coulter,
anguish. Readers feel so close to the characters that, for those 2003, p. 5)
magical moments, they become those characters” (Burroway,
2003, p. 289). From the data as a whole presented in other parts of Coulter’s
Heavy breathing is evident in this excerpt from Wolff ’s (1989) narrative, the reader understands that Vicki’s humorous tone
narrative. In advance of his mother’s marriage, Jack was sent contradicts the anger she feels that ESL has been assigned class-
ahead to live with Dwight. Dwight was attentive, charming, and room space in the mechanic shop. Tone evokes emotion and
jovial toward him while wooing Jack’s mother. But there is an mood. The tone of the telling can evoke any emotion that fits
undercurrent that Jack alone detects: what the writer is trying to accomplish. A tone that matches the
narrative helps to tell the story. A tone that contradicts the narra-
tive can evoke distrust in the reader that suggests the uncertainty
X. and complexity of the underlying events and multiple points of
Dwight made a study of me. He thought about me during the day
view and attitudes about them.
while he grunted over the engines of trucks and generators, and in Richardson (1985) uses tone to offset her choice of the omni-
the evening while he watched me eat, and late at night while he scient third person in her writing of The New Other Woman:
sat heavy-lidded at the kitchen table with a pint of Old Crow and Tone is revealed in many different ways, such as choice of meta-
a package of Camels to support him in his deliberations. He phors, organization of material, how a quotation of a person’s
shared his findings as they came to him. The trouble with me was, experience is framed and treated by the narrator, what and how
I thought I was smarter than everybody else. The trouble with me much the narrator lets who say and so on. Tone, consequently,
was, I thought other people couldn’t tell what I was thinking. The becomes a way for the omniscient writers of qualitative research
trouble with me was, I didn’t think. . . . The trouble with me was to accomplish two interlocking tasks: reduction of their authority
that I had too much free time. Dwight fixed that. (p. 95) over writing for others, and amplification of their credibility as
In this excerpt, the narrator seems to play with authorial distance. writers of interpretive social science. (p. 39)
The scene reads as if it were in the immediate present—the here
Metaphor, Figurative Language, and Theme
and now. Yet the reader also can discern that it was written as an
adult, from the distance of years. The adolescent sensibilities are Use of a unifying metaphor and figurative language increases the
sustained, but the reader knows the focal character has survived chance of provoking intellectual and emotional responses on the
and reflected on his earlier experiences. part of readers. In Vicki’s account just presented, a metaphor

584 educational Researcher


comes out of her mouth and Coulter needs only to select it as part handmade tag that said, “Welcome to the team, Darek!” in
of the account. “DON’T TOUCH ANYTHING” is more than bubble letters. It looked like a girl’s handwriting. . . .
just a statement about the equipment in the shop. It points to . . . I drove in the cab on the way back, watching the clock. I
the larger meaning that these students may not intrude on the couldn’t afford another tardy. Jason noticed me looking at the
clock.
precious culture of Northwest High. Bowden uses figurative
“Don’t worry about it Darek. Like I said, you’re one of us now.
language to bring the reader into the complexities and ambiguity
Just wear your jacket to class.”
of the story: “Yeah, it’s a magic jacket,” said Dirk, waving his hands in the
air. “You magically walk in on time even if you’re late.”
XII. “But you gotta walk right, Darek. Walk in like you own the
place.” (Coulter, 2009, p. 22)
Listen . . . and you can hear them whispering a fable. Look and
you can glimpse the shadows darting down the midnight streets.
. . . In the summer of 2002, the new reform government of Throughout Darek’s story, the basketball symbolizes membership
Mexico opened the secret files, accumulated by the state between and identity. When he throws the ball back to Thai Boy and joins
1952 and 1985. . . . The secret files for this period number eighty his new football buddies, a symbolic act has occurred. Coulter
million. They hold the screams of interrogations, the taps on wanted to convey the sense that Darek’s identification had
phones, the details of executions and hidden graves. . . . People entered a transition without saying so directly. In the same scene,
line up in the streets of Mexico City to glimpse these secret files. Darek is given a letterman’s jacket by the football team, symbol-
They are searching for their own lives and the lives of husbands, izing a new kind of membership. The themes of access, member-
wives, sons, daughters, and lovers who have vanished as if they ship, and exclusion recur throughout the data in Coulter’s study.
had never existed. Of course there are limits to what people can
They might have been stated discursively as warranted assertions,
find out. . . . And then there is an additional barrier, the millions
but instead she implies themes through language and imagery,
of files accumulated by the U.S. government on the killings and
tortures, files that remain sealed. . . . files that chronicle the inti- hoping to create resonance, association, and empathy in the
mate connection between the United States government and the reader, opening up possibilities for naturalistic generalization
slaughter. . . . Grab a shovel, the work begins. Come, down by the (Stake, 1995, 2000). The purpose of narrative research is not to
river, the dead speak. (Bowden, 2004, p. ii) provide a single definitive answer but to open up possibilities for
new questions and ways of thinking in the reader. And the success
In the remainder of his book, Bowden does not so much conjure of a piece of art, according to Eisner (1998), is the persuasiveness
metaphors as unpack those that are commonly used. He shows of the artist’s vision.
how abstractions and circumlocutions (such as “the war on Fiction writers consider it a mistake to let a theme determine
drugs,” “drug czar,” “globalization,” or “free trade”) distract and other elements of the writing, an approach that often results in a
confuse readers and keep them from looking at unpleasant text that is two-dimensional and predictable. Instead, the writing
details, especially those that connect with their own lives. He itself should suggest themes to the reader. This poses a dilemma
admits, “I can’t even produce a metaphor for the drug world any- for the narrative researcher who must address two communities
more. I don’t even like the phrase. . . . It implies that it is a sepa- at once (Becker, 2007; Richardson, 1990). The scholarly com-
rate world” (p. xx). munity expects the report to include discursive arguments that
Constructing a narrative almost always involves invoking some yield a result—a truth claim. In committing to a narrative ren-
theme or moral. Themes that emerge from data can be alluded dering, the researcher makes arguments implicit. We believe that
to—never named explicitly or asserted directly. The following unifying themes should emerge through the researcher’s data
excerpt from Darek’s story introduces an allusive theme: analysis and through the writing process. Using metaphors is a
means to this end. And because the narrative also serves a research
XIII. purpose, the author carefully ties symbolic language to the con-
crete details that make up everyday life in the setting of action.
We were crossing the parking lot to the gym when Jason drove up
with his F 250 full of green and gold teammates. We had to step Placing Action Within Scenes
back as he rolled his window down. . . .
“Get in, Stephens!” he said. I couldn’t move. Jason and Dirk Essential to story construction is to portray action performed by
got out and walked over to me. They made a big show of shoving actors within scenes. Storytelling requires the researcher to
me into the back of the truck, with Dirk pulling and Jason push- describe the particulars of setting, actors, and action (Connelly &
ing. I let myself be coerced like it wasn’t my own decision, hold- Clandinin, 1990). In the details a text becomes compelling and
ing my ball with both hands. . . . Thai Boy just stood there and provides a vicarious experience for the reader. Scenes require rich
watched, his mouth open again. I threw the basketball to him and detail; general exposition or transitions span the time between
shrugged as we drove away. He caught the ball, hitched it on his
scenes. Setting is a vital aspect of the construction because it helps
hip and watched us go. . . .
create a close, believable telling. If the researcher was a participant
. . . “That’s it, Dirk! He needs a jacket!” Jason said. He stood
up and showed me his jacket. He had a big grin on his face. “Put in the events, he can rely on his recollection of his experiences
it on, Darek.” (acknowledging that his own memories are imperfect). If the
“It won’t fit,” I said. I was the biggest guy on the team. researcher was not himself present as the events unfolded, he faces
“Let’s check the tag, see what size it is,” Jason said. He felt difficulties in contextualizing and must compensate by relying on
around for the tag, found it and showed it to me. It was a multiple data sources. The researcher can retrospectively visit the

november 2009 585


place where events took place or are taking place. In addition, he The structures of Wolff ’s and Wolcott’s narratives follow a
can include interview questions designed to evoke a sense of place fairly straightforward path, with the sequence of scenes reflecting
from the perspective of the participants. The methods of inter- the chronology of events in that part of their lives. Wolff begins
viewing proposed by Spradley (1979), for example, encourage at the point when he and his mother broke away from their for-
participants to respond at such a high level of detail that they mer circumstances. At that point he had intended to become a
enable the researcher to “witness” events secondhand and thus different person and signaled that by renaming himself. The
generate data from which scenes can be reconstructed with starting point Wolff has chosen implies that his purpose is not to
­verisimilitude. present his whole life story but to encompass a period in his life
Describing the setting is only the beginning. Actions must when his sense of self was most at risk and might have gone in a
likewise be described at a fine level of detail and preserve the very different direction. The story ends with his enlistment in the
sense of sequence and social interaction. The scenes we have army. He dispenses with his college years in a few pages and his
presented so far reflect both actions within scenes and charac- enlistment in a single line of text. His tone then changes to one
ter inventories, as participants or characters throw basketballs, of brief and simple exposition. However, this alteration in pace
dip French fries in ketchup, drink Old Crow, and smoke and tone still leaves room for multiple interpretations on the
Camels. The description of significant details allows readers to readers’ part.
imagine themselves into those scenes, which then has the effect Wolcott divides his narrative into segments. The first begins
of creating empathy and resonance. with his discovery of Brad on his property. He then considers his
decision to conduct an ethnographic study with Brad as his sub-
Time
ject. He describes his research methods and then presents seg-
Without representing the passage of time, an author has no nar- ments of data organized in themes, such as “getting paid for
rative. The narrative starts at one point and ends at another, but dropping out” and “I’m not going to get caught.”
the arrangement of scenes need not follow chronology. The After the preamble quoted earlier, in Excerpt XIII, Bowden
author can use foreshadowing techniques, flashbacks, and flash- presents another beginning, and then another, before he tells the
forwards to elicit various effects, for example, to keep the reader story of the unsolved murder and all its connections to the drug
interested and expectant. What does this mean for the researcher? war. He also fails to end the narrative in a way that we might
First, her findings have to reflect some kind of arc on which par- expect, thus showing that there is no real ending and no final
ticipants move through time. She must identify key participants understanding of the events he has narrated.
and settings that can become focal points for carrying the action Whatever the researcher’s approach to structuring time in a
forward. She must look for trajectories, plot points, and hinges narrative construction, the structure serves as a cohesive element
that empirically and logically reflect the lived experiences of those throughout the report, tying together elements and perspectives
she has studied. She must be able to recognize the intentions and and providing temporal context for the audience.
goals of key individuals. So many decisions follow: Where does The literary elements described in this section interact with
the story begin? How much time elapses between the beginning each other in complex ways, posing countless combinations of
and the end? How many scenes should be portrayed? How much decisions for the researcher regarding how to manipulate the text.
general description and exposition should be included at the The aim is to achieve various responses in readers without chang-
beginning and end and between scenes for ideal rhythm and ing the underlying story,7 which itself is the result of evidence and
pace? The arrangement of scenes has an effect on the reader’s analysis.
sense of causality, contingency, and explanation (Becker, 2007).
Narrative, Narrative Research, Evidence, and Truth
Readers orient to earlier passages as probable explanations for
later events; thus researchers need to establish continuity, contin- We conclude this article with some remarks on intersections,
gency, and simple causality from their data before they make fractures, and continuities in the practice and theory space of
sequencing decisions in developing their narratives. All these narrative research.
decisions and more are needed to structure the report. The end-
Narrative and Qualitative Research: Evidence and Analysis
ings need not conform to conventions of happy or sad endings or
even to the convention of tying up all the loose ends. Unlike those who draw sharp boundaries around narrative
Coulter’s narrative begins with her first day at Northwest research, differentiating narrative research from qualitative
High School, describing the way the school looks to her as she research, we argue for continuities. We see commonalities in how
drives toward it. She engages in an internal monologue that evidence is generated—through interviews with participants,
establishes some of the significant events that led her here. The detailed observation of the participants’ social worlds, searches of
second scene takes place in the principal’s office, where both she archives—by researchers immersing themselves over the long
and the ESL program are placed in a diminished role in the term in those social worlds. All this research is done in the service
school’s culture. From there, the narrative proceeds generally of revealing as many aspects of those worlds as possible, from as
toward the end of her tenure at the school. However, the scenes many perspectives as possible, recognizing that any one perspec-
in between are presented from the characters’ points of view. A tive will be inadequate. Narrative researchers and qualitative
key event in most stories is the death of their friend Chansy. Each researchers share an orientation to data analysis as a complex
scene has its own arc and sense of time. The overall structure of transaction between researcher and evidence, the ends of which
the narrative is novelistic, with the methodology and review of are provisional and fallible. Both probe the themes, hypotheses,
literature consigned to appendixes. categories, and assertions that emerge from analysis to see how

586 educational Researcher


they stand up to the weight of evidence and counterclaims. Both (Polkinghorne, 1995, 2007). The researcher tests the evolving
judge their work on characteristics of verisimilitude, fidelity, story with the database. When the data conflict with the evolving
coherence, plausibility, usefulness, and evidentiary warrant. They story, or when there are contradictions, the researcher changes
seek to test their accounts with participants and peers. the configuration of the story. Recursive movements are made
At the moment of preparing representations of their work, between the evolving story and the database until the plot begins
however, the paths of narrative and qualitative researchers are to take form. As this happens, the researcher is able to decide
likely to diverge. Narrative researchers use literary devices such as what events and data elements need to be included in the final
we have described to develop time- and process-oriented account. The evolving story embodies the data and correspond-
accounts. They are less likely than qualitative researchers to use ing analysis.
discursive logic to frame their descriptions of the social worlds of Clearly, there are many continuities and areas of convergence
their participants. They do not usually state explicitly the themes among narrative research and qualitative research studies.
they have discovered, or reveal the methods they have used, pre-
Narrative and Fiction
ferring to let the narratives stand on their own literary merits.
Narrative researchers allow multiple interpretations to emerge, Education research generalists worry about whether the word nar-
yet hope to persuade readers by their artistic visions (Eisner, rative implies fiction. They argue that readers of a text called research
1981, 1998). In contrast, qualitative researchers are more likely have legitimate demands for texts that are factual and, by ordinary
to mingle discursive arguments with descriptions, case studies, standards, true. If an author or researcher departs from the literal
and vignettes. facts as known, readers rightfully expect some declaration of how
We also see commonalities in the ways that qualitative and nar- she went about such departures and where those appear in the text.
rative researchers analyze evidence and choose what parts of the Such declarations make it possible for the reader to decide how to
evidence will go into their representations. To support our view, we come to grips with the text and find it credible. In postmodern
first describe in cryptic terms how an interpretivist qualitative novels, however, such declarations are omitted. The author uses
researcher goes about the process. For Erickson (1986), a good literary devices as cues that something nonliteral is going on. In
interpretivist researcher analyzes evidence as follows: He reads and O’Brien’s (1999) novel Going After Cacciato, the narrative begins
rereads the body of evidence as a whole; generates preliminary asser- with realist trappings. Suddenly the reader becomes aware that the
tions (specific statements of what he believes to be true) by inductive author is playing tricks with time, place, and causality, creating a
means; warrants each assertion by first assembling all segments of realm of magical realism. As we described earlier, Bowden varies his
data that confirm the assertion, assembling all the data segments approach from section to section, veering from realism to dream
that seem to disconfirm the assertion; examines extreme cases for states to something like poetry.
how they shed light on patterns; weighs the evidence, discarding or Although narrative research and fiction may share some liter-
redefining assertions that do not stand up to the warranting process; ary techniques, narrative research accounts are renderings of the
looks for an organization system that links assertions to one another results of research (data collected in multiple forms, analyzed, and
(e.g., hierarchies or processes); for each surviving assertion, con- reported) in which the researcher balances the interests of story-
structs a vignette, something like a short story with actors, settings, telling against the inclusion of every literal detail uncovered in
and an arc that demonstrates the truth of the assertion in narrative her research. For example, she renders two research participants
form; and frames assertions and vignettes in interpretive commen- as a single character or reworks two equivalent incidents into a
tary, with general and particular data. single scene. Although such constructions are sometimes called
Erickson recognizes that the generating and warranting of fictionalizing, we believe such a label furthers the confusion; we
assertions does not follow a formula or rule. Rather, he promotes prefer the terms reworking, rendering, or crafting. We argue that
the idea of bounded rationality: The researcher makes decisions the purposes of research are not antithetical to the purposes of
and choices about the course of data generation and analysis with narrative, which include keeping the reader reading to the last
the best thinking at his disposal, critiquing these decisions, exam- page, and that the use of literary elements helps the process.
ining their consequences, and making the process fully transpar- Skeptics of crafting must consider how even strong and formulaic
ent to readers. Similarly, in choosing what evidence to draw on in quantitative studies exhibit additions and subtractions and sel-
writing vignettes, the interpretivist researcher has no algorithm to dom tell the whole story: Interventions are rarely described in
follow. In practice, the event that is transposed into a vignette is sufficient detail for replication; few sampling designs are executed
often representative of a larger set of confirming data instances. as originally intended; the personal stake and ideology of research-
It is representative not in the statistical sense but in being typical, ers are rarely noted. Researchers transform their materials accord-
and perhaps a vivid example, of the larger set of events that war- ing to conventions at every stage of the research process (Becker,
rant the assertion. Researchers in this model (Graue & Walsh, 2007), corresponding to the purposes of their studies. Moreover,
1998) discuss the practice of writing a vignette as a composite of the reports of conventional research—much as researchers in that
material from more than a single observed event, or with com- tradition would like to persuade readers otherwise—are them-
posite characters; they also reveal the crafting decisions in what selves narratives (Barone, 2001c; Richardson, 1990).
Erickson (1986) termed “the life history of the study.”8 Geertz (1973) summed up this view: “Anthropological writ-
Compare Erickson’s model of analytic induction with ings are themselves interpretations, and . . . they are, thus, fic-
Polkinghorne’s method of narrative explanation.9 Again in cryp- tions; fictions, in the sense that they are ‘something made,’
tic form, the narrative researcher begins by developing a story ‘something fashioned’ . . . not that they are false, unfactual, or
from the database and configuring it into an emplotted account merely ‘as if ’ thought experiments” (p. 15).

november 2009 587


Truth and Narrative Research unseriously, that there is no real suffering in the world” (p. 110).
In a world that punishes (writ small) English Language Learners
Most of the literature on narrative research in education, which
with anomie and marginality and (writ large) others with atroc-
is sampled in our first section, is focused on drawing a sharp
ities, disappearances, and poverty, there is a need for witnesses,
distinction between narrative research and other forms of social
however imperfect.
science, both quantitative and qualitative.10 This literature
focuses on positivism as the canonical difference between narra- Notes
tive research and methodologies in similar terrain. Positivism
assumes that a real world exists separate from perceptions and We would like to acknowledge the editors and anonymous reviewers
interpretations of it. Positivism also assumes that a single truth of this article for their thoughtful insights. In addition, we thank the
can be found about the social world and that science demands the following readers who provided feedback on drafts: Gene Glass, David
pursuit of this truth through objective methods. In contrast, the Berliner, and the CTEL writer’s group. To the ESL students of
dominant stream of narrative research theory in education denies “Northwest” High School: Your stories continue to inspire us.
the existence of a single, external reality and declares that the 1
Space precludes discussion of narrative research as a critical and
establishment of objective methods and single, definitive knowl- social project and allows no more than a mention of how narratives work
edge claims is therefore impossible. Instead, narrative researchers to generate emotions and associations in the reader.
emphasize constructing accounts of participants’ subjective expe-
2
We reviewed the available literature to confirm that our article was
not redundant with sources typically accessible to education researchers
rience and pursue particular rather than general understandings
who are generalists. Of course, Clandinin and Connelly (2000),
of phenomena. To understand participants’ subjective experi-
Clandinin (2007), and Richardson (1990) all offer discussions on writ-
ence, researchers cannot rely on rule- and technology-governed ing narrative research texts.
procedures and methods. Knowledge is constructed through 3
Also see Barone (2001c), Denzin (1992), and Richardson (1990).
transactions among researchers, participants, evidence, and the 4
Choosing texts for this article, that is, for rhetorical purposes, is not
social context. the same as choosing events that will be represented in a narrative account.
The emphasis on this canonical division of positivist-informed 5
An omniscient narrator can choose to disclose her perspective and tell
traditional research versus narrative research has consequences. the story through the eyes of a character who, by virtue of being the sto-
The first is that advocates for narrative research tend to downplay ryteller, presumes to know everyone’s thoughts in retrospect. The reader
and even decry11 projects that search for factual knowledge and must suspend disbelief, but, at the same time, the researcher has the abil-
understanding of the social world. Epistemologies of social and ity to enter her own perspective as a character, clarify her role, and even
point out to the reader that she does not know everything. Is the narrator
historical realism, to name two alternative theories of research,
really O’Brien? Yes and no? Was a real person killed in the way the narra-
assume that a real world exists but deny the ability of human
tor described? The reader cannot be sure. Yet the concrete detail of setting,
researchers to capture a singular truth about it. They deny the actors, and actions, along with the precursors and consequences of these
possibility that any single method or perspective might capture the actions, render the narrative persuasive and coherent as a whole.
full complexity of human and social life, and they include as nec- 6
See Josselson (2007).
essary the subjective experiences of participants’ lives. They treat 7
Bal (1997) refers to the underlying story as the fabula, which has a
participants’ actions as the result of their definitions of the situa- fixed structure of relationships between actors, action, and time. In the
tion, their motives, and their intentions. In these epistemologies, fabula of Joseph, for example, the abandonment of Joseph by his broth-
the orientation to knowledge claims is that such claims are flawed, ers in the well must of necessity occur before his journey to Egypt,
provisional, and partial, although they do, at least in part, reflect although the author who translates the fabula into his own text may
some aspect of the social world before them, much in the way that choose to relate the events in an alternative order. A particular author
may choose to emphasize the brothers rather than Joseph, choosing
maps model territory, despite inadequacies in technical methods
Benjamin as the focal character, without damaging the fabula. Different
of measurement and background knowledge.12 Except for this last
authors may choose to emphasize different tones, actors’ intentions, and
feature, the assumptions of these epistemologies are not unlike other literary elements without affecting the fabula. The reader of this
those embraced by narrative research theories in education.13 article may apply this idea to any of the texts described here.
A further consequence is to mark off as legitimate the work of 8In the epistemological assumptions of qualitative models such as

author–researchers such as Wolcott (2002), Rose (1995), and Erickson’s, there is no rule, no algorithm, no abstract principle other
Kozol (1991), who intend to write the warranted but still imper- than bounded rationality, transparency, reflection, and decision making
fect truth about their research topics, using narrative forms. with the best means available to explain how material gets selected in the
The third consequence of canonically divided literature is a representation. One draws on purpose, audience, form, discourse com-
downplaying of what a narrative is. Current publications in nar- munity, discipline, and the like. This is true for both narrative research
rative research devote only a few pages to defining narrative in and qualitative research more generally and will likely never satisfy those
who believe in the possibility of objective methods, formulaic proce-
any way familiar to narrative theory in literary criticism (see,
dures of inference, and definitive accounts.
e.g., Bal, 1997). Finally, the postmodern denial of reality, truth, 9Polkinghorne’s (1995) process of narrative analysis shares many fea-
and truth-seeking abdicates the responsibility of researchers to tures with Maxwell’s (2004) analysis of the causality in social life, with
act as witnesses to the conditions of everyday life for people who Gould’s (1990) method of analysis of causal dynamics, and with the
are silenced or abused. Susan Sontag (2003) wrote, “The reports method of process analysis used in historiography and political science
of the death of reality—like the death of reason . . . the death of case studies.
serious literature,” when considered in light of the effects of 10We introduce this idea with trepidation because we recognize as

wars and atrocities on individual lives, “[suggest], perversely, valid the fear on the part of advocates for any new branch of methodology

588 educational Researcher


that existing dominant traditions will colonize and appropriate some of Doyle, W. (1997). Heard any really good stories lately? A critique of the
its ideas and practices without completely understanding or appreciating critics of narrative in educational research. Teaching and Teacher
what is novel and particular about them. Education, 12(1), 93–99.
11The use of the term epic to describe narrative texts with omniscient Ecker, D. (1966). The artistic process as qualitative problem-solving. In
narrators and conclusive resolutions drips with irony and distaste, as E. W. Eisner & D. W. Ecker (Eds.), Readings in art education
does Van Maanen’s (1988) ridicule of texts that appear objective as “real- (pp. 57–68). Lexington, MA: Xerox College Publishing.
ist tales” (Bakhtin, 1981; Van Maanen, 1988). Eisner, E. W. (1981). On the difference between scientific and artistic
12We draw here on a number of sources: Azevedo, 1997; Putnam, approaches to qualitative research. Educational Researcher, 10(4), 5–9.
1991; and Ziman, 1990. Eisner, E. W. (1998). The enlightened eye: Qualitative inquiry and the
13In addition, we might include the epistemological positions of enhancement of educational practice. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice
American pragmatism that underlie much arts-based research. See, for Hall.
example, Eisner (1981, 1998) and Stake (1995, 2000). Ely, M. (2007). In-forming re-presentations. In D. J. Clandinin (Ed.),
Handbook of narrative inquiry (pp. 567–598). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Emihovich, C. (1995). Distancing passion: Narratives in social science.
References
In J. A. Hatch & R. Wisniewski (Eds.), Life history and narrative
Azevedo, J. (1997). Mapping reality: An evolutionary realist methodology (pp. 37–48). London: Falmer.
for the natural and social sciences. Albany: State University of New York Erickson, F. (1986). Qualitative methods in research on teaching. In
Press. M. Wittrock (Ed.), Handbook of research on teaching. Chicago: Macmillan.
Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays. Austin: Fenstermacher, G. D. (1997). On narrative. Teaching and Teacher
University of Texas Press. Education, 13(1), 119–124.
Bal, M. K. (1997). Narratology: Introduction to the theory of narrative Finley, S. (2005). Arts-based in inquiry: Performing revolutionary peda-
(2nd ed.). Toronto, Ontario, Canada: University of Toronto Press. gogy. In N. K. Denzin & Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), The Sage handbook of
Barone, T. (2000). Aesthetics, politics, and educational inquiry: Essays and qualitative research (3rd ed., pp. 681–694). Thousand Oaks, CA:
examples. New York: Peter Lang. Sage.
Barone, T. (2001a). Pragmatizing the imaginary: A response to a fiction- Freeman, M. (2007). Autobiographical understanding and narrative
alized case study of teaching. Harvard Educational Review, 71(4), inquiry. In J. Clandinin (Ed.), Handbook of narrative inquiry: Mapping
734–741. a methodology (pp. 120–145). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Barone, T. (2001b). Science, art, and the predispositions of educational Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. New
researchers. Educational Researcher, 30(7), 24–27. York: Basic Books.
Barone, T. (2001c). Touching eternity: The enduring outcomes of teaching. Goodson, I. F. (1995). The story so far: Personal knowledge and the
New York: Teachers College Press. political. In J. A. Hatch & R. Wisniewski (Eds.), Life history and nar-
Barone, T. (2007). A return to the gold standard? Questioning the future rative (pp. 89–98). London: Falmer.
of narrative construction as educational research. Qualitative Inquiry, Gould, S. J. (1990). Wonderful life: The Burgess shale and the nature of
13(4), 454–470. history. New York: W. W. Norton.
Becker, H. (2007). Telling about society. Chicago: University of Chicago Graue, M. E., & Walsh, D. J. (1998). Studying children in context:
Press. Theories, methods, and ethics. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Bowden, C. (2004). Down by the river. New York: Simon & Schuster. Josselson, R. (2007). The ethical attitude in narrative research: Principles
Burroway, J. (2003). Writing fiction: A guide to narrative craft. New York: and practicalities. In J. Clandinin (Ed.), Handbook of narrative inquiry:
Longman. Mapping a methodology (pp. 537–566). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Clandinin, D. J. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of narrative inquiry: Mapping Kozol, J. (1991). Savage inequalities: Children in America’s schools. New
a methodology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. York: HarperPerennial.
Clandinin, D. J., & Connelly, F. M. (2000). Narrative inquiry: Experience Maxwell, J. (2004). Causal explanation, qualitative research, and scien-
and story in qualitative research. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. tific inquiry in education. Educational Researcher, 33(2), 3–11.
Clandinin, D. J., & Murphy, M. S. (2007). Looking ahead: Conversations Mayer, R. (2000). What is the place of science in educational research?
with Elliot Mishler, Don Polkinghorne, and Amia Lieblich. In D. J. Educational Researcher, 26(6), 4–10.
Clandinin (Ed.), Handbook of narrative inquiry: Mapping a methodol- Miller, J. (2005). Sounds of silence breaking: Women, autobiography, cur-
ogy (pp. 632–650). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. riculum. New York: Teachers College Press.
Connelly, F. M., & Clandinin, D. H. (1990). Stories of experience and Nespor, J., & Barber, L. (1995). Audience and the politics of narrative.
narrative inquiry. Educational Researcher, 19(5), 2–14. In J. A. Hatch & R. Wisniewski (Eds.), Life history and narrative
Coulter, C. (2003). Snow White, revolutions, the American dream and (pp. 49–62). London: Falmer.
other fairy tales: Growing up immigrant in an American high school. Novitz, D. (1997). The anaesthetics of emotion. In M. Hjort & S. Laver
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Arizona State University. (Eds.), Emotion and the arts. New York: Oxford University Press.
Coulter, C. (2005). Sahar of the Sonoran Desert. Unpublished manu- O’Brien, T. (1990). The things they carried. New York: Broadway Books.
script. O’Brien, T. (1999). Going after Cacciato: A novel. New York: Broadway
Coulter, C. (2009). Narrative construction as social analysis: Going beyond Books.
the literal. Manuscript submitted for publication. Phillips, D. C. (1994). Telling it straight: Issues in assessing narrative
Denzin, N. (1992). The many faces of emotionality: Reading persona. research. Educational Psychologist, 29, 13–21.
In C. Ellis & M. G. Flaherty (Eds.), Investigating subjectivity: Research Phillips, D. C. (1997). Telling the truth about stories. Teaching and
on lived experience (pp. 17–30). Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Teacher Education, 13(1), 101–109.
Denzin, N. (2000). Preface. In M. Andrews, S. D. Sclater, C. Squire, & Polkinghorne, D. E. (1995). Narrative configuration in qualitative anal-
A. Treacher (Eds.), Lines of narrative: Psychosocial perspectives (pp. xi– ysis. In J. A. Hatch & R. Wisniewski (Eds.), Life history and narrative
xiii). New York: Routledge. (pp. 5–24). London: Falmer.

november 2009 589


Polkinghorne, D. E. (2007). Validity issues in narrative research. Van Maanen, J. (1988). Tales of the field: On writing ethnography.
Qualitative Inquiry, 13(4), 471–486. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Putnam, H. (1991). Representation and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Wolcott, H. F. (2002). Sneaky kid and its aftermath: Ethics and intimacy
Press. in fieldwork. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.
Richardson, L. (1985). The new other woman: Contemporary single Wolff, T. (1989). This boy’s life: A memoir. New York: Grove Press.
women in affairs with married men. New York: Macmillan. Ziman, J. M. (1990). Reliable knowledge: An exploration of the grounds for
Richardson, L. (1990). Writing strategies: Reaching diverse audiences. belief in science. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Riessman, C. K. (2008). Narrative methods for the human sciences. AUTHORS
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. CATHY A. COULTER is an assistant professor in the College of
Rose, M. (1995). Possible lives: The promise of public education in America. Education at the University of Alaska, Anchorage, 3211 Providence
New York: Penguin Books. Drive, PSB 224, Anchorage, AK 99508-4614; afcac@uaa.alaska.edu.
Rosiek, J., & Atkinson, B. (2007). The inevitability and importance of Her research focuses on narrative research methodologies and the experi-
genres in narrative research on teaching practice. Qualitative Inquiry, ences of English learners and immigrant children in public schools.
13(4), 499–521.
Sontag, S. (2003). Regarding the pain of others. New York: Farrar, Straus MARY LEE SMITH is a Regents Professor in the Mary Lou Fulton
and Giroux. College of Education at Arizona State University, Box 872411, Tempe,
Spradley, J. P. (1979). The ethnographic interview. New York: Holt, AZ 85287; mlsmith@asu.edu. She is also a consultant in research design.
Rinehart, Winston. Her research focuses on methodology, including integrated research,
Stake, R. E. (1995). The art of case study research. Thousand Oaks, CA: meta-analysis, and research representation.
Sage.
Stake, R. E. (2000). Case studies. In Denzin, N. & Lincoln, Y. (Eds.), Manuscript received April 15, 2008
Handbook of qualitative research (2nd ed., pp. 435–454). Thousand Revisions received October 28, 2008, and February 3, 2009
Oaks, CA: Sage. Accepted February 25, 2009

590 educational Researcher

You might also like