You are on page 1of 2

Reply to Stokes and Purdon: A study of problems encountered

in Granger causality analysis from a neuroscience perspective


Lionel Barnett∗, Adam B. Barrett†, and Anil K. Seth‡
arXiv:1708.08001v1 [stat.ME] 26 Aug 2017

Sackler Centre for Consciousness Science, Department of Informatics, University of


Sussex, Brighton, BN1 9QJ, UK

August 29, 2017

Stokes and Purdon (1) raise several concerns that (1) erroneously state that “Barnett and Seth
about the use of Granger-Geweke causality (GGC) [. . . ] have proposed fitting the reduced model and
analysis in neuroscience. They make two primary using it to directly compute the spectral compo-
claims: nents . . . ” whereas, as mentioned, we derive GGC
from a single full regression (3).
1. That GGC estimates may be severely biased
Stokes and Purdon then note that GGC reflects
or of high variance, and
a combination of ‘transmitter’ and ‘channel’ dy-
2. That GGC fails to reveal the full struc- namics, and is independent of ‘receiver’ dynam-
tural/causal mechanisms of a system. ics. Again, this independence has been previously
identified, as a direct consequence of the invari-
Unfortunately, these claims rest, respectively, on an ance of GGC under certain affine transformations
incomplete evaluation of the literature and a mis- (7). But why should this independence matter?
conception about what GGC can be said to mea- They suggest that it runs “counter to intuitive no-
sure. tions of causality intended to explain observed ef-
Stokes and Purdon explain how bias and variance fects” since, according to them, “neuroscientists
in GGC estimation arise from the use of separate, seek to determine the mechanisms that produce ‘ef-
independent full and reduced regressions. How- fects’ within a neural system or circuit as a func-
ever, this problem has long been recognised (2, 3) tion of inputs or ‘causes’ observed at other loca-
and, moreover, has already been solved by meth- tions”. In fact, this view resonates more strongly
ods which derive GGC from a single full regression. with approaches such as dynamic causal modelling
These methods essentially extract reduced model (DCM)—usually characterised as ‘effective connec-
parameters from the full model via factorisation tivity’ –which attempt to find the optimal mech-
of the spectral density matrix. Well-documented anistic (circuit level) description that explains ob-
approaches include Wilson’s frequency-domain al- served data. GGC, on the other hand, models de-
gorithm (4), Whittle’s time-domain algorithm (3), pendencies among observed responses and is there-
and a state-space approach which devolves to so- fore an example of (directed) ‘functional connec-
lution of a discrete-time algebraic Riccati equation tivity’ (see (8) for a comparison). Essentially, the
(5, 6). Thus, the source of bias and variance dis- distinction is between making inferences about an
cussed in (1) has already been resolved1 . We note underlying physical causal mechanism (DCM) and
∗ l.c.barnett@sussex.ac.uk (corresponding author) making inferences about directed information flow
† abb22@sussex.ac.uk
(GGC; 9). Both address valid questions.
‡ a.k.seth@sussex.ac.uk
1 Therewill still be bias due to nonnegativity of the GGC
Our view is that the real problems associated
sample statistic, which may be countered by standard sur- with GGC analysis of neurophysiological data re-
rogate data methods. side elsewhere: with issues of stationarity, linearity

1
and exogenous influences, as noted in (1), but also
with the noise, sampling rates and temporal/spatial
aggregation engendered by neural data acquisition
(6, 10).
1. Stokes PA, Purdon PL (2017) A study of problems encountered in Granger
causality analysis from a neuroscience perspective. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA
114(34):7063–7072.
2. Chen Y, Bressler SL, Ding M (2006) Frequency decomposition of conditional
Granger causality and application to multivariate neural field potential data. J. Neu-
rosci. Methods 150:228–237.
3. Barnett L, Seth AK (2014) The MVGC multivariate Granger causality toolbox: A
new approach to Granger-causal inference. J. Neurosci. Methods 223:50–68.
4. Dhamala M, Rangarajan G, Ding M (2008) Estimating Granger causality from
Fourier and wavelet transforms of time series data. Phys. Rev. Lett. 100:018701.
5. Barnett L, Seth AK (2015) Granger causality for state-space models. Phys. Rev. E
(Rapid Communications) 91(4):040101(R).
6. Solo V (2016) State-space analysis of Granger-Geweke causality measures with
application to fMRI. Neural Comput. 28(5):914–949.
7. Barrett AB, Barnett L, Seth AK (2010) Multivariate Granger causality and general-
ized variance. Phys. Rev. E 81(4):041907.
8. Seth AK, Barrett AB, Barnett LC (2015) Granger causality analysis in neuroscience
and neuroimaging. J. Neurosci. 35(8):3293–3297.
9. Barnett L, Barrett AB, Seth AK (2009) Granger causality and transfer entropy are
equivalent for Gaussian variables. Phys. Rev. Lett. 103(23):0238701.
10. Barnett L, Seth AK (2017) Detectability of Granger causality for subsampled
continuous-time neurophysiological processes. J. Neurosci. Methods 275:93–121.

You might also like