You are on page 1of 11

Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Tourism Management
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tourman

The impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions for 2009 H1N1 influenza


on travel intentions: A model of goal-directed behavior
Choong-Ki Lee a, *, Hak-Jun Song b, Lawrence J. Bendle a,1, Myung-Ja Kim a, 2, Heesup Han c, 3
a
College of Hotel & Tourism, Kyung Hee University, 1 Hoegi-dong, Dongdaemoon-gu, Seoul 130-701, Republic of Korea
b
Department of Hotel & Convention Management, College of Tourism & Fashion, Pai Chai University, 14 Yeon-Ja 1 Gil, Seo-gu, Daejeon 302-735, Republic of Korea
c
Department of Tourism Management, College of Business Administration, Dong-A University, Bumin-dong 2-ga, Seo-gu, Busan 602-760, Republic of Korea

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Theoretically, in the tourism context this study introduced a new concept of non-pharmaceutical
Received 22 August 2010 intervention (NPI) for influenza, and tested the impact of NPI on the behavioral intention of potential
Accepted 8 February 2011 international tourists. This study also extended the model of goal-directed behavior (MGB) by
incorporating the new concepts of NPI, and the perception of 2009 H1N1. The model found that
Keywords: desire, perceived behavioral control, frequency of past behavior, and non-pharmaceutical interven-
2009 H1N1
tions predicted tourists’ intention but perceptions of 2009 H1N1 had nil effect on desire and
Non-pharmaceutical intervention
intention. Personal non-pharmaceutical interventions were theorized as adaptive behavior of tour-
Model of goal-directed behavior
Travel intention
ists intending to travel during a pandemic which should be supported by tourism operators on
a system-wide basis. Practically, this study dealt with the issue of influenza 2009 H1N1 with the
study findings and implications providing government agencies, tourism marketers, policy-makers,
transport systems, and hospitality services with important suggestions for NPI and international
tourism during pandemics.
Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction 2010, p. 7). Looking back at international tourism arrivals in


2009, which were estimated as declining by 4%, the economic
In June 2009 the United Nations World Tourism Organization crisis, currency variations, and the 2009 H1N1 pandemic were all
identified the outbreak of a novel influenza A (H1N1) virus, later factors depressing outbound travel. With the pandemic impacting
known as 2009 H1N1, as intensifying the harsh effect of the global markets in Northeast Asia and the Americas particularly (UNWTO,
economic crisis on international tourism, however, this was 2010, p. 14), the 2009 H1N1 pandemic was a significant negative
varying by region (UNWTO, 2009a, p. 1). All regions, excepting factor in an “exceptionally challenging” year for international
Africa (þ3%) and South America (þ0.2%) seeing declines in tourism tourism.
demand. By October, the UNWTO’s panel of experts was predicting Yet, little is known about how potential outbound tourists
that after fuel costs, the 2009 H1N1 pandemic, and uncertain behave when they are considering overseas travel during an
currency exchanges rates would have secondary negative impacts Influenza pandemic. Understanding their behavior would help
on long-haul travel in 2010 (UNWTO, 2009b, p. 6). Then, in government agencies, tourism marketers, transport systems, and
January 2010, 2009 H1N1 was classified as a downside risk with hospitality services cope with a crisis more effectively. This study
less overall impact than expected but which could emerge as classified the perceived risk for 2009 H1N1, a novel influenza virus
a threat to international tourism recovery and growth (UNWTO, that emerged in Mexico, as an obstacle which could discourage
tourists from visiting other countries. For instance, if tourists who
are planning international travel perceive risks from 2009 H1N1,
* Corresponding author. Tel.: þ82 2 961 9430; fax: þ82 2 964 2537. such as suffering illness while abroad, they could postpone or
E-mail addresses: cklee@khu.ac.kr (C.-K. Lee), bloodia00@hotmail.com (H.-J. cancel their plans (Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005). We theorized that
Song), bendle@khu.ac.kr (L.J. Bendle), silver@khu.ac.kr (M.-J. Kim), heesup@ in a pandemic, potential tourists would consider voluntary
donga.ac.kr (H. Han).
1 personal non-pharmaceutical interventions (NPI) which could
Tel.: þ82 2 961 9274; fax: þ82 2 964 253.
2
Tel.: þ82 2 961 0549; fax: þ82 2 964 2537. decrease their risk of 2009 H1N1 infection while traveling. Tourists
3
Tel.: þ82 51 200 7427; fax: þ82 51 201 4335. may choose NPI because of the delay in the production of effective

0261-5177/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tourman.2011.02.006
90 C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

antiviral drugs and vaccines, or because of limited vaccine avail- Symptoms of 2009 H1N1, including cough, runny or stuffy
ability in their country (Chaturvedi, 2009). Personal NPI include nose, sore throat, and high fever are similar to seasonal influ-
getting better knowledge of the disease and pandemic; improving enzas as are the inter-person infection paths. Scientists worry
personal hygiene practices while traveling; using social distancing about it being a critical virus which could mutate into a virulent
to avoid suspect people or places; and monitoring personal health deadly form (WHO, 2009a; Jeeninga et al., 2009) like the Influ-
before and after the trip (Nicholl, 2006). enza A H1N1 that ravaged the globe on the heels of the First
Comprehending and predicting travelers’ behavior is a main World War. That lethal strain of influenza, known as the ‘Spanish
issue for tourism marketers, particularly when a certain obstacle for Flu’, killed up to 100 million people between 1918 and 1920,
traveling like an Influenza pandemic exists. The Theory of Reasoned according to later estimates (Kolata, 2001, p. 7). Appearing in
Action (TRA) and the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) are a mild form causing flu-like symptoms of coughs and high fever
frequently used in previous studies for understanding travelers’ for three days with mild mortality, then, reappearing within six
intentions or behavior. However, these theories have limitations. months as a deadly mutation with a high mortality level it
They do not contain the influence of past behavior which may affect spread around the world killing millions rapidly. In the steam
intention and behavior (Leone, Perugini, & Ercolani, 1999) and they transport era the killer disease circled the globe in a year
focus on the cognitive variables without considering affective (Kolata, 2001).
variables associated with behavior (Conner & Armitage, 1998). So, In the air transport era, which yearly moves more than two
the present study employed the model of goal-directed behavior billion passengers around a network of 35,000 commercial airline
(MGB), which considers motivational process, affective process, connections among commercial airports in close to 3500 cities,
and past behavior, to better predict potential travelers’ decision- a lethal disease can spread around the planet rapidly (St. Michael’s
making processes. According to Perugini and Bagozzi (2001) theory Hospital, 2009, p. 17). For example, SARS, called the first emerging
broadening and deepening of this theory is necessary for improving disease of the age of globalization (Omi, 2006, p. ix), moved about
predictions of human behavior in differing contexts. Hence, this the planet on airline jets, appearing in China’s Guangdong Province
research extended the MGB by including perceptions and NPI for during November 2002 and arriving in Toronto, Canada, on 23
2009 H1N1 that were meaningful in explaining human behavior November 2003. A Canadian tourist, who unknowingly contracted
better. the illness while staying in a Hong Kong hotel, flew home while
Using a sample of Korean potential international tourists we authorities were trying to identify the novel virus (Varia et al.,
explain tourists’ decision-making procedures by applying the 2003). The Toronto SARS outbreak caused 44 deaths, hundreds of
MGB theory and a model that includes perceptions of the infections, quarantine for thousands of citizens, billions of dollars in
disease 2009 H1N1 and the NPI which respond to that disease. lost production, and a billion dollars in outbreak control measures
The study had four aims. First, examining potential travelers’ (St. Michael’s Hospital, 2009).
decision-making processes when the risk of 2009 H1N1 infection Criticisms of overreaction, directed in retrospect, at the WHO,
discourages traveling abroad, by developing an extended model various national governments, and the media about the handling
of goal-directed behavior (EMGB) explaining international travel of the 2003 SARS outbreak shows the sensitivity surrounding
intent that includes perceptions of 2009 H1N1 and NPI in the international cooperation for pandemic disease control (Mason,
MGB framework. Second, verifying the superior predictive ability Grabowski, & Du, 2005; McKercher & Chon, 2004). Reactions to
of the EMGB compared with the TRA and TPB. Three, examining SARS included WHO travel warnings for particular geographical
the perceptions of the 2009 H1N1 virus and the NPI for the virus regions, extensive media coverage of the outbreak, travel advi-
among a sample of Korean potential overseas travelers and the sories against Asian countries, and the curtailing tourist’s civil
role of these perceptions in a proposed theoretical framework. rights at some borders and destinations. In the following panic
Four, providing government agencies, tourism marketers, trans- three million people lost their tourism industry jobs, a $20 billion
port systems, and hospitality services with practical suggestions gross domestic product (GDP) decline occurred in China, Hong
for NPI which could mitigate pandemics and benefit the public Kong, Singapore, and Vietnam, and tourism flows across Asia
and tourism businesses. lessened by 70% (McKercher & Chon, 2004). The global
management of emerging infectious diseases occurs in this
2. Background to the study complex environment of fear of an unstoppable killer virus, rapid
mass international travel, and the reluctance to cause national or
2.1. Pandemics and tourism: historical context international economic damage through inappropriate alerts and
and global consequences warnings.

The worldwide caution surrounding the 2009 H1N1 outbreak 2.2. 2009 H1N1: pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical
stems from international apprehension of another global catas- interventions
trophe like the 1918 ‘Spanish Flu’ influenza pandemic or of an
economic shock like the 2003 Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome National management choices for constraining an emerging
corona virus (SARS) epidemic. During March 2009, an unfamiliar influenza virus include pharmaceutical and non-pharmaceutical
influenza-like illness appeared in the Mexico (World Health interventions (Oshitani, 2006; Rizzo & degli Atti, 2008). Pharma-
Organization [WHO], 2009a). In April under international health ceutical choices include using antiviral drugs and fabricating new
protocols the Mexican government reported this to the Pan- vaccines. Several groups of antiviral drugs are useful during an
American Health Organization as an outbreak of a novel influenza influenza pandemic’s early stage. However, drug resistance limits
type virus (Neumann, Noda, & Kawaoka, 2009; WHO, 2009a). Then, the efficacy of the cheaper and more obtainable group of M2
the United States Centers for Disease Control identified the virus as inhibitors, whereas difficulties of distribution, dosage, and supply
a new strain of Influenza A H1N1 (Jeeninga, de Jong, & Berkhout, limit the usefulness of the more expensive group of neuraminidase
2009) now known as 2009 H1N1. Within six months, as the inhibitors. Also, three parameters govern the benefits of pandemic
number of people infected by 2009 H1N1 rapidly increased glob- influenza vaccines: the four to six months to fabricate a new
ally, the WHO quickly increased the pandemic alert for 2009 H1N1 vaccine, the possibility of several doses being needed, and limited
to the high phase six level (WHO, 2009b). production capacity (Finkelstein, Prakash, Nigmatulina, Klaiman, &
C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99 91

Larson, 2009, p. 6; Oshitani, 2006, p. 168). With these constraints diseases among airline passengers while they are in the airline
on the efficacy and availability of pharmaceutical interventions network are not commonly available. Therefore, passive controls
during an influenza pandemic’s early stage, NPI are essential at national borders have limited effectiveness at halting the
for slowing the outbreak (Aledort, Lurie, Wasserman, & Bozzette, arrival or departure of an influenza-like infectious disease and the
2007). near symmetry between international airport-pairs means shared
These NPI comprise administrative control measures and non- risk of export and import of ill passengers. Besides, to impede
mandatory personal protective measures (Raude & Setbon, 2009, an outbreak by several weeks or more would need a 99% effec-
p. 339). Administrative NPI measures include isolating infected tiveness of border controls and internal travel checks (Ferguson
patients, quarantining individuals in contact with the infection, et al., 2006).
hospital infection control, and border control whereas personal The 2009 H1N1 outbreak from Mexico shows how rapidly
NPI measures include social distancing, and personal hygiene and broadly a disease spreads while the authorities scrambled
protection (Oshitani, 2006, p. 169). Social distancing by closing to identify the new virus. Therefore, improving disease recog-
schools, calling off public events, reducing access to public nition, scrutiny, management, and education which limit
transport, and working from home during an outbreak diminishes person-to-person disease transfers in aircraft and at airports,
the chance for person-to-person infection. Hygiene action by requires cooperation beyond national borders (St. Michael’s
washing hands, wearing masks, and techniques for containing Hospital, 2009, p. 111). The complexity of rapid international
coughing and sneezing restrict disease transmission (Finkelstein air transport; the efficacy and availability of antiviral drugs and
et al., 2009). During the SARS outbreak, the administrative NPI vaccines; and the difficulty of NPI like border controls, mean
of contact tracing, quarantine, and isolation were important in that stopping a pandemic is an evolving global challenge. A
restricting the spread of the virus (Omi, 2006). A supplementary pandemic affects tourism-related industries negatively because
NPI was the Internet’s role in keeping the global public up-to- potential tourists experience increases in perceived risk and
date, and linking the medical experts seeking to identify the novel anxiety due to inadequate information about the origins,
virus (Omi, 2006). prevention, and treatments for the disease (Wu, Law, & Jiang,
At the end of 2009, the impact of 2009 H1N1 pandemic on 2010). So, when a pandemic alert is active the intentions of
tourism and hospitality remains unclear as declines in travel tourists to travel internationally, their perception of the disease,
sales occurred simultaneously with the worldwide economic and personal protective NPI measures for the disease need
recession (Department of Health and Human Services, 2009). As further investigation.
May 2009 ended, a high correlation appeared between H1N1
importations and 16 out of the 20 countries with the uppermost 2.3. Theories of behavior and hypothetical relationships
quantity of air passenger arrivals from Mexico. More generally, if
a country received more than 1400 air passenger arrivals from The TRA and TPB are considered as a representative social
Mexico the risk of H1N1 importation increased markedly (Khan psychological theory in the understanding of specific human
et al., 2009). During the 2009 Southern Hemisphere winter, behavior (Zint, 2002). To improve the usefulness of the TRA and
however, fear of the disease probably caused short-term and or TPB, Perugini and Bagozzi (2001) proposed the model of MGB
localized social and economic effects and a temporary decline in which includes all original variables in the TPB but redefines their
tourism in Australia, Argentina, Chile, New Zealand, and Uruguay role as indirectly affecting behavioral intention through desire.
(Department of Health and Human Services, 2009, p. 9). The five They also claimed that motivational, affective, and habitual
countries each used community mitigation measures in processes should be included in the social psychological model in
response to the disease including using antiviral drugs and NPI order to better comprehend human behavior. In terms of moti-
measures. vational process, desire was suggested as a critical factor in
The NPI measures were administrative controls like closing explaining decision formation (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). With
schools, banning public gatherings, and quarantining ill patients. regard to affective process, anticipated affective reactions to
Before the disease appeared in their territories Australia and Chile a specific behavior were recommended as an imperative variable
used thermal screening to detect inbound tourists with raised in decision-making processes (Conner & Armitage, 1998). In terms
surface temperatures and then switched from this containment of habitual process, past behavior was suggested as a significant
phase to a mitigation phase when the disease appeared in their determinant of human decisions (Aarts, Verplanken, & van
populations. Some flights were canceled between Argentina and Knippenberg, 1998; Bentler & Speckart, 1981; Ouellette & Wood,
Mexico; questionnaires and information pamphlets were used at 1998). So, to better understand specific human behavior the
the U.S. and Mexico border; and New Zealand screened passen- MGB incorporates desire, positive and negative anticipated
gers from countries of concern (Department of Health and Human emotions, and past behaviors as well as the original variables of
Services, 2009, p. 7). The 2009 H1N1 outbreak illustrates the the TPB. Therefore, the MGB incorporates desire, positive and
complexity of implementing NPI administrative controls on negative anticipated emotions, and past behaviors besides the
a global basis. For example, 2.35 million air tourists from Mexico original variables of the TPB.
arrived in 1018 cities in 164 countries during March and April A revision of existing social psychological theories is necessary
2008, with a similar pattern estimated during the same months to improve predictions of human behavior in unique contexts
in 2009 when the novel 2009 H1N1 virus emerged (Khan (Ajzen, 1991). Perugini and Bagozzi (2001) describe this process of
et al., 2009). including new constructs as theory broadening and deepening.
There are several complications in detecting and discouraging Taylor (2007) showed that the MGB can explain variance of
travel by infected passengers who may be the vector by which intention and behavior in a particular context by including extra
a virulent influenza virus moves across the global airline network variables as an extended MGB.
(St. Michael’s Hospital, 2009, p. 111). First, the disease incubation In order to understand international tourists’ decision-making
period is longer than the typical journey period. Second, process during the emerging 2009 H1N1 pandemic, this study
passengers who experience symptoms may not admit their added two more constructs (the perception of 2009 H1N1
condition for fear of disrupting their travel arrangements. Third, pandemic and the NPI for 2009 H1N1 pandemic) as an EMGB
fast reliable screening and diagnostic techniques for infectious besides the original framework of MGB.
92 C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

2.4. Attitude, subjective norm, perceived behavioral the MGB, it was theorized that past behavior influenced desire,
control, and desire intention, and actual behavior (Conner & Armitage, 1998;
Ouellette & Wood, 1998). The final dependent variable in
Researchers agree that attitude toward a behavior exerts this study was behavioral intention, not actual behavior. So,
a positive influence on a person’s intention to perform the this study hypothesized that past behavior affects desire
behavior (Baker, Al-Gahtani, & Hubona, 2007; Cheng, Lam, & Hsu, and intention as follows: H7 e past behavior has a positive effect
2006). The TPB asserts that attitude toward a behavior on desire; and H8 e past behavior has a positive effect on
strengthens a person’s intention to perform it, however, the MGB intention.
redefines attitude as affecting intention indirectly through desire.
So, attitude toward a behavior in the MGB affects intention
indirectly through desire (Leone, Perugini, & Ercolani, 2004; 2.7. Relationships between desire, intention, 2009 H1N1,
Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; Prestwich, Perugini, & Hurling, 2008). and non-pharmaceutical interventions
As salient referents influence personal decision-making and
behavior (Bearden & Etzel, 1982; Cheng et al., 2006) a person Bagozzi (1992) claims that the TPB omits desire as a key factor,
considers complying with other people’s opinions when deter- a motivation-based construct leading to intention where desire
mining if they should undertake a behavior. This subjective norm relates closely to intention, so in the MGB desire is a proximal
is the perceived social pressure to perform or not to perform the cause of intention. Whereas the MGB considers other antecedents
behavior (Ajzen, 1991). In the MGB this would not directly fortify as distant causes which desire mediates before they affect
a person’s behavioral intention but effects behavioral intention intention (Bagozzi, 1992). Leone et al. (1999) claimed that the TPB
indirectly through desire (Leone et al., 2004; Perugini & Bagozzi, did not entail motivational commitment but that the intention to
2001; Prestwich et al., 2008). undertake a specific behavior requires desire (Bagozzi, 1992;
The PBC, which refers to individual confidence to carry out Leone et al., 1999). In the MGB desire is regarded as the most
a behavior, is an important factor in forming intention (Ajzen, proximal determinant of intention (Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). So,
1991; Ajzen & Madden, 1986) and affecting decision-making it was hypothesized that desire has a positive effect on intention
formation in the TPB (Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Madden, 1986; to travel overseas, whereas other antecedents (e.g., attitude,
Conner & Abraham, 2001; Taylor & Todd, 1995). In the MGB it subjective norm, positive anticipated emotion, negative antici-
reinforces desire, behavioral intention, and actual behavior pated emotion, and frequency of past behavior) in the MGB affect
(Carrus, Passafaro, & Bonnes, 2008; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; intention through desire: H9 e desire has a positive effect on
Prestwich et al., 2008). Using the literature, three antecedent intention.
variables for attitude, subjective norm, and PBC were hypothe- Perception refers to an individual’s knowledge, information,
sized to have a positive effect on the desire for overseas travel. and experiences which are responsive to their cognition of
These were: H1 e Attitude has a positive influence on desire; objects, behaviors, and events (Anderson, 2004). People form
H2 e Subjective norm has a positive influence on desire; and their attitudes, interests, and opinions through the perceptions
H3 e PBC has a positive influence on desire. Also, it was which they acquire in their day-to-day lives (Oliver, 1997). Some
hypothesized that PBC would positively affect the intention for studies of perceived risks support the possible relationships
overseas travel: H4 e PBC has a positive influence on behavioral between perception of influenza, desire, and intention (Aro,
intention. Vartti, Schreck, Turtiainen, & Uutela, 2009; Brug et al., 2004;
Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998; Wu et al.,
2.5. Relationships between anticipated emotions and desire 2010). Sonmez and Graefe (1998) stated that perceived risks
play a critical role in altering a tourist’s decision-making process.
With regard to affective influence on a decision-making process, Reisinger and Mavondo (2005) claimed that risks perceived by
anticipated affective reaction to performing a behavior would be an tourists negatively affect their travel intention so they are likely
important determinant of intention (van der Pligt & De Vries, 1998; to choose between keeping their travel plans, changing their
Triandis, 1977). In uncertain situations people may have forward- destination choice, altering their travel behavior, or getting
looking emotions toward future behaviors. Gleicher et al. (1995) pertinent information. Thus, an individual’s perception of 2009
identified these anticipated counterfactuals as prefactuals which H1N1 may affect their decision-making process as an interna-
can affect intention and behavior implying that expected emotions tional tourist.
of goal success or failure can be included in the MGB. Perugini and Therefore, in spite of the influenza, some tourists who desire
Bagozzi (2001) showed that both positive and negative anticipated and intend to travel overseas will prepare health procedures in
emotions have a critical role in forming desire. It was hypothesized conjunction with their trip (Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005). Without
that positive and negative emotions significantly affect desire as the pharmaceutical protection of a vaccine they may voluntarily
follows: H5 e positive anticipated emotion has a positive effect on implement personal NPI prior to, during, and following their trip to
desire; and, H6 e negative anticipated emotion has a negative effect mitigate their perceptions of risk (Aledort et al., 2007; Aro et al.,
on desire. 2009; Brug et al., 2004). These include gaining better knowledge
about the disease and the pandemic, actions for improving their
2.6. Relationship between past behavior, desire, and intention personal hygiene while traveling, and decreasing infection possi-
bilities by social distancing from suspect people and places. The
The influence of past behavior was found to have an effect on study posited that perceptions of 2009 H1N1 have a direct or
individual intention in several attitude and behavior studies indirect effect on tourists’ desire and intention to travel through NPI
(Bagozzi & Warshaw, 1992). Specifically, past behavior is often to avoid H1N1 infection as follows. H10 e perception of 2009 H1N1
integrated into a theoretical framework that explains an indi- has a negative influence on desire; H11 e perception of 2009 H1N1
vidual’s decision-making process (Conner & Armitage, 1998). has a positive effect on non-pharmaceutical interventions; H12 e
Past behavior is regarded as a proxy of habit, implying that perception of 2009 H1N1 has a negative effect on intention; and
it influences desire and intention (Bagozzi & Warshaw, H13 e non-pharmaceutical interventions have a positive effect on
1992; Bentler & Speckart, 1981; Fredricks & Dossett, 1983). In intention.
C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99 93

3. Methods passwords for verifying the identity of panelists included in each


sample. They are selected as best-fit participants by response to
3.1. Measures and operational definition of variables a sampling questionnaire and rejected during the survey if they
complete their questionnaire too rapidly (Embrain, 2009). The
A preliminary list of measurement items was generated after an study data were collected during July 2009, when the 2009
extensive review of literature concerning the behavior of interna- H1N1 outbreak was being covered by international and national
tional tourists, theories of human behavior including TRA, TPB, and media in Korea.
MGB, information on Influenza A H1N1, and prevention measures The Internet survey firm distributed questionaires to 990
for influenza diseases (Ajzen, 1985, 1991; Ajzen & Madden, 1986; potential tourists chosen randomly. From these, by using
Bagozzi, Baumgartner, & Pieters, 1998; Bentler & Speckart, 1981; a screening question, we selected those with at least one overseas
Brug et al., 2004; Carrus et al., 2008; Lam & Hsu, 2004, 2006; trip during the past three years. By this procedure 400 ques-
Ministry for Health, Welfare and Family Affairs, Korea, 2009a, tionnaires were collected (40.4%) and after excluding three
2009b; Oh & Hsu, 2001; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; Reisinger & questionnaires as outliers 397 were coded for analysis. Collected
Mavondo, 2005; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998; UNWTO, 2009c; WHO, data were analyzed with the Statistical Package for the Social
2009c). Then to ensure the questionaire’s understandability Sciences (2001) and EQS eStructural Equation Modeling Soft-
tourism scholars and travel industry managers were asked to assess ware (Bentler & Wu, 1995). As a first step in the evaluation of the
the items of measurements and a pretest was also conducted with measurement model, exploratory factor analysis identified the
20 people who experienced an overseas trip during the past three structure of factors and systematically measured variables in
years. Through these processes items that seemed ambiguous were underlying constructs. This lessened multicollinearity or error
reworded for clarity. variance correlations among indicators (Bollen, 1989; Yoon &
The subjects’ attitude toward overseas travel was operational- Uysal, 2005). The structural equation modeling used a two-step
ized with six items (see Appendix) as suggested by previous hybrid method by specifying a measurement model in the
research (Ajzen & Madden, 1986; Bagozzi et al., 1998; Lam & Hsu, confirmatory factor analysis and testing a latent structural model
2004, 2006; Oh & Hsu, 2001). The subjective norm to travel developed from the measurement model (Anderson & Gerbing,
internationally was operationalized with four items (see 1988; Hatcher, 1994; Kline, 2005).
Appendix) as suggested by previous research (Ajzen, 1991;
Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; Oh & Hsu, 2001). The perceived behav-
ioral control was operationalized with four items (Appendix) as 4. Results
suggested by previous research (Ajzen, 1991; Lam & Hsu, 2006).
The perception of Influenza A (H1N1) was operationalized with 4.1. Profile of respondents and measurement model
four items (Appendix) as suggested by previous research
(Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005; Sonmez & Graefe, 1998). The non- The proportion of male and female respondents was 57.4% and
pharmaceutical interventions for Influenza A (H1N1) were oper- 42.6% respectively, most respondents (73.8%) were aged 20e49
ationalized with seven items. These items were derived from the years, and university graduates predominated (64.4%). Respon-
codes and precautions of UNWTO, WHO, and MIHWFA to prevent dents with an international travel plan (78.1%) preferred package
from catching Influenza A (H1N1) (MIHWFA, 2009a, 2009b; tours (55.9%), liked traveling internationally with family (47.6%) or
UNWTO, 2009c; WHO, 2009c). Three of these items included friends (24.4%), and most (79.3%) indicated that vacationing was
actions to improve knowledge of the disease (Appendix). Three their main reason for overseas travel. The preferred destinations
items included actions about personal hygiene and health. And included East Asia (26.2%), Europe (23.9%), Japan (17.4%), China/
one item included action on social distancing. Desire was oper- Hong Kong/Macao (11.8%), and Australia or New Zealand (9.8%). The
ationalized with four items (Appendix) as suggested by previous model was analyzed using EQS 6.1 to establish the degree of reli-
research (Carrus et al., 2008; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001). Positive (4 ability and validity of the confirmatory factor analysis which was
items) and negative (4 items) anticipated emotions (Appendix) composed of 42 observed variables and 9 latent variables. Since
were operationalized with eight items as suggested by previous Mardia’s standardized coefficient (76.5) is greater than the criterion
research (Carrus et al., 2008; Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001; Prestwich of 0.5 (Byrne, 2006) it is considered the data are multivariate non-
et al., 2008). The variables of attitude, subjective norm, perceived normally distributed, so maximum likelihood robust estimation
behavioral control, perception of Influenza, and NPI were rated on associated with the SatorraeBentler was used (Byrne, 1994a,
a 7-point Likert scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly 1994b).
agree (7). However, positive and negative emotions were rated on This approach for non-normality in multivariate data makes
a 7-point Likert scale ranging from not at all (1) to very much (7). corrections to the standard errors, Chi-square, and other fit
The frequency of past behavior was assessed with a single item as indexes (Bentler & Wu, 1995; Byrne, 1994a). Table 1 shows the
suggested by previous research (Bentler & Speckart, 1981; Oh & good fit to the data (RMSEA ¼ 0.029, CFI ¼ 0.973, NFI ¼ 0.899) and
Hsu, 2001), “How many times have you traveled internationally enough internal consistency with Cronbach’s alpha ranging from
in the past 12 months?” 0.853 to 0.961 (Nunnally, 1978). Convergent and discriminant
validity were examined to judge construct validity. All factor
3.2. Data collection and analysis loadings were greater than the minimum criterion of 0.5, with
significant associated t-values and all average variance extracted
With the development of Internet some researchers in and composite reliability values for the multi-item scales
hospitality and tourism fields use online surveys to reach exceeding the minimum criterion of 0.7 and 0.5 respectively (Hair,
broader populations of interest efficiently (Han, Hsu, & Lee, Black, Babin, Anderson, & Tatham, 2006). Thus, convergent validity
2009; Kim & Ok, 2009). This study used data collected by the was fully supported. Also, the average variance value extracted for
top ranking Korean Internet survey firm that utilizes a nation- each construct was greater than the squared correlation coefficient
wide panel of 490,000 online respondents from which repre- for corresponding inter-constructs. These results confirmed the
sentative samples are selected. Their standard procedures use sufficient level of discriminant validity of the measurement model
Korean resident registration numbers matched against personal (Fornell & Larcker, 1981).
94 C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

Table 1
Results of measurement model.

Constructs Correlations among latent constructs (squared)a and reliabilities of constructs

AT SN PBC PAE NAE PI DE NPI BI CRc AVEd


Attitude (AT) 1.000 0.944 0.739
Subjective norm (SN) 0.491 (0.241) 1.000 0.961 0.861
Perceived behavioral 0.255 (0.065) 0.396 (0.157) 1.000 0.841 0.571
control (PBC)
Positive anticipated 0.751 (0.564) 0.510 (0.260) 0.201 (0.040) 1.000 0.916 0.732
emotion (PAE)
Negative anticipated 0.278 (0.077) 0.309 (0.095) 0.188 (0.035) 0.504 (0.254) 1.000 0.867 0.620
emotion (NAE)
Perception of 0.042 (0.002) 0.113 (0.013) 0.019 (0.000) 0.090 (0.008) 0.036 (0.001) 1.000 0.877 0.641
Influenza (PI)
Desire (DE) 0.700 (0.490) 0.579 (0.335) 0.251 (0.063) 0.792 (0.627) 0.496 (0.246) 0.034 (0.001) 1.000 0.917 0.734
Non-pharmaceutical 0.208 (0.043) 0.194 (0.038) 0.133 (0.018) 0.235 (0.055) 0.089 (0.008) 0.584 (0.341) 0.209 (0.044) 1.000 0.885 0.527
interventions (NPI)
Behavioral intention (BI) 0.605 (0.366) 0.656 (0.430) 0.461 (0.213) 0.692 (0.479) 0.428 (0.183) 0.083 (0.007) 0.842 (0.709)b 0.283 (0.080) 1.000 0.938 0.790
Cronbach’s alpha 0.942 0.961 0.853 0.914 0.876 0.882 0.914 0.904 0.933
SB c2 ¼ 981.532, df ¼ 736, p < 0.001, RMSEA ¼ 0.029, CFI ¼ 0.973, NFI ¼ 0.899
a
Correlation coefficients are estimates using a EQS software program.
b
Highest correlations between pairs of constructs.
c
CR ¼ composite reliability; AVE ¼ average variance extracted.
d
Frequency of past behavior was not included in the measurement model because it was a single indicator.

4.2. TRA, TPB, and EMGB tests Chang, 1998) and imply that including perceived behavioral
control did play a significant role in predicting behavioral
In Table 2, the TRA model had a good fit to the data (c2 ¼ 84.549, intention. Second, the TPB model was slightly better in fit
df ¼ 74, p < 0.001, c2/df ¼ 1.143, RMSEA ¼ 0.019; CFI ¼ 0.996; statistics, but the model still lacks the explanatory power of
NFI ¼ 0.972). Attitude toward a behavior and subjective norm behavioral intention as compared to the EMGB. That is, the EMGB
explained 53.4% of the total variance of intention. The TPB model improved R2 largely from 0.573 to 0.793.
had a sufficient fit to the data (c2 ¼ 160.127, df ¼ 128, p < 0.001, c2/ Chi-square tests indicate there was significant difference
df ¼ 1.251, RMSEA ¼ 0.025; CFI ¼ 0.992; NFI ¼ 0.960). Three between these two models (Dc2 (657) ¼ 931.10, p < 0.001). The
antecedent variables (i.e., attitude, subjective norm, and PBC) MGB better accounted for the variance in explaining behavioral
accounted for approximately 57.3% of the variance of intention to intention. This model was superior to the TRA model for explana-
travel internationally. The value of R2 for intention improved tory power (EMGB ¼ 0.793 vs. TRA ¼ 0.534, Dc2 (711) ¼ 1008.41,
slightly from 0.534 to 0.573. Finally, the EMGB showed an excellent p < 0.001). Consistent with other research (Bagozzi & Dholakia,
fit to the data (c2 ¼ 1087.184, df ¼ 785, p < 0.001, c2/df ¼ 1.385, 2006; Carrus et al., 2008; Prestwich et al., 2008; 2008; Taylor,
RMSEA ¼ 0.031; CFI ¼ 0.967; NFI ¼ 0.891). The value of R2 for Bagozzi, & Gaither, 2005) the results showed the EMGB with
intention was 0.793. desire, positive anticipated emotion, negative anticipated emotion,
and frequency of past behavior performs better than the TRA and
TPB models. Enhancing our understanding of the decision process
4.3. Comparing the three models of behavioral intention these results prompt several suggestions.
The TPB is inadequate for explaining behavioral intention to travel
The three competing models, the TRA, the TPB, and the EMGB, internationally, and the processes behind the effect of the predic-
were compared for their relative explanatory power. First, the tors are more intricate than assumed in the TPB (Perugini &
TPB model had a better explanatory power than the TRA model. Bagozzi, 2001).
Specifically, three predictor variables (attitude, subjective norm,
and perceived behavioral control) in the TPB model explained
4.4. Test of hypotheses
approximately 57.3% of the total variance in the behavioral
intention to travel internationally while both attitude and
As shown in Fig. 1, four predictor variables (attitude, subjective
subjective norm jointly explained about 53.4% of the total vari-
norm, positive anticipated emotion, and negative anticipated
ance in the TRA model (Table 2). These findings were consistent
emotion) in the EMGB were positively associated with desire to
with previous research in other settings (Ajzen & Madden, 1986;
travel internationally (bAT/DE ¼ 0.206, t ¼ 2.568, p < 0.01;
bSN/DE ¼ 0.236, t ¼ 3.920, p < 0.01; bPAE/DE ¼ 0.461, t ¼ 4.867,
p < 0.01; bNAE/DE ¼ 0.136, t ¼ 3.244, p < 0.01). This implies an
Table 2
Modeling comparisons.
individual’s desire is a significant function of attitude, subjective
norm, positive anticipated emotion, and negative anticipated
SeB c2 df c2/df RMSEA CFI NFI R2 for BI emotion. However, the findings indicate that PBC, frequency of past
TRA 84.549 74 1.143 0.019 0.996 0.972 0.534 behavior, and perception of 2009 H1N1 did not have a significant
TPB 160.127 128 1.251 0.025 0.992 0.960 0.573
influence on desire (bPBC/DE ¼ 0.017, t ¼ 0.380, not significant;
EMGB 1087.184 785 1.385 0.031 0.967 0.891 0.793
Suggested 3  0.08  0.90  0.90 bFPB/DE ¼ 0.004, t ¼ 0.102, not significant; bPI/DE ¼ 0.046,
value* t ¼ 1.340, not significant). Thus, hypotheses H1, H2, H5, and H6
Note: Suggested values were based on Hair et al. (2006), and Bearden and Etzel
were accepted, but H3, H7, and H10 were rejected.
(1982). RMSEA ¼ root mean square error of approximation; CFI ¼ comparative fit The relationships between the perception of 2009 H1N1 and NPI
index; NFI ¼ nonnormed fit index. were found positive and significant (bPI/NPI ¼ 0.585, t ¼ 7.568,
C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99 95

Attitude Perception of
(AT) 0.585** Non-
Influenza A (7.568)
H1N1 Pharmaceutical
0.206* (PI) Interventions
(2.568) (NPI)
Subjective 0.016 R2: 0.342
Norm 0.236** -0.046 (0.367) 0.100*
(SN) (3.920) (-1.340) (2.308)

Positive 0.461** 0.781**


Desire Behavioral
Anticipated (4.867) (11.684)
(DE) Intention
Emotion
(BI)
(PAE)
0.136** R2: 0.713
(3.244) R2: 0.793

Negative 0.062*
Anticipated -0.004
-0.017 (-0.102) (2.248)
Emotion (-0.380)
(NAE)
0.251** Frequency of
(5.611) Past
Behavior
Perceived (FPB)
Behavioral
Control
(PBC)

Fig. 1. Results of the extended model of goal-directed behavior. Note: Shaded area shows the basic components of the MGB and the two bold ovals with italics are the additional
constructs of PI and NPI that contribute to developing the EMGB.

Table 3
Decomposition of effects with standardized values.

Direct effect Indirect effect Total effect

DE NPI BI DE NPI BI DE NPI BI


AT 0.206** e e e e 0.161** 0.206** e 0.161**
SN 0.236** e e e e 0.184** 0.236** e 0.184**
PBC 0.017 e 0.251** e e 0.013 0.017 e 0.238**
PAE 0.461** e e e e 0.360** 0.461** e 0.360**
NAE 0.136** e e e e 0.106** 0.136** e 0.106**
PI 0.046 0.585** 0.016 e e 0.022 0.046 0.585** 0.039
NPI e e 0.100** e e e e e 0.100**
FPB 0.004 e 0.062** e e 0.003 0.004 e 0.059
DE e e 0.781** e e e e e 0.781**

*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, n/s: not significant Note: AT ¼ attitude; SN ¼ subjective norm; PBC ¼ perceived behavioral control; PAE ¼ positive anticipated emotion; NAE ¼ negative
anticipated emotion; FPB ¼ frequency of past behavior; PI ¼ perception of Influenza A (H1N1); NPI ¼ non-pharmaceutical interventions; DE ¼ desire; BI ¼ behavioral
intention.

p < 0.01). Therefore H11 was supported. Four predictor variables anticipated emotion (0.106), and NPI (0.100). When predicing
(desire, PBC, frequency of past behavior, and NPI) were positively desire the powerful factor was positive anticipated emotion with
associated with intention to travel internationally (bDE/BI ¼ 0.781, a total impact of 0.461, followed by subjective norm (0.236), atti-
t ¼ 11.684, p < 0.01; bPBC/BI ¼ 0.251, t ¼ 5.611, p < 0.01; tude (0.206), and negative anticipated emotion (0.136). The
bFPB/BI ¼ 0.062, t ¼ 2.248, p < 0.01; bNPI/BI ¼ 0.100, t ¼ 2.308, perception of 2009 H1N1 indirectly effected travel intention when
p < 0.01). Thus, hypotheses H9, H4, H8, and H13 were accepted. This it is mediated by personal NPI (see Fig. 1). Although the perception
implies that desire, PBC, frequency of past behavior, and NPI are of 2009 H1N1 had no effect on desire and behavioral intention, the
important in an individual’s intention to travel internationally. NPI for 2009 H1N1 was a significant predictor of behavioral
However, the path from 2009 H1N1 to intention was not significant intention to travel internationally. The four constructs of desire,
(bPI/BI ¼ 0.016, t ¼ 0.367, p > 0.05), not supporting the proposition perceived behavioral control, frequency of past behavior, and NPI,
that 2009 H1N1 has a negative effect on intention. So, H12 was all performed an important role in predicting potential tourists’
rejected. behavioral intention to travel internationally.

4.5. Indirect and total effects 5. Conclusions

As Table 3 shows, when predicting behavioral intention desire 5.1. Travel desire and adaptive behavior
was the important factor with an impact of 0.781, followed
by positive anticipated emotion (0.360), perceived behavioral Knowledge is limited about how the 2009 H1N1 pandemic
control (0.238), subjective norm (0.184), attitude (0.161), negative impacted potential international tourists’ decision-making although
96 C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

it is assumed that tourists’ apprehension of contracting 2009 H1N1 behaviors while taking a trip. This approach may not be shared
while traveling affected the tourism industry negatively. To the by all tourists because socioeconomic characteristics are linked
authors’ awareness, this is the first trial which focused on improving to variations in health outcomes (Raude & Setbon, 2009), so
predictions of international tourists’ travel intention and decision- educational initiatives are needed. For example, guidelines for
making using an extended model of the MGB. In specific, this NPI hygiene should be continually available alongside other
approach included the perception of 2009 H1N1 and the personal safety information in airline onboard publications. Also, tourism
NPI for 2009 H1N1. The EMGB incorporated the construct of desire as operators can improve their online communications concerning
a mediator which considered volitional factors (attitude and pandemic diseases to reassure tourists of their relative safety
subjective norm), non-volitional factors (perceived behavioral and reduce their apprehension about traveling. Second, in
control, the perception of 2009 H1N1, and the NPI for 2009 H1N1), addition to public restrooms, transport and hospitality services
emotional factors (positive anticipated emotion and negative antic- should provide convenient NPI hygiene and information kiosks
ipated emotion), and the frequency of past behavior. Also, including within their facilities which support self-protective health
both the perception of 2009 H1N1 and personal NPI in the EMGB behavior by staff, passengers, or guests. Because personal NPI
were supported by the model’s increased power in predicting are the adaptive behavior of many potential tourists who wish
potential tourists’ intention to travel internationally. to proceed with their journey during a pandemic when uncer-
Showing its superior predictive validity the EMGB accounted for tainty surrounds administrative disease control measures and
significantly more variance in travel intention than either the TRA the efficacy and availability of pharmaceutical interventions. For
or the TPB, which is an improvement in explaining potential example, if designed for high usage these kiosks can provide
international tourists’ intentions. Demonstrating, in this context, multiple hand cleaning devices and multi-lingual displays of
that theory broadening and deepening as described by Perugini and current health risks at regular locations in airport boarding
Bagozzi (2001) is reasonable by integrating new constructs or by lounges.
altering the paths to be more appropriate in the model. So, the Third, similar to green tourism certification, tourism operators
revised model explains a substantial proportion of the total vari- and governments should establish a validation system for NPI
ance of the dependent variable of intention in this study. Also, friendly businesses which continuously support the personal NPI
consistent with previous studies of MGB (e.g., Bagozzi & Dholakia, actions of their staff, passengers or guests. Because the global reach
2006; Carrus et al., 2008; Taylor, 2007) desire as a sufficient of novel influenza-like viruses and the limits of mandatory
impetus for intention formation was the most important latent administrative NPI, such as border control and passenger screening
variable. In the model, the more important determinants to desire techniques, provide tourism businesses with opportunities for
were the emotional factors, specifically positive anticipated good corporate citizenship and for health related competitive
emotion while the other determinants such as the subjective norm, advantages. For example, transport systems and hospitality
attitude, and negative anticipated emotions are less important to services should implement staff training in personal NPI for their
the prediction of desire. It is not problematic that all antecedent own benefit and for them to better assist their passengers and their
variables in the EMGB do not make a considerable contribution to guests. This could be promoted as a “Green & Clean” certification
behavioral intention because the relative importance of individual process developed by cooperation between tourism businesses and
antecedent variables in a model may differ based on given contexts health agencies. Businesses that are accredited with better hygiene
(Sparks & Pan, 2009). support for their passengers or guests should have a positive brand
It is noteworthy the perception of 2009 H1N1 was not a signif- identity in the minds of potential tourists who are planning their
icant (direct) predictor of either desire or behavioral intention next trip.
although some previous research proposed possible relationships Four, government agencies, tourism marketers, transport
among the perceptions of a disease, the response to that disease, systems, and hospitality services should adopt the system-wide
attitude, and intention (Reisinger & Mavondo, 2005; Sonmez & tourism industry approach of a permanent, as opposed to an
Graefe, 1998). However, it did influence international travel inten- episodic, disease mitigation strategy which supports personal
tions indirectly through personal NPI. This Korean data suggests responsibility for staff, passenger or guest health. At a planetary
that the perception of 2009 H1N1 did not constrain the desire for scale influenza pandemics are a continuous risk due to a combi-
international travel among potential tourists as they had some nation of issues. These are the complexity and rapidity of the
adaptive behavior in mind which lowered the infection threat to global air transport; the practical and technological constraints
a level acceptable to them. In effect, personal NPI are an adaptive on border controls and passenger screening; the influenza virus’s
behavior which reinforce the desire that supports their behavioral antigenic changeability; and the limited efficacy of antiviral
intention. This coincides with the findings of Raude and Setbon drugs and vaccines. For example, to combat the complexity of
(2009, p. 342) that the French public has moderately adaptive these issues international and national governmental tourism
beliefs and attitudes about new infectious respiratory illnesses organizations should give health, hygiene, and disease preven-
which lead them to personal NPI as a means for lowering contagion tion equal status with sustainability or security as a policy
risks. priority.
Future research suggestions that reinforce these approaches
5.2. Implications for global tourism and future research include longitudinal studies of the effect of global pandemics on
tourists’ intentions and their approach to personal NPI as pandemic
Recognizing personal NPI for 2009 H1N1 as an adaptive alert levels vary over time. Also, as we focused on Korean tourists,
behavior among tourists infers four significant implications for new research should address variations of nationality and socio-
government agencies, tourism marketers, transport systems, economic status among tourists to discover the communication
and hospitality services which raise opportunities for practical and education issues surrounding pandemic disease awareness,
suggestions. First, the intention to travel internationally is and the use of personal NPI and travel behavior. Finally, identifying
resilient during a global pandemic because potential interna- the NPI hygiene techniques which are practical in diverse tourism
tional tourists perceive personal NPI like hand washing, mask setting and the actions required to modify tourist behavior
wearing, and information gathering as plausible protective accordingly, require research attention.
C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99 97

Appendix.

Operational definitions of the measures

Construct Operational definition


Attitude (AT) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)
I think that traveling internationally is positive.
I think that traveling internationally is useful.
I think that traveling internationally is valuable.
I think that traveling internationally is dynamic.
I think that traveling internationally is attractive.
I think that traveling internationally is enjoyable.*
I think that traveling internationally is delightful.

Subjective norm (SN) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


Most people who are important to me think it is okay for me to travel internationally.
Most people who are important to me support that I travel internationally.
Most people who are important to me understand that I travel internationally.
Most people who are important to me agree with me about traveling internationally
Most people who are important to me recommend traveling internationally.*

Perceived behavioral control (PBC) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


Whether or not I travel internationally is completely up to me.*
I am capable of traveling internationally.
I am confident that if I want, I can travel internationally.
I have enough resources (money) to travel internationally.
I have enough time to travel internationally.
I have enough opportunities to travel internationally.*

Perception of Influenza A (H1N1) (PI) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


It is dangerous to travel internationally because of Influenza A (H1N1).
Influenza A (H1N1) is a very frightening disease.
Compared to SARS and avian flu, Influenza A (H1N1) is more dangerous.
I have much information about Influenza A (H1N1).*
I am afraid of Influenza A (H1N1).
People around me seem to refrain from traveling internationally due to Influenza A (H1N1).*

Desire (DE) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


I want to travel internationally in the near future.
I wish to travel internationally in the near future.
I am eager to travel internationally in the near future.
My wish to travel internationally in the near future can be described desirably.

Frequency of past behavior (FOP) How many times have you traveled internationally in the past 12 months?

Non-pharmaceutical interventions Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


for Influenza A(H1N1) (NPI) I will check the information of on Influenza A (H1N1) by visiting the website of the Korean
Ministry of Foreign Affairs or WTO before traveling internationally.
I will read and check precautions about Influenza A (H1N1) through doctors or health centers
before traveling internationally.
I will prepare a first aid kit for Influenza A (H1N1) before traveling internationally.*
I will get the information about local medical facilities and Korean Embassy for preparing for an
emergency because of Influenza A (H1N1) before traveling internationally.
I will frequently wash my hands while traveling internationally.
I will restrain from touching my eyes, nose, and mouth while traveling.
I will cover my mouth and nose with a tissue when sneezing while traveling internationally.*
I will keep away from those who have the symptoms of Influenza A (H1N1) while traveling internationally.
I will restrain from meeting people for a while after traveling internationally.*
I will carefully keep an eye on my health condition after traveling internationally.

Anticipated emotion (AE) Not At All (1) to Very Much (7)


If I succeed in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be excited.
If I succeed in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be glad.
If I succeed in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be satisfied.
If I succeed in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be happy.
If I succeed in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be proud.*
If I fail in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be unsatisfied.*
If I fail in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be angry.
If I fail in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be disappointed.
If I fail in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be worried.
If I fail in achieving my goal of traveling internationally, I will be sad.

Behavioral intention (BI) Strongly disagree (1)/Strongly agree (7)


I intend to travel internationally in the near future.
I am planning to travel internationally in the near future.
I will make an effort to travel internationally in the near future.
I will certainly invest time and money to travel internationally in the near future.
I am willing to travel internationally in the near future.*

Note: Based on the results of EFA and CFA. * items were excluded from further analyses.
98 C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99

References Fredricks, A., & Dossett, D. (1983). Attitude behavior relations: a comparison of the
FishbeineAjzen and BentlereSpeckart models. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 45(3), 501e512.
Aarts, H., Verplanken, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). Predicting behavior from
Gleicher, F., Boninger, D. S., Strathman, A., Armor, D., Hetts, J., & Ahn, M. (1995). With
actions in the past: repeated decision making or a matter of habit? Journal of
an eye toward the future: the impact of counterfactual thinking on affect,
Applied Social Psychology, 28(15), 1355e1374.
attitudes, and behavior. In N. J. Roese, & J. M. OlsonMahwah (Eds.), What might
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: a theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl,
have been: The social psychology of counterfactual thinking (pp. 283e304).
& J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11e39).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Heidelberg: Springer.
Hair, J. F., Jr., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., Anderson, R. E., & Tatham, R. L. (2006).
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human
Multivariate data analysis. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Decision Processes, 50(2), 179e211.
Han, H., Hsu, L., & Lee, J. (2009). Empirical investigation of the roles of attitudes
Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: attitude,
toward green behaviors, overall image, gender, and age in hotel customers’ eco-
intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social
friendly decision-making process. International Journal of Hospitality Manage-
Psychology, 22(5), 453e474.
ment, 28(4), 519e528.
Aledort, J., Lurie, N., Wasserman, J., & Bozzette, S. (2007). Non-pharmaceutical
Hatcher, L. (1994). A step-by-step approach to using the SAS Systems for factor analysis
public health interventions for pandemic influenza: an evaluation of the
and structural equation modeling. Cary, NC: SAS Institute.
evidence base. BMC Public Health, 7, 208e216.
Jeeninga, R. E., de Jong, M. D., & Berkhout, B. (2009). The New influenza A (H1N1)
Anderson, J. R. (2004). Cognitive psychology and its implications. NY: Worth Pub.
pandemic. Journal of the Formosan Medical Association, 108(7), 523e525.
Anderson, J., & Gerbing, D. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: a review
Khan, K., Arino, J., Hu, W., Raposo, P., Sears, J., Calderon, F., et al. (2009). Spread of
and recommended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103(3), 411e423.
a novel Influenza A (H1N1) Virus via global airline transportation. The New
Aro, A. R., Vartti, A. M., Schreck, M., Turtiainen, P., & Uutela, A. (2009). Willingness to take
England Journal of Medicine, 361(2), 212e214.
travel-related health risks: a study among the Finnish tourists in Asia during the
Kim, W., & Ok, C. (2009). The effects of relational benefits of favorable inequity,
Avian influenza outbreak. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16(1), 68e73.
affective commitment, and repurchase intention in full-service restaurants.
Bagozzi, R. P. (1992). The self-regulation of attitudes, intentions and behavior. Social
Journal of Hospitality & Tourism Research, 33(2), 227e244.
Psychology Quarterly, 55(2), 178e204.
Kline, R. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. NY: Guilford
Bagozzi, R., Baumgartner, H., & Pieters, R. (1998). Goal-directed emotions. Cognition
Press.
and Emotion, 12(1), 1e26.
Kolata, G. (2001). Flu: The story of the great influenza pandemic of 1918 and the search
Bagozzi, R., & Dholakia, U. (2006). Antecedents and purchase consequences of
for the virus that caused it. NY: Touchstone.
customer participation in small group brand communities. International Journal
Lam, T., & Hsu, C. (2004). Theory of planned behavior: potential travelers from
of Research in Marketing, 23(1), 45e61.
China. Journal of Hospitality and Tourism Research, 28(4), 463e482.
Bagozzi, R., & Warshaw, P. (1992). An examination of the etiology of the attitude-
Lam, T., & Hsu, C. (2006). Predicting behavioral intention of choosing a travel
behavior relation for goal-directed behaviors. Multivariate Behavioral Research,
destination. Tourism Management, 27(4), 589e599.
27(4), 601e634.
Leone, L., Perugini, M., & Ercolani, A. (1999). A comparison of three models of
Baker, E. W., Al-Gahtani, S. S., & Hubona, G. S. (2007). The effects of gender and age
attitudeebehavior relationships in the studying behavior domain. European
on new technology implementation in a developing country: testing the theory
Journal of Social Psychology, 29(2e3), 161e189.
of planned behavior (TPB). Information Technology & People, 20(4), 352e375.
Leone, L., Perugini, M., & Ercolani, A. (2004). Studying, practicing, and mastering:
Bearden, W. O., & Etzel, M. J. (1982). Reference group influence on products and
a test of the model of goal-directed behavior (MGB) in the software learning
brand purchase decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, 9(2), 183e194.
domain. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(9), 1945e1973.
Bentler, P., & Speckart, G. (1981). Attitudes “cause” behaviors: a structural equation
McKercher, B., & Chon, K. (2004). The over-reaction to SARS and the collapse of
analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(2), 226e238.
Asian tourism. Annals of Tourism Research, 31(3), 716e719.
Bentler, P., & Wu, E. (1995). EQS for windows: User’s guide. Encino, CA: Multivariate
Mason, P., Grabowski, P., & Du, W. (2005). Severe acute respiratory syndrome, tourism
Software, Inc.
and the media. The International Journal of Tourism Research, 7(1), 11e21.
Bollen, K. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. NY: Wiley.
Ministry for Health, Welfare and Family Affairs. (2009a). The growth trend of
Brug, J., Aro, A. R., Oenema, A., De Zwart, O., Richardus, J. H., & Bishop, G. D. (2004).
Influenza A (H1N1). Retrieved October 21, 2009 from. http://www.mw.go.kr/
SARS risk perception, knowledge, precautions, and information sources, The
front/al/sal0301vw.jsp?.
Netherlands. Emerging Infectious Diseases, 10(8), 1486e1489.
Ministry for Health, Welfare and Family Affairs. (2009b). The code of preventing
Byrne, B. (1994a). Structural equation modeling with EQS and EQS/Windows: Basic
Swine Flu. Retrieved October 21, 2009 from. http://online.mw.go.kr/influenza/
concepts, applications, and programming. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
01_01.jsp.
Byrne, B. (1994b). Testing for the factorial validity, replication, and invariance of
Neumann, G., Noda, T., & Kawaoka, Y. (2009). Emergence and pandemic potential of
a measuring instrument: a paradigmatic application based on the Maslach
swine-origin H1N1 influenza virus. Nature, 459(7249), 931e939.
burnout inventory. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 29(3), 289e311.
Nicholl, A. (2006). Personal (non-pharmaceutical) protective measures for reducing
Byrne, B. (2006). Structural equation modeling with EQS: Basic concepts, applications,
transmission of influenza: ECDC interim recommendations. Retrieved October 29,
and programming. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
2009 from. http://www.eurosurveillance.org/ViewArticle.aspx? ArticleId¼3061.
Carrus, G., Passafaro, P., & Bonnes, M. (2008). Emotions, habits and rational choices
Nunnally, J. (1978). Psychometric theory. NY: McGraw-Hill.
in ecological behaviours: the case of recycling and use of public transportation.
Oh, H., & Hsu, C. (2001). Volitional degrees of gambling behaviors. Annals of Tourism
Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28(1), 51e62.
Research, 28(3), 618e637.
Chang, M. K. (1998). Predicting unethical behavior: a comparison of the theory of
Oliver, R. L. (1997). Satisfaction: A behavioral perspective on the consumer. NY:
reasoned action and theory of planned behavior. Journal of Business Ethics,
McGraw-Hill.
17(16), 1825e1834.
Omi, S. (2006). SARS: How a global epidemic was stopped. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO
Chaturvedi, S. (2009). Pandemic influenza: imminent threat, preparedness and the
Press.
divided globe. Indian Pediatric, 46(2), 115e121.
Oshitani, H. (2006). Potential benefits and limitations of various strategies to
Cheng, S., Lam, T., & Hsu, C. (2006). Negative word-of-mouth communication
mitigate the impact of an influenza pandemic. Journal of Infection and Chemo-
intention: an application of the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Hospi-
therapy, 12(4), 167e171.
tality & Tourism Research, 30(1), 95e116.
Ouellette, J. A., & Wood, W. (1998). Habit and intention in everyday life: the
Conner, M., & Abraham, C. (2001). Conscientiousness and the theory of planned
multiple processes by which past behavior predicts future behavior. Psycho-
behavior: towards a more complete model of the antecedents of intentions and
logical Bulletin, 124(1), 54e74.
behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(11), 1547e1561.
Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. (2001). The role of desires and anticipated emotions in
Conner, M., & Armitage, C. (1998). Extending the theory of planned behavior:
goal-directed behaviors: broadening and deepening the theory of planned
a review of the literature and avenues for future research. Journal of Applied and
behavior. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40(1), 79e98.
Social Psychology, 28(15), 1429e1464.
van der Pligt, J., & De Vries, N. K. (1998). Expectancy-value models of health
Department of Health and Human Services. (2009). Assessment of the 2009 Influenza
behaviour: the role of salience and anticipated affect. Psychology & Health, 13(2),
A (H1N1) pandemic on selected countries in the Southern Hemisphere: Argentina,
289e305.
Australia, Chile, New Zealand and Uruguay. Retrieved December 15, 2009 from.
Prestwich, A., Perugini, M., & Hurling, R. (2008). Goal desires moderate inten-
http://www.flu.gov/professional/global/southhemisphere.html.
tionebehaviour relations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(1), 49e71.
Embrain. (2009). Online survey. Retrieved October 21, 2009 from the official website
Raude, J., & Setbon, M. (2009). Lay perceptions of the pandemic influenza threat.
of Embrain. http://www.embrain.com.
European Journal Epidemiology, 24(7), 339e342.
Ferguson, N., Cummings, D., Fraser, C., Cajka, J., Cooley, P., & Burke, D. (2006).
Reisinger, Y., & Mavondo, F. (2005). Travel anxiety and intentions to travel inter-
Strategies for mitigating an influenza pandemic. Nature, 442(7101), 448e452.
nationally: implications of travel risk perception. Journal of Travel Research,
Finkelstein, S., Prakash, S., Nigmatulina, K., Klaiman, T., & Larson, R. (2009).
43(3), 212e225.
Pandemic influenza: non-pharmaceutical interventions and behavioral changes
Rizzo, C., & degli Atti, M. L. C. (2008). Modeling influenza pandemic and interven-
that may save lives. Retrieved December 15, 2009 from. http://blossoms.mit.
tions. In R. Rappouli, & G. Del Guidice (Eds.), Influenza vaccines for the future (pp.
edu/video/larson2/larson2-state-plans.pdf.
281e296). Basel, Switzerland: Birkhauser Verlag.
Fornell, C., & Larcker, D. (1981). Evaluating structural equation models with unob-
Sonmez, S. F., & Graefe, A. R. (1998). Determining future travel behavior from past
servable variables and measurement error. Journal of Marketing Research, 18(1),
travel experience and perceptions of risk and safety. Journal of Travel Research,
39e50.
37(2), 171e177.
C.-K. Lee et al. / Tourism Management 33 (2012) 89e99 99

Sparks, B., & Pan, G. W. (2009). Chinese outbound tourists: understanding their attitudes, UNWTO. (2010)UNWTO World Tourism Barometer, Vol. 8. (1). Retrieved March 18,
constraints and use of information sources. Tourism Management, 30(4), 483e494. 2010 from. http://unwto.org/facts/eng/pdf/barometer/UNWTO_Barom10_1_en_
Statistical Package for the Social Sciences. (2001). SPSS base 11.0 user’s guide. Chi- excerpt.pdf.
cago, IL: SPSS Inc. Varia, M., Wilson, S., Sarwal, S., McGeer, A., Gournis, E., Galanis, E., et al. (2003).
St. Michael’s Hospital. (2009). The Bio. Diaspora project report 2009: An analysis of Can- Investigation of a nosocomial outbreak of severe acute respiratory syndrome
ada’s vulnerability to emerging infectious disease threats via the global airline trans- (SARS) in Toronto, Canada. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 169(4),
portation network. Retrieved October 21, 2009 from. http://www.biodiaspora.com/. 285e292.
Taylor, S. A. (2007). The addition of anticipated regret to attitudinally based, goal- World Health Organization. (2009a). Clinical features of severe cases of pandemic
directed models of information search behaviours under conditions of uncer- influenza. Retrieved October 21, 2009 from. http://www.who.int/csr/disease/
tainty and risk. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46(4), 739e768. swineflu/notes/h1n1_clinical_features_20091016/en/index.htm.
Taylor, S., Bagozzi, R., & Gaither, C. (2005). Decision making and effort in the self- World Health Organization. (2009b). Pandemic (H1N1) 2009-Update 70. Retrieved
regulation of hypertension: testing two competing theories. British Journal of October 25, 2009 from. http://www.who.int/csr/don/2009_10_16/en/index.
Health Psychology, 10(4), 505e530. html.
Taylor, S., & Todd, P. (1995). Assessing IT usage: the role of prior experience. MIS World Health Organization. (2009c). Global alert and response (GAR): Travel.
Quarterly, 19(4), 561e570. Retrieved October 23, 2009 from the official website of WHO. http://www.who.
Triandis, H. C. (1977). Interpersonal behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. int/csr/disease/swineflu/frequently_asked_questions/travel/en/index.html.
UNWTO. (2009a)UNWTO World Tourism Barometer, Vol. 7. (2). Retrieved March 18, Wu, E., Law, R., & Jiang, B. (2010). The impact of infectious diseases on hotel
2010 from. http://www.unwto.org/facts/eng/pdf/barometer/UNWTO_Barom09_ occupancy rate based on independent component analysis. International Journal
2_en.pdf. of Hospitality Management, 29(4), 751e753.
UNWTO. (2009b)UNWTO World Tourism Barometer, Vol. 7. (3). Retrieved March 18, Yoon, Y., & Uysal, M. (2005). An examination of the effects of motivation and
2010 from. http://www.unwto.org/facts/eng/pdf/barometer/UNWTO_Barom09_ satisfaction on destination loyalty: a structural model. Tourism Management,
3_en.pdf. 26(1), 45e56.
UNWTO. (2009c). Influenza A (H1N1): No restrictions on travel recommended. Zint, M. (2002). Comparing three attitude-behavior theories for predicting
Retrieved October 21, 2009 from. http://www.unwto.org/media/news/en/press science teachers’ intentions. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(9),
_det.php?id¼4071. 819e844.

You might also like