You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/281711355

Committed Relationships and Enhanced Threat Levels: Perceptions of Coach


Behaviour, the Coach-Athlete Relationship, Primary Appraisals, and Coping
among Athletes.

Article  in  International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching · February 2016


DOI: 10.1177/1747954115624825

CITATIONS READS

18 539

6 authors, including:

Andrew R Levy Rudi A Meir


Edge Hill University Southern Cross University
67 PUBLICATIONS   2,034 CITATIONS    120 PUBLICATIONS   1,337 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Jon Radcliffe John Perry


Leeds Trinity University Mary Immaculate College
36 PUBLICATIONS   140 CITATIONS    78 PUBLICATIONS   1,281 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Improving Physical Activity in Cardiac and Pulmonary Rehabilitation (IPAiR Trial) View project

Appearance and Performance Enhancing Drug (APED) Use in Recreational Sports: A Behavioural Science Perspective View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Rudi A Meir on 23 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Original Research
International Journal of Sports Science
& Coaching
2016, Vol. 11(1) 16–26
Committed relationships and enhanced ! The Author(s) 2016
Reprints and permissions:
threat levels: Perceptions of coach sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav
DOI: 10.1177/1747954115624825
behavior, the coach–athlete relationship, spo.sagepub.com

stress appraisals, and coping among


athletes

Adam R Nicholls1, Andrew R Levy2, Leigh Jones3, Rudi Meir4,


Jon N Radcliffe5 and John L Perry1

Abstract
The purpose of this study was to assess an a priori model that included perceptions of coach behavior, coach–athlete
relationship, stress appraisals, and coping. A total of 274 athletes from the United Kingdom, Austalia, and Hong Kong
completed relevant measures that assessed each construct. Our results revealed that perceptions of coach behavior
were associated with aspects of the coach–athlete relationship and stress appraisals. In particular, closeness was posi-
tively associated with challenge appraisals and negatively with threat appraisals. However, commitment was positively
associated with threat, indicating that there might be some negative implications of having a highly committed coach–
athlete relationship. Further, commitment was also positively associated with disengagement-oriented coping, which has
previously been linked to poor performance and lower goal-attainment. Applied practitioners could monitor athlete’s
perceptions of the coach–athlete relationship, particularly commitment levels, and provide training in appraising stress
and coping to those who also score highly on threat and disengagement-oriented coping, but low on task-oriented
coping.

Keywords
Commitment, disengagement-oriented coping, task-oriented coping

the emotional responses (i.e. happiness, anxiety, or


Introduction
anger) and coping.4 However, little is known about
Participating in competitive sport is associated with how the coach–athlete relationship may influence
athletes reporting a variety of stressors, such as appraisals of stress, and whether the coach–athlete
errors, performance, and concerns about the outcome relationship is related to coping. This is surprising
of a competition.1 A recent meta-synthesis of the stress because researchers found a relationship between
and sport literature2 included a taxonomic classifica- coach behavior and coping.5,6 In this study, we
tion of stressors encountered by athletes, which tested an a priori model that included coach behavior,
revealed that coach’s behavior and interactions along
with a coach’s personality were salient stressors for Reviewer: Britt Brewer (Springfield College, MA, USA)
athletes. Indeed, scholars revealed that a coach’s 1
Department of Sport, University of Hull, Health and Exercise Science,
behavior influences how an athlete perceives his or
Hull, UK
her relationship with that coach, and that this rela- 2
Edge Hill University, UK
tionship is associated with an athlete’s happiness.3 3
University of Wales, Newport, UK
Given that an athlete’s perception of his or her rela- 4
Southern Cross University, Australia
5
tionship is associated with happiness and that coaches Leeds Trinity University, UK
are a source of stress,2 it is plausible to assume that
Corresponding author:
perceptions of the coach–athlete relationship would Adam R Nicholls, Department of Sport, University of Hull, Health and
also be related to how an athlete evaluates stress and Exercise Science, Hull, UK.
coping, given that appraisal is thought to determine Email: A.Nicholls@hull.ac.uk
Nicholls et al. 17

the coach–athlete relationship, stress appraisals, and athlete relationship as the 3 þ 1 Cs, which comprises of
coping among a sample of athletes. closeness (e.g. the extent to which value, support, and
care for each other), commitment (e.g. the coach and
athlete’s intent to maintain the relationship), comple-
Coach behavior
mentarity (e.g. how the behaviors of the coach and ath-
How a coach behaves can influence agressive beha- lete correspond to each other), and co-orientation (e.g.
viors,7 thoughts,8 and anxiety9 among his or her ath- the coach and athlete establishing common views
letes. It is therefore important that coaches behave in a regarding the athlete’s progression).
way that athletes perceive as being positive or support- The importance of the coach–athlete relationship
ive. Høigaard10 identified positive coach behaviors should not be underestimated, given that successful
among a sample of elite Norwegian footballers, report- coach–athlete relationships can result in superior
ing that supportive behaviors included providing posi- coaching,16 coach and athlete well-being,17 and
tive feedback (e.g. behaviors that recognize and reward better self-concept.18 Understanding more about the
good performances), training and instruction (e.g., antecedents and outcomes of the coach-athlete rela-
coach behaviors that enable an athlete to improve), tionship is important for the development of coaching
and democratic behaviors (e.g., allowing team members practices. One psychological construct associated with
to make decisions). coach–athlete relationship is happiness.3 Happiness is
Other research distinguished between supportive and an emotion that reflects a person’s positive state of
unsupportive coaching behaviors. Using Côté et al.’s their overall psychological well-being.4 Indeed,
Coaching Behavior Scale for Sport (CBS),11 Nicolas5 Lazarus4 stated that emotions are generated by
deemed supportive coaching behaviors as including appraisals. As such, although Lafrenière and col-
emotional/relational and structural/instrumental com- leagues3 did not measure appraisal, their findings indi-
ponents. Conversely, unsupportive coaching occurred cate that appraisals are related to the coach–athlete
when coaches shouted, manipulated, threatened, or relationship, give that emotions occur as a conse-
upset athletes, which is likely to be perceived as the quence of appraisals.
coach exerting unwanted pressure.11 Coach behavior
also is linked to athlete perceptions of the coach-athlete
Appraisal
relationship.3 Indeed, Lafrenière3 found a positive rela-
tionship between autonomy supportive coach behaviors Athletes will make judgments about their environment
and the athlete’s relationship quality with the coach. in relation to valued personal goals. This evaluative
These scholars also found a negative relationship process is known as primary appraisal.4 Peacock and
between controlling coach behaviors and the athlete’s Wong19 identified three primary appraisals and three
relationship with the coach. Although Lafrenière3 made secondary appraisals. Primary appraisals included
an important contribution to the literature regarding threat (i.e. the anticipation of future harms), challenge
how coach behaviors may influence the athlete’s rela- (i.e. the anticipation of future gains), and centrality (i.e.
tionship quality with his or her coach, it could be the perceived importance of a situation or event).
argued the method of assessing coach behavior is lim- Secondary appraisal refers to an evaluation of percep-
ited. For example, only two forms of coach behavior tions of control and coping options available to the
were assessed (i.e. autonomy supportive behaviors and athlete.4 Peacock and Wong19 identified three different
controlling behaviors), which were measured by only types of secondary appraisal: controllable-by-self (i.e.
three and six items, respectively. The CBS11 provides the extent to which the athlete can control the situ-
a more detailed assessment of coaching behavior. ation), controllable-by-others (i.e. the extent to which
people close to the athlete can control the situation),
and uncontrollable-by-anyone (i.e. the extent to which
The coach–athlete relationship
no-one can control the situation).
Jowett and Cockerill12 suggested that the coach–athlete Of particular relevance to the current study is the
relationship refers to all situations in which a coach’s recent literature on challenge and threat states, which
and athlete’s thoughts, feelings, and behaviors are are similar to how Lazarus4 conceptualized these pri-
inter-related. The affiliation between the coach and mary appraisals. Indeed, a study by Moore and col-
the athlete is dynamic,12 meaning that both the coach leagues20 found that those who experienced challenge
and the athlete can influence the coach–athlete relation- states exhibited superior performance, felt less anxious,
ship. There are several conceptualizations of the coach– and engaged in less conscious processing, in addition to
athlete relationship,13–15 with Jowett’s model13 being having longer quiet eye durations. These results were
the most widely used and the guiding framework for echoed by Turner and colleagues21 who found that the
this current study. Jowett13 conceptualized the coach– cricketers exhibiting challenge states performed better
18 International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 11(1)

than those who reported threat states. In addition to and complementarity, but negative paths between
appraisals of challenge or threat states influencing per- unsupportive coaching behaviors and these three
formance and anxiety, they have also been theoretic- coach–athlete relationship constructs. This is because
ally4 and empirically associated with coping, along Lafrenière3 reported a positive relationship between
with secondary appraisals.22 autonomy coaching behaviors and athlete perceptions
of the coach–athlete relationship, but a negative path
between controlling coach behaviors and the coach–
Coping athlete relationship constructs. We also predicted posi-
According to Lazarus and Folkman,23 coping refers to tive paths between supportive coach behavior and chal-
all conscious cognitive and behavioral efforts to manage lenge, and unsupportive coaching behaviors and threat,
external or internal demands that a person appraises as but negative paths between supportive coaching behav-
taxing his or her resources. Although coping can be clas- iors and threat and unsupportive coaching behaviors
sified into many different dimensions, the taxonomy pro- and challenge. This is hypothesis is based on
posed by Gaudreau and Blondin24 is widely used in the Lafrenière et al.’s3 finding that controlling behaviors
sport literature. Gaudreau and Blondin24 classified were negatively associated with happiness, but autono-
coping within three higher-order dimensions: task- mous coaching behaviors were positively associated,
oriented, distraction-oriented, and disengagement- although these findings were insignificant. However,
oriented coping. The purpose of task-oriented strategies given that challenge appraisals are associated with
is to change or master a stressful situation, whereas dis- pleasant emotions and threat appraisals with unpleas-
traction-oriented coping direct the athlete’s attention ant emotions,25 the athletes who experienced happiness
onto an unrelated aspect of the sporting task. Finally, in the Lafrenière3 study are more likely to have experi-
disengagement-oriented coping strategies involve ath- enced a challenge rather than a threat appraisal. Due to
letes stopping achieving their goals. the lack of published research, we did not make predic-
tions regarding the paths between the coach–athlete
relationship and centrality.
Summary and hypotheses Similarly, we predicted positive paths between close-
Our hypotheses are presented in Figure 1, with a unbro- ness, commitment, and complementarity with challenge
ken line representing a positive relationship and a appraisals, but negative paths between these three con-
broken line inferring a negative relationship. We pre- structs and threat appraisals based on the notion that
dicted that there would be positive paths between sup- these constructs were positively related to the pleasant
portive coaching behavior and closeness, commitment, emotion happiness. This could imply that the situation is

Coach Behavior Coach-Athlete Primary Stress Secondary Stress Coping


Relationship Appraisal Appraisal

Challenge

Controllable-by-
Self

Closeness Task-Oriented
Coping
Supportive Coach
Controllable-by-
Behaviors
Others

Revised Path Model Commitment Centrality Distraction-


Oriented Coping
Unsupportive Uncontrollable-
Coach Behaviors .25by-anyone
Complementarity Disengagement-
Oriented Coping
Stressfulness
.32
Threat

Positive Path
-------- Negative Path

Figure 1. Hypothesized path model for coach behavior, coach–athlete relationship, stress appraisal, and coping.
Nicholls et al. 19

more likely to have been appraised as a challenge rather Participants responded to the stem ‘‘How frequently
than a threat.25 We also predicted that there would be do you experience the following coach behaviors?’’ A
positive paths from closeness, commitment, and comple- question classified as from the supportive coaching
mentarity to task-oriented coping, but negative paths behaviors was ‘‘The coach(es) most responsible for
from these three constructs to distraction- and disen- my physical training and conditioning provides me
gagement-oriented coping. This is because both high with structured training sessions’’ and ‘‘the coach(es)
scores in closeness, commitment, and complementarity most responsible for my mental preparation provides
are thought to be associated with athletic excellence,26 as advice on how to perform under pressure.’’ Examples
is task-oriented coping.27 In accordance with Nicholls,22 of unsupportive coaching behaviors were ‘‘my head
we predicted that there would be positive paths between coach yells at me when angry’’ and ‘‘my head coach
both controllable-by-self and controllable-by-others and shows favoritism to others.’’ Questions were answered
task-oriented coping, but that these paths would be on a seven-point Likert-type scale, which ranged from
negative to distraction- and disengagement-oriented 1 ¼ never to 7 ¼ always. Côté and colleagues11
coping. Further, the paths between both uncontrolla- reported Cronbach’s alpha coefficients of between
ble-by-anyone and stressfulness to distraction- and dis- 0.85 and 0.96 from a sample of 205 athletes. Little
engagement-oriented coping would be negative, where independent research has been conducted to establish
the paths from these secondary appraisal constructs to the validity of the CBS. Jurko and colleagues28
task-oriented coping would be negative. Finally, we reported that each scale of the CBS could explain sub-
hypothesized that there would be a positive path from stantial variance through exploratory factor analysis.
challenge appraisals, controllable-by-self, and controlla- They did not perform a full confirmatory factor ana-
ble-by-others to task-oriented coping and from threat lysis though.
appraisals, uncontrollable-by-anyone, and stressfulness
to distraction- and disengagement-oriented coping. We Coach–athlete relationship. The 11-item Coach Athlete
also predicted negative paths from threat, uncontrolla- Relationship Questionnaire (CART-Q)29 assessed the
ble-by-anyone, and stressfulness to task-oriented coping athletes’ perceptions of closeness (i.e. the extent to
and from challenge, controllable-by-self, and controlla- which the athlete feels close to his or her coach), com-
ble-by-others to both distraction- and disengagement- mitment (i.e. the degree to which athletes intend to
oriented coping, based on previous findings.22 maintain their working relationship with their coach),
and complementarity (i.e. co-operative actions) with
their coach. Participants responded to the stem ‘‘This
Method questionnaire aims to measure the quality and content
of the coach–athlete relationship. Please read carefully
Participants the statements below and circle the answer that indi-
A total of 274 athletes who resided in the United cates whether you agree or disagree.’’ An example of
Kingdom (n ¼ 176), Hong Kong (n ¼ 56), and question assessing closeness was ‘‘I trust my coach,’’
Australia (n ¼ 42) (male n ¼ 200, female n ¼ 73, unspeci- whereas ‘‘I am committed to coach’’ was from the com-
fied n ¼ 1), aged between 16 and 45 years of age mitment scale, and ‘‘When I am coached by my coach,
(Mage ¼ 21.59, SD ¼ 4.45) participated in the study. I adopt a friendly stance’’ represents a question from
Participants were from team (n ¼ 250) and individual the complimentary scale. Participants responded to
sports (n ¼ 24), including both contact sports (n ¼ 216) these questions on a 7-point Likert-type scale, which
and non-contact sports (n ¼ 58). Our sample consisted ranged from 1 ¼ strongly disagree to 7 ¼ strongly agree.
of 188 Caucasian, 31 African-Caribbean, 30 Asian, and Jowett and Ntoumanis29 found that all aspects of the
25 athletes from other ethnic origins. The athletes in coach–athlete relationship significantly predicted rela-
our sample competed at international (n ¼ 81), national tionship satisfaction, which provided some support for
(n ¼ 54), county (n ¼ 38), club (n ¼ 36), and beginner construct validity. The same authors also reported
(n ¼ 60) levels. Five athletes did not specify their skill Cronbach alpha coefficients of 0.86 for closeness, 0.83
level. for commitment, and 0.78 for complementarity. Similar
findings were presented by Yang and Jowett,30 who used
relationship satisfaction as construct validation. Their
Measures paper also examined the factorial properties of the 11-
Coach behavior. The 47-item CBS11 assessed the ath- item CART-Q, which provided a stronger model fit than
letes’ perceptions of seven of their coach’s behaviors. the 13- and 29-item versions.
Thirty-nine of the questions were classified as support-
ive coaching behaviors, compared to eight of the ques- Stress appraisals. The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM)19
tions that were classified as unsupportive behaviors.5 measured three primary appraisals (i.e. challenge,
20 International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 11(1)

threat, and centrality), three secondary appraisals (con- internal consistency because it has fewer assumptions
trollable-by-self, controllable-by-others, and uncontrolla- than alpha, problems associated with inflation of inter-
ble-by-anyone), and stressfulness (i.e. overall feeling of nal consistency are less likely, and point estimates and
stress). Participants were instructed to ‘‘please respond confidence intervals can be calculated.34 Bivariate cor-
according to how you view this situation right now.’’ relations were used to examine relationships between all
An example of a question relating to challenge appraisals variables, using the effect size (r) to make a judgment
was ‘‘Is this going to have a positive impact on me?’’ on their meaning.35 Zhu35 suggested using a criteria of
Conversely, an example of a question measuring threat 0–0.19 ¼ no correlation, 0.2–0.39 ¼ low correlation,
was ‘‘Will the outcome of this situation be negative?’’ The 0.4–0.59 ¼ moderate correlation, 0.6–0.79 ¼ moderately
responses on the SAM range from 1 ¼ not at all to high correlation, and #0.8 ¼ high correlation.
5 ¼ extremely. Peacock and Wong19 reported internal To test how well the hypothesized model (Figure 1)
consistencies ranging from .65 to .90. It should be noted fit our data, we performed a path analysis in Mplus 7.36
that the Cronbach alpha score of .65 was for threat, A range of indicators of model fit was used to supple-
which was reported in one of three studies. In the other ment !2. Hu and Bentler’s recommendations of CFI
two studies within that paper, the Cronbach alphas for close to .95, TLI close to .95, SRMR close to .08, and
threat were.75 and .73. Perry31 conducted confirmatory RMSEA close to .05 were used as guidelines for good
factor analysis and exploratory structural equation model fit, while acknowledging the recommendations
modeling on the SAM and demonstrated sound factorial by Marsh and colleagues,37 who encouraged research-
validity, including measurement invariance. ers to avoid interpreting these as golden rules. To assess
mediation, we used 5000 bootstrapped samples, which
Coping. We used the Coping Inventory for Competitive does not hold assumptions regarding sampling distribu-
Sport (CICS)32 to assess how the athletes were coping tion38 and provides standard errors and confidence
before their competition. The CICS has been success- intervals.
fully used to examine pre-competitive coping and
assesses 10 coping subscales categorized within task-,
distraction-, and disengagement-oriented coping.33
Results
Participants reported how their coping ‘‘corresponds Data were initially screened for missing data (<1%)
to what you are doing now,’’ with questions answered outliers and univariate normality, which presented no
on a 5-point scale, which ranged from 1 ¼ not at all to issues with skewness (<2) or kurtosis (<7) across all
5 ¼ very strongly. Although Gaudreau and Blondin32 variables. Table 1 presents the means, standard devi-
did not report the Cronbach alpha coefficients for the ations, minimum and maximum scores, and omega
higher-order dimensions, the individual coping strate- point estimates and confidence intervals. Omega esti-
gies ranged from .67 to .87. Perry31 presented support mates and confidence intervals were calculated using
for the factorial validity and measurement invariance. the MBESS package39 in R40 with 1000 bootstrap sam-
ples. Omega point estimates and intervals supported the
internal consistency of all subscales with the exception
Procedure
of the stressfulness subscale of the stress appraisal
Letters were distributed to coaches and participants, measure. Consequently, results pertaining to this scale
which explained the purpose of the study and the were treated with caution.
requirements for those interested in participating, Pearson bivariate correlations assessed the relation-
after ethical approval was obtained from a University ships among coach behavior, coach–athlete relationship,
Ethics Committee. Participants were asked to complete stress appraisal, and coping strategies. Pearson correl-
an assent form if they wished to participate in the study. ations were used in favor of the latent factor correlations
Each participant received a questionnaire pack and the from structural equation modeling because the amount
questionnaires were completed in the clubhouse of of latent variables examined at this stage would have
sports clubs in the presence of a trained research assist- required a sample size far larger than was available.
ant, and within three hours of a competition starting. Bivariate correlations are presented in Table 2. All
As such, each participant completed the questionnaires aspects of coach behavior correlated positively with the
in the following order: CBS,11 CART-Q,29 challenge 3Cs of the coach–athlete relationship with the exception
and threat items of the SAM,19 and the CICS.32 of negative personal rapport, which correlated negatively
with all aspects of the coach–athlete relationship. The
positive correlations were largely moderate in size
Data analysis
(rs ¼ .29 to .69, p < .01), while negative correlations
Preliminary data analysis screened for outliers, normal- were typically low (rs ¼ $.19 to $.29, p < .01). All posi-
ity, and omega. Omega was the preferred assessment of tive coach behaviors exhibited a low positive correlation
Nicholls et al. 21

Table 1. Descriptive statistics, univariate normality estimates, internal consistency.

Variable M SD Min Max Skew Kurt Omega (95% CI)

Coach behavior
Physical training 5.08 1.34 1.00 7.00 $.79 .15 .90 (.88, .92)
Technical skills 5.39 1.19 1.50 7.00 $.67 $.04 .94 (.92, .95)
Mental preparation 4.54 1.51 1.00 7.00 $.40 $.51 .95 (.93, .96)
Goal setting 4.22 1.59 1.00 7.00 $.26 $.65 .96 (.95, .97)
Competition strategies 5.31 1.19 1.43 7.00 $.77 .27 .92 (.90, .94)
Personal rapport 5.01 1.36 1.33 7.00 $.54 $.32 .89 (.87, .92)
Negative personal rapport 2.42 1.28 1.00 7.00 1.58 2.42 .89 (.85, .92)
Coach–athlete relationship
Closeness 5.74 1.23 1.00 7.00 $1.29 1.41 .92 (.90, .94)
Commitment 5.14 1.29 1.00 7.00 $.96 .66 .84 (.81, .88)
Complementarity 5.37 1.23 1.00 7.00 $.82 .57 .76 (.69, .81)
Stress appraisal
Threat 2.26 .81 1.00 4.25 .24 $1.01 .60 (.52, .65)
Challenge 3.48 .86 1.50 5.00 $.18 $.74 .78 (.72, .81)
Centrality 2.95 .83 1.00 5.00 $.18 .04 .68 (.57, .73)
Control – Self 3.86 .79 1.50 5.00 $.42 $.39 .78 (.73, .83)
Control – Others 3.41 .94 1.00 5.00 $.06 $.73 .79 (.72, .83)
Uncontrollable 2.18 1.04 1.00 4.75 .59 $.75 .84 (.80, .87)
Stressfulness 2.59 .63 1.00 4.25 .04 $.11 .23 (not pos)
Coping strategies
Task-oriented coping 3.36 .55 1.87 5.00 $.24 $.23 .84 (.79, .87)
Mental imagery 3.57 .77 1.50 5.00 $.30 $.46 .65 (.57, .71)
Effort expenditure 3.97 .86 1.00 5.00 $1.08 1.38 .70 (.61, .77)
Thought control 3.45 .80 1.00 5.00 $.37 .17 .62 (.54, .70)
Seeking support 2.89 .84 1.00 5.00 .22 $.46 .71 (.65, .76)
Relaxation 3.13 .87 1.00 5.00 .04 $.48 .77 (.71, .82)
Logical analysis 3.33 1.00 1.00 5.00 $.86 .21 .80 (.74, .84)
Distraction-oriented coping 2.33 .73 1.00 4.50 .51 .07 .82 (.77, .86)
Distancing 2.59 .90 1.00 4.75 .49 $.23 .74 (.68, .80)
Mental distraction 2.35 .93 1.00 5.00 .58 $.03 .80 (.75, .85)
Disengagement-oriented coping 2.22 .70 1.00 4.00 .50 $.35 .73 (.61, .79)
Venting unpleasant emotions 2.70 .89 1.00 5.00 .17 $.60 .76 (.70, .80)
Resignation/disengagement 1.74 .87 1.00 4.00 1.10 .06 .82 (.78, .86)
Note: Coach behavior and stress appraisal are measured on 7-point scales; stress appraisal and coping strategies are measured on 5-point scales.
Omega confidence intervals could not be calculated for the stressfulness subscale, as the matrix was not-positive-definite.

with task-oriented coping (rs ¼ .17 to .25, p < .01), and p < .01), and stressfulness (r ¼ .20, p < .01), and nega-
negative personal rapport was positively related to dis- tively with control-self (r ¼ $.29, p < .01) and control-
traction-oriented coping (r ¼ .23, p < .01) and disengage- others (r ¼ $.23, p < .01).
ment-oriented coping (r ¼ .28, p < .01). The most The coach–athlete relationship was significantly
significant relationships between coach behavior and associated with stress appraisal. Specifically, closeness
stress appraisal were the positive correlations of all posi- and complementarity were correlated moderately posi-
tive coach behaviors with the exception of goal setting tively with challenge (r ¼ .42 and .55, p < .01), control-
and a challenge appraisal (rs ¼ .16 to .32, p < .01). There self (r ¼ .45 and .53, p < .01), and control-others (r ¼ .44
were also positive correlations between all positive coach and .54, p < .01). Closeness and complementarity were
behaviors and control-others appraisal (rs ¼ .18 to .40, negatively associated with threat (r ¼ $.24 and $.35,
p < .01). Negative personal rapport correlated positively p < .01) and uncontrollable (r ¼ $.26 and $.44,
with threat (r ¼ .33, p < .01), uncontrollable (r ¼ .24, p < .01). Complementarity presented the strongest
22 International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 11(1)

Table 2. Bivariate correlations for coach behavior, coach–athlete relationship, stress appraisal, and coping.
Coach–athlete relationship Coping Stress appraisal

Close Comm Compl Task Distract Diseng Threat Chall Central ContSelf ContOth Uncont Stress

Coach behavior
Physical training .50** .52** .39** .17** .05 $.06 .03 .27** .27** .19** .35** .02 .13*
Technical skills .64** .64** .55** .20** $.12 $.20** $.03 .29** .10 .28** .40** $.09 .07
Mental Prep .49** .55** .40** .19** $.04 $.07 .01 .16** .09 .16** .29** .00 .03
Goal setting .45** .56** .29** .17** .11 .03 .12* .06 .23** .06 .18** .20** .17**
Comp strategies .59** .62** .49** .20** $.12 $.17** $.05 .30** .11 .27** .38** $.08 .04
Personal rapport .67** .69** .59** .25** $.12 $.17** $.15* .32** .08 .36** .38** $.21** .06
Negative rapport $.29** $.19** $.25** $.00 .23** .28** .33** $.19 .09 $.29** $.23** .24** .20**

Coach–athlete relationship

Stress appraisal Coping Close Comm Compl

Threat $.24** $.01 $.35** $.12* .41** .41** Task .28** .19** .38**
Challenge .42** .22** .55** .47** $.04 $.22** Distraction $.08 $.05 $.04
Centrality .10 .18** .04 .27** .25** .10 Disengagement $.20** $.12 $.20**
Control – self .45** .26** .53** .44** $.12 $.30**
Control – others .44** .28** .54** .38** $.03 $.18**
Uncontrollable $.26** .05 $.44** $.23** .29** .38**
Stressfulness $.01 .10 $.07 .18** .38** .28**

*Statistically significant at p < .05.


**p < .01.

relationship of the coach–athlete relationship variables coping. There were, however, some significant direct
with coping. Specifically, it was related to task-oriented paths. Specifically, unsupportive coach behaviors posi-
coping (r ¼ .38, p < .01). Relationships between stress tively predicted centrality (" ¼ .65, 95% CI ¼ .50, .80,
appraisal and coping were low to moderate. The stron- p < .001), and stressfulness (" ¼ .36, 95% CI ¼.11, .60,
gest correlations were between task-oriented coping p < .001), but negatively predicted controllable-by-self
with challenge (r ¼ .47, p < .01), control-self (r ¼ .44, (" ¼ $.35, 95% CI ¼ $.50, $.20, p < .001). Supportive
p < .01), and control-others (r ¼ .38, p < .01), distrac- behaviors presented a significant positive path with
tion-oriented coping with threat (r ¼ .41, p < .01) and uncontrollable-by-anyone (" ¼ .22, 95% CI ¼ .06, .38,
stressfulness (r ¼ .38, p < .01), and disengagement- p < .001). Of the coach–athlete relationship variables,
oriented coping with threat (r ¼ .41, p < .01) and stress- commitment presented a significant positive path with
fulness (r ¼ .38, p < .01). disengagement-oriented coping (" ¼ .24, 95% CI ¼ .07,
To guard against departure from multivariate nor- .40, p < .001) and complementarity negatively predicted
mality, the robust maximum likelihood estimator both distraction- (" ¼ $.21, 95% CI ¼ $.37, $.04,
(MLR) was used in all model testing. The path model p < .001) and disengagement-oriented coping (" ¼
found in Figure 1 represented a reasonable fit to the $.36, 95% CI ¼ $.54, $.17, p < .001).
data but with a significant !2, low TLI, and high error To examine mediation, 5000 bootstrap replications
(RMSEA): !2(17) ¼ 40.86, p ¼ .001, CFI ¼ .973, TLI ¼ were conducted and indirect and direct effects analyzed.
.834, SRMR ¼ .039, RMSEA ¼ .080 (90% CI ¼ .049, This method presents 95% confidence intervals for each
.112). Examination of the path estimates identified sev- estimate. The absence of a zero in the confidence inter-
eral non-significant paths (p > .05). Consequently, these vals indicates a significant effect. The results of the medi-
paths were removed from the model. The resultant ation analysis between the coach–athlete relationship
model presented improved model fit: !2(50) ¼ 60.75, variables and coping are presented in Table 3. Stress
p ¼ .142, CFI ¼ .988, TLI ¼ .975, SRMR ¼ .052, appraisal did not mediate the relationship between any
RMSEA ¼ .031 (90% CI ¼ .000, .056). This model is coach–athlete relationship variable and coping strate-
presented in Figure 2. This figure does not include gies. Further analysis of indirect effects was conducted
direct paths between coach behavior and secondary to determine if the coach–athlete relationship mediated
appraisals and coping. Nor does it include paths the relationship between coach behavior and coping.
between coach–athlete relationship variables and The relationship between positive coach behaviors and
Nicholls et al. 23

Coach Behavior Coach-Athlete Primary Stress Secondary Stress Coping


Relationship Appraisal Appraisal

.23 Challenge
.30
Controllable-by- -.10
Self
.38 .41
Closeness Task-Oriented
.41 Coping
Supportive Coach .40
-.36 Controllable-by-
Behaviors
.26 Others .35
Commitment Centrality Distraction-
.26 Oriented Coping
.38
Unsupportive -.30 Uncontrollable-
-.16
Coach Behaviors .25by-anyone
-.53 Complementarity .51 Disengagement-
.18 Oriented Coping
.22
Stressfulness
.32
Threat
.48 .26
Positive Path
-------- Negative Path

Figure 2. Revised path model showing only significant (p < .05) paths.
Note: Direct paths between coach behavior and secondary appraisal, coach behavior and coping, and coach–athlete relationship and
coping have been omitted for clarity.

Table 3. Direct, indirect, and total effects of coach–athlete relationship variables on coping in the original path model.
Direct Via challenge Via threat Total effect

Closeness ! Task-oriented coping .42 [.11, .72] .05 [$.04, .13] $.02 [$.11, .06] .44 [.10, .79]
Commitment ! Task-oriented coping $.32 [$.53, $.10] $.01 [$.07, .06] .01 [$.04, .07] $.31 [$.55, $.07]
Complementarity ! Task-oriented coping $.06 [$.37, .26] .04 [$.07, .14] $.01 [$.07, .04] $.03 [$.35, .28]
Closeness ! Distraction-oriented coping .20 [$.23, .63] .02 [$.05, .08] $.05 [$.16, .07] .17 [$23, .56]
Commitment ! Distraction-oriented coping $.22 [$.52, $.08] $.00 [$.03, .03] .03 [$.05, .11] $.19 [$.48, .10]
Complementarity ! Distraction-oriented coping $.28 [$.56, $.01] .01 [$.06, .08] $.03 [$.10, .04] $.29 [$.55, $.04]
Closeness ! Disengagement-oriented coping .14 [$.11, .38] $.03 [$.08, .03] $.11 [$.23, .02] .01 [$.22, .23]
Commitment ! Disengagement-oriented coping .24 [.01, .47] .00 [$.03, .04] .07 [$.03, .17] .31 [.10, .52]
Complementarity ! Disengagement-oriented coping $.50 [$.69, $.32] $.02 [$.08, .04] $.06 [$.18, .05] $.59 [$.75, $.42]

task-oriented coping was positively mediated by close-


ness (g ¼ .12 [95% CI ¼ .00, .35]). The effect from nega-
Discussion
tive coach behavior on disengagement-oriented coping We assessed the relationships between perceived coach
was mediated by complementarity (g ¼ .26 [95% behavior, athlete’s perceptions of closeness, commitment,
CI ¼ .15, .38]). We then examined the indirect effects and complementarity, along with stress appraisals and
between coach behavior and coping, mediated by stress coping. Overall, some of the hypothesized paths were sup-
appraisal. The indirect effect on disengagement-oriented ported, indicating that some of these constructs are
coping mediated by threat appraisal from positive related, but there were also unexpected significant find-
coaching behavior (g ¼ .08 [95% CI ¼ .01, .15]) and ings. These included the relationship between commit-
negative coaching behavior (g ¼ .19 [95% CI ¼ .09, ment and threat appraisals, along with commitment and
.30]) was significant. Finally, the mediating effects of coping (e.g. task- and disengagement-oriented coping).
the coach–athlete relationship on the relationship There were positive paths from supportive
between coach behavior and stress appraisal were coaching behaviors to closeness, commitment, and com-
assessed. Results indicated no significant indirect effects. plementarity. This compliments the work of Lafrenière
24 International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 11(1)

and colleagues.3 Only one of the negative paths that we negative consequences of being in a highly committed
predicted from unsupportive coaching behaviors to the coach–athlete relationship, which has previously been
three coach–athlete relationship scales was significant, unconsidered. When athletes are in a highly committed
which was the path to complementarity. This finding is relationship with their coach, they might be more con-
only in partial agreement with Lafrenière3 who found a cerned about letting their coach down and therefore
negative relationship between controlling forms of coach experience higher levels of threat. Although not focusing
behaviors and athlete perceptions of the coach–athlete on the coach–athlete relationship, Nicholls41 reported
relationship. The insignificant paths between unsupport- that young golfers experienced threat in regards to let-
ive perceptions of coach behavior with both closeness and ting their parents down by not performing well.
commitment would imply that athletes still feel a bond Furthermore, there was also a positive path from com-
with their coach and plan to continue working with the mitment to disengagement-oriented coping and a nega-
coach despite feeling the coach is unsupportive. In certain tive path to task-oriented coping which were unexpected.
circumstances, especially team sports, athletes have little Task-oriented coping has been positively associated with
or no say on who their coach is and could only end the goal attainment,42 superior performance,27,43 and higher
coach–athlete relationship by changing teams. As such, coping effectiveness,44 whereas disengagement-oriented
the athletes felt committed to their coach, because they coping is negatively associated with such constructs.
had little choice regarding working with a new coach. It These findings also illustrate the possible negative associ-
should be noted that the vast majority of the athletes in ations of a highly committed coach–athlete relationship.
the present sample were from team sports, so it could be It should also be noted, however, that commitment was
interesting to compare the effects of unsupportive coach positively associated with controllable-by-self, indicating
behaviors among team versus individual sport athletes. that a committed coach–athlete relationship instills a
The path from supportive coaching behaviors to chal- belief that the athlete can manage stressful situations
lenge appraisals and the path from unsupportive coach- on their own. Additional research is therefore warranted
ing behaviours were both significant and positive. to explore both the positive and negative consequences
Incidentally, the path weighting of unsupportive coach- of having a highly committed coach–athlete relationship.
ing behaviors to threat was higher than the weighting for Only some of our hypothesized paths between
supportive coaching behaviors to challenge. This finding appraisal and coping were supported. The path
illustrates the impact that unsupportive coaching behav- between challenge and task-oriented coping was posi-
ior can have on athlete’s perception of a situation. tive and the path between challenge and disengage-
Coaches should consider the impact of their behavior ment-oriented coping was negative. Further, the path
and the detrimental consequences of such unsupportive between threat and disengagement-oriented coping
behavior. Threat is associated with undesirable conse- was positive, which are all in agreement with
quences such as increased anxiety19 and decreased per- Nicholls,22 who also found that only some of the
formance.20 The finding that there was a significant path hypothesized paths were significant. The notion that
between unsupportive coaching behaviors and threat challenge is associated with adaptive forms of coping,
could imply that coaches can generate perceptions of such as task-oriented coping, but is less associated
threat among their athletes, although given that this is with athletes using more distraction- or disengage-
a cross-sectional study, research is required to verify this. ment-oriented coping, was partially supported.
We also found a negative path between supportive coach- Similarly, although threat appraisals are associated
ing behaviors and perceptions of threat, implying that with athletes using more disengagement-oriented
there is a negative association between these constructs. coping, it is not associated with athletes using less
Although it appears that coach behavior might not gen- task-oriented coping strategies.
erate challenge appraisals among athletes, it could be that
it reduces that occurrence of threat appraisals.
Limitations
Other than closeness, the hypothesized paths
between the coach–athlete relationship and appraisals This study explored perceptions of coach behavior and
were not supported. These findings, however, illustrate the association of such perceptions with the coach–ath-
the importance of the athlete’s perception of closeness lete relationship and stress appraisals. However, it is
to coach, because it was positively associated with chal- possible that the athletes’ perceptions of such coach
lenge, but negatively with threat. However, commit- behaviors may be biased, so future research could
ment and complementarity were not associated with assess actual coach behaviors in relation to perceptions
challenge, and commitment was positively associated of the coach–athlete relationship and stress appraisals.
with threat. That is, when the athlete was committed Furthermore, we employed a cross-sectional design,
to working with his or her coach, threat levels were and the constructs we assessed are all recursive and
higher. This findings illustrates that there might be dynamic processes.11,19,29,32 As such, we were unable
Nicholls et al. 25

to so assess how these relationships unfolded over time, associated with the Department of Sport, University of Hull,
which would make for an interesting and useful piece of Health and Exercise Science, Hull, UK.
research. While we have acknowledged the known val-
idity of the measures used, this is largely related to the Declaration of Conflicting Interests
factorial validity. There is little testing of construct and The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with
criterion validity on the self-report measures used in respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this
this study. In particular, the coach behavior scale article.
would benefit from such scrutiny.
Funding
Recommendations The author(s) received no financial support for the research,
The findings from this study illustrate that perceptions authorship, and/or publication of this article.
of coach behavior are associated with how an athlete
perceives his or her relationship with the coach and References
the appraisal of situations. It is therefore paramount 1. Nicholls AR, Jones CR, Polman RCJ, et al. Stressors,
that coaches consider their behavior and maximize coping, and emotion among professional rugby union
their level of supportive behavior, whilst minimizing players during training and matches. Scand J Med Sci
unsupportive coaching behaviors. This may appear an Sports 2009; 19: 113–120.
obvious recommendation, but our data suggests that 2. Arnold R and Fletcher D. A research synthesis and taxo-
coaches were being perceived to behave in an unsup- nomic classification of the organizational stressors
portive manner among some athletes, which suggests encountered by sport performers. J Sport Exercise
that this type of behavior is evident among coaches. Psychol 2012; 34: 397–429.
Although it may seem appealing to want to maximize 3. Lafrenière M-AK, Jowett S, Vallerand RJ, et al. Passion
for coaching and the quality of the coach–athlete rela-
all aspects of the coach–athlete relationship, this is
tionship: The mediating role of coaching behaviours.
one of the first studies to suggest that there might be
Psychol Sport Exercise 2011; 12: 144–152.
some undesirable consequences of such an approach, 4. Lazarus RS. Stress and emotion: A new synthesis.
particularly in relation to commitment. Although it is New York: Springer, 1999.
important that both the coach and the athlete are com- 5. Nicolas M, Gadureau P and Franche V. Perception of
mitted to the relationship, coaches could speak to their coaching behaviors, coping, and achievement in a sport
athletes and provide re-assurances about factors that competition. J Sport Exercise Psychol 2011; 33: 460–468.
might cause threat (e.g. the outcome of competitions) 6. Ntoumanis N, Biddle SJH and Haddock G. The mediat-
in highly committed coach–athlete relationships. ing role of coping strategies on the relationship between
achievement motivation and affect in sport. Anxiety
Stress Coping 1999; 12: 299–327.
Conclusion 7. Chow GM, Murray KE and Feltz DL. Individual, team,
and coach predictors of players’ likelihood to aggress in
We found support for a number of paths assessed in
youth soccer. J Sport Exercise Psychol 2009; 31: 425–443.
this study, indicating that coach behaviors are asso-
8. Zourbanons N, Harzigeorgiadis A, Tsiakaras SC, et al.
ciated with the coach–athlete relationship and apprai- A multimethod examination of the relationship between
sals. Further, aspects of an athlete’s perception of the coaching behaviour and athletes inherent self-talk.
coach–athlete relationship are related to appraisals and J Sport Exercise Psychol 2010; 32: 764–785.
coping. Although supportive coaching behaviors were 9. Ledochowski L, Unterrainer C, Ruedl G, et al. Quality of
not positively associated with challenge appraisals, they life, coach behaviour and competitive anxiety in winter
were negatively associated with threat, and unsupport- youth olympic games participants. Br J Sport Med 2012;
ive coaching behaviors were positively associated with 46: 1044–1047.
threat appraisals. As such, coaches might be able to 10. Høigaard R, Jones GW and Peters DM. Preferred coach
reduce threat levels among their athletes by monitoring leadership behaviour in elite soccer in relation to success
their behavior and eliminating unsupportive coaching and failure. Int J Sport Sci Coach 2008; 2: 241–250.
11. Côté J, Yardley J, Hay J, et al. An exploratory examin-
behaviors. Finally, this is one of the first studies to sug-
ation of the coaching behavior scale for sport. Avante
gest that a strong coach–athlete relationship might have
1999; 5: 82–92.
some undesirable consequences, given that commitment 12. Jowett S and Cockerill IM. Incompatibility in the coach–
was positively associated with threat. athlete relationship. In: Cockerill IM (ed.) Solutions in
sport psychology. London: Thompson Learning, 2002,
Author’s note pp.16–31.
The author John L. Perry, at the time of this research was 13. Jowett S, Paull G and Pensgaard AM. Coach–athlete
affiliated from Leeds Trinity University, UK and now he is relationship. In: Taylor J and Wilson GS (eds) Applying
26 International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching 11(1)

sport psychology: Four perspectives. Champaign, IL: and initial validation. Scand J Med Sci Sport 2004; 14:
Human Kinetics, 2005, pp.153–170. 245–257.
14. LaVoi NM. Expanding the interpersonal dimension: 30. Yang SX and Jowett S. Examination of the Psychometric
Closeness in the coach–athlete relationship. Int J Sport Properties of the Chinese Coach Athlete Relationship
Sci Coach 2007; 4: 497–512. Questionnaire (CART-Q). Int J Coach Sci 2010; 4: 73–89.
15. Poczwardowski A, Henschen KP and Barott JE. The ath- 31. Perry JL, Nicholls AR, Clough PJ, et al. Assessing model
lete and the coach: Their relationship and its meaning. fit: Caveats and recommendations for confirmatory
Results of an interpretive study. Int J Sport Psychol 2002; factor analysis and exploratory structural equation mod-
33: 114–140. eling. Measure Phys Educ Exercise Sci 2015; 19: 12–21.
16. Lyle J. Sports coaching concepts: A framework for coa- 32. Gaudreau P and Blondin J-P. Development of a ques-
ches. Oxon: Routledge, 2002. tionnaire for the assessment of coping strategies
17. Chelladurai P. Coach leadership: A review. Int J Sport employed by athletes in competitive sport settings.
Psychol 1990; 21: 328–354. Psychol Sport Exercise 2002; 3: 1–34.
18. Jowett S and Cramer D. The prediction of young ath- 33. Nicholls AR, Perry JL and Calmeiro L. Pre-competitive
letes’ physical self from perceptions of relationships with achievement goals, appraisal, emotions, and coping in
parents and coaches. Psychol Sport Exercise 2010; 11: sport. J Sport Exercise Psychol 2014; 36: 433–445.
140–147. 34. Dunn TJ, Baguley T and Brunsden V. From alpha to
19. Peacock EJ and Wong PTP. The stress appraisal measure omega: A practical solution to the pervasive problem of
(SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive internal consistency estimation. Br J Psychol 2013; 105:
appraisal. Stress Med 1990; 6: 227–236. 399–412.
20. Moore LJ, Wilson MR, Vine SJ, et al. Champ or chump?: 35. Zhu W. Sadly, the earth is still round (p < 0.05). J Sport
Challenge and threat states during pressurized competi- Health Sci 2012; 1: 9–11.
tion. J Sport Exercise Psychol 2013; 35: 551–562. 36. Muthén LK and Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide, 7th ed.
21. Turner MJ, Jones MV, Sheffield D, et al. Who thrives Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén, 2012.
under pressure? Predicting the performance of elite acad- 37. Marsh HW, Hau K-T and Wen Z. In search of golden
emy cricketers using the cardiovascular indicators of rules: comment on hypothesis testing approaches to
challenge and threat states. J Sport Exercise Psychol setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers in over-
2013; 35: 387–397. generalising Hu & Bentler’s (1999) findings. Struct Equat
22. Nicholls AR, Polman RCJ and Levy AR. A path analysis Model 2004; 11: 320–341.
of stress appraisals, emotions, coping, and performance 38. Hayes AF. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical medi-
satisfaction among athletes. Psychol Sport Exercise 2012; ation analysis in the new millennium. Commun
13: 263–270. Monograph 2009; 76: 408–420.
23. Lazarus RS and Folkman S. Stress, appraisal and coping. 39. Kelley K and Lai K. MBESS: MBESS. R Package
New York: Springer, 1984. Version 3.3.2, 2012, http://CRAN.R-project.org/
24. Gaudreau P and Blondin J-P. Different athletes cope dif- package¼MBESS.
ferently during sport competition: A cluster analysis of 40. R Development Core Team. R: A Language and
coping. Personality Individual Difference 2004; 36: Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna,
1865–1877. Austria, 2012, http://www.R-project.org/.
25. Nicholls AR, Levy AR, Jones L, et al. An exploration of 41. Nicholls AR, Holt NL and Polman RCJ. A phenomeno-
the two-factor schematization of relational meaning and logical analysis of coping effectiveness in golf. Sport
emotions among professional rugby union players. Int J Psychol 2005; 19: 111–130.
Sport Exercise Psychol 2011; 9: 1–14. 42. Schellenberg BJI, Gaudreau P and Crocker PRE. Passion
26. Jowett S. On enhancing and repairing the coach–athlete and coping: Relationships with changes in burnout and
relationship. In: Jowett S and Jones M (eds) The psych- goal attainment in collegiate volleyball players. J Sport
ology of coaching. Leicester: British Psychological Exercise Psychol 2013; 35: 270–280.
Society, 2005, pp.14–26. 43. Doron J and Gaudreau P. A point-by-point analysis of
27. Gaudreau P, Nicholls AR and Levy AR. The ups and performance in a fencing match: Psychological processes
downs of sports performance: An episodic process ana- associated with winning and losing streaks. J Sport
lysis of within-person associations. J Sport Exercise Exercise Psychol 2014; 36: 3–13.
Psychol 2010; 32: 298–311. 44. Nicholls AR, Polman RCJ, Levy AR, et al. The mediat-
28. Jurko D, Tomljanović M and Čular D. Initial validation ing role of coping: A cross-sectional analysis of the rela-
of coach behavior scales in volleyball. Split, Croatia: Sport tionship between coping self-efficacy and coping
SPA, 2010, pp.47–50. effectiveness among athletes. Int J Stress Manage 2010;
29. Jowett S and Ntoumanis N. The Coach–Athlete 17: 181–192.
Relationship Questionnaire (CART-Q): Development

View publication stats

You might also like