You are on page 1of 10

Pain, 49 (I 992) 22 I-230 221

0 1992 Elsevier Science Publishers B.V. All rights reserved 0304-3959/92/$05.00

PAIN 02000

Efficacy of multidisciplinary pain treatment centers:


a meta-analytic review *

Herta Flor a,b,Thomas Fydrich ’ and Dennis C. Turk ’


“ Department of Clinical and Physiological Psychology, University of Tiibingen, Tiibingen (Germany), ” Fachhereich Psychologie,
Unirxersityof Marhurg, Marburg (Germany), ’ Department of Psychology, Ukersity of Heidelberg, Heidelberg (Germany),
and ’ Pain El,aluation and Treatment Institute, Departments of Psychiatry and Anesthesiology, Unir,ersity of Pittsburgh School of Medicine,
Pittsburgh, PA, (USA)

(Received 2 April 1991, revision received 9 July 1991, accepted 12 September 1991)

Summary Sixty-five studies that evaluated the efficacy of multidisciplinary treatments for chronic back pain
were included in a meta-analysis. Within- and between-group effect sizes revealed that multidisciplinary treatments
for chronic pain are superior to no treatment, waiting list, as well as single-discipline treatments such as medical
treatment or physical therapy. Moreover, the effects appeared to be stable over time. The beneficial effects of
multidisciplinary treatment were not limited to improvements in pain, mood and interference but also extended to
behavioral variables such as return to work or use of the health care system. These results tend to support the
efficacy of multidisciplinary pain treatment; however, these results must be interpreted cautiously as the quality of
the study designs and study descriptions is marginal. Suggestions for improvement in research designs as well as
appropriate reports of research completed are provided.

Key words: Meta-analysis; Chronic back pain; Multidisciplinary treatment

Introduction are always raised and each review concludes with the
old bromide - more and better research is needed
There has been a rapid proliferation in the number before definitive conclusions can be drawn.
of pain clinics since their inception over a quarter of a In addition to methodological concerns raised, con-
century ago. Many questions have been raised as to the clusions about the efficacy of pain clinic treatment also
relative efficacy and cost effectiveness of these clinics have to be viewed with some caution as they have been
(Stieg and Turk 1988; Cicala and Wright 1989). A based on subjective impressions and personal opinions
growing number of papers have appeared in the litera- regarding the efficacy of therapeutic approaches in
ture reviewing the relative effectiveness of these clinics general and in comparison to alternative approaches.
in treating patients with persistent pain. A number of Simply put, the usual approach consists of aggregating
qualitative reviews (Block 1982; Aronoff et al. 1983; the results of studies in a narrative manner sometimes
Kerns et al. 1983; Turner and Roman0 1984; Linton referred to as a box-score or vote-count approach
1986; Malone and Strube 1988) have generally sup- (Glass 1976).
ported the efficacy of pain clinic treatment (however, There are a number of problems inherent in the
see Trifiletti 19841, although methodological concerns box-score approach. The narrative reviewer attempts to
‘make sense’ out of an array of studies and reports his
or her conclusions usually without articulating the rules
* This research was supported by Grants No. FI 156/l and F1 156/2 by which evidence was evaluated. The standards for
by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft to the first author. deciding such things as which studies to include, which
Correspondence to: Dr. Herta Flor, Department of Clinical and
dependent measures should be given greatest weight,
Physiological Psychology, University of Tubingen, Gartenstrasse 29, how to reconcile differences in outcome on different
W-7400 Tiibingen, Germany. dependent measures, and how to abstract the evidence
222

from them may shift from study to study and from exceeded another by one-half of a standard deviation.
reviewer to reviewer (Mintz 1983). Furthermore, the This result can be interpreted as a specific treatment
adequacy of the box-score approach is limited because performing one-half of all standard deviations better
it ignores the magnitude of effects across all available than an alternate treatment or no treatment.
studies and across all variables. This approach uses A valuable feature of the computed effect size is
only a fraction of the available data. that it is comparable to a standardized Z score and
Because of the subjective nature of qualitative re- thus can be converted to percent improvement. Thus,
views, two reviewers can examine the same studies and an effect size of 0.50 for a treatment indicates that the
come to very different conclusions. For example, two average treated patient fared better than 69% of the
sets of reviewers examined the utility of the MMPI in untreated patients. Individual effect sizes can be aver-
predicting response of pain patients to different treat- aged across studies to provide a global portrait of the
ments. One United States commission, Social Security relationship or they can be further examined as depen-
Administration Commission on the Evaluation of Pain dent variables moderated by type of treatment in ques-
(US Dept. Hlth Hum. Serv. 19871, concluded that “... tion or a host of other methodological variables (e.g.,
the MMPI also has not yet demonstrated the kind of pertinent demographic data, quality of study) or mod-
predictive validity essential to precise measurements erator variables such as patient demographics, diagno-
about pain, nor has it yet yielded precise predictors of sis, compensation status (Wolf 1986).
success in treatment or rehabilitation program has Meta-analysis has begun to be widely accepted in
not as yet progressed to where it can serve as an medicine (Jenicek 1989; Mann 1990) and has received
acceptable measure for the assessment of clinical pain attention in the pain treatment outcome literature for
as it relates to disability” (p. 84). By way of contrast, different pain problems (e.g., headaches: Blanchard et
another reviewer of the same literature arrives at a al. 1980; Holroyd and Penzien 1986, 1990), the efficacy
conclusion that appears to be diametrically opposed, of preparatory information for surgery (e.g., Hathaway
namely, “The collective findings of empirical studies ... 1986; Suls and Wan 1989), coping strategies (e.g., Fer-
lend strong support to inclusion of the MMPI in the nandez and Turk 1989), and the efficacy of specific
assessment of patients being treated for chronic pain” pain treatments (e.g., acupuncture: Blanchard et al.
(Snyder 1990). To illustrate further how the nature of 1980; Pate1 et al. 1989) for diverse pain problems (e.g.,
reviews can lead to different conclusions, we can con- Malone and Strube 1988).
sider the recent meta-analytic review conducted by The number of dependent measures, diversity of
Fernandez and Turk (1989). These investigators patient characteristics, and criteria of success of pain
demonstrated how completely opposite conclusions can clinic outcome studies that have appeared in the litera-
be drawn based on reviewing the same studies examin- ture make both narrative and meta-analytic reviews
ing the efficacy of different pain coping strategies but especially difficult. Some of the problems with these
using qualitative versus quantitative (meta-analytic) box-score approaches were noted earlier. However,
methods. advances in the use of quantitative review methods
In pursuit of greater objectivity in the review pro- (meta-analyses) seem to offer a number of advantages
cess, a set of statistical techniques known collectively as and should contribute to a more systematic under-
‘meta-analysis’ was developed (Glass et al. 1981). standing of the effectiveness of pain clinics. The pri-
Meta-analyses consist of the integration and synthesis mary purpose of this paper is to use meta-analytic
of research through statistical analysis of individual methods to determine the relative effectiveness of mul-
studies. Perhaps the most innovative aspect of meta- tidisciplinary treatment for patients with chronic low
analytic applications is that they afford the use of back or heterogeneous pain, two of the largest groups
inferential statistics in a domain where conclusions of patients treated in pain clinics.
were formerly tested on simple summation and opinion
and thereby did not control for the subjective interpre-
tation of the reviewer. Meta-analyses reduce (but do Methods
not totally eliminate) the subjectivity that is inherent in
traditional narrative reviews.
Studies included
The effect size of a study is given by a basic formula Studies were identified by searching data bases including MED-
that incorporates the difference in mean outcomes of LARS and PSYCHINFO. reviewing relevant journals (e.g., Archkes
the experimental (treated) group and control (un- of Physical and Medical Rehabililarion, Pain, Journal of Consulting
treated) group, divided by the standard deviation of the and Clinical Psycholom), and tracing publications by searching the
reference sections of relevant reviews and papers. Studies were
control group. Computations based on this formula
included if they used an interdisciplinary treatment approach in a
result in the determination of the effects of treatment multidisciplinary pain clinic, included empirical data. and were pub-
in standard deviation units. Thus, for example, an lished from 1960 to 1990. A thorough review of the literature from
effect size of 0.50 reveals that one treatment condition 1960 to 1990 revealed more than 300 pain outcome studies that
223

TABLE I control groups were not reported. Additionally, within-group effect


sizes permitted a standardized control between different studies.
STUDY CHARACTERISTICS
Furthermore, within-group effect sizes reveal to what extent the
change in treatment groups exceeds the change in control groups.
Mean S.D. Range N
Following the suggestion of Nicholson and Berman (19831, in cases
Number of comparisons 12.09 11.61 l-60 65 in which the results were reported as non-significant and no other
Internal validity 1.20 0.59 o- 3 65 information was available, effect size was conservatively estimated as
Percentage excluded 16.12 20.05 o-54 12 0.
Percentage declined 14.01 IS.07 3-65 18 Following computation of effect sizes for each individual depen-
Percentage dropouts dent variable, mean effect sizes for each study were computed. The
in experimental group 14.98 13.68 O-48 37 study effect sizes were derived by averaging all variables for which
Percentage dropouts effect sizes were reported. Study effect sizes were used because the
in control group 9.64 16.58 o-47 10 utilization of several, potentially highly correlated measures from
Percentage loss of subjects one study has been criticized (Wortman and Bryant 1985; Bangert-
in experimental group 28.50 22.36 O-87 44 Drowns 1986). In addition to computing effect sizes the studies were
Percentage loss of subjects evaluated and coded on a number of criteria.
in control group 25.53 21.31 O-60 14 General information. This information included the study identi-
fication number, the journal, and the year of publication.
For the coding of internal validity see text. N = number of studies
Sum& information. The site of pain was coded according to the
included.
classification of the International Study for the Association of Pain
(Merskey 1986). Additional categories added were back pain, hetero-
geneous pain, and predominantly back pain ( > = 50%). The dura-
included a host of multidisciplinary interventions. Of these studies, a
tion of pain was coded in months. In addition, subject recruitment
majority dealt with recurrent headaches which have been extensively
strategy (clinic, advertising, solicited by experimenter, referred, com-
reviewed elsewhere (Blanchard et al. 1980; Chapman 1986; Holroyd
bination of the above) was coded. Patient characteristics including
and Penzien 1986, 1990; Keefe et al. 1986). One-hundred-and-sixteen
(a) age, (b) marital status, (cl education level, fd) SES, (e) employ-
studies were found that included some kind of psychological treat-
ment status, if) compensation, (g) litigation, (hf number of previous
ment for chronic back or heterogeneous pain. Twenty-six of these
surgeries and (i) medication use were also coded. Furthermore, the
treatment studies made use of one or more psychological treatment
basis for the exclusion or inclusion of patients was noted.
paradigms, for instance biofeedback (e.g., Nouwen 1983; Bush et al.
Design-related irzjbrmufion. Experimental design information was
1985) or they were not conducted in a pain clinic settting (cf., Turner
used in determining the methodological quality of the study. It
1982) and were therefore not included. An additional 25 publications
included the type of group assignment (random, matching, conve-
either did not provide any empirical data fn = 9), were single-case
nience sample). The internal validity of the study was coded accord-
presentations fn = 8) or were predictive studies with only one assess-
ing to 4 criteria (based on Glass et al. 1981): 0 = no statistical
ment at post treatment (n = 7). In summary, 65 of these 300 studies
analysis, highly non-equivalent groups, obvious bias; 1 = weak, non-
(22%) met inclusion criteria that qualified them for inclusion in the
existent matching, disproportionate mortality, inadequate statistics;
meta-analysis. The studies incorporated in the meta-analysis are
2 = random assignment but differential mortality, failed randomiza-
listed in the appendix. Table I displays descriptive information about
tion, well designed matching study; 3 = random assignment, mortality
the study population.
< 15% and same between groups, still good if data on terminators
are included or equivalence of dropouts and non-dropouts is estab-
Coding of series lished. The exclusion rates and the percentage of patients who
Each dependent variable of the 6.5 studies was coded by two declined to enter the study, the number of experimental and control
independent raters (H.F. and T.F.). Inter-rater reliabilities were groups, and the dropout rate per group were also recorded.
computed for a random sample of n = 13 studies and n = 50 effect Treatment-related information. The treatment-related informa-
sizes. Cohen’s kappa coefficient, based on Cicchetti (1976) ranged tion included the type of treatment experimental group. Because of
from 0.75 to 1.00 with a median kappa of 0.88. For the numerical great diversity and difficulties in differentiating some of the treat-
value of effect size a 94% agreement between raters was obtained. ments, type of treatment was not considered as a separate variable in
Disagreements in the studies were resolved by discussing the criteria this study. The comparison groups were coded as follows: (1) no
and refining them if necessary. treatment. (2) waiting list, (3) medical treatment, (4) placebo treat-
In order to have comparable effects for these studies, effect sizes ment, (5) second treatment, and (6) other. Further variables coded
were computed that relate the effects of the treatment groups with were the setting (inpatient, outpatient, combination of inpatient and
those of a control group. As noted, effect sizes can be seen as outpatient), the location of the treatment (pain clinic, university
analogous to standard deviations. They were calculated by subtract- department, other clinic, private practice), the primary department
ing the experimental groups post-treatment mean from that of the of the treatment provider (psychiatry, psychology, other medical), the
control group and dividing by the control group’s standard deviation amount of family invoivement, the training of the therapist who
(between-group effect size: Smith and Glass 1980). If means and provided the psychological treatment, the treatment format (individ-
standard deviations were not available, effect sizes were computed ual or group), the duration of the treatment, type and duration of
by using available F or r values or by estimating the data from concurrent medical treatment.
graphic displays or P values, as described by Glass et al. (Glass et al. Dependent variables. Overall. 704 dependent variables were coded
1981). by type: somatic, psychophysiological, behavioral, and verbal-subjec-
In addition, effect sizes for within-group changes over the course tive. Within the verbal-subjective modality pain intensity, interfer-
of the treatments, and - if data on control groups were available - ence, mood. and other measures (such as MMPI, SCL-90) were
within-group effect sizes for the control groups were calculated by differentiated. The behavioral variables included return to work, use
subtracting the post-treatment mean from the pretreatment mean of the health care system, medication intake, observable pain behav-
and dividing by the pretreatment standard deviation. This strategy iors, and activity levels.
permitted computation of standardized values for change on depen- The quality and reactivity of the outcome measures were coded
dent variables even if control groups were not used or data on following the suggestions of Glass et al. (iY81). A variable received a
723

quality rating of I if the measure showed low reliability and validity TABLE 11
or if the measure was specifically designed for the study without data
PATIENT CHARACTERISTIC‘S
on reliability and validity. A quality rating of 2 was assigned if
reliability and validity were adequately described or if a measure
Mean Median S.D. Range N
with known reliability and validity was used. Reactivity was coded
according to the three following categories: (I) all physiological Mean age in years 44.Y3 44.91 4.46 34.5- 56.0 56
measures, laboratory data, blinded ratings or behavioral assessments. Percent female 50. II) 52.00 IX.14 0 - X6 57
medical record data: (2) standardized measures with little connection Pain duration in months X5.43 60.00 113.h’) I3 -7% 46
with the treatment or therapist (e.g.. diary data): (3) patient self-re- Percentage working 34.17 25.25 32.1 I 0 -100 2x
port of improvement, therapist improvement ratings. instruments Percent married 77.f15 77.00 0.20 56 100 27
with obvious relationship to treatment outcome. Twenty-nine per- Average years in school I I.98 IZ.lO 0.70 IO - I3 IO
cent received a good quality rating, 36q a low quality rating, and for Percentage with
the remainder the quality could not be determined. The reactivity of litigation 2t1.53 x.20 24.45 0 - 63 20
the measures was generally high (58%); only 3% received a low Percentage ou
reactivity rating, and the remainder was considered to be moderately compensation 5 I .hJ 53.00 24.5’) 0 63 20
reactive. Percentage with
Eff;‘c’t size.\. Effect sizes were compared along a number of ;’ = I sui-gery 54.50 50.00 lY.33 28 -IO0 23
dimensions. First, the mean effect size for each of the treatments Mean number of
under review was computed to determine the overall effect of the surgeries 1.76 I .50 0.86 0.4- 4.60 74
treatment provided. The long-term effects of the treatments were Pcrcentagc mcd-
evaluated by computing separate effect sizes for the time from the cation use x4.5 I X6.00 12.12 53 ~100 17
completion of treatment up to 6 months and the period ranging from
N = number of studies included.
7 months to 7 years post treatment. Next. the changes for each type
of dependent measure (e.g.. verbal-subjective, behavioral) were ana-
lyzed. For these computations, effect sizes based on individual out- of hl patients per study. Table II presents important demographic
come measures rather than study effect sizes were used. Additional and clinical characteristics of the patients. These data show that
analyses were performed for selected variables, such as return to those patients for whom information was available were severely
work and use of the health care system. Finally, the effect sizes were disabled with high rates of unemployment, a large number of com-
correlated with sample and design characteristics as well as study pensation and litigation cases. many previous surgeries. and high
characteristics to determine predictors of the effect sizes for the medication use. In most of the studies (85%) regular pain clinic
various treatments. patients not specifically referred for a study served as subjects.

Treatment characteristics
Characteristics of studies included The majority of the treatments included a combination of psycho-
The number of dependent variables per study ranged from I to logical interventions, medical treatments. and physical or occupa-
3. Between I and 60 effect sizes or percentage change measures tional therapy. The average duration of the treatments was 7 weeks
were obtained per study with an average of 12. The number of (SD. = 6.42. N = 52) with a range from I to 31 weeks. The average
experimental groups ranged from I to 3 (92% of the studies had only number of hours spent in treatment ranged from 4 to 264 h. with an
one) and the number of control groups from 0 to 2 (with 65% having average of Yh h (S.D. = 7X. N = 51). For inpatient programs which
no control group). lacked information on the exact number of treatment hours. an
The most common type of treatment provided was multimodal average of 6 treatment hours/day for 7 days/week was assumed.
(60%); the most frequent type of control group was one composed of Approximately 80% of the treatments were conducted by multidisci-
patients whose insurance company refused to pay or who could not plinary treatment teams. The majority of the studies (50%) were
pay for other reasons (28%). The typical assignment to the experi- performed in inpatient settings; 289; were in an outpatient setting
mental and control groups was that a convenience sample consisting and I3V were mixed. The remainder of the studies (N = 6) provided
of patients treated was chosen (75%). Thus, the internal validity of no information on the type of setting.
the studies using the Glass et al. (1981) criteria was relatively low.
with only 8% of the studies receiving a rating of 3 (high internal
validity) and 86.5% receiving a low validity rating. Results

Or~erall treatment effects


Patient characteristics
Overall 3089 patients were included in the meta-analysis. The The overall effect sizes for the studies without dif-
number of patients per study ranged from 5 to 467, with an average ferentiating between type and quality of measure and

TABLE 111

MEAN EFFECT SIZES BY TIME OF ASSESSMENT

Follow-up Treatment group ES Control group ES Between-group ES Treated group Control group
(within-group) (within-group) (Cr, change) t% change)

Mean S.D. Range N Mean SD. Range N Mean SD. Range N Mean SD. N Mean S.D. N

Short term t < = 6 mo.)


I.51 1.4’) 0.25-7.98 34 0.03 0.19 - 0.2OHJ.42 7 0.62 0.47 -0.46-I.24 I3 47 27 46 6 I8 II
Long term (> 6 mo.)
I.31 1.06 0.23-5.79 24 0.44 0.47 0.08-I 4 0.x1 O.hh 0.04-2.43 I2 4x 21 2’) 24 21 IO

N = number of studies included


225

TABLE IV The mean percentage change at short-term follow-up


EFFECT SIZES BY TYPE OF CONTROL GROUP was 40.07 (S.D. = 27.36, N = 46) for the experimental
and 6.11 (S.D. = 17.77, N = 46) for the control groups.
Mean S.D. Range N At long-term follow-up these percentages changed to
Treatment denied 0.90 0.71 0.12-2.43 8 48.34 (S.D. = 20.60, N = 29) for the treated and 23.65
Waiting list 0.62 0.31 0.17-1.14 6 (S.D. = 20.72, N = 10) for the control groups (but note
Pure medical treatment 0.83 0.56 -0.12-1.24 5
the small N for the control group). These results sug-
Placebo 0.97 0.13 0.88-1.06 2
Physical therapy 0.41 0.30 0 -0.73 6
gest that the groups treated in a multidisciplinary pain
clinic demonstrate at least twice the change reported
N = number of studies included. by control groups.

other study parameters, are presented in Table III. In


Comparison to different control groups
order to determine short-term and long-term effects of
the treatments, assessments were divided in up to 6 When treatment effects were compared to different
months after completion of treatment (short-term fol- types of control groups it became evident that those
low-up) and longer than 6 months post treatment patients who declined treatment differed most from
(long-term follow-up). the treated patients with an average effect size of 0.90
Table III displays three types of overall effect sizes (see Table IV). When compared to unimodal non-
for the study population. Examination of this table medical or conventional medical treatments the exam-
reveals that both the between-group and the within- ined multimodal programs seem to be superior, allow-
group effect sizes are large and are maintained at the ing the tentative conclusion that combined treatments
short-term and long-term follow-ups. The effect sizes in general are superior to the use of single-treatment
at both follow-ups are smaller when compared to con- components. Differences in effect size between the
trol groups than within treatment; however, they re- various control groups, however, were not significant.
main substantial. The majority of the control group
effect sizes do not differ significantly from zero. Treatment effects by outcome measure
An effect size of 0.62 for the between-group com- The frequencies of utilization of the five different
parison at short-term follow-up (mean: 5 weeks post categories of dependent measures show that behavioral
treatment) indicates that a treated patient is function- and verbal-subjective measures of pain are most widely
ing on the average 0.60 standard deviations or 30% used. The pattern of the within-group effect sizes re-
better than a patient from the control group (per- veals that patients show the greatest improvement on
centage based on a binomial effect size display: cf., self-report measures of pain (Table V>. Importantly,
Rosenthal and Rubin 1982). The effect size of 0.81 at however, they demonstrate substantial effects on the
follow-up (on the average 95 weeks post treatment) more objective physical and behavioral measures as
amounts to an improvement of 38%. If this value is well.
compared to the within-group effect size, the effect A more detailed analysis of the behavioral class of
size increases to 1.51 at short-term and 1.31 at long- outcome variables revealed that there were substantial
term follow-up, respectively. Expressed in average im- effects of the treatments on such important measures
provement, these results translate to an improvement as return to work and use of the health care system.
of 60% at short-term follow-up and 55% at long-term For example, the between-groups effect size of 0.67 for
follow-up. return to work indicates that the treated patients have
Computations of percentage improvement were pos- a much greater likelihood (68% vs. 32%) to return to
sible for more studies than computations of effect size. work than the untreated patients. Comparable effects

TABLE V

EFFECT SIZES BY OUTCOME MEASURES

Within-group ES Between-group ES Treated (% change) Controls (%) change)

Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N


Somatic 0.94 0.50 37 - - 58 29 44 _ _ _
Psychophysiological 2.04 1.26 5 0.84 0.26 4 47 10 5 11 10 4
Behavioral 1.19 0.99 158 0.65 0.70 115 54 47 216 10 26 52
Pain 1.59 1.91 97 0.70 0.66 73 37 23 116 4 18 44
Interference 0.50 0.43 20 1.10 0.67 12 23 14 23 -7 6 3
Mood 1.91 3.11 10 0.63 0.33 3 20 22 12 12 23 4
Other subjective 0.76 0.82 104 0.47 0.48 61 24 21 139 10 17 38

N = number of studies included.


226

were evident for use of medication, overall pain behav- there was, however, a small significant negative corre-
iors, and activity levels (all 65% vs. 35% improvement). lation between reactivity of a measure and between-
group effect size (r (271) = - 0.14, P < 0.05).
It might be assumed that longer treatment times
Correlation of study characteristics with effect sizes
lead to greater improvement. However, the correlation
An interesting question is the determination of vari-
between duration of treatment and effect size was not
ables associated with effect size (Table VI>. Several
significant (r (12) = - 0.11). Thus, longer treatments
variables have been associated with poor outcome, for
do not necessarily produce stronger effects.
example, chronicity (Swanson et al. 19791, age (Holroyd
and Penzien 19861, litigation or compensation (Block et
al. 1980; Guck et al. 1985). Correlations between effect
size and age, duration of pain, litigation or compensa-
Discussion
tion were not significant (r (23) = -0.24 for between-
group effect size and age; r (19) = -0.31 for between-
group effect size and duration; r (8) = -0.23 for be- Overall the results of this meta-analysis provide
tween-group effect size and compensation, and r (7) = support for the conclusion that multidisciplinary pain
- 0.25 for- between-group effect size and litigation). clinics are efficacious. Even at long-term follow-up,
Caution is needed in interpreting the non-significant patients who are treated in such a setting are function-
effects for these variables as they are based on a small ing better than 75% of a sample that is either un-
number of studies. Despite the few studies, the low treated or that has been treated by conventional, uni-
correlations between treatment success and litigation modal treatment approaches. Although the largest ef-
and compensation suggest that these factors may not. fect sizes were obtained when treated patients were
as has been suggested, show serious impediments to compared to those whose insurance company declined
successful treatment in pain clinics (Turk and Rudy payment or who could not pay themselves, effect sizes
1990). based on these comparison groups were not signifi-
Other variables thought to be of relevance for the cantly different from effect sizes based on the compari-
evaluation of treatment outcome studies are the num- son of treatments with waiting list or purely medical
ber of dropouts per study as well as the overall attri- treatments. Conventional physical therapy is somewhat
tion rate (which includes dropouts, those excluded better than no treatment or medical treatment but
from treatment, and those who do not return to for inferior to multidisciplinary approaches (commonly a
follow-ups). Neither the percentage of dropouts, r (12) combination of medical, physical therapy, and psycho-
= 0.01, NS, nor the overall attrition rate, r (15) = logical approaches).
- 0.30, NS, were significantly related to between-group Importantly, the effects for pain clinics are not
effect sizes. As noted, the small sample sizes preclude simply limited to the subjective perception of the pa-
firm conclusions. tients but also extend to objective behavioral variables
Somewhat surprisingly, the quality of the studies such as return to work or decreased use of the health
was not significantly related to treatment outcome (r care system. For example, patients treated in a multi-
(23) = 0.14 for the correlation between group effect disciplinary pain clinic are almost twice (68% vs. 36%)
size and internal validity). However, the type of group as likely to return to work than the untreated or
assignment was marginally related with non-random unimodally treated patients. Similarly impressive are
assignment yielding higher between-group effect sizes the data on the pretreatment to post-treatment change:
(r (25) = 0.32, P < 0.10). The quality of the dependent an overall effect size of 1.35 can be interpreted as an
variables was not significantly related to effect size: improvement of 56%. This figure is closely paralleled

TABLE VI
EFFECT SIZES ON SPECIFIC BEHAVIORAL LEVELS

Within-group ES Between-group ES Treated (% change) Untreated (‘% change)

Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N Mean S.D. N


Work 1.35 0.71 30 0.67 0.55 17 43 28 43 2s 32 9
Medication 1.78 1.19 48 0.61 0.63 30 63 27 54 21 21 7
Health care use 0.44 0.29 16 0.47 0.5 1 15 35 16 21 4 1x 3
Activity 0.92 0.66 27 0.63 0.87 16 53 66 49 13 32 14
Pain behavior 0.59 0.44 29 0.61 0.85 13 62 67 34 0 4 x

N = number of variables included in the analysis.


227

by the average 43% improvement (meaning 43% more forthcoming, this issue must be considered as unre-
patients are working after as compared to before treat- solved at this time.
ment) computed from the return to work data noted in Several issues discussed in the meta-analysis by Mal-
the studies for which effect size could not be com- one and Strube (Mann 19901 were not addressed in
puted. The findings on return to work are especially this review. In order to minimize problems created by a
important. small number of studies with very heterogeneous pain
Although it might be argued that over one-half of problems, this study was limited to patients who at-
the patients treated in pain clinics do not return to tended a multidiscipinary pain clinic, i.e., patients who
work, the duration of pain (over 7 years), the magni- suffered predominantly from chronic back pain. Gen-
tude of the problem, extent of disability of patients eralizations to other types of pain should, therefore,
included in these studies, and the tremendous cost of not be made. We refrained from an analysis of the
disability payments suggest that a 43% saving for this comparative efficacy of different types of multidisci-
intractable pain population would save third-party pay- plinary interventions because the number of studies
ers billions of dollars not to mention the incalculable included would preclude statistical analysis. As Hol-
reduction in suffering of the patients and their fami- royd and Penzien (1989) noted, merely ocular compar-
lies. isons of means based on a sample for which type of
The conduction of this meta-analysis was signifi- treatment, type of pain, and type of outcome measure
cantly hampered by the fact that most studies failed to are confounded, will not yield meaningful information
provide means and standard deviations for the relevant on the efficacy of multidisciplinary pain treatments.
outcome measures. In a large percentage of cases they Further evaluation of treatment outcome studies
had to be determined from proportions or had to be would be facilitated if authors adhered to the following
estimated from graphic displays. guidelines: (a) comprehensive patient sample descrip-
There are several qualifications that must be noted tions according to the criteria detailed in the Method
in interpreting the results based on to this meta-analy- section of this article; (b) use of adequate control
sis. First, the majority of the studies were conducted groups, preferably with random assignment; (cl use of
without appropriate control groups. Even when control well-established outcome measures on all levels rele-
groups were used, they were rarely appropriate and vant for pain; and Cdl provision of means, standard
most often composed of a convenience sample. Typi- deviations, and sample sizes for all outcome measures.
cally, patients who dropped out of treatment, whose Despite the limitations noted, several conclusions
insurance company refused to pay or patients who did can be drawn from this meta-analytic study. First,
not enter treatment for other reason were used as multidisciplinary pain treatments are superior to uni-
controls. There might be significant differences be- modal treatments and no treatment. Second, the bene-
tween patients who accept treatment and those who ficial effects from pain clinics are maintained over an
decline, for whatever reason. This type of non-random extended period of time. Importantly, the improve-
control group is likely to inflate effect size, as is evi- ments documented are not only reflected on subjective
dent from the positive correlation of non-random as- ratings of pain but also more objective measures such
signment and effect size. Thus, the appropriateness of as return to work and use of the health care system.
using those who decline treatments as a comparison These results are quite impressive and add credence to
group is open to question and needs greater considera- the relative and incremental validity of multidisci-
tion and discussion. Other limitations include the qual- plinary pain clinic treatment.
ity of studies and the reactivity of measures. Moreover,
only a minority of studies provided descriptions of the
patient sample, and information on study design and
Appendix
analysis. The advantages of meta-analysis are severely
reduced by inadequate study descriptions, dissimilar
dependent measures and diagnostic groups (cf., Hol- Studies included in the meta-analysis
royd and Penzien 1989). Anderson, T.P., Cole, T.M., Gullickson, G., Hudgens, A. and
Roberts, A.H., Behavior modification of chronic pain: a treat-
Another issue to note is the substantial attrition of
ment program by a multidisciplinary team, Clin. Orthop., 129
subjects observed over the course of the treatments (1977) 96-100.
and especially at long-term follow-up. In some studies, Arawaka, K., Outpatient pain clinic, J. Kans. Med. Sot., 82, (1981)
as many as 87% did not complete the entire study. It is 292-294.
not clear how these high attrition rates affect outcome Block, A., Kremer, E. and Gaylor, M., Behavioral treatment of
chronic pain: variables affecting treatment efficacy, Pain, 8 (1980)
(cf., Turk and Rudy 1990). The lack of a significant
367-375.
correlation between effect size and attrition in this Cassisi, J.E., Sypert, G.W., Salamon, A. and Kapel, L., Independent
study sample would, however, suggest that the effect of evaluation of a multidisciplinary rehabilitation program for
dropout may not be significant. Unless further data are chronic low back pain, Neurosurgery, 25 (1989) 877-883.
Chapman. S.. Brena. S. and Bradford. A., Treatment outcome in a addiction: a multitherapeutic approach. Comprehensive Psychiat..
chronic pain rehabilitation program. Pain, 1 I (1981) 255-268. 20 (1979) 55-60.
Cinciripini, P.M. and Floreen. A.. An evaluation of a behavioral Kleinke. C.L.. and Spangler, AS., Predicting treatment outcome of
program for chronic pain. J. Behav. Med., 5 (1982) 375-3X9. chronic back pain patients in ;I multidisciplinary pain clinic:
DeBenedettis, G.. A new strategy for chronic pain control: prelimi- methodological issues and treatment implications. Pain. 33 (1988)
nary results. Part II. J. Neurosurg. Sci., 23 (1979) 191-200. 4lL4X.
DeGood, D., A behavioral pain-management program: expanding Linssen. A.G. and Zitman, F.G.. Patient evaluation of a cognitive
the psychologist’s role in a medical setting. Prof. Psychol.. 3X behavioral group program for patients with low back pain. Sot.
(1979) 491-502. Sci. Med.. IY (IYX4) 1361 1367.
Dolce. J.J., Cracker, M.F. and Doleys, D.M., Predicion of outcome Linton. S.J. and Goetestam. K.G.. A controlled study of the effects
among chronic pain patients. Behav. Res. Ther.. 24 (1986) 31% of applied relaxation and applied relaxation plus operant proce-
31’). dures in the regulation of chronic pain. Br. J. Clin. Psychol.. 23
Doleys, D.M., Cracker. M. and Patton, D.. Response of patients with (19X4) 201-29’).
chronic pain to exercise quotas, Phys. Ther., 62 (1982) 111 l-l 114. Linton. S.L. and Melin. L.. Applied relaxation in the management of
Duckro. P.L.. Margolis, R.B., Tait, R.C. and Korytnyk, N., Long-term chronic pain, Behav. Psychother., 11 (1983) 337-350.
follow-up of chronic pain patients, Inc. J. Psychiat. Med.. 15 Linton. S.L., Melin, 1.. and Stjernlocf, K.. The effect5 of applied
(lY85) 2X3-291. relaxation and operant activity training on chronic pain. Behav.
Flor, H.. Haag, G.. Turk. D.C. and Koehler, H.. Efficacy of EMG Psychother.. 13 (lYX5) X7-100.
biofeedback, pseudotherapy and conventional medical treatment Lutz. R.W.. Silhert, M. and Olshan. N.. Treatment outcome and
for chronic rheumatic back pain, Pain, 17 (1983) 21-31. compliance with therapeutic regimens: long-term follow-up of a
Flor, H., Haag, G. and Turk, D.C.. Long-term efficacy of EMG multidisciplinary pain program. Pain. 17 (19X.7) 301-30X.
biofeedback for chronic rheumatic back pain, Pain. 27 (19X6) Malec, J., Cayner. J.J. and Harvey, R.F., Pain management: long-term
195-202. follow-up of an inpatient program. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehah.. 62
Fordycc, W.E., Fowler. R.S., Lehmann. J.F.. DeLateur, B., Sand. (IYXI) 36Y-372.
P.L. and Trieschmann. R.B., Operant conditioning in the trea- Mayer. T.G., Gatchel. R.J.. Kishino, N.. Keeley. J.. Mayer. H.,
ment of chronic pain, Arch. Phys. Med.. 54 (1973) 399-406. Capra. P. and Mooney. V., A prospective short-term study of
Frederickson, B.E.. Trief, P.M.. VanBeveren. P., Yuan. H.A. and chronic low back pain patients utilizing novel objective functional
Baum. G., Rehabilitation of the patient with chronic back pain: a measures. Pain, 2.5 (1986) 53-6X.
search for outcome predictors, Spine. 13 (19%) 351-353. Mayer. T.G.. Gatchel. R.J.. Mayer. f1.. Kishino. N.D.. Keeley. J.K.
Gottlieb. H.. Alperson. B.L., Schwartz, A.H.. Beck. C. and Kee. S., and Mooney. V., A prospective two-year study of functional
Self-management for medication reduction in chronic low-back restoration in industrial low back injury, J. Am. Med. Assoc.. 25X
pain. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab., 69 (1988) 442-44X. (1987) 17h3-17h7.
Gottlieb. M.. Strite, L.. Keller, R.. Madorsky, A., Hockersmith, V.. McArthur, I)., Cohen, M.J.. Gottlieh. 11.J.. Naliboff. B.D. and
Kleeman, M. and Wagner, J.. Comprehensive rehabilitation of Schandler, S.L.. Treating chronic low-back pain. 1. Admissions to
patients having chronic low back pain, Arch. Phys. Med. Rehah.. initial follow-up. Pain. 29 (lYX7a) l-22.
5X (1977) 101-108. McArthur. D.. Cohen, M.J.. Gottlieh, H.J., Nalihoff, B.D. and
Guck, T.P.. Skultety. F.M., Meilman, P.W. and Dowd, E.T., Multi- Schandler. S.L.. Treating chronic low-back pain. 11. Long-term
disciplinary pain center follow-up study: evaluation with a no- follow-up, Pain. 29 (lY87h) 23-3X.
treatment control group, Pain, 21 (1985) 295~306. Melin. L. and Linton. S.J., A follow-up study of a comprehcns1ve
Hirkip%i, K.. Jiirvikoski, A., Mellin, G. and Hurri, H., A controlled behavioral treatment programme for chronic pain patients, Be-
study on the outcome of inpatient and outpatient treatment of hav. Psychother.. 16 (19x8) 313-322.
low back pain, Stand. J. Rehab. Med., 21 (1989) X1-89. Moore, J.A. and Chaney. E.F.. Outpatient group treatment of chronic
Hammonds, W., Brena, S.F. and Unikel, I.P.. Comparison for work- pain: effects of spouse involvement. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.. 53
related injuries and rehabilitation of patients with chronic pain, (19X5) 326-334.
South. Med. J., (1978) 664-666. Moore, M.E.. Berk, S.N. and Nypaver, A., Chronic pain: inpatient
Hazard, R.G.. Fenwick, J.W., Kalisch, S.M., Redmond, J.. Reeves. treatment with small group effects, Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab.. 65
V.. Reid. S. and Frymoyer, J.W., Functional restoration with (I 984) 356~361.
behavioral support, Spine, 14 (19X9) 157-161. Newman, R.I.. Seres. J.L.. Yospe. L.P. and Garlington, B.. Multidis-
Heinrich. R.L.. Cohen, M.J., Naliboff, B.D., Collins, G.A. and ciplinary treatment of chronic pain: long-term follow-up of low-
Bonebakker, A.D.. Comparing physical and behavior therapy for back pain patients, Pain, 4 (197X) 2X3-292.
chronic low hack pain on physical abilities, psychological distrers Painter, J.R., Seres, J.L. and Newman. R.I.. Assessing benefits oi a
and patient’s perception, J. Behav. Med.. 8 (19X5) 61-78. pain center: why some patients regress, Pain, 8 (IYXO) 101-I 13.
Herman, E. and Baptiste. S., Pain control mastery through group Parris. W.C.V., Jamison, R.N. and Vasterling. J.J.. Follow-up study
experience, Pain, 10 (19X1) 79-86. of a multidisciplinary pain center, J. Pain Sympt. Manag.. 2 (19X7)
Ignelzi. R.. Sternbach, R. and Timmermanns. G.. The pain-ward 145-151.
follow-up analyses, Pain, 3 (1977) 277-280. Ritchie. R.J. and Sternhach. A token economy system for the treat-
Keefe, F.J.. Block, A.R., Williams, R.B. and Sunvit, R.S., Behavioral ment of chronic low back pain. J. Behav. Ther., 7 (lY7h) 341-343.
treatment of chronic low back pain: clinical outcome and individ- Roberts. A. and Reinhardt. L.. The behavioral management 01
ual differences in pain relief, Pain, 11 (1981) 221-231. chronic pain: long-term follow-up with comparison groups, Pain.
Kerns, R.D., Turk. D.C., Holzman. A.D. and Rudy. T.E., Compari- x (19X0) 151-162.
son of cognitive-behavioral and behavioral approaches to the Rybstein-Blinchik, E. and Grzesiak. R., Reinterpretative cognitive
outpatient treatment of chronic pain, Clinical J. of Pain, (1986) strategies in chronic pain management. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab..
195-203. 60 (1979) 609-612.
Khatami. M. and Rush, A.J., A one year fullow-up of the multimode1 Rybstein-Blinchik. E.. Effects of different cognitive coping strategies
treatment for chronic pain, Pain, 14 (1982) 45-52. on chronic pain experience, J. Behav. Med.. 2 (1979) 93-101.
Khatami, M., Woody, G. and O’Brien, C., Chronic pain and narcotic Salzherg. C.. Hanley. D.. Jennison. J. and Chang-Zern. H.. Back
229

school: a valuable rehabilitation tool for the management of low Chapman, S.L., A review and clinical perspective on the use of EMG
back pain, Pain Manag., 22 (1988) 78-84. and thermal feedback for chronic headaches, Pain, 27 (1986)
Sarno, J.E., Chronic pain and psychic conflict, Stand. J. Rehab. 1-44.
Med., 8 (1976) 143-153. Cicala, R.S. and Wright, H., Outpatient treatment of patients with
Seres, J. and Newman, R., Results of chronic low-back pain at the chronic pain: an analysis of cost savings, Clin. J. Pain, 5 (1989)
Portland Pain Center, J. Neurosurg., 45 (1976) 32-36. 223-226.
Shealy, C.N., Holistic management of chronic pain, Topics Clin. Cicchetti, D.V., Assessing inter-rater reliability for rating scales:
Nurs., 2 (1980) 1-18. resolving some basic issues, Br. J. Psychiat., 129 (1976) 452-456.
Simmons, J.W., Avant, W.S., Demski, J.D. and Parisher, D., Deter- Fernandez, E. and Turk, D.C., The utility of cognitive coping strate-
mining successful pain clinic treatment through validation of cost gies for altering pain perceptions: a meta-analysis, Pain, 38 (1989)
effectiveness, Spine, 13 (1988) 342-344. 123-136.
Smith, G.T., Huges, L.B., Duvall, R.D. and Rothman, S., Treatment Glass, G., Primary, secondary, and meta-analysis of research, Educ.
outcome of a multidisciplinary center for management of chronic Res., 5 (1976) 3-8.
pain: a long-term follow-up, Clin. J. Pain, 27 (1988) 47-49. Glass, G., McGaw, B. and Smith, M.L., Meta-analysis in Social
Snow, B.R., Gusmorino, P., Pinter, I., Jimenez, A. and Rosenblum, Research, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, 1981.
A., Multidisciplinary treatment of physical and psychosocial dis- Guck, T.P., Skultety, F.M., Meilman, P.W. and Dowd, E.T., Multi-
abilities in chronic pain patients: a follow-up report, Bull. Hosp. disciplinary pain center follow-up study: evalutation with a no-
Joint Dis. Orthop. Inst., 48 (1988) 52-61. treatment control group, Pain, 21 (1985) 295-306.
Stans, L., Goossens, L., Van Houdenhove. B., Adraensen, H., Ver- Hathaway, D., Effect of preoperative instruction on postoperative
straeten, D.. Vervaeke, M. and Fannes, V., Evaluation of a brief outcomes: a meta-analysis, Nurs. Res., 35 (1986) 269-275.
chronic pain management program: effects and limitations, Clin. Holroyd, K.A. and Penzien, D.B., Client variables and the behavioral
J. Pain. 5 (1089) 317-322. treatment of recurrent tension headache: a meta-analytic review,
Sturgis. ET., Schaefer, C.A. and Sikora, T.L., Pain center follow-up J. Behav. Med., 9 (1986) 515-536.
study of treated and untreated patients, Arch. Phys. Med. Rehab., Holroyd, K.A. and Penzien, D.B., Meta-analysis minus the analysis: a
65 t I9841 30 I-303. prescription for confusion. Pain, 39 (1989) 359-361.
Swanson, D., Swenson W., Maruta. T. and McPhee, M., Program for Holroyd, K.A. and Penzien, D.B., Pharmacological versus non-
managing chronic pain. 1. Program description and characteristics pharmacological prophylaxis of recurrent migraine headache: a
of patients. Mayo Clin. Proc., 51 (1976a) 401-408. meta-analytic review of clinical trials, Pain, 42 (1990) 1-14.
Swanson, D., Floreen, A. and Swenson, W., Program for managing Jenicek, M., Meta-analysis in medicine: where we are and where we
chronic pain. II. Short term results, Mayo Clin. Proc., 51 (1976b) want to go, J. Clin. Epidemiol., 42 (1989) 35-44.
4OY-411. Jessup, B.A., Relaxation and biofeedback. In: P.D. Wall and R.
Swanson. D., Maruta. T. and Swenson, W., Results of behavior Melzack (Eds.), Textbook of Pain. Churchill Livingstone, Edin-
modification in the treatment of chronic pain, Psychosom. Med.. burgh, 1989, pp. 989-1000.
41 (1979) 55-61. Keefe, F.J., Gil, K.M. and Rose, S.C., Behavioral approaches in the
Taylor. C., Zlutnick, S., Corley, M. and Flora. J., The effects of multidisciplinary management of chronic pain: programs and
detoxification, relaxation and brief supportive therapy on chronic issues, Clin. Psychol. Rev., 6 (1986) 87-l 13.
pain, Pain, 8 (1980) 319-329. Kerns, R.D., Turk, D.C. and Holzman, A.D., Psychological treat-
Tollison, C.D., Kriegel, M.L., Datterthwaite, J.R., Hinnant, D.W. ment for chronic pain: a selective review, Clin. Psychol. Rev.. 3
and Turner, K.P., Comprehensive pain center treatment of low (1983) 15-26.
back workers’ compensation injuries, Orthop. Rev., IX (1989) Linton, S.J., Behavioral remediation of chronic pain: a status report,
1115-1126. Pain, 24 (1986) 125-141.
Tyre. T.E. and Anderson, D.L., Inpatient management of chronic Love. A.W. and Peck, C.L., The MMPI and psychological factors in
pain patient: one-year follow-up study, J. Fam. Pratt., 12 (1981) chronic low back pain: a review, Pain, 28 (1987) l-12.
XIY-827. Malone, M.D. and Strube, M.J., Meta-analysis of non-medical treat-
Wang, J.K., Ilstrup, D.M., Nauss. L.A., Nelson, D.O. and Wilson, ments for chronic pain, Pain, 4 (1988) 231-244.
P.R., Outpatient pain clinic: a long-term follow-up study, Minn. Mann, C., Meta-analysis in the breech. Science, 299 (1990) 476-480.
Med.. 63 (1980) 663-666. Merskey, H., Classification of chronic pain: description of chronic
pain syndromes and definition of pain terms, Pain, 3 (1986)
Sl-S225.
Mintz, J.. Integrating research evidencee: a commentary on meta-
analysis, J. Cons. Clin. Psychol., 51 (1983) 71-75.
References
Nicholson, R.A. and Berman, J.S., Is follow-up necessary in evaluat-
ing psychotherapy?, Psychol. Bull., 93 (1983) 261-278
Aronoff, G.M., Evans, W.O. and Enders, P.L., A review of follow-up Nouwen, A., EMG biofeedback used to reduce standing levels of
studies of multidisciplinary pain units, Pain, I6 (1983) 1-11. paraspinal muscle tension in chronic low-back pain. Pain, I7
Bangert-Drowns, R.L., Reviews of the developments in meta-analytic (1983) 335-360.
method, Psychol. Bull., 99 (1986) 388-399. Nouwen, A. and Solinger. W., The effectiveness of EMG biofeed-
Blanchard, E.B., Andrasik, F., Ahles, T.A., Teders, S.J. and O’Keefe, back training in low-back pain, Biofeedback Self-Regul., 4 (1979)
D.M., Migraine and tension headache: a meta-analytic review, 103-111.
Behav. Ther., 11 (1980) 613-631. Patel, M., Gutzwiller, F., Paccaud, F. and Marazzi, A., A meta-analy-
Block. A.R., Multidisciplinary treatment of chronic low back pain: a sis of acupuncture for chronic pain, Intern. J. Epidem., I8 (1989)
review, Rehab. Psychol., 27 (1982) 51-63. 900-906.
Block, A., Kremer, E. and Gaylor, M., Behavioral treatment of Rosenthal, R. and Rubin, D.B., A simple, general purpose display of
chronic pain: variables affecting treatment efficacy, Pain, 8 (1980) magnitude of experimental effect, J. Educ. Psychol., 79 (1982)
367-375. 166-169.
Bush. C., Ditto, B. and Feuerstein, M., A controlled evaluation of Smith, M. and Glass, G., Meta-analytic research on class size and its
paraspinal EMG biofeedback in the treatment of chronic low relationship to attitudes and instructions, Am. Educ. Res. J., 17
back pain, Hlth Psychol., 4 (1985) 307-321. (1980) 419-433.
Snyder. D.K., Assessing chronic pain with the Minnesota Multiphasic ment outcome studies: referral patterns, failure to enter treat-
Personality Inventory. In: T.W. Miller (Ed.), Chronic Pain, Vol. 2, ment and attrition, Pain, 43 (1990) 7-26.
International Universities Press, Madison, CT, 1990, pp. 181-214. Turner, J.A., Comparison of group progressive-relaxation training
Stieg, R.L. and Turk, D.C., Chronic pain syndrome: demonstrating and cognitive-behavioral group therapy for chronic low back pain,
the cost-benefit of treatment, Clin. J. Pain, 4 (1988) 58-63. J. Cons. Clin. Psychol., SO (1982) 757-765.
Suls, J. and Wan, Ch. K., Effects of sensory and procedural informa- Turner, J.A. and Romano, J.M., Evaluating psychologic interventions
tion on coping with stressful1 medical procedures and pain: a for chronic pain: issues and recent developments. Advan. Pain
meta-analysis, J. Cons. Clin. Psychol., 57 (1989) 372-379. Res. Ther.. 7 (1984) 257-296.
Swanson. D., Maruta, T. and Swenson, W., Results of behavior United States Department of Health and Human Services, Report of
modification in the treatment of chronic pain, Psychosom. Med., the Commission on the Evaluation of Pain (SSA Publ. No.
41 (1979) 55-61. 64-031). US Government Printing Office, Washington, DC. 1987.
Trifiletti, R.J., The psychological effectiveness of pain management Wolf, F.M.. Meta-Analysis, Sage, Beverly Hills, CA, 1986.
procedures in the context of behavioral medicine and medical Wortman, P.M. and Bryant, F.B.. School desegregation and black
psychology, Cert. Psychol. Monogr., 109 (1984) 251-278. achievement: an integrative review, Social. Meth. Res.. I3 (1085)
Turk. D.C. and Rudy, T.E.. Neglected factors in chronic pain treat- 289-324.

You might also like