You are on page 1of 9

RESEARCH ARTICLES

Determinants of Dual-Task Training Effect Size


in Parkinson Disease: Who Will Benefit Most?
Carolien Strouwen, PhD, Esther A. L. M. Molenaar, MSc, Liesbeth Münks, MSc, Sanne Broeder, MSc,
Pieter Ginis, PhD, Bastiaan R. Bloem, PhD, Alice Nieuwboer, PhD, and Elke Heremans, PhD

Background and Purpose: Dual-task interventions show positive benefited most from the dual-task training, irrespective of the type of
effects in people with Parkinson disease (PD), but it remains unclear training and type of dual-task outcome.
which factors determine the size of these benefits. As a secondary Video Abstract available for more insights from the authors (see
analysis of the DUALITY trial, the aim of this study was to assess Video, Supplemental Digital Content 1, available at: http://links.lww.
the determinants of the effect size after 2 types of dual-task practice. com/JNPT/A242).
Methods: We randomly allocated 121 participants with PD to re- Key words: cognition, dual task, gait, human movement system,
ceive either integrated or consecutive dual-task training. Dual-task prediction
walking performance was assessed during (i) a backward digit span
task (digit), (ii) an auditory Stroop task (Stroop), and (iii) a func- (JNPT 2019;43: 3–11)
tional mobile phone task. Baseline descriptive, motor, and cognitive
variables were correlated with the change in dual-task gait velocity INTRODUCTION
after the intervention. Factors correlated with the change in dual-task
gait velocity postintervention (P < 0.20) were entered into a stepwise
forward multiple linear regression model.
T he DUALITY trial, a large-scale multicenter study on the
effects of dual-task (DT) training in people with Parkin-
son disease (PD), recently showed that 2 different training
Results: Lower dual-task gait velocity and higher cognitive capacity programs had equally positive effects on DT motor and cog-
(Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition [ScopaCog]) nitive performance without increasing fall risk.1 In this trial,
at baseline were related to larger improvements in dual-task gait consecutive task training (CTT, training 2 tasks separately) was
velocity after both integrated and consecutive dual-task training for contrasted with integrated task training (IDT, practicing 2 tasks
all 3 tasks (β[gait] = −0.45, β[ScopaCog] = 0.34, R2 = 0.23, P < simultaneously). Both regimens were assumed to improve DT
0.001, for digit; β[gait] = −0.52, β[ScopaCog] = 0.29, R2 = 0.26, outcomes via different mechanisms: (1) by increasing single-
P < 0.001, for Stroop; and β[gait] = −0.40, β[ScopaCog] = 0.30, task automaticity or fluidity (CTT) and 2) by improving actual
R2 = 0.18, P < 0.001, for mobile phone task). task integration (IDT). It was also expected that in IDT a better
Discussion and Conclusions: Participants with PD who showed a retention of practice would be found at the expense of a higher
slow dual-task gait velocity and good cognitive functioning at baseline fall risk, a hypothesis that was not confirmed. These very sim-
ilar outcomes of training bring up a clinical dilemma, namely
Neuromotor Rehabilitation Research Group, Department of Rehabilitation how clinicians can determine the best training approach for
Sciences, KU Leuven, Belgium (C.S., L.M., S.B., P.G., A.N., E.H.); and each individual. Therefore, this study aimed to inform clin-
Department of Neurology, Nijmegen Centre for Evidence Based Prac- ical decision-making by increasing insight into DT training
tice (E.A.M.) and Department of Neurology, Donders Institute for Brain, responses.
Cognition and Behaviour (B.R.B.), Radboud University Medical Center,
Nijmegen, the Netherlands. Thus far, a first exploratory analysis has been conducted
This work was funded by the Jacques and Gloria Gossweiler Foundation and to compare the training response of subgroups based on di-
the Malou-Malou funds of the King Baudouin foundation. Elke Heremans chotomous scoring of the presence of falls, gait freezing,
is a postdoctoral researcher and Sanne Broeder a PhD researcher of the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) stage II or III, or having cognitive im-
Research Foundation Flanders. The funding sources were not involved in
study design or execution. pairment (a cutoff score at the MoCA of 27). Results showed
This study presents results from 121 participants included in the DUALITY that no differences were apparent between these subgroups.1
trial conducted in 2 clinical centers in Belgium and the Netherlands, regis- Cross-sectional studies in participants with PD show that DT
tered on clinicaltrials.gov as NTC01375413. gait velocity, irrespective of training, is predicted by several
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
motor, cognitive, and personal factors.2 Our own recent study
Supplemental digital content is available for this article. Direct URL citation
appears in the printed text and is provided in the HTML and PDF versions identified the combination of single-task (ST) gait velocity
of this article on the journal’s Web site (www.jnpt.org). and the score on the Alternating Intake Test, a test of ex-
Correspondence: Elke Heremans, PhD, Department of Rehabilitation Sci- ecutive function, as the key factors determining DT gait ve-
ences, KU Leuven, Tervuursevest 101 bus 1501, 3001 Leuven, Belgium locity in PD.3 In persons with stroke, improvement in gait
(Elke.Heremans@kuleuven.be)
Copyright C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA.
velocity after training has been found to be associated with
ISSN: 1557-0576/19/4301-0003 age and gait velocity at the beginning of the intervention.4 In
DOI: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000247 older individuals, both ST and DT variabilities were important

JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019 3

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Strouwen et al JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019

in predicting DT practice effects.5 Furthermore, executive tive tasks (ie, a backward digit span task [digit] and an auditory
function (inhibitory control, mental set shifting, and atten- Stroop task [Stroop]) and 1 functional task (mobile phone task,
tional flexibility) appeared to be an important factor determin- requiring typing in the date of the test on a mobile phone) were
ing changes in ST mobility after 12 months of intervention.6 To used to assess DT performance while walking,19 with the in-
date, no studies have been performed in participants with PD to struction of focusing equally on both tasks. The backward digit
identify factors that determine the size of DT training effects. span task was trained during both types of dual-task training,
Cognitive dysfunction has previously been found to re- whereas both other tasks were untrained to capture the transfer
late to learning deficits in people with PD.7-9 Individuals with effects of training. Reaction times for these tasks were assessed
freezing of gait (FOG) were shown to suffer from more se- both under ST and DT conditions. Descriptors included (i) age,
vere cognitive dysfunction10-12 and consequently to experi- (ii) disease duration, (iii) history of recurrent falls in the previ-
ence greater deficits in learning than nonfreezers.11,13,14 The ous year (yes/no), (iv) levodopa-equivalent dose (LED),24 and
impaired learning profile of persons with FOG as well as the (v) presence of a deep brain stimulator (DBS) (yes/no). Cog-
fact that the frequency of FOG is known to increase under DT nitive scores consisted of (i) Mini-Mental State Examination
conditions15 predicts that IDT may be less effective in this sub- (MMSE),22 (ii) Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA),25
group of individuals. In addition, IDT may be contraindicated (iii) Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB),26 (iv) Scales for Out-
for individuals in the later disease stages and with a higher fall comes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition (ScopaCog),27 (v) Al-
risk to guarantee safety during training.12,16-18 ternating Names Test,28 (vi) Alternating Intakes Test,28 and
The main aim of this study was to identify which factors (vii) ST and DT reaction times on the cognitive tasks at base-
determine the change in DT gait velocity after DT training. line. Motor scores included (i) H&Y stage,21 (ii) Movement
Based on previous studies in other groups, gains in DT per- Disorders Society Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—
formance were hypothesized to be lower in those with initial part 3 (MDS-UPDRS-III),29 (iii) New Freezing of Gait Ques-
slower gait velocity, more FOG, more advanced disease stage tionnaire (NFOG-Q),30 (iv) Activities-specific Balance Con-
and age, and a higher prevalence of falling. The second aim of fidence scale (ABC),31 and (v) ST and DT gait velocity at
this study was to assess whether treatment benefits depended baseline.
on the type of DT training strategy that was used. As atten- Testing was repeated 4 times over a period of 6 months
tional resources were challenged to a greater extent in IDT, of follow-up and was performed in the gait laboratories of the
it was expected that participants with cognitive problems, in- Neuromotor Research Group at KU Leuven or of the Rad-
cluding those with FOG, and participants with an increased boud University Medical Center. Baseline performance was
risk of falling would benefit less from this type of training. assessed at 2 different times, separated by a control period of
6 weeks. After the second assessment, all participants under-
METHODS went 6 weeks of DT intervention. Immediately afterward, the
training effect was assessed. After 12 weeks of follow-up, a fi-
Participants nal assessment investigated potential retention effects. Change
This study presents results from 121 participants in- in gait velocity after training was calculated by subtracting the
cluded in the DUALITY trial conducted in 2 clinical centers in average DT gait performance during the 2 baseline tests from
Belgium and the Netherlands (registered on clinicaltrials.gov the DT gait performance during the first postintervention test.
as NTC01375413).19 Inclusion criteria were (a) diagnosis of
PD according to the UK Brain Bank criteria20 ; (b) H&Y stage Intervention
3 or less in the subjective best “on” phase of the medication As described in the DUALITY trial protocol,19 for all
cycle21 ; (c) being able to walk for 10 minutes continuously; (d) participants training consisted of 3 identical components: (1)
the presence of DT interference as established by a structured gait practice, (2) auditory cognitive exercise, and (3) functional
checklist19 ; (e) a score 24 or more on the MMSE22 ; (f) stable training. Gait practice involved at-home gait exercises, aimed
medication over the past 3 months or stable DBS settings over to improve gait quality. Exercises depended on the clinical
the past year; and (g) no hearing or visual problems that could need of the individual and included, for example, exercises
cause interference with testing or training. The study was eth- such as focusing on the taking big steps, focusing on heel
ically approved in both centers (Belgium: CME KU Leuven, strike, turning, and other key parameters of gait. Difficulty
B322201213165/S53419, and the Netherlands: CMO Regio level was gradually increased over the training program. Cog-
Arnhem-Nijmegen, NL39530.091.12) and participants signed nitive exercises addressed verbal fluency, discrimination and
an informed consent form before study participation. decision-making, working memory, mental tracking, and reac-
tion time. The intervention included four 30-minute practice
Predictors and Outcome Measures sessions per week, of which 2 were performed in the pres-
Testing was performed as described in the previously ence of a physical therapist who was specifically trained and
published protocol of the DUALITY trial.19 All indepen- experienced in treating individuals with PD. Participants were
dent variables were derived from the first baseline assessment randomly assigned to either CTT or IDT.1 During CTT, 15
and included disease descriptors, cognitive scores, and motor minutes of gait exercises, and 15 minutes of cognitive training
scores. In all participants, ST and DT gait velocity (cm/sec) at exercises, were provided while participants were seated on a
preferred speed was assessed before and after the intervention chair. In contrast, IDT consisted of motor-cognitive tasks dur-
by means of the GAITRite Walkway System,23 whereby walk- ing the entire session. All training sessions were performed
ing started and ended 1 m before/after the walkway. Two cogni- while participants where “on” medication.

4 
C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019 Determinants of Dual-Task Training Effect Size in Parkinson Disease

Statistical Analysis tual correlation higher than r = 0.70 were not entered into the
Data analysis was performed with IBM SPSS, version model to avoid multicollinearity.
22 (IBM Corp, Armonk, New York).32,33 Missing data were
due to sporadic dropout or technical issues and were not in- RESULTS
cluded in the analysis. A regression analysis was performed
to examine factors that determine the effect of DT training Descriptive Data
on the change in gait velocity while performing (1) digit, (2) Descriptive data are presented in Table 1. No differences
Stroop, and (3) mobile phone tasks. Following the main re- were found between the 2 training groups concerning descrip-
gression analysis, an additional subanalysis was performed to tive, cognitive, and motor predictors. As previously reported,
identify determinants in both types of training separately. Cor- the change in DT performance after training did not differ
relations between determinants and outcome variables were between the groups.1
assessed using simple linear regression models.32,33 For the
main analysis, as well as for the secondary subanalysis, fac- Change in DT Gait Velocity—Digit in All
tors were included into a stepwise forward multivariate linear Participants
regression model when they were correlated with the outcome Table 2 presents the factors that exhibit univariate cor-
variable, with a P value less than 0.20. Variables with a mu- relations at P < 0.20 with the outcome variable, and were

Table 1. Descriptive Data for Independent and Dependent Variables of the Total Group, the Consecutive Task Training
Group, and the Integrated Task Training Group
Total Group CTT IDT CTT vs IDT:
N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) N Mean (SD) P Value
Descriptive
Age, y 121 65.93 (9.22) 65 66.05 (9.30) 56 65.80 (9.19) 0.886
Disease duration, y 121 8.67 (5.83) 65 8.89 (6.30) 56 8.41 (5.29) 0.653
Falling in previous year, yes/no 121 40/81 65 22/43 56 18/38 0.843
LEDa , mg/24 h 121 610.00 [387.50-865.80] 65 710 [427.50-931.67] 56 612.99 (396.10) 0.060
DBSa , n 121 0 [0-0] 65 0 [0-0] 56 0 [0-0] 0.733
Cognitive
MMSE (0-30) 121 27.94 (1.59) 65 27.88 (1.65) 56 28.02 (1.53) 0.630
MoCAa (0-30) 121 26 [24-28] 65 25.71 (2.82) 56 25.93 (2.76) 0.665
FAB (0-18) 121 15.76 (1.99) 65 15.77 (1.97) 56 15.75 (2.03) 0.958
ScopaCog (0 – 43) 121 27.02 (5.67) 65 27.26 (5.36) 56 26.73 (6.05) 0.611
ANTa , s 120 24.19 [17.12-35.35] 65 24.00 [18.30-36.78] 55 24.38 [16.14-34.60] 0.202
AITa , s 120 35.01 [25.86-46.50] 65 39.00 [27.35-50.00] 55 32.50 [23.20-43.10] 0.255
ST reaction time, digit span 118 874.50 [559.17-1234.69] 63 852.52 [566.46-1204.32] 55 896.49 [525.53-1271.37] 0.694
taska , s
ST reaction time, Stroop task, s 119 1174.26 (201.98) 63 1177.80 (181.84) 56 1170.29 (224.10) 0.841
DT reaction time, digit span 117 795.48 [556.23-1168.84] 63 762.82 [520.31-1116.81] 54 940.84 (488.45) 0.444
taska , s
DT reaction time, Stroop task, s 118 1194.56 (199.24) 63 1191.05 (146.86) 55 1198.59 (247.41) 0.839
Motor
H&Ya (0-5) 120 2 [2-3] 65 2 [2-3] 55 2.00 [2-3] 0.691
MDS-UPDRS-III (0-132) 120 32.39 (12.79) 65 32.51 (13.71) 55 32.25 (11.73) 0.915
NFOG-Qa (0-28) 121 0.00 [0.00-13.00] 65 0.00 [0.00-13.00] 56 2.00 [0.00-14.00] 0.567
ABCa (0-100) 121 78.75 [66.88-92.50] 65 78.25 (17.50) 56 78.13 [61.72-92.34] 0.391
ST gait velocity, cm/s 119 106.70 (20.07) 64 107.38 (19.68) 55 105.91 (20.66) 0.692
DT gait velocity during digit 120 90.57 (20.56) 65 90.32 (19.14) 55 90.87 (22.30) 0.885
span task, cm/s
DT gait velocity during Stroop 121 92.07 (20.83) 65 92.63 (19.95) 56 91.43 (21.96) 0.752
task, cm/s
DT gait velocity during mobile 121 79.41 (22.08) 65 79.59 (22.17) 56 79.21 (22.18) 0.924
phone task, cm/s
Outcomes
Change in DT gait velocity 105 10.28 (14.55) 55 11.20 (14.34) 50 9.28 (14.86) 0.503
during digit span task, cm/s
Change in DT gait velocity 114 11.30 (13.51) 61 10.34 (13.56) 53 12.41 (13.50) 0.417
during Stroop task, cm/s
Change in DT gait velocity 114 7.96 (13.50) 61 9.30 (13.64) 53 6.41 (13.30) 0.256
during mobile phone task,
cm/s
Abbreviations: ABC, Activities-specific Balance Confidence scale; AIT, Alternating Intake Test; ANT, Alternating Names Test; CTT, consecutive task training; DBS, deep brain
stimulator; DT, dual task; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; H&Y, Hoehn and Yahr stage; IDT, integrated task training; LED, levodopa-equivalent dose; MDS-UPDRS-III, Unified
Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—motor part; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NFOG-Q, New Freezing of Gait Questionnaire;
ScopaCog, Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition; SD, standard deviation; ST, single task.
a
In the event of abnormally distributed data, the median [interquartile range] is provided.


C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA 5

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Strouwen et al JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019

Abbreviations: ABC, Activities-specific Balance Confidence scale; AIT, Alternating Intakes Test; ANT, Alternating Names Test; DBS, deep brain stimulator; DT, dual task; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; H&Y, Hoehn and
Yahr stage; LED, levodopa-equivalent dose; MDS-UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—motor part; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NFOG-Q, New Freezing of
included into a stepwise forward multivariate linear regres-

r = −0.37

r = −0.36

r = −0.27
P < 0.001

P < 0.001

P = 0.004
DT Gait
Velocity
sion model. The change in DT gait velocity during the digit
span task after training was correlated with the MMSE, MoCA,
FAB score, ScopaCog, ST and DT reaction time on the digit
r = −0.20

r = −0.18
P = 0.04

P = 0.06
Velocity
ST Gait

span task, MDS-UPDRS-III, NFOG-Q, and ST and DT gait


velocity during the digit span task at baseline. Stepwise for-
ward linear regression (Table 3) revealed that lower DT gait
velocity during the digit span task at baseline (β = −0.45;
ABC


Table 2. Total Group Results for Pearson Correlation Coefficients Resulting From the Univariate Analysis Significant at Level P < 0.20
Motor

P < 0.001) and higher performance on the ScopaCog, reflect-


ing better cognitive functioning (β = 0.34; P < 0.001), related
r = −0.14

r = −0.17
NFOG-Q

P = 0.15

P = 0.07

significantly to the change in DT performance after training


(R2 = 0.23; P < 0.001). The differences between the observed


and predicted values based on the multiple linear regression
UPDRS III
r = −0.16

r = −0.15
P = 0.11

P = 0.12
MDS-

analysis are shown in the Figure for the digit span (top), Stroop

(center), and mobile phone tasks (bottom).


H&Y

Change in DT Gait Velocity—Stroop in All


Participants
DT Reaction

P = 0.06
r = 0.19

Change in DT gait velocity during the Stroop task was


Time

correlated with the MMSE, MoCA, FAB score, ScopaCog,


MDS-UPDRS-III, NFOG-Q, and ST and DT gait velocity dur-
ST Reaction

ing the Stroop task at baseline (Table 2). After forward linear
P = 0.01
r = 0.27
Time

regression (Table 3), lower DT gait velocity during the Stroop


task at baseline (β = −0.52; P < 0.001), higher ScopaCog


performance (β = 0.29; P = 0.002), and lower MDS-UPDRS-
AIT

III scores, reflecting better motor function (β = −0.25; P =


0.005), remained significant determinants for change in DT


Gait Questionnaire; r, regression coefficient; ScopaCog, Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition; ST, single task.

gait velocity during the Stroop task after training (R2 = 0.26;
ANT


Cognitive

P < 0.001). The difference between the observed and pre-


dicted values based on the multiple linear regression analysis
ScopaCog

P = 0.03

P = 0.05

P = 0.08
r = 0.22

r = 0.18

r = 0.16

is shown in the Figure.

Change in DT Gait Velocity—Mobile Phone


P = 0.19

P = 0.08
r = 0.13

r = 0.17
FAB

in All Participants

Univariate analysis (Table 2) showed that the LED,


P = 0.03

P = 0.03

P = 0.03

MoCA, ScopaCog, and DT gait velocity during the mobile


r = 0.22

r = 0.21

r = 0.20
MoCA

phone task at baseline were correlated with change in DT gait


velocity during the mobile phone task after training. Forward
P = 0.10

P = 0.06
r = 0.16

r = 0.18
MMSE

linear regression (Table 3) revealed that lower DT gait veloc-


ity during the mobile phone task at baseline (β = −0.40; P <


0.001), higher ScopaCog performance (β = 0.30; P = 0.002),
and higher LED (β = 0.19; P = 0.03) significantly deter-
DBS

mined change in DT gait velocity during the mobile phone task


(R2 = 0.18; P < 0.001). The difference between the observed
P = 0.01
r = 0.23
LED

and predicted values based on the multiple linear regression


analysis is shown in the Figure.


Descriptive

Falls

Consecutive Versus Integrated Task Training


Group
Duration
Disease

Pearson correlation coefficients at P < 0.20 resulting


from the univariate analysis in consecutive and integrated task


training groups are shown in the Appendix. In the CTT group,
Age

significant determinants of change in DT gait velocity during


the trained digit span task (Table 4) were lower baseline DT gait
velocity during Stroop
velocity during digit

velocity during the digit span task (β = −0.37; P = 0.007) and


span task (n =103)

mobile phone task


Change in DT gait

Change in DT gait

Change in DT gait

higher performance on the ScopaCog (β = 0.31; P = 0.021)


velocity during
task (n = 112)

(R2 = 0.14; P = 0.007). Results for gait velocity were sim-


(n = 114)

ilar in the IDT group (R2 = 0.38; p < 0.001) (β = −0.51;


P < 0.001). However, in this group higher MoCA scores

6 
C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019 Determinants of Dual-Task Training Effect Size in Parkinson Disease

Table 3. Total Group Results From the Multiple Regression Analysis With Stepwise Forward Regression
Significance of
β [95%CI] Standardized β t Value P Value VIF R2 Model
Change in DT gait velocity during
digit span task (n =103)
Constant 14.08 [0.21 to 27.94] 2.02 0.05 0.23 <0.001
DT gait velocity during digit span −0.297 [−0.414 to −0.180] − 0.45 − 5.04 <0.001 1.07
task
ScopaCog 0.837 [0.404 to 1.270] 0.34 3.83 <0.001 1.07
Change in DT gait velocity during
Stroop task (n = 112)
Constant 32.01 [15.84 to 48.17] 3.93 <0.001 0.26 <0.001
DT gait velocity during Stroop task −0.33 [−0.44 to −0.22] − 0.52 − 5.83 <0.001 1.18
ScopaCog 0.68 [0.26 to 1.09] 0.29 3.22 0.002 1.19
UPDRS-III −0.26 [−0.44 to −0.08] − 0.25 − 2.85 0.005 1.15
Change in DT gait velocity during
mobile phone task (n = 114)
Constant 3.97 [−8.08 to 16.01] 0.65 0.52 0.18 <0.001
DT gait velocity during mobile −0.25 [−0.37 to −0.13] − 0.40 − 4.28 <0.001 1.22
phone task
ScopaCog 0.74 [0.28 to 1.19] 0.30 3.20 0.002 1.25
LED 0.006 [0.001 to 0.011] 0.19 2.24 0.03 1.03
Abbreviations: CI, confidence interval; DT, dual task; LED, levodopa-equivalent dose; MDS-UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—motor part; ScopaCog =
Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition; VIF, variance inflation factor.

(β = 0.45; P < 0.001) instead of performance on the Sco- ies in older individuals and persons with stroke.4,6 As well,
paCog determined better gains in DT gait velocity during the these results are supported by the relationship between cog-
digit span task. nition and DT gait velocity and between dual-task deficits
For the CTT group, change in DT gait velocity during and reduced movement automaticity in older individuals and
the untrained Stroop task (Table 4) was solely related to a in persons with PD.3,34-36 As a result, participants with PD
lower DT gait velocity during Stroop at baseline (β = −0.27; showed a larger increase in frontostriatal networks than healthy
P = 0.04) (R2 = 0.05; P = 0.04). In contrast, in the IDT controls.37 Participants with PD were previously shown not to
group change in DT gait velocity was related to lower DT gait be able to activate cerebellar areas involved in dual tasking to
velocity during the Stroop at baseline (β = −0.62; P < 0.001) the same extent as their healthy counterparts.38 Participants
as well as to higher performance on the ScopaCog (β = 0.35; with limited cognitive reserve, in particular, may no longer
P = 0.004) and lower MDS-UPDRS-III score (β = −0.26; have the cognitive resources to optimally gain from DT train-
P = 0.03) (R2 = 0.39; P < 0.001). ing. As well, cognitive dysfunction has been linked to learn-
In the CTT group, only a higher LED (β = 0.28; P = ing deficits in people with PD,7-9 particularly in participants
0.03) determined change in DT gait velocity during the mo- with FOG.10-14 These individuals have specific alterations in
bile phone task significantly (R2 = 0.06; P = 0.03). In the IDT functional connectivity in dual-task-related regions involving
group on the other hand, lower DT gait velocity during the mo- the precuneus and the striatum,39 and this may partly explain
bile phone task at baseline (β = −0.37; P = 0.006) and higher why cueing is of value for reducing FOG.40 In the current
MoCA score (β = 0.34; P = 0.01) were both significant deter- study, FOG severity correlated univariately with the outcome
minants (R2 = 0.17; P = 0.003) (Table 4). Overall, cognitive variables, but in contrast to our hypotheses, it was not main-
factors played an important part in predicting improvements tained as a primary factor determining the gains found after
in the IDT group. It is of note that a higher variance was ex- DT training. Instead, cognitive function, irrespective of FOG,
plained by the prediction models in the IDT group (R2 ranged was a consistent and significant determinant. The fact that the
between 0.17 and 0.39 compared with between 0.05 and 0.14 cognitive load was different between the 3 tasks may explain
in the CCT group). why different factors did not contribute equally to changes in
each of the tasks.
DISCUSSION Beyond cognition, DT training benefits were also deter-
The main result of the current study was that both motor mined by motor factors, including DT gait velocity at baseline
and cognitive baseline factors contributed to the prediction of and MDS-UPDRS-III scores. Contrary to our hypothesis,
DT training gains. More specifically, people with a lower DT people with PD with a low initial gait velocity showed the
gait velocity at baseline and better cognitive function were largest improvements after training. This can be explained by
more likely to experience a greater benefit. In contrast to our their larger potential to improve compared with people who
hypothesis, this finding was irrespective of the type of training started at higher levels and by the fact that the training level
(ie, IDT vs CTT) and type of dual-task outcome. was adapted specifically to the participants’ gait velocity,
The predictive effect of the motor and cognitive base- which was the primary outcome measure of the DUALITY
line factors is in line with our hypotheses and previous stud- trial. It is also in line with previous work by Strouwen et al3


C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA 7

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Strouwen et al JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019

were less likely to improve, particularly when performing


the auditory Stroop task. This is in line with previous trials
investigating the effect of exercise on other outcomes, such
as balance and upper limb movements, which indicate that
learning effects may be hampered in subgroups with greater
disease severity.41,42 One potential explanation for this finding
could be that individuals with greater disease severity have
a higher fall risk, and may as such deliberately walk slower
and/or focus less on the cognitive task as they may prioritize
safety over improvement under dual-task conditions. Future
interventions, including subgroup analyses comparing the
effects of interventions on participants with lesser versus
greater disease severity, would be extremely valuable in the
field of PD rehabilitation. Other factors, such as disease stage,
age, and prevalence of falling, did not contribute to the effect
size. As for the mobile phone task, LED was an additionally
important factor in determining training benefit, as higher
medication intake was associated with larger improvements in
the CTT group. Both the Stroop and mobile phone tasks were
untrained tasks and thus training benefits could be considered
as transfer effects. As generalization of learning is specifically
impaired in some subgroups of PD,14 this may explain why
a higher MDS-UPDRS-III score was found to be related
with less DT gains in the untrained Stroop task. Dopamine
replacement therapy has been previously demonstrated
to reinforce learning in persons with PD,43,44 which was
confirmed by the current results on the mobile phone task.
The second aim of the current study was to investigate
whether the factors determining DT training gains depended
on the type of practice. We hypothesized that IDT would de-
mand higher attentional load than CTT.45 Therefore, it was
expected that participants with cognitive problems, falling,
more advanced disease stage, and FOG would benefit less
from integrated compared with consecutive dual-task train-
ing. This hypothesis was only partially confirmed. We indeed
found that cognitive outcomes were related to training im-
provements more frequently after CTT than IDT Participants
with a higher cognitive status, measured by either MoCA or
ScopaCog, showed larger effects after IDT in both trained and
untrained tasks compared with participants with lower cogni-
tive scores. In contrast, falling, disease stage, and FOG were
not significant in determining benefits of IDT or CTT.
A limitation of the current study is that the explained
variance was low, and predicted outcomes corresponded only
moderately to the observed outcomes. Importantly, the predic-
tion of the explained variance was higher in the IDT group
compared with the CTT group. This finding indicates that DT
Figure. Differences between observed and predicted values training gains are also explained by other factors that were
based on the multiple linear regression analysis for 5 not included in this study, particularly in the CTT group. In
dependent variables (total group): digit span task (top), the DUALITY trial by Strouwen et al,1 it was found that the
Stroop task (center), and mobile phone task (bottom). total number of falls was higher in the CTT than in the IDT
group. As such, it is likely that fear of falling could have been
a contributing factor explaining the differences in explained
showing that single-task gait velocity had the highest associ- variance between groups. Furthermore, a previous study found
ation with dual-task gait velocity, suggesting that the loss of that depression was important in predicting DT gait interfer-
motor function is the most important explanatory factor of ence in PD.2 In addition, other factors such as motivation,
dual-task impairment in PD. In contrast, the higher MDS- activity level, training frequency, and training intensity should
UPDRS-III score correlated negatively with the DT training be taken into account in future studies predicting DT training
benefits, indicating that people with a greater disease severity effects.

8 
C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019 Determinants of Dual-Task Training Effect Size in Parkinson Disease

Table 4. Consecutive Task Training Versus Integrated Task Training Group Results From the Multiple Regression
Analysis With Stepwise Forward Regression
Consecutive Task Training Group Integrated Task Training Group
Significance Significance of
Standardized β P Value R2 of Model Standardized β P Value R2 Model
Change in DT gait velocity during
digit span task (n = 55/48)
DT gait velocity during digit span − 0.368 0.007 0.14 0.007 − 0.509 <0.001 0.38 <0.001
task
ScopaCog 0.311 0.02 – –
MoCA – – 0.450 <0.001
Change in DT gait velocity during
Stroop task (n = 61/53)
DT gait velocity during Stroop task − 0.265 0.04 0.05 0.039 − 0.623 <0.001 0.39 <0.001
ScopaCog – – 0.347 0.004
MDS-UPDRS-III – – − 0.263 0.03
Change in DT gait velocity during
mobile phone task (n = 61/53)
LED 0.281 0.03 0.06 0.028 – – 0.17 0.003
DT gait velocity during mobile – – − 0.367 0.01
phone task
MoCA – – 0.336 0.01
Abbreviations: DT, dual task; LED, levodopa-equivalent dose; MDS-UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—motor part; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment;
ScopaCog, Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition

Despite these limitations, the current study offers valu- REFERENCES


able information regarding the implementation of dual-task 1. Strouwen C, Molenaar EALM, Münks L, et al. Training dual tasks together
training in the rehabilitation of individuals with PD. Lower or apart in Parkinson’s disease: results from the DUALITY trial. Mov
Disord. 2017;32(8):1201-1210.
dual-task gait velocity at baseline and higher cognitive reserve 2. Rochester L, Nieuwboer A, Baker K, et al. Walking speed during single
were found to be the most important predictors of the train- and dual tasks in Parkinson’s disease: which characteristics are important?
ing effect for both IDT and CTT. As factors such as FOG, Mov Disord. 2008;23(16):2312-2318.
falling, and disease duration did not contribute significantly to 3. Strouwen C, Molenaar EA, Keus SH, et al. Are factors related to dual-task
performance in people with Parkinson’s disease dependent on the type of
improvements in gait velocity after training, dual-task training dual task? Parkinsonism Relat Dis. 2016;23:23-30.
should be offered to individuals with various clinical profiles, 4. Goldie PA, Matyas TA, Kinsella GJ, Galea MP, Evans OM, Bach TM. Pre-
including those with greatest disease severity. Disease severity diction of gait velocity in ambulatory stroke patients during rehabilitation.
measured by the MDS-UPDRS-III did not predict improve- Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80(4):415-420.
ments on the trained task after either CTT or IDT and on both 5. Strobach T, Gerstorf D, Maquestiaux F, Schubert T. Does initial perfor-
mance variability predict dual-task optimization with practice in younger
trained and untrained tasks after CTT. It had a significant effect and older adults? Exp Aging Res. 2015;41(1):57-88.
only on the untrained Stroop task after IDT. Transfer to un- 6. Gothe NP, Fanning J, Awick E, et al. Executive function processes predict
trained tasks was compromised after IDT in patients with lower mobility outcomes in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2014;62(2):285-290.
cognitive functioning, reflecting the inherent greater cognitive 7. Deroost N, Kerckhofs E, Coene M, Wijnants G, Soetens E. Learning
sequence movements in a homogenous sample of patients with Parkinson’s
challenge related to IDT in comparison to CTT. disease. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(10):1653-1662.
8. Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, Kerckhofs E. Does implicit learn-
ing in non-demented Parkinson’s disease depend on the level of cognitive
CONCLUSIONS functioning? Brain Cogn. 2009;69(1):194-199.
9. Price A, Shin JC. The impact of Parkinson’s disease on sequence learn-
DT gait velocity at baseline and cognitive function, as ing: perceptual pattern learning and executive function. Brain Cogn.
assessed by the MoCA and the ScopaCog scale for global 2009;69(2):252-261.
cognitive function, are the most important determinants of the 10. Amboni M, Cozzolino A, Longo K, Picillo M, Barone P. Freezing of gait
effect of DT training on DT gait velocity. Whereas individuals and executive functions in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord.
with PD seem to benefit from consecutive dual-task train- 2008;23(3):395-400.
11. Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, et al. Impaired implicit sequence
ing regardless of disease severity, their cognitive capacity and learning in Parkinson’s disease patients with freezing of gait. Neuropsy-
UPDRS-III scores are important factors to determine whether chology. 2013;27(1):28-36.
they would be eligible for integrated DT training. 12. Heremans E, Nieuwboer A, Spildooren J, et al. Cognitive aspects of freez-
ing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: a challenge for rehabilitation. J Neural
Transm (Vienna). 2013;120(4):543-557.
13. Vandenbossche J, Deroost N, Soetens E, et al. Freezing of gait in
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Parkinson’s disease: disturbances in automaticity and control. Front Hum
Neurosci. 2012;6:356.
We would like to thank all patients who participated in 14. Heremans E, Nackaerts E, Vervoort G, Broeder S, Swinnen SP, Nieuw-
the study. boer A. Impaired retention of motor learning of writing skills in


C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA 9

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Strouwen et al JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019

patients with Parkinson’s disease with freezing of gait. PLoS One. patients with early Parkinson’s disease: results from a multicenter clinical
2016;11(2):e0148933. trial. Mov Disord. 2002;17(4):758-763.
15. Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Desloovere K, Vandenberghe W, Kerckhofs 30. Nieuwboer A, Rochester L, Herman T, et al. Reliability of the new freezing
E, Nieuwboer A. Freezing of gait in Parkinson’s disease: the impact of of gait questionnaire: agreement between patients with Parkinson’s disease
dual-tasking and turning. Mov Disord. 2010;25(15):2563-2570. and their carers. Gait Posture. 2009;30(4):459-463.
16. Rascol O, Perez-Lloret S, Damier P, et al. Falls in ambulatory non- 31. Dal Bello-Haas V, Klassen L, Sheppard MS, Metcalfe A. Psychometric
demented patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Neural Transm (Vienna). properties of activity, self-efficacy, and quality-of-life measures in indi-
2015;122(10):1447-1455. viduals with Parkinson disease. Physiother Can. 2011;63(1):47-57.
17. Bekkers EMJ, Dijkstra BW, Dockx K, et al. Clinical balance scales indi- 32. Field A. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics. 4th ed. London,
cate worse postural control in people with Parkinson’s disease who exhibit England: Sage Publications; 2013.
freezing of gait compared to those who do not: a meta-analysis. Gait 33. Portney L, Watkins M. Foundations of Clinical Research: Application to
Posture. 2017;56:134-140. Practice. 3rd ed. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson-Prentice Hall; 2009.
18. Heinzel S, Maechtel M, Hasmann SE, et al. Motor dual-tasking deficits 34. Hausdorff JM, Schweiger A, Herman T, Yogev-Seligmann G, Giladi N.
predict falls in Parkinson’s disease: a prospective study. Parkinsonism Dual-task decrements in gait: contributing factors among healthy older
Relat Disord. 2016;26:73-77. adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2008;63(12):1335-1343.
19. Strouwen C, Molenaar EA, Keus SH, et al. Protocol for a randomized com- 35. Holtzer R, Verghese J, Xue X, Lipton RB. Cognitive processes related to
parison of integrated versus consecutive dual task practice in Parkinson’s gait velocity: results from the Einstein Aging Study. Neuropsychology.
disease: the DUALITY trial. BMC Neurol. 2014;14(1):61. 2006;20(2):215-223.
20. Hughes AJ, Daniel SE, Kilford L, Lees AJ. Accuracy of clinical diagno- 36. Wu T, Hallett M. A functional MRI study of automatic movements in
sis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain. 2005;128(10):2250-2259.
cases. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1992;55(3):181-184. 37. Gratwicke J, Jahanshahi M, Foltynie T. Parkinson’s disease dementia: a
21. Hoehn MM, Yahr MD. Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. neural networks perspective. Brain. 2015;138(6):1454-1476.
Neurology. 1967;17(5):427-442. 38. Gao L, Zhang J, Hou Y, Hallett M, Chan P, Wu T. The cerebellum in
22. Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Examination: a com- dual-task performance in Parkinson’s disease. Sci Rep. 2017;7:45662.
prehensive review. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1992;40(9):922-935. 39. Vervoort G, Heremans E, Bengevoord A, et al. Dual-task-related con-
23. Menz HB, Latt MD, Tiedemann A, Mun San Kwan M, Lord SR. Re- nectivity changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuroscience.
liability of the GAITRite R walkway system for the quantification of 2016;317:36-46.
temporo-spatial parameters of gait in young and older people. Gait 40. Spildooren J, Vercruysse S, Heremans E, Galna B, Verheyden G, Vervoort
Posture. 2004;20(1):20-25. G, Nieuwboer A. Influence of cueing and an attentional strategy on freez-
24. Deuschl G, Schade-Brittinger C, Krack P, et al. A randomized trial of deep- ing of gait in Parkinson disease during turning. J Neurol Phys Ther.
brain stimulation for Parkinson’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2006;355(9):896- 2017;41(2):129-135
908. 41. Stephan MA, Meier B, Zaugg SW, Kaelin-Lang A. Motor sequence learn-
25. Dalrymple-Alford JC, MacAskill MR, Nakas CT, et al. The MoCA: well- ing performance in Parkinson’s disease patients depends on the stage of
suited screen for cognitive impairment in Parkinson disease. Neurology. disease. Brain Cogn. 2011;75(2):135-140.
2010;75(19):1717-1725. 42. Canning CG, Sherrington C, Lord SR, et al. Exercise for falls pre-
26. Cohen OS, Vakil E, Tanne D, Molshatzki N, Nitsan Z, Hassin-Baer S. vention in Parkinson disease: a randomized controlled trial. Neurology.
The Frontal Assessment Battery as a tool for evaluation of frontal lobe 2015;84(3):304-312.
dysfunction in patients with Parkinson disease. J Getr Psychiatry Neurol. 43. McNeely ME, Duncan RP, Earhart GM. Medication improves bal-
2012;25(2):71-77. ance and complex gait performance in Parkinson disease. Gait Posture.
27. Kulisevsky J, Pagonabarraga J. Cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s dis- 2012;36(1):144-148.
ease: tools for diagnosis and assessment. Mov Disord. 2009;24(8):1103- 44. Nackaerts E, Vervoort G, Heremans E, Smits-Engelsman BC, Swinnen
1110. SP, Nieuwboer A. Relearning of writing skills in Parkinson’s disease: a
28. Hyde T, Fritsch T. Assessing executive function in Parkinson disease: the literature review on influential factors and optimal strategies. Neurosci
alternating names test. Part I. Reliability, validity, and normative data. Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(3):349-357.
Parkinsonism Relat Disord. 2011;17(2):100-105. 45. Strouwen C, Molenaar EA, Münks L, et al. Dual tasking in Parkin-
29. Siderowf A, McDermott M, Kieburtz K, Blindauer K, Plumb S, Shoulson son’s disease: should we train hazardous behavior? Expert Rev Neurother.
I. Test-retest reliability of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale in 2015;15(9):1031-1039.

10 
C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.

JNPT r Volume 43, January 2019

APPENDIX. Pearson Correlation Coefficients Resulting From the Univariate Analysis Significant at Level P < 0.20—Consecutive Versus Integrated Task
Training Group
Descriptive Cognitive Motor
Disease ST Reaction DT Reaction MDS-UPDRS ST Gait DT Gait
Age Duration Falls LED DBS MMSE MoCA FAB ScopaCog ANT AIT Time Time H&Y III NFOG-Q ABC Velocity Velocity

C 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA


Consecutive task training group
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – r = 0.19a – – r = 0.19 – r = 0.22 – – r = 0.27 r = 0.25 – – – – – r = −0.29
digit span task (n = 55) P = 0.17 P = 0.17 P = 0.11 P = 0.05 P = 0.07 P = 0.03
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – r = 0.224 – – r = 0.19 – – – – – – – – – – – r = −0.27
Stroop task (n = 61) P = 0.08 P = 0.14 P = 0.04
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – r = 0.281 – – – – – – – – – – – – – – r = −0.24
mobile phone task (n = 63) P = 0.03 P = 0.06
Integrated task training group
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – – – r = 0.26 r = 0.25 – r = 0.21 – – r = 0.31 – – – r =−0.19 – r =−0.34 r = −0.46
digit span task (n = 48) P = 0.06 P = 0.08 P = 0.14 P = 0.03 P = 0.19 P = 0.02 P = 0.001
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – – – r = 0.28 r = 0.23 r = 0.19 r = 0.25 – – – – – r =−0.19 r =−0.20 – r =−0.36 r = −0.46
Stroop task (n = 53) P = 0.04 P = 0.10 P = 0.18 P = 0.07 P = 0.18 P = 0.16 P = 0.01 P = 0.001
Change in DT gait velocity during – – – – – – r = 0.27 – r = 0.22 – – – – – – – – – r = −0.31
mobile phone task (n = 53) P = 0.05 P = 0.11 P = 0.03
Abbreviations: ABC, Activities-specific Balance Confidence scale; AIT, Alternating Intakes Test; ANT, Alternating Names Test; DBS, deep brain stimulator; DT, dual task; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; H&Y, Hoehn and
Yahr stage; LED, levodopa-equivalent dose; MDS-UPDRS-III, Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; r, regression coefficient; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; NFOG-Q, New
Freezing of Gait Questionnaire; ScopaCog, Scales for Outcomes in Parkinson’s Disease-Cognition; ST, single task.
a
Factors which were correlated in one group but not in the other are underlined.

Copyright © 2019 Academy of Neurologic Physical Therapy, APTA. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
11
Determinants of Dual-Task Training Effect Size in Parkinson Disease

You might also like