You are on page 1of 12

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/11929801

A review and evaluation of research on the deaf-blind from perceptual,


communicative, social and rehabilitative perspectives

Article  in  Scandinavian Audiology · February 2001


DOI: 10.1080/010503901300112176 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

49 1,490

2 authors, including:

Jerker Rönnberg
Linköping University
268 PUBLICATIONS   7,002 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Ease of Language Understanding View project

Sign language, perception and cognition View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Jerker Rönnberg on 30 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Jerker Rönnberg 1
Erik Borg2 A review and evaluation
of research on the
deaf-blind from
perceptual, communicative,
social and rehabilitative
perspectives
1
Department of Behavioural Sciences, This paper reviews research on deaf-blind individuals, primarily
Linköping University, and The from behavioral and communicative points of view. Inclusion in the
Swedish Institute for Disability population of deaf-blind is qualiŽ ed by describing a variety of
Research, Linköping and Örebro subgroups and genetically based syndromes associated with deaf-
Universities; 2Ahlsén Research blindness. Sensory assessment procedures – based primarily on
Institute, Örebro University, Örebro residual capacities – are appraised. Consequences for everyday life
Medical Centre Hospital, Sweden are described brie y. Non-sensory, alternative classiŽ catory
schemes and procedures are presented and the results from behavior
KEY WORDS: modiŽ cation procedures used for correcting maladaptive behaviors
alphabetic codes, deaf-blind, sensory are summarized. Methods for communicating tactilely are described
assessment procedures, sign and evaluated. Attention is also drawn to some suggestions regarding
acquisition, tactile communication learning of alphabetic codes and sign acquisition. Finally, sugges-
tions for future research are proposed.
RECEIVED/ACCEPTED:
June 20, 2000/October 26, 2000 Scand Audiol 2001;30:67– 77

ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE:


Jerker Rönnberg, Department of
Behavioural Sciences, Linköping
University, SE–581 83 Linköping,
Sweden.
Fax: ‡46 13 282145,
e-mail: JR@ibv.liu.se

Introduction functional assessment procedures are presented.


The literature on mobility and most work on
The aim of this review is to present primarily the technical aids for mobility are excluded since, to
last 10–20 years of behavioral and communica- a large extent, they focus on the isolated lack of
tive research on deaf-blind individuals. The vision. The next section (3) is a brief description
review is selective in the sense that we have of some general problems encountered in every-
focused on subject categories such as “deaf- day life; (4) alternative functional classiŽ cation
blind”, “Usher” and other genetically relevant and description of the functions of the deaf-blind
syndromes, combined with additional terms such are discussed; (5) intervention and (re)habilita-
as “perceptual”, “cognitive” and “social”. These tion issues and examples are presented and
concepts and terms have guided us in formal evaluated; (6) a section on various approaches
searches of databases (e.g. Eric, Medline, and of teaching speech, sign and textured symbols to
PsychINFO) and in the way that we sought to the deaf-blind is presented. Finally (7), the paper
uncover research along the continuum from is concluded by suggestions for future behavior-
residual signal processing capacities to commu- al research in the area.
nicative and rehabilitative issues.
The review is organized such that (1), a
characterization of the population and some of
the variety of its genetically determined sub-
1. Population
groups are presented and (2), some sensory and In the United States, where most of the research
ã 2001 Taylor & Francis
68 J Rönnberg and E Borg

on deaf-blind subjects has been carried out, the Usher’s syndrome has been subdivided into
legal deŽ nition of blindness is visual acuity for three types (Kimberling & Möller, 1995). Type I
distant vision 20/200 (corresponding to 0.1 in represents an autosomal recessive disorder
acuity in Nordic terms) for the better eye. The where the individuals are congenitally deaf
deŽ nition of deafness is that the threshold (bilaterally), inviting the use of sign language
exceeds 90 dB HL (for the 0.5–2 kHz range). from the very beginning of their linguistic
The prevalence of people being both deaf and development. Also, Type I individuals typically
blind in the United States is approximately have a defective balance organ, forcing them to
10 000. The etiology underlying this dual sensory rely on proprioception for walking. Type II
condition varies. It is also important to separate patients do have access to limited hearing from
those who were born deaf and blind from those birth (non-progressive), which will lead sponta-
who have acquired deaf-blindness as adults. They neously to a linguistic development based on
differ in many respects. The latter group dom- speech and speech-based communication
inates in number, but the former has a larger methods. Usher Type III individuals is a
variety of additional handicaps, often mental and category distinguished from Type II patients by
motor disorders, and requires substantial and a rapid progressive decline in hearing and vision.
individually tailored (re)habilitation programs. Very few studies have addressed the psycho-
The Nordic deŽ nition for being deaf-blind is that social implications of the Usher syndrome, but
“a person is deaf-blind when (s)he has a severe Miner (1995) has written an illustrative paper
degree of a combined loss of vision and hearing (based on interviews of 39 people with Usher
function”. It is further stated that deaf-blind people Type I) on the multiple adaptation problems
have great difŽ culty in obtaining information from associated with progressive RP. Usher indivi-
television, radio, newspapers and books, that the duals often experience depression in adolescence
individuals cannot use different kinds of support (Miner, 1995), and psychosis has been reported
and technical visual and hearing aids in a simple (Vernon, 1969). Also, relatively little attention
way. Deaf-blindness causes extreme difŽ culties in has been paid to the psychological sequelae of
school and further education, in work, family and deafness and blindness (e.g. Cooper, 1979).
social situations, as well as in partaking in cultural There are also other genetically determined
activities. Thus, the Nordic deŽ nition (NNH, 1993) syndromes such as CHARGE, where reports
stresses the functional features of seeing and suggest that the oculo-auriculo-vertebral spec-
hearing and acknowledges the fact that an exact trum is associated with heart malformation,
nominal deŽ nition is not generally accepted. Deaf- mental retardation and many other anomalies
blind interpreters are needed for communication (Tellier et al., 1998; VanMeter & Weaver,
with others and special support facilities are 1996). Deaf-blindness can also be part of
required in order for the deaf-blind to participate the deŽ nition of, for example, the Wolfram
in education. The number of deaf-blind people in syndrome, associated with juvenile diabetes
Sweden (a total population of nine million people) insipidus, diabetes mellitus, optic atrophy and
is estimated to be around 1200; 12% of them have sensorineural deafness (e.g. Barrett & Bundey,
total loss of vision and hearing, and 50% have 1997 for a review) and with the Yemenite deaf-
limited vision or hearing (Borg et al., 1999). If the blind hypopigmentation syndrome (Hennekam
condition is congenital, for example from mothers & Gorlin, 1996).
infected with rubella, many of these individuals However, there is a variety of other causes for
appear to be developmentally retarded. the majority of deaf-blind individuals in our
Usher’s syndrome is a genetic disease with society, including sensory losses due to the
congenital hearing loss or deafness, but with the ageing of sensory organs, dual sensory dysfunc-
cardinal symptom of progressive blindness due tion due to in icted brain damage and meningitis
to retinitis pigmentosa (RP). RP gives a chronic affecting an already-blind person, to mention
in ammation of the retina, which produces black some common causes.
patches affecting darkness vision, gives cataracts
and typically results in “tunnel vision” during
the teenage years. The size of the tunnel in
2. Sensory and functional assess-
“tunnel vision” typically varies between 5 and
ment procedures
10 degrees of visual angle. Relatively few studies that focus on sensory

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
Selective review of the deaf-blind 69

capacities in the deaf-blind have addressed In a related, but broader attempt at character-
clinical assessment procedures such as auditory izing the early communication abilities of deaf-
testing of deaf-blind, developmentally retarded blind individuals with orthopedic impairments,
children. Similarly, testing of brightness sensi- microtechnology was employed with reinforce-
tivity has also been carried out. This general ment techniques to help the child advance
approach may seem counterintuitive, as the through a series of levels of early communica-
children in question are more or less deaf or tion: from getting attention, requesting things,
blind. However, the point of these studies is to making choices and, Ž nally, using symbols to
utilize residual hearing or vision, although express preferences. Three cases were used to
limited, and to attempt to establish whatever illustrate the different paths that these indivi-
functional consequences there might be, and duals took to reach the Ž nal, early communica-
what remaining sensory capabilities can be tion level (Schweigert & Rowland, 1992).
utilized for compensation and rehabilitation. Obviously, operant conditioning techniques
In one of the studies (Friedlander et al., 1973), allow us to assess with some precision what
operant conditioning techniques were employed residual sensory capacities there are in deaf-
in such a way that the children (7.5 years) were blind, otherwise untestable children. This type of
to pull a lever in two ways, one giving sound as a information-gathering is of importance for the
reinforcer (a piece of music played at 80 dB assessment of boundary conditions that may
SPL), the other giving no sound. The children impose restrictions on intervention practices.
were tested both with and without hearing aids, In addition, the psychophysical properties of
and with and without tactile vibrator support, to the cutaneous senses, i.e. phantom sensations
examine the total time taken to receive a sound and movement illusions, can be used as a basis
reinforcer, as well as how selective they were in for assessment of sound localization in the deaf-
the choice of responses, i.e. how many times blind (Borg, 1997). Localization of sound by
music was chosen (in percentage of total means of some vibrator system can be almost as
choices). The procedure provides usable data effective as localization by hearing, given that
for children who would, in principle, be un- head movements are allowed (Richardson,
testable by means of conventional audiological 1982). In addition, work by Borg and colleagues
methods. (1999) has demonstrated that it is possible to
In a later study along the same theme of hard- optimize a vibrator system such that detection
to-test deaf-blind multihandicapped children, a with a Ž xed head position can be made. Further
scheme for behavioral observations has been research is needed here before the full clinical
developed where parents and teachers (care- implications of this work can be realized.
givers) check systematically for different types Furthermore, reports from successful cochlear
of behavior in relation to different types of implants in some subjects with deaf-blindness
auditory stimuli. This procedure is a useful aid have been reported several times in recent years.
when studying at which frequencies and inten- A patient with Bechet’s disease has beneŽ ted
sities the children respond, their most typical from implantation (Nucleus 22) (Szilvassy et al.,
responses, and their most intense responses 1998), and it has been shown in a larger sample
(Kersman & Napier, 1982). of deaf-blind that an implant is one of the most
Friedlander & Knight (1973) tested 16 re- worthwhile projects to entertain (Saeed et al.,
tarded, preschool post-rubella children with an 1998). Finally certain visual tests, such as the
operant procedure using potential preferences Fagan test, designed to assess higher-order
for lights of various intensity, compared to visual functions (i.e. a preferential looking
darkness. The procedure was very similar to that technique), have also been used to catch pre-
used by Friedlander et al. (1973). The results viously unnoticed residual visual capacities in
suggest that one can Ž nd a surprisingly high light mentally retarded subjects.
sensitivity in so-called “legally blind” children Thus, although there exist traditional auditory
who, when on their own, had no purposive and visual assessment procedures, as well as
behavior repertoire, but engaged only in self- various neurophysiological and imaging tech-
stimulatory behavior or were extremely hyper- niques (brain stem audiometry, fMRI and PET),
active. Nevertheless, their operant behaviors the agenda of assessing remaining capacities
were relatively precise. in the deaf-blind seems to put extraordinary

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
70 J Rönnberg and E Borg

demands on and stimulate the use of new it is not too difŽ cult to detect the direction of the
innovative assessment procedures. source of a tapping. Wooden  oors are good
transmitters of vibrations, and within a family
characteristic  oor vibrations may identify the
different family members.
3. Some everyday consequences of Outdoors, heavy trafŽ c and other loud en-
sensory losses vironmental sounds can be felt which could be
From a general behavioral and communicative facilitated with an in ated balloon or umbrella.
perspective, the lack of sensory information Use and training of remaining senses are
concerns a large variety of aspects of life, important. Particularly for mobility, any remain-
particularly those which are beyond arm’s ing vision may be extremely important to the
length. Clinical observation suggests that a deaf person. The breeze of the wind, the warmth
common complaint is that other people do not of a body, a radiator, sunlight, all give important
greet the deaf-blind individual or inform about environmental information. The proposal that
their presence. Telephone signals, doorbell some deaf-blind people can identify most in-
sounds and trafŽ c situations are other examples dividuals by their scent is, however, an exag-
of events where the deaf-blind receive little or no geration (Sauerburger, 1993). In a recent study,
information. Also, echolocation is not typically we (Rönnberg et al., submitted) have developed
used as a means of detecting objects and moving an interview instrument to assess the relative
in relation to objects in the surroundings. In importance of different kinds of sensory infor-
addition, the deaf-blind situation is sometimes mation used for detection and localization of
made even more difŽ cult by balance problems objects. Preliminary data suggest no self-evident
(Sauerburger, 1993). primacy of the odor sense (Rönnberg et al., sub-
Nevertheless, deaf-blind individuals may have mitted). Technical aids (e.g. vibrator matrices
some residual vision or hearing. The most for “seeing” (Bach-y-Rita, 1972, 1982) or
important sense may be even a narrow angle of vibrating elements for transmitting speech, e.g.
remaining vision. In some instances, this may be Tactaid 7) have been developed as aids for the
used for manual communication (see section 6 deaf or blind. However, it is not clear that they
for a separate treatise of communication modes). are optimal for the deaf-blind. Cochlear im-
Slight remaining audition may facilitate identi- plants, for example, are more important in most
Ž cation and localization, and echolocation can cases to the deaf-blind for monitoring of the
be used even if there is only remaining hearing in environment than for communication (Chute &
a narrow frequency range, not useful for speech Nevins, 1995; Miner, 1995).
communication. The kinesthetic and tactile Thus, it is important to note that although
senses are, however, the most important for the there may be examples of successful application
ability to move. These senses can be supported of technical aids to the deaf or the blind, the
by spatial memory representations (Jansson, combined loss puts extraordinary demands on
1999), which allows the individuals to walk ingenious engineering, drawing on an analysis of
through areas with which they are familiar. the everyday ecological demands on information
The ability of blind and deaf-blind people to processing. In this sense, we believe, research is
detect or avoid objects has been subjected to still in its infancy.
considerable research and speculation. The
hypothesis of a special sense, sometimes named
“facial vision”, was Ž nally disproved in 1947 by
Worchel & Dallenbach. They found that this
4. Alternative functional classiŽ ca-
well-documented ability was due to remaining
tion and description
vision and hearing, and not to some mystical In one major study (Dunlap, 1985) it was
other sense. After sufŽ cient blindfolding and investigated whether the deaf-blind can be
occlusion of the ear canals no ability to avoid functionally better classiŽ ed and described on
objects remained. However, the cutaneous the basis of certain activity dimensions rather
senses can be used for detection, localization than hearing and vision status. A large relatively
and to some extent identiŽ cation of events in the heterogeneous sample of 251 deaf-blind indivi-
surroundings. By placing two hands on the table duals was described along 11 activity dimen-

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
Selective review of the deaf-blind 71

sions, based on an instrument of 199 skill or (Kiernan & DuBose, 1974) is inspired from
activity statements (Dunlap et al., 1982). A several other sources. Although reliability and
cluster analysis revealed that all individuals validity estimates have been reported to be
were best clustered into three groups. Those relatively satisfactory, the sample sizes for these
activity variables that counted most when dis- alternatives have been limited (Sisson et al.,
criminating high from intermediate from low 1987). In some instances, typical assessment
functioning individuals were motor, linguistic, procedures developed for the blind or for the
leisure and social activities (in that order), rather deaf have been adopted, but they obviously lack
than the speciŽ c auditory and visual deŽ cits. the genuine comparability that one seeks.
ClassiŽ cation of individuals based on multi- In addition to these instruments, there is a
variate discriminant analysis resulted, on aver- body of research on play in handicapped
age, in 84% correctly classiŽ ed individuals, i.e. children, which suggests that the relationships
assigning each individual a membership in one among the children’s play level and cognitive/
of the three groups. This information provides an communicative processes can be relatively well
interesting basis from which to depart when understood. In this respect, Fewell & Rich
different (re)habilitation activities are planned, (1987) found surprisingly high correlations
taking into account other disabling conditions among a set of play assessment procedures and
than vision and hearing loss. It was also obvious cognitive, communicative and social measures
from the study that at least part of the functional in 17 deaf-blind children.
classiŽ cation data could be explained on the Still other methodological solutions include
basis of presence or absence of, for example, behavior checklists and observational tech-
cerebral palsy or mental retardation. Further niques. Here, a variety of developmental scales
research must take into account the complex has been created at the different deaf-blind
interactions between several disabling condi- institutes in the United States. However, there
tions, other skills, and the sensory deŽ cits. exist several problems with these behavior
Related to these general assessments of the checklists, one of which is quantiŽ cation. Some
deaf-blind individual is also functional intellec- of the problems have been overcome by using
tual/cognitive assessment. For several reasons, the standard American Association on Mental
normal IQ-testing is not appropriate. Operant DeŽ ciency Adaptive Behavior Scale (AAMD-
techniques presented above seem far-fetched and ABS) with the deaf-blind (Sisson et al., 1987).
would not catch the subtleties of cognition. Direct observation of behavior has also been
Many problems are connected to IQ-assess- used, with ratings of videotaped sequences along
ments. There is no easy way of gathering different dimensions (e.g. Wolf-Schein, 1993).
normative data for the multiply disabled. Norms Thus, the overall recommendation in the area
presuppose similar testing conditions and simi- is to use combinations of standardized tests,
larities across a number of background variables developmental scales and direct observation to
among individuals. This is hard to achieve for obtain a reasonably balanced overall assessment
the deaf-blind population which is heteroge- of a deaf-blind individual. This is reasonable,
neous both in terms of life-experiences, cogni- given the various methodological problems
tive consequences due to variations in residual associated with each cognitive/intellectual as-
sensory capacities, as well as their impact on sessment procedure.
intellectual and linguistic development, not to
mention the practical problems of test adminis-
tration (Sisson et al., 1987).
Despite the multitude of methodological
5. (Re)habilitation and intervention
problems, there exist some alternative intellec- The predominant therapeutic method in the
tual assessment methods that deserve mention- context of rehabilitation and intervention is the
ing in this review. First, the Deaf-Blind Program behavior modiŽ cation set of techniques. These
and Ability Screening Test (Henry & Lyall, techniques have in common that they build on
1973), based on Gesell’s developmental theory modiŽ cation of behavior through operant con-
(which is actually very much about motor ditioning (reinforcing certain behaviors but not
development), whereas the second instrument, others) and classical conditioning (associating an
the Peabody Intellectual Performance Scale event with an aversive consequence). The target

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
72 J Rönnberg and E Borg

behaviors for modiŽ cation are the various the interesting features of this issue is a proposed
maladaptive routines and behaviors often dis- Ž ve-stage model of the disease and the issue of
played by the deaf-blind, the inability to acquire early vs late detection of the disease, and
social and self-help skills, as well as the problem associated counseling for vocational careers,
of not having a vocation. The research examples genetic family counseling, etc. It is known that
vary widely (Sisson et al., 1987, for a review). Usher patients, to a higher degree than congeni-
In one relatively recent study, a behavior tally deaf-blind individuals, experience psycho-
modiŽ cation procedure was used to treat a 25- logical problems (e.g. Miner, 1995; Vernon,
year-old, deaf-blind, mentally retarded man for 1969). This can be understood on the grounds
his self-choking behavior. Water misting of the that awareness of an initially relatively intact
subject’s face was used as an aversive condition- function (i.e. vision in this case) – which is
ing technique, combined with a verbal “no” successively deteriorating – is more traumatic,
every time the subject was about to choke. With compared to the case when the individuals were
this procedure, they succeeded in eliminating his born without a particular function. Here, the
self-destructive behavior. He was free from self- individual has no “normal” state with which to
choking at an 8-month follow-up test (Peine et compare, hence reducing the psychological
al., 1991). distress involved in losing the function.
Acquisition of adequate self-feeding skills in Thus, in general it has been suggested that
the deaf-blind have also been addressed by educational work on learning skills and activities
means of different types of reinforcement with deaf-blind children should follow three
procedures, even if the research in this particular stages: (a) a co-active stage, the adult and child
subarea is relatively sparse (Luiselli, 1993). The working together as one; (b) the co-operative
latter has also used reinforcement plus “time- stage, the adult performing the activity alongside
out” procedures in the treatment of hitting, the child; and (c) the reactive stage, where the
punching and scratching in rubella patients child can perform the activity independently.
(= children). Other examples of modiŽ cation of The general curriculum may be split into Ž ve
aggressive behaviors include the modiŽ cation of main areas: (1) communication skills, (2) cog-
chronic spitting (Lawrence & Drabman, 1984). nitive development, (3) social and emotional
In a further study on two deaf-blind cases, the development, (4) motor and self-care skills and
use of positive reinforcement procedures (tokens (5) sensory development (Murdoch, 1986).
to buy soda pop) has demonstrated that it is
possible to modify behavior in such a way that
the frequency of social and object-relevant target
behavior increased, whereas the frequency of
6. Linguistic and communicative
self-injury and self-stimulating behaviors de-
aspects
creased (van Hasselt et al., 1989). Sisson et al. The majority of the research conducted on the
(1993) went even further to examine the linguistic and communicative abilities of the
potential transfer effects of behavior modiŽ ca- deaf-blind concerns the so-called ‘Tadoma’
tion techniques that emphasize differential re- method. The Tadoma user places his/her hand
inforcement of adaptive behaviors. It was shown on the speaker’s face with the thumb held lightly
that, for example, time on-task and behaviors in against the mouth of the speaker with the other
a vocational setting could be improved. Other Ž ngers spread over the neck and jaw. A skilled
domains of research include modiŽ cation of Tadoma user can comprehend connected speech
severe sleeping problems in the multiply dis- with an accuracy that allows for a relatively easy
abled (Lancioni et al., 1999) and efforts at  ow of conversation by monitoring lip and jaw
training orientation ability in the deaf-blind movements, air ow from the mouth and vibra-
(Lancioni et al., 1990). tions of the neck (Reed et al., 1992; see Reed,
Although relatively little attention has been 1995, for an overview). However, none of these
drawn to the rehabilitative aspects of Usher studies addresses cognitive or social conditions
patients, in 1978 American Annals of the Deaf explicitly. Focus is on purely perceptual, lin-
had already published a special issue on these guistic, and to some extent communicative,
types of problems (McCay et al., 1978), with a variables.
particular emphasis on the family unit. One of Some of the results are summarized by Reed

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
Selective review of the deaf-blind 73

(1995): a Tadoma user’s speech perception can letters. For example, A can be coded on the left
be compared with the speech perception which is Ž fth Ž ngernail by means of a short tap, B by
the case for normal hearing individuals in means of a longer pressure in the same location,
conditions where the signal-to-noise ratio is 0– and C on the same Ž nger by a short tap between
6 dB. Segmentally, a Tadoma user can perceive the second and third joints (Thurlow, 1986).
phonemes deŽ ned by manner of articulation Research on the development of mechanical
rather than place of articulation. Perceptual cues Ž nger spelling hands (i.e. an aid for learning the
for Tadoma users are: larynx vibrations, lip manual alphabet) for people who are deaf-blind
movements, jaw movements and air puffs/ has also been reported (Jaffe, 1994).
streams. By means of this method, the deaf- Further linguistic research emphasizes sign
blind have no apparent problems in developing language and how signs can be communicated
appropriate grammatical distinctions (Chomsky, tactilely. In one study, tactile iconicity of signs
1986). Intelligibility of the Tadoma user’s has been investigated for deaf, blind, normal
speech production (of sentences) is in the range hearing and visually impaired individuals. Ico-
of 60–70%, the rate of speech is about half that nicity refers not only to the physical similarity
for a normal hearing individual, the fundamental between a sign in the hand and an external object
frequency variation is limited, across a sentence or referent, but is also related to the associative
the pause-to-vocalization onset time is extre- values that exist between signs and referents.
mely high. Generally, speech production of the Some signs have, for different reasons, different
deaf-blind is reminiscent of the speech produc- evocative powers with respect to lexical entries.
tion of a congenitally deaf speaker (Monsen, Iconicity has also proved to be an important
1978). Naive subjects use Tadoma as effectively factor with regard to acquisition of sign language
as experienced users with regard to simple for autistic and for developmentally retarded
discrimination tasks. These results compare well children (e.g. GrifŽ th & Robinson, 1980; Kon-
with independent data on so-called ‘Tactiling’, stantareas et al., 1978; Lloyd & Fristoe, 1978). It
where subjects read speech by means of applying has also been shown that there is high agreement
the hand onto the speaker’s throat/collarbone. among raters of this sign language variable for
The initial effect on such tactilely supplemented the blind and for the normally sighted. For the
lip-reading is direct and is not substantially deaf-blind, recommendations are put forward
improved by means of training (cf. Öhngren et with respect to the order in which signs should be
al., 1992; Rönnberg, 1993, 1995; Rönnberg et taught tactilely (e.g. verbs are higher on tactile
al., 1998). iconicity). There are also indications as to the
It should be noted that the research on Tadoma developmental sequence in which signs are
and Tadoma-related perception is based on acquired (GrifŽ th et al., 1983; GrifŽ th &
speech as the linguistic source of information. Robinson, 1984). What seems to be a relevant
There exist several other means of transferring future task for research in this context is a project
linguistic information for the deaf-blind. Here, which properly scrutinizes the developmental
further research on the abilities of the deaf-blind sequence for the deaf-blind, and which experi-
has been carried out in connection with percep- mentally evaluates different methods of sign
tion of tactile discriminability of alphabetic acquisition from the perspective of concept
codes. One of the results is that it is relatively formation and communicative ability in general.
harder to learn to discriminate Braille script Interesting Swedish research on turn-taking in
than, for example, the Moon code. Braille is dialogues between deaf-blind persons has re-
based on raised dots in one or more of six cently been published (Mesch, 1998).
positions in a standard Braille cell, while the Related to this research is the notion of co-
Moon-code consists of Ž ve forms which can active signing (i.e. taking the hand of the child to
represent most of the letters in the alphabet by form the sign) which, in fact, can lead to
presenting the forms in different spatial orienta- interactive signing between the deaf-blind and
tion. For deaf-blind individuals without experi- the tutor (i.e. the sensory impaired person feels
ence of letter-shapes, it was found that a simple the sign in his hand, and then responds with a
tactile T-code was the most effective and easy sign; Watkins & Clark, 1991). Here, the material
way to learn to communicate. The T-code is (i.e. a vocabulary sample, which is disseminated
arranged such that a Ž nger can represent three throughout the United States) is on video and has

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
74 J Rönnberg and E Borg

been constructed with respect to parameters such Thus, the literature offers several interesting
as function and meaningfulness of words for lines of enquiry but so far lacks generality. Case
children and families: words found in early studies, which seem to be the most common way
vocabularies of non-disabled children, words of reporting research in the area, by deŽ nition
with signs that are concrete and iconic, etc. It is impose severe restrictions on the Ž ndings ob-
urged that training with such vocabularies tained; but even with such restrictions, the
should stimulate the child to generate new signs research is informative and poses both theore-
and move from co-active to interactive signing. tical and practical implications. One theme,
Further research along these lines has been recurring across studies, is the suggestion that,
reported recently, where four co-active move- despite the severe intellectual and sensory
ment phases are stressed for making possible the limitations of the deaf-blind population, rela-
transition to acquisition of language: resonance, tively high competence can be achieved in
co-active movement, non-representational refer- different ways. Here, the Tadoma method
ence and deferred imitation (Wheeler & GrifŽ n, represents a challenge for the deaf-blind indivi-
1997). dual with some experience of speech (e.g. early
Still further communication research has meningitis) at one end of a continuum of
addressed the issue of acquiring linguistic severity, and textured symbols for the individual
capacities from the perspective of textured with severe intellectual impairments at the other
symbols. In one particular report (Murray- end of the continuum. Research on tactile signs
Branch et al., 1991), two individuals with severe and co-active signing would fall somewhere
intellectual (1.5 years of mental age) and dual between these endpoints. Thus, a pertinent issue
sensory impairments were taught textured sym- in the context of different communication
bols. A textured symbol is deŽ ned as a paired methods for the deaf-blind would be the evalua-
association between a texture and an object or tion of relative efŽ cacy among communication
activity. The symbol is easy to discriminate, methods.
permanent and instantaneously discernable
through touch, without unnecessary tactile ex-
ploration (Gibson, 1966). It need not be oriented
in space to be correctly perceived; the texture is
7. Concluding discussion
uniform across the surface area. For example, From an international perspective, the popula-
corrugated cardboard can be associated with tion of deaf-blind has received little research
cola, a sponge with an orange, sandpaper with attention. The research on the deaf-blind can be
grapes, etc. An ecological inventory was used to characterized by heterogeneity in subject popu-
determine the individual’s expressive needs. lation, methodology and scientiŽ c ambition.
Teaching occurred frequently (a) by establishing This typically manifests itself in case studies,
the association between textured symbol and whether it concerns research on communication
referent, (b) by using the symbols for requesting methods or behavior modiŽ cation studies. There
objects and (c) by using the symbols for choice- exist larger studies, but then they are typically
making and to establish discrimination between descriptive and not causally oriented.
textures in the context of functional activities. One important general constraint is the type of
This research is interesting since it points to the deaf-blindness under scrutiny. In principle, one
fact that the individuals could actually use turn- can separate three classes of deaf-blind indivi-
taking during requesting (handing over a texture duals: congenitally deaf-blind (including a vari-
to the intervenor as part of a request), despite the ety of syndromes), Usher (congenitally deaf,
fact that they were assessed as having an becoming blind) and adventitiously deaf and
intelligence only equivalent to the sensory- blind adults. This is not always acknowledged in
motor level. Sensory-motor intelligence should the studies cited, which presumably leads to
not, according to traditional Piagetian (1952) important constraints on generalization – in
developmental psychology, allow for symbol- addition to the other functional dimensions and
use and communicative turn-taking. According disabling conditions characterizing differences
to Piaget (1952), this ability to decenter and to among deaf-blind individuals.
take the role of another person should appear Although methodological limitations seem
much later in a normal child’s development. pervasive from the perspective of experimental

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
Selective review of the deaf-blind 75

and behavioral science, there are still many 5. The Tadoma method has received relatively
interesting leads that can be utilized for further large research efforts. However, a study that
research. We will mention a few examples, compared Tadoma with other natural tactile
which build on some of the central issues in speech perception methods such as Tactiling
the review. (which essentially represents time-intensity
tactile complements to lip-reading) would be
1. Further research on the potential of tactile very informative. It would be of help to
detection and localization of sound sources initially assess the rate at which propositions
should be pursued to the level of clinical are speech-read using the two methods, and
application. Research on the relative impor- also to track longitudinally the learning
tance of different sensory strategies for curves.
detection and localization of events and 6. Suggestions in the literature that certain
objects has just begun and should be pursued. tactile signs should be easier to acquire than
The development of techniques that reveal others should be investigated further on the
undiscovered, high-level sensory capacities deaf-blind themselves within a longitudinal
seems particularly challenging. The complex- framework – and on a larger sample of
ity of the interaction between the combined individuals, preferably in comparison with
sensory loss, high-level capacities, interven- the natural development of sign acquisition in
tion technique and technical aids for commu- the congenitally deaf. This would answer the
nication in a real-life setting is beginning to be important questions raised with respect to the
recognized. The research is still very much in “natural” unfolding of sign acquisition in
its infancy. Here, we also believe that modern children (cf. Petito & Marentette, 1991). This
information technology offers promise for would also have implications for intervention
detection and localization based on other and teaching sign to the deaf-blind.
types of concepts, e.g. in “smart” environ-
ments.
2. Research on the various bases for classi-
Ž cation (e.g. sensory vs activity-based) is
important, as they imply very different Acknowledgements
(re)habilitation methods and foci of interven- Writing of this paper was supported by a grant to
tion. Activity-based approaches transcend the Ž rst author from the Swedish Council for
some of the methodological problems im- Social Research (95-0023:1B), and by a grant to
posed by assessment of perceptual and in- the second author from the Swedish Council for
tellectual capacities. They emphasize instead Social Research (94-0053:3C). The helpful
the social and adaptive aspects of behavioral comments by two anonymous reviewers are
development, and the vital importance of gratefully acknowledged.
scrutinizing cognitive and higher-order pre-
requisites that foster genuine dialogue.
3. Related to point 2, we have seen no attempts
at evaluating combinations of behavioral References
modiŽ cation methods with different kinds of 1. Bach-y-Rita P. Brain mechanisms in sensory substitu-
communication methods. Such multi-group, tion. New York: Academic Press, 1972.
2. Bach-y-Rita P. Sensory substitution in rehabilitation.
comparative control studies should be encour- In: Illis L, Sedgwick M, eds. Rehabilitation of the
aged, perhaps inspired by activity-based neurological patient. Oxford: Blackwell ScientiŽ c
classiŽ cation systems. Publications, 1982:361– 83.
3. Barret TG, Bundey SE. Wolfram’s (DIMOAD) syn-
4. The successful turn-taking behavior of se- drome. J Med Genet 1997;3:838– 41.
verely mentally retarded deaf-blind children, 4. Borg E. Cutaneous senses for detection and localization
using textured surfaces, challenges traditional of environmental sound sources: a review and tutorial.
Scand Audiol 1997;26:195– 206.
cognitive theory on the development of 5. Borg E, Rönnberg J, Neovius L. Monitoring the
symbolic capacities. A continued “dialogical” environment: sound localization equipment for deaf-
perspective on these capacities can have the blind people. Acta Otolaryngol 1999;119:146– 9.
6. Chomsky C. Analytic study of the Tadoma method:
potential of further discovering hidden com- language abilities of three deaf-blind subjects. J Speech
municative capacities. Hear Res 1986;29:332– 47.

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
76 J Rönnberg and E Borg

7. Chute PM, Nevins J. Cochlea implants in people who frequency spitting in a multiply handicapped and
are deaf-blind. J Vis Impairment Blindness 1995:297. mentally retarded adolescent. Child Fam Behav Ther
8. Cooper AF. The psychological problems of the deaf 1984;6:45– 55.
and blind. Scott Med J 1979;24:105– 7. 29. Lloyd L, Fristoe M. Transparency of manual signs used
9. Dunlap WR. A functional classiŽ cation system for the with individuals having severe communication impair-
deaf-blind. Am Ann Deaf 1985;130:236– 43. ment. Paper presented at the 11th Gatlinburg Con-
10. Dunlap WR, Evelsizer RL, McQuiddy D, Powers AR. ference on Research in Mental Retardation, Gatlinburg,
Severely handicapped progress inventory. University, TN, March 8, 1978.
AL: College of Education, University of Alabama, 30. Luiselli JK. Training self-feeding skills in children who
1982. are deaf and blind. Behav Modif 1993;17:457– 73.
11. Fewell RR, Rich JS. Play assessment for examining 31. McCay V, Craig WN, Letourneau SN. Am Annal Deaf
cognitive, communication, and social skills in multi- 1978.
handicapped children. J Psychoeduc Assess 1987;2: 32. Mesch J. Sign Language in tactile form (TeckenspraÊ k i
107–18. taktil form). PhD thesis, Stockholm University, 1998.
12. Friedlander BZ, Knight DA. Brightness sensitivity and 33. Miner ID. Psychosocial implications of Usher syn-
preference in deaf-blind retarded children. Am J Ment drome, Type I, throughout the life cycle. J Vis
DeŽ c 1973;78:323– 30. Impairment Blindness 1995; May/June:287– 96.
13. Friedlander BZ, Silva DA, Knight MS. Selective 34. Monsen RB. Toward measuring how well hearing-
responses to auditory and auditory-vibratory stimuli impaired children speak. J Speech Hear Res 1978;
by severely retarded deaf-blind children. J Aud Res 21:197–19.
1973;13:105– 11. 35. Murdoch H. Helping the deaf-blind child in class. Br J
14. Gibson J. The senses considered as perceptual systems. Spec Educ 1986;13:75– 7.
Boston: Houghton-Mif in, 1966. 36. Murray-Branch J, Udavari-Solvner A, Bailey B.
15. GrifŽ th P, Robinson JH. A comparative and normative Textured communication systems for individuals with
study of the iconicity of signs rated by three groups. severe and intellectual and dual sensory impairments.
Am Ann Deaf 1980;126:440– 9. Language Speech Hearing Serv Schools 1991;22:260–
16. GrifŽ th PL, Robinson JH. Perceptions of tactile and 8.
visual iconicity by blind and sighted groups. J Vis 37. Nordiska nämnden för handikappfraÊ gor (NNH). Döv-
Impairment Blindness 1984;78:481– 7. blindas livsvillkor i Norden inför aÊr 2000. Vällingby:
17. GrifŽ th PL, Robinson JH, Panagos JH. Tactile NNH, 1993:8–93.
iconicity: signs rated for use with deaf-blind children. 38. Peine HA, Liu L, Blakelock H, Jenson WR, Osborne
TASH J 1983;8:26– 38. JG. The use of contingent water misting in the
18. Hennekam RCM, Gorlin RJ. ConŽ rmation of the treatment of self-choking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry
Yemenite (Warburg) deaf-blind hypopigmentation syn-
1991;22:225– 31.
drome. Am J Med Genet 1996;65:146– 8.
39. Petito LA, Marentette P F. Babbling in the manual
19. Henry V, Lyall JH. Ability screening and program
mode: evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science
placement for deaf-blind children and adults. Volta Rev
1991;251:1493– 6.
1973;75:227– 31.
40. Piaget J. The origins of intelligence in children. New
20. Jaffe DL. Evolution of mechanical Ž ngerspelling hands
for people who are deaf-blind. J Rehabil Res Dev 1994; York: W. W. Norton, 1952.
31:236–44. 41. Potenski DH. Use of black light in training retarded,
21. Jansson G. Spatial orientation and mobility of the multiply handicapped, deaf-blind children. J Vis
visually impaired. In: Silverstone B, Lang MA, Impairment Blindness 1983;77:347– 8.
Rosenthal B, Fraye EE, eds. The Lighthouse handbook 42. Reed CM, Durlach NI, Delhorne LA. Natural methods
on visual impairment and rehabilitation. New York: of tactual communication. In Summers IR, ed. Tactile
The Lighthouse and Oxford University Press, 1999. aids for the hearing impaired. London: Whurr Publish-
22. Kersman SM, Napier D. Systematic procedures for ers, 1992:218– 30.
eliciting and recording responses to sound stimuli in 43. Reed CH. Tadoma: an overwiev of research. In: Plant
deaf-blind multihandicapped children. Volta Rev 1982; G, Spens KE, eds. Profound deafness and speech
84:226–37. communication. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd, 1995.
23. Kiernan DW, DuBose RF. Assessing the cognitive 44. Richardson B. Using the skin for purpose of sound
development of pre-school deaf-blind children. Educ localization. In: Gatehouse RW, ed. Localization of
Visual Handicap 1974;6:103– 5. sound. Amphore Press, 1982:155– 68.
24. Kimberling WJ, Möller C. Clinical and molecular 45. Rönnberg, J. Cognitive characteristics of skilled tactil-
genetics of Usher syndrome. J Am Acad Audiol ing: the case of GS. Eur J Cogn Psychol 1993;5:19– 33.
1995;6:63– 72. 46. Rönnberg J. Perceptual compensation in the deaf and
25. Konstantareas MM, Oxman J, Webster CD. Iconicity: blind: myth or reality? In: Dixon R, Bäckman L, eds.
effects on the acquisition of sign language by autistic Compensating for psychological deŽ cits. Hillsdale, NJ:
and other severely dysfunctional children. In: Siple P, Lawrence Erlbaum, 1995:251–74.
ed. Understanding language through sign language 47. Rönnberg J, Andersson U, Lyxell B, Spens K.
research. New York: Academic Press, 1978:213– 35. Vibrotactile speechreading support: cognitive prerequi-
26. Lancioni GE, O’Reilly MF, Basili G. Review of sites for training. J Deaf Studies Deaf Educ 1998;3:
strategies for treating sleep problems in persons with 143–56.
severe or profound mental retardation or multiple 48. Saeed SR, Ramsden RT, Axden PR. Cochlear im-
handicaps. Am J Ment Retard 1999;104:170– 86. plantation in the deaf-blind. Am J Otol 1998;19:774– 7.
27. Lancioni, GE, Olivia D, Bartolini T. A blind woman 49. Sauerburger D. Independence without sight or sound.
who is mentally retarded promoting and sharing the Suggestions for practitioners working with deaf-blind
occupational engagement of a dependent deaf-blind adults. New York: AFB Press, 1993.
adolescent. Behav Res Treat 1990;5:149– 57. 50. Schweigert P, Rowland C. Early communication and
28. Lawrence JS, Drabman R. Suppression of chronic high- microtechnology: instructional sequence and case

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001
Selective review of the deaf-blind 77

studies of children with severe multiple disabilities. 56. VanMeter TD, Weaver DD. Oculo-auriculo-vertebral
Argumentative Altern Commun 1992;8:273– 86. spectrum and the CHARGE association: clinical
51. Sisson LA, Hersen M, Van Hasselt VB. Improving the evidence for a common pathogenetic mechanism. Clin
performance of youth with dual sensory impairment – Dysmorphol 1996;5:187– 96.
analyses and social validation of procedures to reduce 57. van Hasselt VB, Hersen M, Egan BS, McKelvey JL,
maladaptive responding in vocational and leisure Sisson LA. Increasing social interactions in deaf-blind
settings. Behav Ther 1993;24:553– 71. severely handicapped young adults. Behav Modif 1989;
52. Sisson LA, Van Hasselt VB, Hersen M. Psychological 13:257–72.
approaches with deaf-blind persons: strategies and 58. Vernon M. Usher’s syndrome: congenital deafness and
issues in research and treatment. Clin Psychol Rev progressive loss of vision. J Chronic Dis 1969;22:133–
1987;7:303– 28. 51.
53. Szilvassy J, Czigner J, Jori J, Toth F, Szilvassy Z, 59. Watkins S, Clark TC. A coactive sign system for
Mieszkowski JM, Kiss JG. Cochlear implantation of a
children who are dual-sensory impaired. Am Ann Deaf
Hungarian deaf and blind patient with discharging ears
1991;136:321– 136.
suffering from Bechet’s disease. J Laryngol Otol 1998;
112:169– 71. 60. Wheeler L, GrifŽ n HC. A movement-based approach to
54. Tellier AL, Cormier-Daire V, Abadie V, Amiel J, language development in children who are deaf-blind.
Sigaudy S, Bonnet D, de Lonlay-Debeney P, Morris- Am Annals Deaf 1997;142:387– 90.
seau-Durand MP, Hubert P, Michel JL, Jan D, Dollfus 61. Worchel P, Dallenbach KM. “Facial vision”: perception
H, Baumann C, Labrune P, Lacombe D, Philip N, obstacles by the deaf-blind. Am J Psychol 1947;60:
LeMerrer M, Briard ML, Munnich A, Lyonnet S. 502–53.
CHARGE syndrome: Report of 47 cases and review. 62. Wolf-Schein EG. Assessing the “untestable” client:
Am J Genet 1998;76: 402–9. ADLO. Dev Dis Bull 1993;21:53– 70.
55. Thurlow WR. Some comparisons of characteristics of 63. Öhngren G, Rönnberg J, Lyxell B. Tactiling: a usable
alphabetic codes for deaf-blind. Hum Factors 1986;28: support system for speechreading? Br J Audiol 1992;
175–86. 26:167–73.

× × ×
Scandinavian Audiology Vol 30 No 2 2001

View publication stats

You might also like