You are on page 1of 20

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 1997, 30, 279–298 NUMBER 2 (SUMMER 1997)

THE USE OF POSITIVE AND NEGATIVE


REINFORCEMENT IN THE TREATMENT OF
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR
CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA, WAYNE W. FISHER, GREGORY P. HANLEY,
MATTHEW L. REMICK, STEPHANIE A. CONTRUCCI, AND TAMMERA L. AITKEN
KENNEDY KRIEGER INSTITUTE AND
JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

We identified 3 clients whose destructive behavior was sensitive to negative reinforcement


(break from tasks) and positive reinforcement (access to tangible items, attention, or
both). In an instructional context, we then evaluated the effects of reinforcing compliance
with one, two, or all of these consequences (a break, tangible items, attention) when
destructive behavior produced a break and when it did not (escape extinction). For 2
clients, destructive behavior decreased and compliance increased when compliance pro-
duced access to tangible items, even though destructive behavior resulted in a break. For
1 client, extinction was necessary to reduce destructive behavior and to increase compli-
ance. Subsequently, when the schedule of reinforcement for compliance was faded for all
clients, destructive behavior was lower and fading proceeded more rapidly when compli-
ance produced multiple functional reinforcers (i.e., a break plus tangible items or atten-
tion) and destructive behavior was on extinction. The results are discussed in terms of
the effects of relative reinforcement value and extinction on concurrent operants.
DESCRIPTORS: concurrent operants, developmental disabilities, negative reinforce-
ment, positive reinforcement, functional analysis, escape, response covariation

Negative reinforcement plays a significant reinforcement would be beneficial for a large


role in the maintenance of problematic be- proportion of individuals who engage in de-
havior (Iwata, 1987). Iwata, Pace, et al. structive behaviors. Toward that end, a num-
(1994) conducted 152 functional analyses ber of treatments, such as functional com-
and found that self-injurious behavior (SIB) munication training (FCT; Carr & Durand,
was sensitive to social negative reinforcement 1985; Fisher et al., 1993; Wacker et al.,
in the form of escape from demands or other 1990), reinforcement of compliance (Carr,
aversive stimulation for 38.1% of individu- Newsom, & Binkoff, 1980; Steege, Wacker,
als. Derby et al. (1992) found that the de- Berg, Cigrand, & Cooper, 1989), instruc-
structive behavior (e.g., aggression, SIB) of tional fading (Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree,
48% of clients seen in an outpatient clinic & McIntyre, 1993; Piazza, Moes, & Fisher,
was sensitive to social negative reinforce- 1996; Zarcone et al., 1993), noncontingent
ment. Results of these epidemiological stud- escape (Vollmer, Marcus, & Ringdahl,
ies suggest that the development of treat- 1995), and task alteration (Dunlap, Kern-
ments for behavior maintained by negative Dunlap, Clarke, & Robbins, 1991; Mace et
al., 1988; Weeks & Gaylord-Ross, 1981;
This investigation was supported in part by Grant Zarcone, Iwata, Mazaleski, & Smith, 1994),
MCJ249149-02 from the Maternal and Child Health have been demonstrated to effectively reduce
Service of the U.S. Department of Health and Human
Services. We thank Jennifer Zarcone for her helpful responding shown or hypothesized to be
comments on this manuscript. maintained by escape from demands. These
Requests for reprints should be sent to Cathleen C. treatments vary along a number of proce-
Piazza, Neurobehavioral Unit, The Kennedy Krieger
Institute, 707 N. Broadway, Baltimore, Maryland dural dimensions; however, the studies all
21205. share one common element: the use of ex-

279
280 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

tinction for escape-maintained aberrant be- reinforcement associated with destructive be-
havior. havior. Previous investigations on response
In fact, escape extinction alone has been covariation have shown that increasing com-
demonstrated to be effective in reducing SIB pliance through reinforcement can result in
independent of other treatment compo- concomitant decreases in problem behavior
nents. Iwata, Pace, Kalsher, Cowdery, and without manipulating the consequences for
Cataldo (1990) demonstrated the effective- destructive behavior (Parrish, Cataldo, Kol-
ness of an escape extinction procedure with ko, Neef, & Egel, 1986; Russo, Cataldo, &
6 individuals whose SIB was sensitive to es- Cushing, 1981). In other investigations, re-
cape as reinforcement. The extinction pro- sults of a functional analysis were used to
cedure consisted of continuing the learning determine the reinforcer for compliance. For
trial and physically guiding the individual to example, Horner and Day (1991) demon-
complete tasks contingent on SIB. Although strated that task initiation was high and de-
escape extinction has been demonstrated to structive behavior was low when escape was
be effective in reducing aberrant behavior, allowed contingent on task initiation, even
the use of extinction may be associated with when destructive behavior also resulted in
a number of problems, such as extinction escape. However, low levels of destructive
bursts (Iwata et al., 1990), increases in be- behavior were not maintained when the
havioral variability (Goh & Iwata, 1994), or schedule of reinforcement (escape) for com-
difficulties continuing tasks due to an indi- pliance was thinned.
vidual’s size or strength (Pace, Ivancic, & Jef-
Lalli and Casey (1996) also evaluated the
ferson, 1994; Piazza et al., 1996).
effects of escape as reinforcement for com-
Despite the potential problems associated
pliance and destructive behavior with 1 cli-
with escape extinction, it is unclear whether
ent. When compliance was reinforced on a
reductions in escape-maintained destructive
fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedule and destructive
behavior could be produced without it (i.e.,
when escape continues to be available for de- behavior was reinforced on a variable-ratio
structive behavior). Iwata (1987) suggested (VR) 5 schedule, compliance increased and
that analysis of behavior maintained by neg- destructive behavior decreased. As the sched-
ative reinforcement should consider not only ule of reinforcement for compliance was
the activity that is terminated (negative re- thinned, destructive behavior increased and
inforcement) but also the activity that is ini- compliance decreased. However, when the
tiated (positive reinforcement) coincident authors reinforced compliance with both es-
with escape. Zarcone, Fisher, and Piazza cape and adult attention, they were able to
(1996) examined the effects of ‘‘free time’’ thin the schedule of reinforcement for com-
contingencies and found that a break plus pliance (i.e., to an FR 10) while maintaining
access to preferred items resulted in greater low rates of destructive behavior and high
increases in compliance than a break alone. levels of compliance, even though destruc-
It is possible that similar manipulations tive behavior continued to produce escape
could be used with differential reinforce- on a VR 5 schedule. One potential expla-
ment procedures to increase compliance and nation for these results may be that the
decrease problem behavior. client’s aberrant behavior appeared to be sen-
Thus, one potential approach to the treat- sitive to both escape and attention as rein-
ment of behavior maintained by negative re- forcement. During treatment, compliance
inforcement may be to greatly increase re- produced both reinforcers, whereas destruc-
inforcement for compliance relative to the tive behavior produced escape only (atten-
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 281

tion was unavailable during the escape in- old boy who had been diagnosed with mild
terval). mental retardation, mild cerebral palsy, and
Results of the investigation by Lalli and pervasive developmental disorder. His de-
Casey (1996) suggest that, for clients whose structive behavior was aggression. Andy was
destructive behavior is multiply maintained, ambulatory and could follow simple instruc-
access to multiple functional reinforcers fol- tions (e.g., ‘‘Put the lotion in the basket’’).
lowing compliance may increase compliance He communicated using one- to three-word
and decrease destructive behavior even when sentences and was often echolalic. Carly was
destructive behavior continues to produce an 8-year-old girl who had been diagnosed
escape. Such a finding is consistent with re- with mild mental retardation. Her destruc-
search on concurrent operants in that in- tive behavior included aggression and dis-
creasing the quality of reinforcement for one ruption. She could communicate verbally,
response (e.g., compliance) may increase the but her communication was often unintel-
likelihood of that response relative to the ligible due to articulation problems. She was
other response (e.g., destructive behavior). ambulatory and could follow simple one-
In the current investigation, we attempted and two-step instructions. Ben was a 9-year-
to replicate and extend previous research on old boy who had been diagnosed with a sei-
the effects of manipulating the consequences zure disorder, oppositional defiant disorder,
for compliance on destructive behavior. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and
First, we identified 3 clients whose destruc- dysthymic disorder. His destructive behavior
tive behavior was sensitive to negative rein- included aggression and disruption. Ben
forcement (break from tasks) and positive re- communicated in complete sentences, could
inforcement (access to tangible items, atten- follow complex instructions (e.g., ‘‘Go in
tion, or both). Next, we evaluated the effects your room and find your blue shoes’’), and
of providing a break contingent on either ambulated without assistance.
compliance or destructive behavior during
instructional tasks. We also compared the ef- Data Collection and Interrater Agreement
fects of a break for compliance to the effects During all sessions, trained observers used
of a break plus access to the tangible item laptop computers to record each occurrence
that had been identified during the func- of destructive behavior. Andy’s destructive
tional analysis. Finally, we evaluated the ex- behavior was defined as punching, slapping,
tent to which the schedule of reinforcement pinching, kicking, pulling hair, scratching,
for compliance could be thinned using a and forcefully pulling on other’s clothing.
break as reinforcement versus using a break Carly’s destructive behavior was aggression
plus social positive reinforcement (tangible (hitting, kicking, scratching, pinching, hair
items, attention, or both). These manipula- pulling, headbutting, and throwing objects
tions were conducted with and without ex- at people) and disruption (throwing objects,
tinction for destructive behavior. banging on surfaces, knocking objects off
surfaces, and breaking objects). Ben’s de-
structive behavior was defined as aggression
GENERAL METHOD
(hitting, kicking, and scratching) and dis-
Participants ruption (banging and kicking surfaces,
Three individuals with severe behavior throwing objects, and breaking objects). Ob-
problems were admitted to an inpatient unit servers also recorded occurrences of compli-
specializing in the assessment and treatment ant behavior, which was defined as comple-
of destructive behavior. Andy was a 7-year- tion of an instruction following a verbal or
282 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

gestural prompt. Percentage compliance was request or engaged in destructive behavior.


calculated by dividing the number of com- If the client complied with the instruction
pliant responses by the total number of de- following a verbal or gestural prompt, he or
mands and multiplying by 100%. she received praise from the therapist. If the
Two independent observers scored the tar- client displayed destructive behavior, the
get responses simultaneously but indepen- therapist removed the materials and ignored
dently during 46%, 69%, and 42% of func- the client for 30 s (i.e., the client was per-
tional analysis sessions for Andy, Carly, and mitted to escape the task). Both academic
Ben, respectively. Interrater agreement was and self-care tasks were presented to all cli-
assessed during 79%, 55%, and 50% of de- ents. During attention sessions, the client
mand analysis sessions and 41%, 66%, and was given toys and was asked to play quietly.
64% of fading sessions for Andy, Carly, and The therapist provided a reprimand contin-
Ben, respectively. Agreement coefficients gent on each destructive behavior displayed
were calculated by partitioning each session by the client. All other responses were ig-
into 10-s intervals and dividing the number nored. In the toy play sessions, the therapist
of exact agreements on the frequency of be- played with the client and provided praise
havior by the sum of agreements plus dis- every 30 s contingent upon the first 5-s pe-
agreements multiplied by 100%. Mean riod in which no destructive behavior oc-
agreement was 96%, 97%, and 99% for de- curred. During tangible sessions, the client
structive behavior for Andy, Carly, and Ben was allowed to play with preferred activities
during the functional analysis. During the or items for 2 min prior to the start of the
demand analysis, mean agreement was 99% session. The activity and items were selected
for destructive behavior and 98% for com- based on parental report that activity or item
pliance for Andy, 91% for destructive be- removal resulted in destructive behavior. An-
havior and 98% for compliance for Carly, dy’s preferred activity was to have the ther-
and 99% for destructive behavior and 99% apist swing him around, Carly’s preferred
for compliance for Ben. During fading, object was access to television, and Ben’s was
mean agreement was 99% for destructive be- a computer game. When the session began,
havior and 98% for compliance for Andy, the therapist terminated the activity (Andy)
97% for destructive behavior and 97% for or withdrew the object (Carly and Ben). Fol-
compliance for Carly, and 99% for destruc- lowing each occurrence of destructive behav-
tive behavior and 97% for compliance for ior, the therapist engaged in the activity
Ben. (Andy) or returned the item for 30 s (Carly
and Ben).
PHASE 1: FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS Additional functional analysis sessions
were conducted for Carly and Ben because
Procedure
the results of the multielement analyses were
Functional analyses, using procedures somewhat unclear. A sequential pairwise
similar to those described by Iwata, Dorsey, analysis was conducted with Carly in which
Slifer, Bauman, and Richman (1982/1994), each test condition was compared to the toy
were conducted with all clients. The 10-min play condition (Iwata, Duncan, Zarcone,
conditions consisted of demand, social at- Lerman, & Shore, 1994). The conditions
tention, toy play, and tangible. During de- during the pairwise comparisons were simi-
mand sessions, the therapist used sequential lar to those of the multielement analysis ex-
verbal, gestural, and physical prompts every cept that different tangible items (books and
10 s until the client either complied with the photographs) were used. The tangible items
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 283

were changed because we observed that Car- ior in the tangible condition (M 5 0.5) and
ly engaged in destructive behavior on the liv- near-zero rates of destructive behavior dur-
ing unit when these items were removed. ing the toy play condition (M 5 0.1). Dur-
Rates of destructive behavior in the de- ing the sequential pairwise analysis, she con-
mand condition declined over the course of sistently engaged in destructive behavior
the analysis for Ben. However, we observed during social attention (M 5 8.9), tangible
that he engaged in destructive behavior (M 5 2.1), and demand (M 5 8.4) condi-
when he was instructed to complete his daily tions and engaged in low rates of behavior
self-care tasks. Therefore, we used a reversal during the toy play condition. These results
design (Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, & Roane, suggested that Carly’s destructive behavior
1995) to evaluate his sensitivity to escape as was maintained by escape from demands, at-
a reinforcer. Only self-care tasks were pre- tention, and access to tangible items.
sented during these sessions. During Ben’s multielement functional
analysis, destructive behavior occurred con-
Results sistently during the tangible condition (M 5
Results of the functional analyses appear 1.9 responses per minute), at variable rates
in Figure 1. For Andy, rate (responses per during the demand condition (M 5 0.4),
minute) of destructive behavior was consis- and not at all in social attention and toy play
tent during the demand (M 5 2.5) and tan- conditions. During the subsequent analysis
gible (M 5 1.4) conditions. In fact, Andy using a reversal design, his mean rates of de-
displayed the most efficient rates of respond- structive behavior were 1.5 during the two
ing in the tangible condition, contacting re- demand phases and 0 during the toy play
inforcement almost immediately upon re- phase. These results suggested that Ben’s de-
moval of the activity. Rates of responding structive behavior was maintained by escape
were less efficient in the demand condition from self-care tasks and access to a tangible
(i.e., he engaged in more destructive behav- item.
ior than was necessary to escape each de-
mand). However, he escaped almost every PHASE 2: DEMAND ANALYSIS
demand that was presented. Rates of de- Procedure
structive behavior were variable during the
social attention condition (M 5 8.5). In the The escape-maintained destructive behav-
toy play condition, rates of destructive be- ior of all clients was assessed during a series
havior were initially high, but then decreased of phases in which consequences for com-
to near zero (M 5 1.2). These results sug- pliance and destructive behavior were ma-
gested that Andy’s destructive behavior was nipulated. The conditions conducted with
maintained by escape from demands and ac- each client were specific to the client’s re-
cess to an activity. His sensitivity to atten- sponse to each treatment. During all con-
tion as reinforcement was less clear. ditions, (a) sequential verbal, gestural, and
Carly’s rates of destructive behavior were physical prompts were used once every 10 s
initially low during all conditions of the until the client complied with the demand
multielement functional analysis until Ses- or engaged in destructive behavior; (b) com-
sion 23. At that point, rates of destructive pliance resulted in praise from the therapist;
behavior increased in the social attention and (c) destructive behavior that occurred
and demand conditions (M 5 4.5 and 4.3 during a 30-s break was ignored. The con-
for social attention and demand, respective- ditions are described below. Each condition
ly). She engaged in some destructive behav- (e.g., differential reinforcement of compli-
284 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

Figure 1. Number of destructive responses per minute during the analogue functional analyses for Andy
(top panel), Carly (middle panel), and Ben (bottom panel).

ance without extinction) was given an ab- for compliance did not appreciably alter the
breviated name (e.g., praise/break), in which rate of demands (i.e., compliance did not
the first word (e.g., praise) refers to the con- result in a break from the tasks).
sequence for compliance, and the second DRA without extinction (break/break).
word (e.g., break) refers to the consequence During this condition, compliance resulted
for destructive behavior. in a 30-s break from the task. Destructive
Differential reinforcement of compliance behavior also resulted in a 30-s break from
(DRA) without extinction (praise/break). This the task.
condition was identical to the demand con- DRA without extinction (tangible/break).
dition conducted during the functional anal- In this condition, compliance resulted in 30
ysis. Compliance resulted in praise from the s of access to the tangible item used in the
therapist, and destructive behavior resulted tangible condition of the functional analysis
in a 30-s break from the task (all task ma- (books and photographs for Carly and a
terials were removed, and the therapist did computer game for Ben). We were con-
not interact with the client). Praise delivered cerned that swinging Andy (the activity used
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 285

in the functional analysis) would not repre- phase consisted of a comparison of praise/
sent a long-term treatment option as he grew break and break/break. Because destructive
in size. Therefore, we substituted items that behavior did not decrease during the break/
we believed had sensory properties similar to break condition, we then compared tangi-
swinging (access to bouncing on a big ther- ble/break with break/break. In the third
apy ball or to a sit-n-spin). It should be not- phase, extinction was added to both condi-
ed that providing access to the tangible item tions (tangible/extinction vs. break/extinc-
for 30 s also constituted a 30-s break from tion). In the next phase, extinction was with-
the task. Destructive behavior resulted only drawn (tangible/break vs. break/break). Fi-
in a 30-s break from the task. nally, extinction was reintroduced in both
DRA with extinction (break/extinction). In conditions (tangible/extinction vs. break/ex-
this condition, compliance resulted in a 30-s tinction) to demonstrate functional control
break from tasks, and no differential conse- of the extinction component.
quence occurred for destructive behavior
(extinction). If destructive behavior occurred Results
during the instructional sequence, the three- Results of Andy’s demand analysis are
step prompting procedure continued. shown in Figure 2. The top panel depicts
DRA with extinction (tangible/extinction). number of destructive responses per minute,
During this condition, compliance resulted and the bottom panel depicts percentage of
in 30 s of access to the tangible item as de- compliance. When compliance resulted in
scribed above, and no differential conse- praise and destructive behavior resulted in a
quence occurred for destructive behavior break (praise/break), rates of destructive be-
(extinction). havior remained stable (M 5 1.7 responses
During the demand analyses, a combina- per minute) and compliance was low (M 5
tion of multielement and reversal designs 12.6%). However, rates of destructive be-
was used for Andy. The first phase consisted havior were near zero (M 5 0.02 responses
of a multielement analysis in which praise/ per minute) and compliance was higher (M
break was compared to tangible/break. To 5 84.5%) when compliance resulted in ac-
isolate the effects of providing Andy with a cess to the tangible item, even though de-
break for compliance (independent of access structive behavior resulted in a break (tan-
to the activity), the multielement phase was gible/break). To evaluate the effects of the
then followed by a phase of break/break. Be- break alone, independent of the tangible
cause destructive behavior decreased in the item, a phase was conducted in which de-
break/break condition, we reversed to praise/ structive behavior and compliance both re-
break and then returned to break/break to sulted in a break (break/break). Initially,
demonstrate functional control of the break/ rates of destructive behavior were at baseline
break treatment. levels but then dropped to near zero (M 5
Two multielement phases were conducted 0.5 responses per minute). In addition, com-
for Carly. The first phase was a comparison pliance was initially low but then increased
of praise/break and break/break. Because de- (M 5 51.3%). Rates of destructive behavior
structive behavior did not decrease during increased during the reversal to praise/break
the break/break condition, the second phase (M 5 0.7 responses per minute), and com-
was a comparison of tangible/break and pliance was variable (M 5 50.1%). When
break/break. we returned to the break/break condition,
A combination of multielement and re- rates of destructive behavior dropped to near
versal designs was used for Ben. The first zero (M 5 0.7 responses per minute), and
286 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

Figure 2. Number of destructive responses per minute (top panel) and percentage of compliance (bottom
panel) during Andy’s demand analysis.

compliance, although somewhat variable, in- and the rates of destructive behavior and
creased (M 5 51.7%). compliance were similar (M 5 1.8 and 1.0
The results of Carly’s demand analysis are for destructive behavior and M 5 35.6%
depicted in Figure 3. During the first phase, and 37.4% for compliance during the
the effects of praise/break were compared praise/break and break/break conditions, re-
with break/break. Rates of destructive be- spectively). Adding a tangible item to the
havior and compliance were similar across break for compliance during the next phase
the two conditions. Mean rate of destructive produced little change in behavior (M 5 0.7
behavior was 4.7 responses per minute for responses per minute and 53.4% for de-
praise/break and 6.4 for break/break; mean structive behavior and compliance, respec-
percentages of compliance were 28.1 for tively). When extinction was added to both
praise/break and 17.6 for break/break. In the conditions, destructive behavior decreased to
next phase, the effects of break/break were zero across both conditions (M 5 1.6 and
compared to tangible/break. The rates of de- 0.6 for break/extinction and tangible/extinc-
structive behavior (M 5 3.4) were lower and tion, respectively) and compliance increased
compliance (M 5 36.3%) was higher during (M 5 90.4% and 90.4% for break/extinc-
the second phase of the break/break condi- tion and tangible/extinction, respectively).
tion. Rates of destructive behavior (M 5 When the extinction component was with-
0.4) were near zero and compliance was drawn, destructive behavior occurred consis-
highest (M 5 65.2%) during the tangible/ tently in both conditions (M 5 0.9 and 0.5
break condition. for break/break and tangible/break, respec-
Results of Ben’s demand analysis are de- tively) and compliance decreased (M 5
picted in Figure 4. During the first phase, 51.9% and 61.7% for break/break and tan-
praise/break was compared to break/break, gible/break, respectively). Finally, when ex-
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 287

Figure 3. Number of destructive responses per minute (top panel) and percentage of compliance (bottom
panel) during Carly’s demand analysis.

Figure 4. Number of destructive responses per minute (top panel) and percentage of compliance (bottom
panel) during Ben’s demand analysis (EXT 5 extinction).
288 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

Table 1 for Andy in the break/break and tangible/


The Schedule and Duration of Reinforcement at Each break conditions because both procedures
Step During Fading
had been effective in reducing destructive
Number of Duration of behavior, and we wanted to determine
Steps demands reinforcement (s) whether those effects would be maintained
1 1 30 under both procedures during fading. Six
2 2 30 sessions were conducted in each condition,
3 3 45 break/break and tangible/break, with rein-
4 5 60
5 8 90 forcement for compliance on an FR 1 sched-
6 12 120 ule (Step 1). As the schedule of reinforce-
7 16 180 ment was thinned, extinction was added to
8 20 270
both conditions. The fading goal was
achieved in the tangible/extinction condition
but not in the break/extinction condition.
tinction was again added to the two condi- Therefore, the tangible reinforcer for com-
tions, destructive behavior decreased (M 5 pliance was added in the break/extinction
0.2 and 0.1 for break/extinction and tangi- condition.
ble/extinction, respectively) and compliance Fading was implemented for Carly in the
increased in both conditions (M 5 95% and tangible/break condition because this treat-
100% for break/extinction and tangible/ex- ment produced the greatest decreases in de-
tinction, respectively). structive behavior and the highest levels of
compliance. Eight sessions were conducted
PHASE 3: FADING
in the tangible/break condition with rein-
Procedure forcement for compliance on an FR 1 sched-
After the demand analysis was completed ule (Step 1). As fading progressed, her de-
with each client and destructive behavior structive behavior increased, and we added
had been reduced to near-zero levels, the extinction for destructive behavior. When
schedule of reinforcement for compliance the schedule of reinforcement was thinned
was thinned (see Table 1). Fading included further, destructive behavior increased again,
increasing the number of demands the client and we added attention (verbal praise, tick-
needed to complete to obtain reinforcement les, and pats on the back) to the break and
when the client’s rate of destructive behavior tangible items (Lalli & Casey, 1996).
was at or below 10% of baseline levels for Fading was introduced for Ben simulta-
two consecutive sessions. If the criterion was neously in the break/extinction and tangible/
exceeded for two consecutive sessions, the extinction conditions because both
number of demands was decreased to the procedures had been effective in reducing
previous step. As the demand requirement destructive behavior. Five sessions were con-
was increased, the duration of access to re- ducted in each condition, with reinforce-
inforcement was also increased. The goal for ment for compliance on an FR 1 schedule
all 3 clients was completion of 20 tasks (ap- (Step 1). When destructive behavior in-
proximately the number of demands pre- creased in the break/extinction condition,
sented in baseline) followed by a 4- to 5-min the tangible item was added for compliance.
reinforcement period (i.e., Step 8), which A combination of multielement and re-
was a reasonable work/break schedule based versal designs was used during fading for
on input from their school placements. Andy. The first multielement phase consist-
Fading was implemented simultaneously ed of a comparison of fading during the
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 289

break/break and tangible/break conditions. creased from one to three (Steps 1 to 3) dur-
In the next phase, extinction was added to ing the break/break procedure. However,
both conditions (break/extinction vs. tangi- when he was required to complete five de-
ble/extinction). Extinction was withdrawn in mands (Step 4) to receive reinforcement, de-
the next phase, followed by a return to the structive behavior increased dramatically and
break/extinction and tangible/extinction was maintained above the fading criteria
conditions to demonstrate functional control even when the number of demands was de-
of the extinction component. Finally, when creased to one (Step 1). A similar pattern
destructive behavior increased during the was observed during the tangible/break con-
break/extinction condition, the tangible item dition: Destructive behavior remained low
was added to the break for compliance (tan- and compliance remained high until the
gible/extinction). An ABABCBC design was number of demands was increased to three
used during fading for Carly. Fading was in- (Step 3). Mean rate of destructive behavior
troduced in the tangible/break condition during the first phase for break/break was
(A). As fading progressed, extinction for de- 0.7 and mean percentage of compliance was
structive behavior was added (B). Extinction 43.7. Mean rate of destructive behavior dur-
then was withdrawn (A) and reintroduced ing the first phase for tangible/break was 0.6
(B). Next, attention (C) was added to the and mean percentage of compliance was
tangible/extinction condition, withdrawn 62.9. Because fading under both conditions
(B), and then reintroduced (C). A multi- was unsuccessful, extinction was introduced
element design was used during fading for for destructive behavior across both condi-
Ben, in which the effects of break/extinction tions. When compliance resulted in a break
were compared to tangible/extinction. When and destructive behavior was on extinction
rates of destructive behavior increased dur- (break/extinction), destructive behavior ini-
ing the break/extinction condition, the tan- tially increased, then decreased to near zero
gible item was added to the break for com- (first panel, M 5 1.0) but compliance was
pliance (tangible/extinction). unchanged (second panel, M 5 43.4%).
When compliance resulted in the tangible
Results item and destructive behavior was on ex-
The data for destructive behavior and tinction (tangible/extinction), destructive
compliance when reinforcement for compli- behavior decreased rapidly (third panel, M
ance was faded are depicted in Figures 5, 6, 5 0.1) and compliance increased (fourth
and 7. The first and second panels of Figure panel, M 5 80.3%). When extinction was
5 depict the data for destructive behavior withdrawn, destructive responses increased
and compliance, respectively, when fading (first panel, M 5 2.0) and compliance de-
was initiated in the break/break condition. creased (second panel, M 5 11.4%) in the
The third and fourth panels of Figure 5 de- break/break condition. Withdrawal of ex-
pict the data for destructive behavior and tinction in the tangible/break condition also
compliance, respectively, when fading was resulted in an increase in destructive behav-
initiated in the tangible/break condition. ior (third panel, M 5 1.4) and a decrease in
The data for each condition are depicted in compliance (fourth panel, M 5 20.2%). Ex-
separate panels to improve readability, al- tinction was then reintroduced, and destruc-
though the analysis was conducted in a mul- tive responses decreased in the tangible/ex-
tielement design. Andy’s destructive behavior tinction condition (third panel, M 5 0.4)
remained low and compliance remained but remained variable in the break/extinc-
high as the number of demands was in- tion condition (first panel, M 5 1.1). In ad-
290 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

Figure 5. Number of destructive responses per minute (first panel) and percentage of compliance (second
panel) when fading of the schedule of reinforcement was initiated in the break/break condition with Andy, and
number of destructive responses per minute (third panel) and percentage of compliance (fourth panel) when
fading of the schedule of reinforcement was initiated in the tangible/break condition with Andy. The numbers
and arrows between the graphs for destructive behavior and compliance indicate the fading steps (EXT 5
extinction).

dition, compliance increased in the tangible/ behavior decreased rapidly to near-zero levels
extinction condition (fourth panel, M 5 (first panel, M 5 0.2), and overall levels of
72.8%) but was more variable in the break/ compliance were slightly higher than in the
extinction condition (second panel, M 5 previous phase (second panel, M 5 60.5%).
36.3%). The tangible item was then added Figure 6 shows treatment effects during
as a reinforcer for compliance during the fading for Carly’s destructive behavior and
break/extinction condition, and destructive compliance. We were able to increase the
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 291

Figure 6. Number of destructive responses per minute (top panel) and percentage of compliance (bottom
panel) when fading of the schedule of reinforcement was initiated in the tangible/break condition with Carly.
The numbers and arrows between the graphs for destructive behavior and compliance indicate the fading steps
(EXT 5 extinction; Soc Attn 5 social attention).

number of demands to three (Step 3) during compliance increased slightly (M 5 47.3%).


the tangible/break procedure while main- When attention was added for compliance,
taining low levels of destructive behavior. destructive behavior decreased (M 5 0.2)
However, destructive behavior increased in and compliance increased (M 5 76.8%).
the second session in which Carly was re- When attention was withdrawn, destructive
quired to complete five demands (Step 4) to behavior increased (M 5 4.6) and compli-
receive reinforcement. Mean rate of destruc- ance decreased (M 5 35.6%). Finally, when
tive behavior was 2.0 and mean percentage attention was added, destructive behavior
of compliance was 53.1 during the tangible/ decreased (M 5 0.7), compliance increased
break phase. When we added extinction, de- (M 5 64.5%), and the fading goal was
structive behavior decreased (M 5 0.6) and achieved.
compliance increased (M 5 64.4%). When Ben’s data for destructive behavior and
extinction was withdrawn, destructive be- compliance are depicted in Figure 7. Fading
havior again increased (M 5 3.0) and com- proceeded rapidly when compliance resulted
pliance decreased (M 5 32.8%). When ex- in the tangible item and extinction was im-
tinction was again implemented, destructive plemented for destructive behavior (M 5 0
behavior was maintained (M 5 1.6) and for destructive behavior, M 5 98.2% for
292 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

Figure 7. Number of destructive responses per minute (top panel) and percentage of compliance (bottom
panel) when fading of the schedule of reinforcement was initiated in the break/extinction and tangible/extinction
conditions with Ben. The numbers and arrows between the graphs for destructive behavior and compliance
indicate the fading steps (EXT 5 extinction).

compliance). However, when compliance re- structive behavior was maintained by escape
sulted in a break and extinction was imple- from demands and access to an activity. Car-
mented for destructive behavior, destructive ly’s destructive behavior was maintained by
behavior increased (M 5 1.3) when he was escape from demands, access to tangible
required to complete five demands (Step 4) items, and attention. Ben’s destructive be-
to receive reinforcement, although his com- havior was maintained by escape from de-
pliance remained high (M 5 95%). At that mands and access to tangible items. There-
point, the tangible item was added for com- fore, we examined the effects of providing
pliance, and Ben’s destructive behavior de- these reinforcers for destructive and compli-
creased (M 5 0) and compliance remained ant behavior during instructional situations
high (M 5 97%). with and without extinction. For Andy, but
not for Carly or Ben, compliance increased
and destructive behavior gradually decreased
DISCUSSION to near-zero levels when compliance was re-
In the current investigation, the function- inforced with a break, even though destruc-
al analyses of 3 clients indicated that their tive behavior also continued to produce a
destructive behavior was maintained by mul- break. The combination of a break and ac-
tiple sources of reinforcement. Andy’s de- cess to tangible items contingent upon com-
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 293

pliance produced immediate behavioral one response almost exclusively if it results


change (decreases in destructive behavior in (a) a higher rate or amount of reinforce-
and increases in compliance) for Andy and ment (Catania, 1963), (b) more immediate
Carly. However, for Andy and Carly, it was reinforcement (Chung & Herrnstein, 1967),
necessary to add extinction to the treatment or (c) higher quality reinforcement (Miller,
as the schedule of reinforcement for com- 1976).
pliance was thinned. For Ben, destructive When compliance or destructive behavior
behavior was maintained until it was placed resulted in a 30-s break (break/break), the
on extinction, regardless of whether compli- destructive behavior for 2 of 3 clients (Carly
ance resulted in a break or a break plus ac- and Ben) did not decrease. One reason may
cess to the tangible item. have been that even though reinforcement
This investigation replicates and extends for the two responses (destructive behavior
the literature on the relation between com- and compliance) was the same (a 30-s break
pliance and problem behavior in several from work), there may have been differences
ways. First, the results for Andy and Carly between the two conditions that favored de-
are consistent with previous findings show- structive behavior. The criterion for rein-
ing that reinforcement of compliance can forcement of compliance was task comple-
produce concomitant decreases in destruc- tion, which produced a break (i.e., post-
tive behavior (Horner & Day, 1991; Lalli & ponement of the next task). Destructive be-
Casey, 1996), even when destructive behav- havior, on the other hand, produced a break
ior continues to produce reinforcement in in the absence of task completion. This
the form of a break. When compliance pro- could help to explain the results for Carly
duced a break (Andy) or a break plus a tan- and Ben, whose destructive behavior did not
gible item (Carly), destructive behavior de- decrease when both compliance and destruc-
creased even though it continued to produce tive behavior produced a break from work,
a break. In addition, during fading for Andy but not those for Andy, whose destructive
and Carly and during the demand analysis behavior decreased to near-zero levels under
for Ben, applying extinction to destructive this schedule arrangement.
behavior resulted in increases in compliance, The results of the demand analysis for
even when the consequence for compliance Andy were consistent with those of Horner
remained unchanged. and Day (1991), who found that task initi-
One potential explanation of these find- ation was high and destructive behavior was
ings is that the relative rates of compliance low when both behaviors resulted in a break
and destructive behavior were a function of (break/break condition). It is possible that
the relative value of the reinforcement pro- Andy’s preference for compliance over de-
duced by each response (Catania, 1992). In structive behavior when each produced a
a concurrent operants arrangement, two or 30-s break was due to other reasons, such as
more responses, each correlated with a the amount of physical effort required for
schedule of reinforcement, are available at each response. However, the effort for the
the same time. Variables that affect relative two responses did not appear to be different;
response rates in such arrangements include no attempt was made to measure the relative
response effort or difficulty as well as rein- effort associated with these responses, so this
forcement rate, delay, amount, and type explanation remains speculative.
(Mazur, 1994). When each response is re- During the demand analysis, when com-
inforced on a ratio schedule, as in the cur- pliance resulted in a 30-s break and access
rent investigation, individuals often display to tangible reinforcement but destructive be-
294 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

havior resulted in a break only (tangible/ turned to an FR 1 for compliance and de-
break), compliance was associated with high- structive behavior.
er quality reinforcement (multiple functional During fading, the schedule of reinforce-
reinforcers) than destructive behavior was. ment for compliance was decreased rapidly
Under this arrangement, Andy and Carly, during the tangible/extinction condition for
but not Ben, preferred compliance over de- Ben due to low rates of destructive behavior.
structive behavior. These idiosyncratic re- However, similar to the results for Andy, de-
sults are consistent with those reported by structive behavior increased as the schedule
Neef, Mace, Shea, and Shade (1992), who of reinforcement for compliance was
examined the effects of reinforcer rate and thinned during fading in the break/extinc-
quality on time allocation to arithmetic tion condition. As with Andy, we added the
problems during concurrent variable-interval higher quality reinforcer (i.e., the tangible
schedules. Neef et al. found that 2 students item) and maintained zero levels of destruc-
generally responded for the higher quality tive behavior. Thus, the results during fading
reinforcer, even when that high-quality al- suggest that for clients whose destructive be-
ternative was associated with leaner sched- havior is multiply controlled, providing ac-
ules of reinforcement. However, the 3rd stu- cess to multiple functional reinforcers for
dent demonstrated a highly variable pattern compliance may improve treatment out-
of responding, sometimes allocating re- come.
sponses to the higher quality reinforcer and There are several clinical implications of
these findings. First, the primary goals of
at other times responding on the richer
treatment for these 3 clients were (a) low
schedule of reinforcement. Thus, when qual-
rates of destructive behavior, (b) high levels
ity of reinforcement is manipulated, it may
of compliance, and (c) a reasonable schedule
be difficult to predict how a given individual
of demands and reinforcement (i.e., 20 de-
will allocate responding, perhaps because
mands followed by 4 to 5 min of reinforce-
quality of reinforcement is more difficult to ment). For all 3 clients, these goals were met
quantify than other parameters such as rate. only when compliance produced multiple
As the density of reinforcement for com- functional reinforcers (a break plus tangible
pliance was thinned during fading for Andy reinforcers, attention, or both) in combina-
(break/break and tangible/break conditions) tion with extinction (i.e., discontinuation of
and for Carly (tangible/break condition), de- reinforcement for destructive behavior).
structive behavior increased and compliance Placing destructive behavior on extinction
decreased. This may have occurred because and reinforcing compliance with the conse-
destructive behavior continued to result in a quence that had previously maintained de-
break on an FR 1 schedule, and the differ- structive behavior (i.e., escape) were not suf-
ences between the rates of reinforcement for ficient to meet our final treatment goal (20
the two responses increased as the schedule demands and 4 to 5 min of reinforcement)
for compliance was faded. Under this sched- for these 3 clients. Thibault et al. (1995)
ule arrangement, quality of reinforcement treated the severe behavior problems of 30
was higher for compliance, but rate of re- clients using differential reinforcement of an
inforcement was greater for destructive be- alternative (DRA) response (communica-
havior. However, the schedule of reinforce- tion) and extinction. Even though the in-
ment may not fully explain these results be- vestigators reinforced communication with
cause control over destructive behavior was the reinforcer that had previously main-
not regained when the schedule was re- tained destructive behavior, they successfully
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 295

faded the schedule of reinforcement for the when extinction is not implemented for de-
alternative response in only 5 of 30 appli- structive behavior. In addition, if teachers or
cations. By contrast, when communication caregivers cannot provide reinforcement for
was reinforced and a punishment procedure compliance consistently, access to higher
was added, they were able to fade the sched- quality reinforcement and extinction for de-
ule of reinforcement in 100% of the cases. structive behavior may be effective in reduc-
One potentially important clinical implica- ing destructive behavior even when rein-
tion of the current results is that reinforce- forcement is not provided on a continuous
ment of an alternative response with multi- schedule.
ple functional reinforcers and extinction for Finally, in the current investigation, func-
destructive behavior may provide an effective tional reinforcers that maintained destructive
alternative to punishment when DRA with behavior in other social contexts (attention,
a single functional reinforcer proves to be tangible items) were added to the reinforcer
ineffective. that maintained destructive behavior in the
Second, we evaluated the individual con- instructional context (i.e., escape). However,
tribution of reinforcement to treatment ef- most individuals with escape-maintained de-
fectiveness because each reinforcement com- structive behavior do not also display de-
ponent (i.e., the break, the tangible item, structive behavior maintained by other tan-
and attention) was introduced separately. gible reinforcers or attention (i.e., they do
For 2 clients (Andy and Carly), reinforce- not display multiply controlled destructive
ment for compliance (with a break for Andy behavior). For individuals with destructive
and a break plus a tangible item for Carly) behavior that is maintained by a single con-
on an FR 1 schedule without extinction for sequence (e.g., escape), future research might
destructive behavior resulted in decreases in be directed toward determining whether it is
destructive behavior and increases in com- possible to increase the effectiveness of a
pliance. For all clients, extinction for de- DRA procedure by incorporating multiple
structive behavior and access to higher qual- alternative reinforcers that are identified on
ity reinforcement (break plus social positive the basis of a reinforcer assessment, such as
reinforcement) for compliance resulted in the one described by Fisher et al. (1992).
the greatest increases in compliance when Steege et al. (1989) used reinforcers that had
the schedule of reinforcement for compli- been identified through a preference assess-
ance was thinned. ment and obtained increases in compliance
In the natural environment, reinforce- and decreases in destructive behavior com-
ment for compliance and extinction for de- parable to those in the current investigation.
structive behavior may not occur consistent- However, the procedure used by Steege et al.
ly (e.g., teachers or caregivers may allow es- involved extinction for destructive behavior
cape for some destructive behavior or may in addition to reinforcement for compliance
not provide reinforcement for every compli- on an FR 1 schedule. Thus, future investi-
ant behavior). If teachers or caregivers can- gators may wish to examine the extent to
not withhold reinforcement for destructive which reinforcers identified via preference
behavior, the findings from the demand (Pace, Ivancic, Edwards, Iwata, & Page,
analysis of the current investigation suggest 1985) and choice (Fisher et al., 1992) as-
that, for some clients (in this case, Andy and sessments might also be effective for (a)
Carly), providing access to higher quality re- achieving increases in compliance and de-
inforcement on a dense schedule can result creases in destructive behavior without the
in decreases in destructive behavior even use of extinction and (b) increasing the like-
296 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

lihood that destructive behavior will remain Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Dis-
abilities, 2, 3–20, 1982)
low as reinforcement for compliance is fad- Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman,
ed. D. C., & Shore, B. A. (1994). A sequential, test-
control methodology for conducting functional
analyses of self-injurious behavior. Behavior Mod-
REFERENCES ification, 18, 289–306.
Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J.
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing be- R., Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., Rodgers, T. A.,
havior problems through functional communica- Lerman, D. C., Shore, B. A., Mazaleski, J. L.,
tion training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Goh, H. L., Cowdery, G. E., Kalsher, M. J.,
18, 111–126. McCosh, K. C., & Willis, K. D. (1994). The
Carr, E. G., Newsom, C. D., & Binkoff, J. A. (1980). functions of self-injurious behavior: An experi-
Escape as a factor in the aggressive behavior of mental-epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied
two retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240.
Analysis, 13, 101–117. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery,
Catania, A. C. (1963). Concurrent performances: A G. E., & Cataldo, M. F. (1990). Experimental
baseline for the study of reinforcement magni- analysis and extinction of self-injurious escape be-
tude. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Be- havior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23,
havior, 6, 299–300. 11–27.
Catania, A. C. (1992). Learning. Englewood Cliffs, Lalli, J. M., & Casey, S. D. (1996). Treatment of
NJ: Prentice Hall. multiply controlled problem behavior. Journal of
Chung, S.-H., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1967). Choice Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 391–395.
and delay of reinforcement. Journal of the Exper- Mace, F. C., Hock, M. L., Lalli, J. S., West, P. B.,
imental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 67–74. Pinter, E., & Brown, D. K. (1988). Behavioral
Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M., momentum in the treatment of noncompliance.
Northup, J., Cigrand, K., & Asmus, J. (1992). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 123–141.
Brief functional assessment techniques to evaluate
Mazur, J. E. (1994). Learning and behavior. Engle-
aberrant behavior in an outpatient setting: A sum-
wood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
mary of 79 cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
Miller, H. L. (1976). Matching-based hedonic scaling
ysis, 25, 713–721.
in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
Dunlap, G., Kern-Dunlap, L., Clarke, S., & Robbins,
of Behavior, 26, 335–347.
F. R. (1991). Functional assessment, curricular
Neef, N. A., Mace, F. C., Shea, M. C., & Shade, D.
revision, and severe behavior problems. Journal of
(1992). Effects of reinforcer rate and reinforcer
Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 387–397.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. quality on time allocation: Extensions of matching
P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A compar- theory to educational settings. Journal of Applied
ison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers Behavior Analysis, 25, 691–699.
for persons with severe and profound disabilities. Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. B. A., & Page, T. J. (1985). Assessment of stim-
Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jef- ulus preference and reinforcer value with pro-
ferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional foundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied
communication training with and without extinc- Behavior Analysis, 18, 249–255.
tion and punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., & Jefferson, G. (1994).
Analysis, 26, 23–36. Stimulus fading as treatment for obscenity in a
Goh, H., & Iwata, B. A. (1994). Behavioral persis- brain-injured adult. Journal of Applied Behavior
tence and variability during extinction of self-in- Analysis, 27, 301–305.
jury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Be- Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Cowdery, G. E., Andree, P.
havior Analysis, 27, 173–174. J., & McIntyre, T. (1993). Stimulus (instruction-
Horner, R. H., & Day, M. D. (1991). The effects of al) fading during extinction of self-injurious es-
response efficiency on functionally equivalent cape behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
competing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior 26, 205–212.
Analysis, 24, 719–732. Parrish, J. M., Cataldo, M. F., Kolko, D. J., Neef, N.
Iwata, B. A. (1987). Negative reinforcement in ap- A., & Egel, A. L. (1986). Experimental analysis
plied behavior analysis: An emerging technology. of response covariation among compliant and in-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 361–378. appropriate behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. Analysis, 19, 241–254.
E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a func- Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996).
tional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Be- Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior
havior Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from and demand fading in the treatment of escape-
ESCAPE-MAINTAINED DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR 297

maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G.,
Behavior Analysis, 29, 569–572. Berg, W., Reimers, T., Cooper, L., Cigrand, K.,
Russo, D. C., Cataldo, M. F., & Cushing, P. J. & Donn, L. (1990). A component analysis of
(1981). Compliance training and behavioral co- functional communication training across three
variation in the treatment of multiple behavior topographies of severe behavior problems. Journal
problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 14, of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
209–222. Weeks, M., & Gaylord-Ross, R. (1981). Task diffi-
Steege, M. W., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Cigrand, culty and aberrant behavior in severely handi-
K. K., & Cooper, L. J. (1989). The use of be- capped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
havioral assessment to prescribe and evaluate treat- ysis, 14, 449–463.
ments for severely handicapped children. Journal Zarcone, J. R., Fisher, W. W., & Piazza, C. C. (1996).
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 23–33. Analysis of free-time contingencies as positive ver-
Thibault, M., Hagopian, L., Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. sus negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Be-
C., Harrell, R., Gerencser, J., & Patterson, H. havior Analysis, 29, 247–250.
(1995, May). Further investigation of the effects of
Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Mazaleski, J. L., & Smith,
functional communication training with and with-
R. G. (1994). Momentum and extinction effects
out extinction and punishment: A summary of thirty
cases. Poster presented at the 21st annual conven- on self-injurious escape behavior and noncompli-
tion of the Association for Behavior Analysis, ance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27,
Washington, D.C. 649–658.
Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Jagtiani,
(1995). Noncontingent escape as treatment for S., Smith, R. G., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). Ex-
self-injurious behavior maintained by negative re- tinction of self-injurious escape behavior with and
inforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, without instructional fading. Journal of Applied
28, 15–26. Behavior Analysis, 26, 353–360.
Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments Received August 26, 1996
to extended experimental analyses in the evalua- Initial editorial decision November 4, 1996
tion of aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Be- Final acceptance January 31, 1997
havior Analysis, 28, 561–576. Action Editor, Brian A. Iwata

STUDY QUESTIONS

1. What were the general purposes of the study?

2. Why were additional functional analyses conducted with Carly andBen, and what did these
analyses entail?

3. Describe the consequences delivered (but not their various combinations) for compliance
and destructive behavior across the various treatment and fading conditions.

4. How were demand frequencies and reinforcement interval lengths modified during the fading
phase?
298 CATHLEEN C. PIAZZA et al.

5. The participants were exposed to a variety of different arrangements during the demand
analysis and fading phases. Below is a table indicating these various combinations. Complete
the table by indicating for each paticipant whether a given condition produced a therapeutic
effect (1), did not produce such an effect (2), or was not experienced by the individual (n/a).

Andy Carly Ben


Demand Demand Demand
analysis Fading analysis Fading analysis Fading
Praise/break
Break/break
Tangible/break
Break/EXT
Tangible/EXT
Tangible1praise/EXT

6. Based on the above, which single component seemed to have the greatest therapeutic effect?

7. Across participants, the praise/break condition seemed to be uniformly ineffecive, whereas


both the break/break and the tangible/break conditions were effective for at least 1 partici-
pant. Based on the contingencies in effect during these three conditions, why might one
expect the praise/break condition to be the least effective?

8. What do the authors describe as two important clinical implications of their results?

Questions prepared by Iser DeLeon and Michele Wallace, University of Florida

You might also like