You are on page 1of 22

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2001, 34, 17–38 NUMBER 1 (SPRING 2001)

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING


FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH
FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING
GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H. THOMPSON
THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

We evaluated four methods for increasing the practicality of functional communication


training (FCT) by decreasing the frequency of reinforcement for alternative behavior.
Three participants whose problem behaviors were maintained by positive reinforcement
were treated successfully with FCT in which reinforcement for alternative behavior was
initially delivered on fixed-ratio (FR) 1 schedules. One participant was then exposed to
increasing delays to reinforcement under FR 1, a graduated fixed-interval (FI) schedule,
and a graduated multiple-schedule arrangement in which signaled periods of reinforce-
ment and extinction were alternated. Results showed that (a) increasing delays resulted
in extinction of the alternative behavior, (b) the FI schedule produced undesirably high
rates of the alternative behavior, and (c) the multiple schedule resulted in moderate and
stable levels of the alternative behavior as the duration of the extinction component was
increased. The other 2 participants were exposed to graduated mixed-schedule (unsignaled
alternation between reinforcement and extinction components) and multiple-schedule
(signaled alternation between reinforcement and extinction components) arrangements in
which the durations of the reinforcement and extinction components were modified.
Results obtained for these 2 participants indicated that the use of discriminative stimuli
in the multiple schedule facilitated reinforcement schedule thinning. Upon completion
of treatment, problem behavior remained low (or at zero), whereas alternative behavior
was maintained as well as differentiated during a multiple-schedule arrangement consist-
ing of a 4-min extinction period followed by a 1-min reinforcement period.
DESCRIPTORS: functional analysis, functional communication training, differential
reinforcement of alternative behavior, reinforcement schedules, delay to reinforcement

The development of interventions based are the same as those found to be responsible
on the results of functional analyses (Iwata, for maintenance of problem behavior. When
Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/ positive treatment effects are observed, the
1994) has become a hallmark of current re- initially dense schedule of reinforcement is
search on the assessment and treatment of sometimes thinned to some predetermined
problem behavior. Once the function of a and presumably practical goal.
problem behavior is identified, an interven- Several methods have been developed for
tion is designed that typically consists of re- thinning noncontingent schedules of rein-
inforcement (e.g., either noncontingent, forcement. For example, Vollmer et al.
Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, Smith, & Mazales- (1993) presented a method for thinning non-
ki, 1993, or contingent on an alternative re- contingent reinforcement (NCR) in the form
sponse, Carr & Durand, 1985) and extinc- of attention from a continuous schedule (i.e.,
tion, in which stimuli delivered or withheld fixed-time [FT] 10 s) to a relatively thin
schedule (FT 5 min). This type of NCR
This research was supported in part by a grant from
schedule thinning was used successfully in
the Florida Department of Children and Families. We several subsequent investigations (e.g., Ha-
thank Jeffrey Hickman and April Worsdell for their gopian, Fisher, & Legacy, 1994; Marcus &
assistance in conducting various aspects of this study. Vollmer, 1996). A slightly different procedure
Reprints may be obtained from Brian Iwata, Psy-
chology Department, The University of Florida, was described by Lalli, Casey, and Kates
Gainesville, Florida 32611. (1997). They set the initial NCR schedule

17
18 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

based on the mean latency to the first prob- scribed a thinning procedure that involved
lem behavior observed during baseline and gradually increasing the interval of time (de-
subsequently increased the FT intervals by ei- lay) between a communication response and
ther 30, 60, or 90 s. As an alternative to the delivery of positive reinforcement. This
fixed-increment procedures for thinning procedure resulted in a 90% reduction in
NCR schedules, Kahng, Iwata, DeLeon, and problem behavior at the terminal schedule in
Wallace (2000) used an adjusting-interre- only 5 of 12 applications. Punishment, in ad-
sponse-time (IRT) procedure, in which the dition to the FCT intervention, was necessary
NCR schedule was initially set and then later for maintaining 90% reductions in problem
thinned based on the mean IRT from pre- behavior at the terminal schedule in all appli-
ceding sessions. The authors observed that cations.
the adjusting-IRT procedure was somewhat Fisher, Thompson, Hagopian, Bowman,
more efficient in reaching a terminal schedule and Krug (2000) noted that the delay pro-
than was the fixed-increment procedure. cedure may weaken the contingency be-
The procedures described above for thin- tween communicative responding and rein-
ning NCR schedules each may have partic- forcement as the delay interval is increased.
ular advantages (e.g., efficiency, ease of im- This contingency-weakening effect inherent
plementation); however, the procedures are to delay procedures (Lattal, 1984) may result
similar in that they are all time based (i.e., in extinction of the newly acquired com-
the delivery of reinforcers under all three ar- municative response as well as recovery of
rangements is based purely on the passage of the historical problem behavior. Following
time). Differential-reinforcement-of-other- successful reduction of attention-maintained
behavior (DRO) schedules can be thinned problem behavior through FCT, Fisher et al.
in a similar manner, in that the duration of increased the delay to reinforcement follow-
time during which the target behavior must ing occurrences of alternative (communica-
be absent is gradually increased (e.g., from tive) responses. In the absence of the delay
5 s to 15 min, Repp & Deitz, 1974). By and during initial brief delays, alternative re-
contrast, schedule-thinning procedures used sponses were maintained at just above four
for NCR or DRO are not applicable to dif- per minute. However, as the delay was grad-
ferential-reinforcement-of-alternative-behav- ually increased to 30 s, the rate of the alter-
ior (DRA) schedules, such as those arranged native behavior dropped to near zero. In ad-
in functional communication training dition, problem behavior occurred some-
(FCT) (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985), be- what more frequently than did alternative
cause DRA schedule thinning requires that behavior under the 30-s reinforcement delay
some appropriate responses will be rein- in two of the final four sessions. Although
forced and others will not. Thus, schedule- FCT with a 30-s delay was a successful in-
thinning procedures that may be appropriate tervention in that problem behavior was
for time-based interventions (NCR and greatly reduced from its baseline rate, poten-
DRO) may compromise the integrity of in- tial hazards of the contingency-weakening
terventions involving a reinforcement con- effects of the delay procedure also were ap-
tingency for a particular response. parent in the data.
Few methods for thinning schedules of re- A slightly different procedure for FCT
inforcement within FCT treatment packages schedule thinning that may be more likely
have been formally evaluated. In a large-scale to maintain alternative responding is to pre-
analysis of FCT outcomes, Hagopian, Fisher, sent reinforcement on an initially dense
Sullivan, Acquisto, and LeBlanc (1998) de- fixed-interval (FI) schedule of reinforcement
SCHEDULE THINNING 19

and to subsequently thin the FI schedule. FI sponses (the manual sign for ‘‘hugs’’ or the
schedules involve the delivery of a reinforcer manual sign for ‘‘games’’). The two drawings
for a response after a specified amount of and their associated contingencies were al-
time has elapsed since the last reinforced re- ternated every 30 s and were successful in
sponse (Ferster & Skinner, 1957) and have producing highly discriminated responding
been demonstrated to support various hu- (following training, participants emitted
man behaviors as schedules were thinned only the response that would be reinforced
(Orlando & Bijou, 1960; Schroeder, 1972; at that moment).
Weiner, 1969), presumably because the con- The arrangement used by Fisher et al.
tingency between responding and reinforce- (1998) was a multiple schedule (Herrick, My-
ment was maintained. ers, & Korotkin, 1959), which represents an
One limitation of FI schedules is that re- attractive alternative to reinforcement delays
sponding may be maintained at high levels and FI arrangements during FCT schedule
during the intervals in which reinforcement thinning. That is, reinforcement for alternative
is unavailable. In fact, in an early study on responding could be decreased during FCT by
the operant performance under FI schedules correlating distinctive stimuli with reinforce-
of children with developmental disabilities, ment and extinction within a multiple-sched-
Orlando and Bijou (1960) observed either ule arrangement and then increasing the rel-
scalloping (an increase in response rate near ative duration of the extinction component.
the end of the interval) or stable and high Because reinforcement is delivered immediate-
rates of responding (similar to those ob- ly following an alternative response in the re-
served under fixed-ratio [FR] schedules). inforcement component, the response–rein-
Thus, in the context of FCT interventions, forcer relation is maintained. In addition, the
FI arrangements may lead to undesirably schedule-correlated stimuli may generate more
high rates of alternative behavior in the ab- efficient responding when reinforcement is
sence of reinforcement as the FI schedule is available as well as less responding when re-
thinned. This response pattern could be dis- inforcement is unavailable.
ruptive to others in a home or classroom as We evaluated several strategies for thin-
well as annoying to the person responsible ning DRA schedules in the present study.
for delivering reinforcement, especially as DRA interventions consisting of FCT and
the interval is about to expire. extinction for problem behavior were imple-
Undesirably high levels of responding be- mented with 3 participants based on the re-
tween opportunities for reinforcement might sults of functional analyses. Subsequently
be decreased through the use of distinctive (Study 1), the effects of reducing reinforce-
stimuli correlated with the availability and ment for appropriate behavior were observed
unavailability of reinforcement (Bijou & Or- under three conditions with 1 participant:
lando, 1961; Long, 1962). For example, (a) an FR 1 with increasing delays, (b) a
Fisher, Kuhn, and Thompson (1998) estab- graduated FI schedule, and (c) a graduated
lished discriminative control over two alter- multiple schedule. In Study 2, the effects of
native responses during FCT with 2 partic- altering the durations of the reinforcement
ipants. Training involved pairing specific and extinction components under (a) a
stimuli (a drawing of the participant inter- mixed schedule (no stimuli correlated with
acting with an adult or a drawing of the par- the reinforcement and extinction compo-
ticipant playing with toys) with particular nents) and (b) a multiple schedule (distinc-
forms of positive reinforcement (attention or tive stimuli correlated with the reinforce-
access to toys) for different alternative re- ment and extinction components) were ob-
20 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

served on the problem and alternative be- wheelchair), communicated via gestures, and
haviors of 2 participants. could follow one-step instructions. She en-
gaged in SIB (banging hands, wrists, and
arms against hard surfaces) and aggression
GENERAL METHOD
(slapping, punching, pinching, scratching,
Participants and Setting and kicking others). Staff reported that these
Three individuals who lived in a state res- behaviors occurred at high rates throughout
idential facility for persons with develop- the day in the absence of any identifiable
mentally disabilities participated. They had antecedents. Staff also noted that Julie often
been referred for assessment and treatment was aggressive towards peers and that the
of various forms of self-injurious behavior typical response was for a staff member to
(SIB) or aggression. Karen was a 29-year-old tell Julie to stop and to sit within an arm’s
woman who had been diagnosed with pro- reach when peers were nearby.
found mental retardation. She was ambula- All sessions were conducted in therapy
tory, communicated via gestures, and could rooms at the day-treatment program located
follow one-step instructions. She engaged in on the grounds of the residential facility. Ses-
SIB consisting of face, head, and body hit- sion rooms contained tables, chairs, and oth-
ting and hand biting. Staff reported that her er materials as needed (see below). Sessions
SIB occurred intermittently throughout the lasted for 10 to 12 min and were conducted
day but was more likely when she was de- three to six times daily, 4 to 5 days per week.
nied access to food (i.e., trips to restaurants,
cafeterias, or stores in which food was visible Response Measurement and Reliability
were reported to occasion SIB). Data were collected on participants’ SIB,
Jake was a 34-year-old man who had been aggression, and alternative responses, and on
diagnosed with profound mental retardation. therapists’ delivery of reinforcement. Karen’s
Jake was ambulatory, communicated through SIB was defined as hitting her face, head, or
either gestures or signs (but rarely did so spon- torso with any part of her hand and as force-
taneously), and followed two-step instructions. ful contact between her teeth and hand
He engaged in SIB in the form of hand and (hand biting rarely occurred). Jake’s SIB was
arm biting and occasionally engaged in ag- defined as closure of his teeth on any part
gression (biting, pinching, and limb twisting). of his hand, arm, or sides of fingers. Jake’s
Jake’s SIB resulted in severely callused hands aggression was defined as twisting another’s
and forearms and, at times, produced open skin or limbs, forcefully striking others with
wounds and bruising. Staff reported no iden- hands, or closing his teeth on another’s skin.
tifiable antecedents to these problem behav- Julie’s SIB was defined as forceful contact be-
iors, which often occurred in ‘‘bursts.’’ Due to tween her hand, wrist, or arm and a hard
Jake’s size (he was 6 ft tall and weighed ap- surface (e.g., table, wheelchair); her aggres-
proximately 190 lb) and the intensity of his sion was defined as hitting, kicking, punch-
outbursts, staff reported that they often pro- ing, or scratching others.
vided preferred materials (food, radio, puzzles) Alternative responses were selected based
to ‘‘calm him down.’’ on recommendations by speech and lan-
Julie was a 31-year-old woman who had guage specialists who worked regularly with
been diagnosed with profound mental retar- the participants. Voice-output microswitches
dation, Angelmann’s syndrome, cerebral pal- were selected for Karen and Julie. Alternative
sy, and a seizure disorder. Julie was not am- responses were scored each time the partic-
bulatory (she spent most of her day in a ipant’s hand depressed the microswitch such
SCHEDULE THINNING 21

that the programmed sound (‘‘more, please’’ treatment and schedule-thinning procedures
for Karen; ‘‘talk to me, please’’ for Julie) was were evaluated in a reversal design.
emitted. The manual sign ‘‘more’’ was se-
lected as Jake’s alternative response, which Functional Analysis and Initial
was defined as repeatedly touching the fin- DRA Evaluation
gertips of both hands together (another re- An analysis based on procedures described
sponse was not scored until there was at least by Iwata et al. (1982/1994) was conducted
a 1-s period between fingertip touches). to determine if Karen’s SIB was sensitive to
Alternative responses were scored as either edible reinforcement. The functional analysis
prompted (by a therapist) or unprompted was arranged in a pairwise manner, in which
(independent) and as occurring under either only two (test and control) conditions were
reinforcement or extinction components compared (Iwata, Duncan, Zarcone, Lerman,
during the mixed- and multiple-schedule & Shore, 1994). In one condition, the pu-
conditions (see below). Data were collected tative reinforcer was delivered contingent on
by trained observers on handheld computers problem behavior (contingent reinforcement,
(Assistant Model A102) during continuous CR); in a second condition, the same rein-
10-s intervals and were summarized as num- forcer was available noncontingently (non-
ber of responses per minute. Interobserver contingent reinforcement, NCR). As noted
agreement was assessed by having a second above, Karen’s SIB predominantly occurred
observer collect data simultaneously but in- during situations in which access to foods or
dependently during 48% of the sessions snacks was delayed or denied. Nonfat pop-
across all participants. Agreement data were corn was chosen as the reinforcer for her eval-
collected during at least 30% of the sessions uation because it was a typical snack in her
in each condition for each participant. Ob- home. During the CR condition, instances of
servers’ records were compared on an inter- SIB produced 10-s access to a plate of pop-
val-by-interval basis, and agreement percent- corn. During the NCR condition, the plate
ages were calculated by dividing the smaller of popcorn was continuously available. A
number of responses recorded in each inter- therapist was present during the NCR con-
val by the larger number of responses; these dition but did not deliver any consequences
fractions then were averaged and multiplied following SIB. The CR condition of the
by 100%. Mean interobserver agreement for functional analysis served as the baseline for
all behaviors across the 3 participants was
evaluating the effects of the DRA contingen-
96.9% (range, 80.4% to 100%).
cy.
During the DRA condition, the micro-
STUDY 1: switch was placed on the table, and an FR
EVALUATION OF 1 schedule of reinforcement was arranged
INCREASED DELAYS, such that each occurrence of the alternative
FI SCHEDULES, AND response (pressing a microswitch that emit-
MULTIPLE SCHEDULES ted the sound ‘‘more, please’’) resulted in 10-
PROCEDURE s access to popcorn, whereas SIB no longer
A functional analysis, evaluation of a produced access to reinforcement (i.e., ex-
DRA procedure, and an assessment of three tinction). At the beginning of the initial ses-
types of schedule-thinning procedures were sion, Karen was physically guided to emit
conducted with Karen. The conditions of the alternative response, and reinforcement
the functional analysis were arranged in a was delivered for the prompted response.
multielement design, and the effects of the Physical prompts were gradually replaced by
22 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

gestural prompts, which were then faded us- increased following two consecutive sessions
ing a delay procedure during the initial ses- in which SIB remained at or below 85% of
sion of each DRA (FR 1) condition (1 s was the baseline mean (see Figure 1 for the grad-
added to the delay following emission of uated intervals). Occurrences of SIB pro-
each prompted response until the response duced no programmed consequences.
occurred independently). A return to the CR
condition, in which reinforcement was de- Multiple Schedule: FR 1 Extinction
livered contingent upon occurrences of SIB,
was conducted (the microswitch was not Following a return to the FR 1 condition
available in this condition) and was followed without delays, the effects of a multiple
by a return to the DRA condition. The schedule were evaluated with Karen. The
DRA condition then was used as a baseline multiple schedule consisted of two distinct
for evaluating the effects of the three sched- components: an FR 1 component in which
ule-thinning procedures on SIB and alter- each alternative response produced reinforce-
native responding. ment (10-s access to popcorn) and an ex-
tinction component in which alternative re-
FR 1 Delay sponses produced no reinforcement. During
During this condition, gradually increas- both components, SIB resulted in no pro-
ing delays were inserted between the emis- grammed consequences. A distinct stimulus
sion of an alternative response (pressing the was present during each component: A
microswitch) and delivery of the reinforcer round (20 cm) white laminated card was
(10-s access to popcorn). Responses during present on the table during the FR 1 com-
the delay were not reinforced, and the delays ponent, and a rectangular (20 cm by 30 cm)
were not signaled (i.e., the therapist did not red laminated card was present during the
deliver any instructions to the participant or extinction component. Initially, the compo-
indicate in any other way that reinforcement nent durations were set at 45 s and 15 s for
was unavailable). The initial delay was 1 s; FR 1 and extinction, respectively. When the
this delay was increased following two con- session began, the white card was present for
secutive sessions in which SIB remained at 45 s, and each alternative response resulted
or below 85% of the baseline mean (see Fig- in 10-s access to popcorn. The white card
ure 1 for the graduated delays). Occurrences
was then replaced with the red card for the
of SIB produced no programmed conse-
next 15 s, during which reinforcement was
quences.
withheld for all behavior. Basically, the ther-
Fixed Interval apist responded to instances of the alterna-
Following a return to the FR 1 condition tive response only when the white card was
without delays, reinforcers (10-s access to present and ignored all responding in the
popcorn) were delivered for alternative re- presence of the red card. Alternative respons-
sponses according to an FI schedule. For in- es that occurred in the presence of the white
stance, under the FI 25-s schedule, the first and red cards were scored separately. Sched-
response that occurred after 25 s elapsed ule thinning was accomplished by gradually
from either the start of the session or from altering the durations of the components fol-
a previous reinforcer delivery produced a re- lowing two consecutive sessions in which
inforcer. Responses that occurred prior to SIB remained at or below 85% of the base-
the end of the interval were not reinforced. line mean (see Figure 1 for graduated com-
The initial FI value was 1 s; this interval was ponent durations).
SCHEDULE THINNING 23

Figure 1. Number of self-injurious and alternative responses per minute (top panel), and the percentage of
reinforcers earned and the percentage of alternative responses resulting in reinforcement (bottom panel) during
Karen’s functional analysis, treatment assessment, and evaluation of schedule-thinning procedures.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION given continuous access to the same rein-


The top panel of Figure 1 shows Karen’s forcer (NCR condition). These data showed
rate of SIB and alternative responses during that Karen’s SIB was sensitive to food as re-
the functional analysis, initial DRA evalua- inforcement and were consistent with anec-
tion, and assessment of the three schedule- dotal reports and observed instances of prob-
thinning procedures. lem behavior in her home. However, this as-
sessment does not rule out the possibility
Functional Analysis and Initial that problem behavior was also maintained
DRA Evaluation by other sources of reinforcement that are
High rates of SIB were observed (M 5 typically examined in more thorough func-
7.7 responses per minute) when reinforce- tional analyses (e.g., Iwata et al., 1982/
ment was delivered contingent on occur- 1994).
rences of SIB (CR condition). By contrast, The DRA (FR 1) intervention resulted in
little or no SIB occurred when Karen was an immediate decrease in Karen’s rate of SIB
24 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

(M 5 0.2) and a gradual increase in her rate the alternative response appeared to be
of alternative responding (M 5 4.2; only un- weakening under the increasing delays, SIB
prompted alternative responses are shown in also reemerged at low but variable rates. This
the figures for all participants). A return to pattern of alternative and problem behavior
contingent reinforcement for SIB resulted in was similar to that observed by Fisher et al.
an immediate increase in SIB (M 5 7.3) to (2000) under similar conditions of reinforce-
levels observed in the initial CR condition. ment delay.
A return to the DRA condition again re- Supplemental calculations were included
sulted in an immediate and sustained de- to evaluate additional effects of the schedule-
crease in SIB (M 5 0.1) and maintenance thinning procedures (bottom panel of Figure
of the alternative response (M 5 4.9). These 1). The percentage of reinforcers earned was
data indicated that the DRA intervention in calculated by dividing the actual number of
which SIB was placed on extinction and an reinforcers earned within a session by the to-
alternative response was reinforced on an FR tal number of reinforcers that were available
1 schedule was successful in eliminating SIB (or that could have been earned) within a
and strengthening the alternative response. session and multiplying by 100%. For ex-
However, the DRA component resulted in ample, in a 10-min session in which an FR
unacceptably high rates of the alternative re- 1 schedule without a delay is arranged, 60
sponse (e.g., a response about every 12 s). reinforcers (one every 10 s) are available. If
This outcome seemed impractical (necessi- the participant emitted five alternative re-
tating almost continuous therapist involve- sponses per minute and earned 50 of the 60
ment) and particularly prone to failures of reinforcers, then the percentage of reinforc-
treatment integrity. Therefore, several strat- ers earned would be 83%. Karen earned
egies for thinning the schedule of reinforce- most of the available reinforcers during base-
ment and making the intervention more line (CR; M 5 79%) and DRA FR 1 with-
practical to implement were evaluated. The out delay (M 5 72%) conditions. However,
goal of these strategies was to produce an as the delay between the alternative response
arrangement under which (a) problem be- and reinforcement increased during the FR
havior either did not occur or occurred at 1 delay condition, the percentage of rein-
rates much lower than those observed in forcers earned began to decrease and even-
baseline and (b) the newly acquired alter- tually reached zero.
native response was maintained but did not An additional measure, the percentage of
occur at excessively high rates. alternative responses that resulted in rein-
forcement, was calculated by dividing the
FR 1 Delay number of reinforcers earned by the number
This arrangement, in which delays were of alternative responses within each session
programmed between occurrences of the al- and multiplying by 100%. During the DRA
ternative response and the delivery of rein- FR 1 (without delay) condition, the mean
forcement, resulted in a temporary increase percentage of alternative responses that re-
in the rate of the alternative response as the sulted in reinforcement was 87% (it was not
delay was increased to 8 s. However, as the 100% under these conditions because some
delay to reinforcement was increased further responding occurred while Karen had access
(to 16 s and then to 25 s), alternative re- to the reinforcer). As the delay between al-
sponding decreased sharply and appeared to ternative responding and reinforcement in-
be extinguished (no alternative responses oc- creased, the percentage of alternative re-
curred in four of the last five sessions). As sponses that resulted in reinforcement be-
SCHEDULE THINNING 25

came more variable. It appeared that, al- for the last five sessions of the condition was
though more alternative responses were 91%). These data are interesting in that, un-
being emitted under the FR 1 delay condi- der the FR 1 delay condition, the percentage
tion, at times, few of these responses resulted of alternative responses that produced rein-
in reinforcement. Taken together, these data forcement was higher than that observed un-
suggest that the FR 1 delay condition re- der the FI condition (M 5 65% and 23%,
sulted in a weakening of the contingency be- respectively), and fewer of the available re-
tween alternative responding and reinforce- inforcers were earned under the FR 1 delay
ment that was sufficient to extinguish the condition than under the FI condition (M
newly acquired alternative response and 5 40% and 77%, respectively). This dis-
evoke problem behavior. crepancy seems most likely related to the
subtle contingency-weakening (FR 1 delay)
Fixed Interval and contingency-strengthening (FI) effects
As the delay interval increased under the of these two procedures for thinning rein-
FR 1 delay procedure, the likelihood that an forcement schedules within DRA interven-
alternative response would be followed by tions.
reinforcement decreased. This contingency- In many ways, the FI schedule resulted in
weakening effect of the FR 1 delay proce- the desired pattern of behaviors because SIB
dure might be circumvented by program- remained near zero (M 5 0.1 responses per
ming the delays following reinforcement and minute), alternative responding was main-
by providing subsequent reinforcement im- tained, and the overall availability of rein-
mediately following the first response that forcement was decreased. However, the FI
occurs after a specified time interval expires. arrangement engendered a different but still
This subtle change in procedures results in problematic situation in that the alternative
an FI schedule of reinforcement. A return to response occurred at extremely high rates
the FR 1 condition without delay was con- (over three times that observed during the
ducted prior to the evaluation of FI sched- FR 1 condition). This response pattern
ules and resulted in (a) SIB returning to could be disruptive in the home or work-
zero, (b) increased alternative responding shop (or classroom) as well as disturbing to
that became stable (M 5 4.1 responses per the person responsible for delivering rein-
minute), (c) the majority of available rein- forcement, especially as the interval is about
forcers being earned (M 5 63%), and (d) a to expire.
high percentage of alternative responses re-
sulting in reinforcement (M 5 94%). The Multiple Schedule: FR 1 Extinction
initiation of the FI schedule of reinforce- High rates of alternative responding that
ment for alternative responding resulted in occur during the FI schedule (i.e., those that
a small and immediate increase followed by do not result in reinforcement) may be re-
a sharp increase in the rates of alternative duced by enhancing discrimination between
responding that eventually became stable the availability and unavailability of rein-
(mean for the last five sessions of the con- forcement through the pairing of distinctive
dition was 12.5 responses per minute). Even (discriminative) stimuli with the two con-
though the percentage of alternative respons- ditions. This type of arrangement, in which
es that produced reinforcement was low stimuli are correlated with alternating peri-
(mean percentage for the last five sessions of ods of reinforcement and extinction, results
the condition was 7%), Karen earned most in a multiple schedule.
of the available reinforcers (mean percentage Prior to introducing the multiple-schedule
26 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

condition, a return to the FR 1 condition ment was effective in (a) maintaining low
was conducted and produced patterns simi- rates of SIB while (b) maintaining a strong
lar to those observed previously (i.e., zero contingency between alternative responding
SIB, moderate and stable rates of alternative and reinforcement, (c) maintaining low lev-
responding). As the multiple-schedule com- els of alternative responding when reinforce-
ponent durations were changed, alternative ment was not available, and (d) decreasing
responding during the FR 1 and extinction the overall availability of reinforcement (and
components was measured separately and re- necessary supervision) from continuous to
vealed different patterns of responding dur- one fifth of the time.
ing the two components. A stable rate of
alternative responding consistent with, al-
STUDY 2:
though slightly higher than, that observed in
EVALUATION OF
previous FR 1 conditions was observed
SCHEDULE-CORRELATED
throughout the FR 1 component of the mul-
STIMULI
tiple-schedule condition (M 5 5.2). As can
be seen from the data in the bottom panel PROCEDURE
of Figure 1, Karen earned larger percentages Functional analyses, evaluations of DRA
of reinforcement (M 5 83%) under the procedures, and assessments of schedule-
multiple schedule arrangement than under correlated stimuli were conducted with Jake
the previous FR 1 condition (M 5 72%). and Julie. The functional analyses were
Alternative responding during the extinction arranged in multielement designs, the effects
component initially increased to extremely of the treatment were assessed in reversal
high rates (mean for the first five sessions designs, and the effects of schedule-
was 26.7 responses per minute); however, as correlated stimuli were assessed in
the component durations were altered across multielement and reversal designs. Two
successive sessions (and more alternative re- therapists alternated within sessions for both
sponses contacted relevant contingencies in participants, except during the multielement
the presence of different stimuli), the rate of evaluations of schedule-correlated stimuli,
alternative responses decreased to near zero when one therapist conducted the multiple-
during the extinction component (mean for schedule sessions and a second therapist
the last five sessions was 0.9 responses per conducted the mixed-schedule sessions.
minute). Reduced rates of SIB under the ex-
tinction component and the efficient alter- Functional Analyses and
native responding observed during the FR 1 DRA Evaluations
component resulted in most of the alterna- A functional analysis (Iwata et al., 1994)
tive responses producing reinforcement (M similar to that described for Karen was con-
5 55%). ducted to determine if Jake’s SIB (his ag-
In the final multiple-schedule arrange- gression was measured but not included in
ment, 1-min periods of reinforcement for al- the contingency class) was maintained by ed-
ternative responding alternated with 4-min ible and material reinforcement and if Julie’s
periods of extinction. Under these condi- SIB and aggression were maintained by at-
tions, SIB remained at zero, and most alter- tention. In one condition, the putative re-
native responses were emitted when rein- inforcer was delivered contingent on prob-
forcement was available (few were emitted lem behavior (CR); in the other condition,
when reinforcement was unavailable). It ap- the same reinforcer was available noncontin-
pears that the multiple-schedule arrange- gently (NCR). As noted previously, Jake’s
SCHEDULE THINNING 27

SIB usually resulted in access to preferred A return to the CR condition, in which re-
items. On several occasions, snacks or activ- inforcement was delivered contingent upon
ities such as listening to his radio were pro- occurrences of SIB, was conducted with Julie
vided following episodes of SIB. Therefore, (the microswitch was unavailable in this con-
M&Mst and a radio were chosen as rein- dition) and was followed by a return to the
forcers during his assessment and treatment. DRA condition.
Reports from staff and results from struc-
tured observations suggested that Julie’s Evaluation of Schedule-Correlated Stimuli:
problem behaviors often occurred when staff Multiple Versus Mixed Schedules
members were not directly interacting with Karen’s data (Study 1) showed that a mul-
Julie and that her problem behavior often tiple-schedule arrangement, in which alter-
resulted in strong reactions (e.g., verbal rep- native responses resulted in reinforcement
rimands) from peers and staff. Therefore, at- only during signaled interspersed periods of
tention (statements of concern, comments time, was effective in maintaining low levels
on her play behavior, and brief physical con- of problem behavior and reducing the over-
tact) was included as reinforcement through- all rate of alternative responding while main-
out Julie’s evaluations. During Jake’s sessions, taining its strength as a discriminated oper-
no materials other than the session furniture ant. Her data also suggested that the stimuli
and reinforcers were included in the session correlated with the FR and extinction sched-
room. During all of Julie’s sessions, drawing ules may have been instrumental in main-
materials were located on the table. During taining high rates of alternative responding
the CR condition, instances of SIB (Jake) or during the FR component and low rates
SIB and aggression (Julie) produced 10-s ac- during extinction. However, similar patterns
cess to a plate of M&Mst and a radio (Jake) of behavior may have been observed simply
or 10-s access to social interaction (Julie). by introducing gradually longer extinction
During the NCR condition, the materials or periods in the absence of any schedule-cor-
social interaction were continuously avail- related stimuli. Therefore, the importance of
able, and problem behavior did not result in the schedule-correlated stimuli was evaluated
any programmed consequences. An extinc- with Jake and Julie through a comparison of
tion condition, in which attention was com- multiple-schedule (alternation between sig-
pletely unavailable throughout the session, naled periods of reinforcement and extinc-
also was included for Julie. tion) and mixed-schedule (alternation be-
The CR condition of the functional anal- tween unsignaled periods of reinforcement
ysis was used as a baseline to evaluate the ef- and extinction) arrangements. As was the
fects of a DRA intervention in which pressing case for Karen, the multiple schedule con-
a microswitch that emitted the sound ‘‘talk to sisted of two components: an FR 1 com-
me, please’’ (Julie) or the manual sign ‘‘more’’ ponent (white card) in which each alterna-
(Jake) was reinforced while problem behavior tive response produced 10-s access to rein-
was placed on extinction. During the DRA forcement, and an extinction component
condition, an FR 1 schedule was arranged (red card) in which alternative responses
such that each instance of the alternative re- produced no reinforcement. No experience
sponse resulted in 10-s access to reinforce- with the red or white cards was arranged in
ment. Physical and gestural prompts were conditions prior to the evaluation of mixed
used initially to occasion the alternative re- and multiple schedules. Initially, the com-
sponse but were gradually removed during the ponent durations were set at 45 s and 15 s
first treatment session, as described for Karen. for FR 1 and extinction, respectively. Alter-
28 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

Figure 2. Number of self-injurious (top panel) and alternative (bottom panel) responses per minute during
Jake’s functional analysis, treatment assessment, and evaluation of schedule-correlated stimuli.

native responses that occurred in the pres- Jake. Prior to the evaluation of schedule-
ence of the white and red cards were scored correlated stimuli, probes of the terminal
separately. The mixed-schedule condition in- multiple schedule (i.e., alternation between
volved the same contingencies; however, the 60 s of FR 1 and 240 s of extinction) were
schedule-correlated stimuli (white and red conducted to determine if gradual schedule
cards) were not present in these sessions. thinning was actually necessary. Following
During both conditions, SIB resulted in no these terminal-schedule probes, a return to
programmed consequences. Schedule thin- the continuous FR 1 condition was con-
ning was accomplished by gradually altering ducted, followed by the comparison of mul-
the durations of the components following tiple (conducted by Therapist 1) and mixed
two consecutive sessions in which SIB re- (conducted by Therapist 2) schedules. Dur-
mained at or below 85% of the baseline ing this comparison, the duration of the
mean (see Figures 2 and 3 for graduated schedule components was held constant (45
component durations). Procedural elements s of FR 1 and 15 s of extinction). Once the
particular to each of the participants are de- relative effectiveness of the multiple and
scribed below. mixed schedules was demonstrated, the
SCHEDULE THINNING 29

Figure 3. Number of self-injurious and aggressive behaviors (top panel) and alternative responses (bottom
panel) per minute during Julie’s functional analysis, treatment assessment, and evaluation of schedule-correlated
stimuli.

schedule-correlated stimuli were incorporat- lie while attending to a magazine (no eye
ed into the mixed-schedule sessions con- contact or interaction occurred unless an al-
ducted by Therapist 2. This addition, in es- ternative response was emitted); during the
sence, changed the mixed schedule into a extinction (red card) component, the thera-
multiple schedule. Finally, the multiple- pist sat with his or her back towards Julie
schedule sessions were conducted until low while attending to a magazine. Prior to con-
levels of SIB occurred under the terminal- ducting the series of multiple- and mixed-
schedule arrangement. schedule sessions in a reversal design, the
Julie. Following the initial evaluation of multiple-schedule component durations
schedule-correlated stimuli (red and white were altered to match those operating in the
cards) in a multielement design, supplemen- mixed schedule (60 s of FR 1 and 120 s of
tal schedule-correlated stimuli were added to extinction). Finally, the multiple-schedule
the multiple-schedule condition beginning sessions were conducted until low levels of
with Session 101. During the FR 1 (white SIB occurred under the terminal-schedule
card) component, the therapist sat facing Ju- arrangement.
30 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION patterns of problem and alternative behav-


The top panels of Figures 2 and 3 show iors were observed for both participants
rates of problem behavior during the func- when the reinforcers identified in their func-
tional analyses, DRA evaluations, and as- tional analyses were withheld following in-
sessments of schedule-correlated stimuli for stances of problem behavior and delivered
Jake and Julie, respectively. The bottom pan- for alternative responses.
els of Figures 2 and 3 show rates of alter- Evaluation of Schedule-Correlated Stimuli:
native responding under FR 1 and extinc- Multiple Versus Mixed Schedules
tion throughout the analyses.
Jake. Prior to evaluating the effects of
Functional Analyses and Initial schedule-correlated stimuli, responding was
DRA Evaluations observed under the terminal multiple-sched-
ule arrangement, in which 1-min periods of
Jake. Rates of SIB were high during the test signaled FR 1 were alternated with 4-min
condition in which food and a radio were periods of signaled extinction. Under this
available following instances of SIB (M 5 condition (labeled terminal-schedule probe
4.1), but no SIB was observed when the same on Figure 2), Jake engaged in high rates of
reinforcement was available continuously. SIB (M 5 3.4) even though SIB was placed
These data support the hypothesis that Jake’s on extinction. Jake also continued to engage
SIB was maintained by tangible items. During in the alternative response during both the
the DRA condition, immediate decreases in FR 1 (M 5 3.7) and extinction (M 5 2.8)
SIB (M 5 0) and increases in alternative re- components; however, alternative respond-
sponding (M 5 4.3) were observed. ing approached zero during the extinction
Julie. High rates of both SIB and aggres- components in the latter part of each ses-
sion were observed (M 5 21.1) in the con- sion. These data indicated that an abrupt
tingent attention condition (CR). By con- shift to a thin schedule of reinforcement for
trast, low rates of SIB and aggression were alternative behavior was unwarranted, and
observed when therapist attention was either we returned to the FR 1 schedule (DRA) to
unavailable throughout the session (extinc- reestablish low rates of SIB and stable rates
tion; M 5 1.5) or available continuously of alternative responding prior to evaluating
(NCR; M 5 0.8). Thus, results of Julie’s the effects of schedule-correlated stimuli.
functional analysis indicated that her prob- During the multiple-schedule arrange-
lem behavior was maintained by attention. ment, in which 45-s periods of signaled FR
Due to the variability of Julie’s problem be- 1 were alternated with 15-s periods of sig-
havior during the functional analysis (and naled extinction, Jake engaged in low rates
the brevity of her assessment), a CR baseline of SIB (M 5 0.2), low rates of alternative
was conducted prior to the DRA evaluation responding during the extinction compo-
(M 5 11.8). During the DRA condition, nents (M 5 0.4), and high rates of the al-
immediate decreases in problem behavior ternative response during the FR 1 schedule
(M 5 1.9) and increases in alternative re- component (M 5 4.9). Presumably, discrim-
sponses (M 5 6.0) were observed. These ef- ination between the red and white cards had
fects were replicated by observing behavior developed during the terminal-schedule
during a return to baseline (CR; M 5 17.5) probes, as evidenced by initial zero rates of
and a return to the DRA condition (M for the alternative response during the extinc-
SIB and aggression 5 1.3; M for alternative tion component of the multiple schedule.
responses 5 5.1). Thus, clear shifts in the During the mixed arrangement conducted
SCHEDULE THINNING 31

by a second therapist, in which schedule-cor- schedule. Both multiple-schedule conditions


related stimuli were not present, Jake en- resulted in low rates of SIB (M 5 0.04 and
gaged in high rates of SIB (M 5 2.4) and 0.3) and alternative responding (M 5 0.8 and
alternative responding during both the FR 1 1.4) during signaled extinction periods and
(M 5 4.3) and extinction (M 5 12.3) com- stable levels of alternative responding during
ponents. These data show that the addition signaled FR 1 periods (M 5 5.2 and 5.2) for
of the schedule-correlated stimuli resulted in Therapists 1 and 2, respectively.
two important outcomes: (a) Alternative re- The remainder of Jake’s assessment in-
sponding occurred almost exclusively during volved the gradual lengthening of extinction
the FR 1 component under the multiple- periods. Rates of SIB were somewhat vari-
schedule arrangement but almost indiscrim- able across this phase of the assessment, but
inately throughout the sessions under the were consistently lower than those observed
mixed-schedule arrangement, and (b) SIB in baseline. An increase in SIB was observed
occurred at near-zero rates under the mul- each time a longer extinction component
tiple-schedule arrangement but at higher duration was introduced (see arrows below
rates under the mixed-schedule arrangement. the horizontal axis of the top panel in Figure
Although both the multiple and mixed 2), suggesting that changes in the multiple-
schedules programmed extinction continu- schedule component durations should have
ously for SIB and intermittently for alter- been more gradual. However, this conclu-
native responding (i.e., for 15 s out of every sion is speculative given the overall variabil-
minute), few alternative responses contacted ity in alternative responding observed
extinction in the multiple-schedule condi- throughout this phase in the absence of
tion (about 8%) whereas a larger percentage schedule changes. Nevertheless, low rates of
of alternative responses (about 74%) con- SIB (and zero rates of aggression) were ob-
tacted extinction in the mixed-schedule con- served at the terminal schedule, in which 1-
dition. In addition, higher rates of aggres- min periods of reinforcement alternated
sion were observed under the mixed-sched- with 4-min periods of extinction. Rates of
ule condition (M 5 2.4) relative to the mul- alternative responding became less variable
tiple-schedule condition (M 5 0.1) under the extinction component and even-
(aggression had not been observed in Jake’s tually were extinguished (zero in 32 of the
assessment until this phase of the evalua- last 40 sessions), whereas alternative re-
tion). Thus, it appears that nonreinforce- sponding was maintained at a stable rate in
ment of the newly acquired alternative re- the FR 1 component throughout the entire
sponse (engendered by the absence of salient assessment (M 5 5.1). At the end of the
stimuli correlated with reinforcement and assessment, Jake was not engaging in SIB (or
extinction in the mixed-schedule condition) other problem behaviors such as aggression)
may have had the undesirable effect of evok- and was emitting alternative responses only
ing other behavior that either had an estab- at times in which the likelihood of reinforce-
lished (SIB) or presumed (aggression) his- ment was high.
tory of producing similar reinforcement Julie. The multiple- and mixed-schedule
(Goh & Iwata, 1994). conditions both maintained relatively low
Further evidence of control exerted by the rates of aggression and SIB throughout the
schedule-correlated stimuli was demonstrated multielement evaluation (M 5 1.4 and 2.1,
by introducing these stimuli in sessions con- respectively) relative to baseline. Although
ducted by the second therapist; this function- Julie’s alternative responding was highly var-
ally changed the mixed schedule to a multiple iable within the multielement comparison, a
32 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

few general patterns emerged. Higher rates ted under the FR 1 schedule component as
of the alternative response were observed reinforcement was available for 1 of every 5
during the FR 1 component in the multiple- min within the multiple schedule.
schedule condition (M 5 6.4) than in the
mixed-schedule condition (M 5 4.1), and Within-Session Analysis
lower rates of the alternative response were If intermittent schedules are used to in-
observed during the extinction component crease the practicality or acceptability of
in the multiple-schedule condition (M 5 DRA interventions, and if the components
7.9) than in the mixed-schedule condition of these schedules are not discriminable to
(M 5 11.8). These data suggest that the the individuals for whom these treatments
schedule-correlated stimuli (colored cards) are planned, two undesirable patterns of be-
exerted some control over alternative re- havior may emerge. Both of these patterns
sponding; however, the effects may have were evident in Julie’s data and can be seen
been mitigated by multiple treatment inter- in Figure 4, which shows the cumulative
ference engendered by the multielement de- number of alternative responses across
sign or simply were weak due to the sched- schedule components (e.g., FR 1 and ex-
ule-correlated stimuli not being sufficiently tinction) within 10-s bins for three sessions
salient. As a result, changes were made in from her assessment of schedule-correlated
the experimental design (repeated observa- stimuli. These three particular sessions were
tions within a single condition were con- selected because they provide clear examples
ducted in lieu of rapid alternation between of likely patterns of performance under these
two conditions) and the saliency of the different schedule arrangements.
schedule-correlated stimuli (the therapist During Mixed Session 107, alternative re-
oriented towards or away from Julie during sponding continually occurred during the
the FR 1 and extinction conditions, respec- initial 1-min FR 1 component; however, af-
tively). ter contacting the extinction contingency in
During the initial multiple-schedule con- the second component, alternative respond-
dition, relatively high rates of the alternative ing never recovered. In the absence of stim-
response were observed during the FR 1 uli associated with the resumption of rein-
component (M 5 11.7) relative to the ex- forcement for alternative responses, periods
tinction component (M 5 5.7), whereas of extinction for newly acquired alternative
rates of SIB remained low (M 5 1.0). These responding may have weakened the contin-
patterns were reversed during the mixed- gency sufficiently to extinguish this behavior.
schedule condition, in that relatively higher If this pattern continued for an extended pe-
alternative response rates were observed dur- riod, behaviors that historically produced
ing extinction (M 5 9.2) than during FR 1 similar reinforcement might reemerge.
(M 5 2.9). A return to the multiple-sched- A second undesirable pattern that may be
ule condition replicated the initial pattern in engendered by the absence of schedule-cor-
which higher rates of the alternative response related stimuli was evident in the data for
were observed during the FR 1 component. Mixed Session 112, in that high levels of
Julie’s data replicate the effects of schedule- alternative responding may occur during a
correlated stimuli observed with Jake (and scheduled extinction period and culminate
suggested by Karen’s data). At the end of the in reinforcement. Successively higher rates of
study, Julie’s SIB remained near zero (mean responding observed within each extinction
for the last 10 sessions was 0.8) and most of period during Session 112 were followed by
her alternative responses (74.9%) were emit- access to reinforcement when responding
SCHEDULE THINNING 33

Figure 4. Cumulative number of alternative responses across schedule components for three sessions from
Julie’s assessment of schedule-correlated stimuli. Open symbols represent two mixed-schedule sessions; filled
symbols represent a single multiple-schedule session.

continued into the FR 1 component. This unacceptably high rates of responding dur-
pattern seemingly increased the likelihood ing extinction result in reinforcement (e.g.,
that high rates of alternative behavior con- Mixed Session 112); rather, the alternative
tinued to occur under extinction and is un- response predominantly occurred when and
desirable for the same reason that FI sched- only when reinforcement was available. Al-
ules may not be preferred: High rates of the though the clinical outcome of Julie’s eval-
alternative behavior (even though they may uation is compromised somewhat by the
not be reinforced) may be disruptive in cer- continued high rates of alternative respond-
tain contexts (e.g., classrooms, restaurants). ing at the end of the study, her data clearly
The pattern of behavior evident in the demonstrate the importance of schedule-cor-
data from Multiple Session 106 exemplifies related stimuli when attempting to decrease
the benefits of including schedule-correlated the availability of reinforcement within
stimuli. During the signaled FR 1 compo- DRA interventions.
nents, alternative responding occurred at
steady rates. However, when the contingency
as well as the schedule-correlated stimuli GENERAL DISCUSSION
changed from reinforcement to extinction, Based on data obtained during pairwise
responding rapidly ceased. These data show functional analyses, which showed that the
that the alternative response was not com- problem behaviors (SIB and aggression) of 3
pletely eliminated following a period of ex- individuals were maintained by social-posi-
tinction (e.g., Mixed Session 107), nor did tive reinforcement, DRA interventions com-
34 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

monly described as FCT were successful in Results obtained with the delay procedure
decreasing the frequency of problem behav- (Karen) replicated those of basic research on
iors and increasing the frequency of alter- the effects of delays to reinforcement (Lattal,
native behaviors. Several methods for thin- 1984; Williams, 1976) as well as those re-
ning the schedule of reinforcement for alter- ported by Fisher et al. (2000) and Hagopian
native behavior were then evaluated. A re- et al. (1998), in that increasing delays were
inforcement-delay procedure resulted in associated with decreases in the target or al-
extinction of the alternative response and ternative behavior and increases in problem
slight recovery of SIB for 1 participant (Ka- behavior. However, responding has been
ren). An FI schedule resulted in unaccept- maintained during delays to reinforcement
ably high rates of the alternative response for when a brief (Schaal, Schuh, & Branch,
the same participant. A mixed schedule re- 1992) or continuous (Lattal; Richards,
sulted in indiscriminate alternative respond- 1981) signal was present during the delay.
ing (Jake and Julie) and recovery of SIB For example, Vollmer, Borrero, Lalli, and
(Jake only). Finally, a multiple schedule in- Daniel (1999) showed that, in the absence
volving the alternation of signaled periods of of a stimulus signaling a delay to reinforce-
reinforcement and extinction was successful ment for appropriate behavior, their partic-
in decreasing the overall rate of alternative ipant engaged in problem behavior (which
responding and its rate of reinforcement produced immediate reinforcement); these
while maintaining near-zero levels of prob- results were reversed with the addition of a
lem behavior for the 3 participants. continuous signal during delays to reinforce-
The thinning of reinforcement schedules ment. These data provide some evidence of
for alternative behavior is rarely demonstrated the importance of signaled delays to rein-
or discussed in most research on FCT; nev- forcement in FCT interventions.
ertheless, procedures for accomplishing this Signaled delays, however, may pose some
goal are important to the integrity of FCT limitations. Hagopian et al. (1998) reported
interventions under more natural conditions. that a brief signal at the beginning of delay
Some authors have reported that rates of com- periods resulted in limited success (only
munication occur at manageable levels im- 42% of participants reached the terminal
mediately following training (Durand, 1999; goal under these conditions). Even under
Durand & Carr, 1991, 1992); however, this continuously signaled delays, contingency-
outcome is not evident in most studies in weakening effects may be evident (Lattal,
which data on communication are presented 1984), in that rates of responding are lower
(e.g., Campbell & Lutzker, 1993; Carr & Du- during delayed reinforcement relative to
rand, 1985; Fisher et al., 1993; Frea & conditions of immediate reinforcement (in-
Hughes, 1997; Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, Con- dependent of the length of the delay or the
trucci, & Maglieri, 1997; Shirley, Iwata, signaling procedure). By shifting the delay
Kahng, Mazaleski, & Lerman, 1997; Shukla from between the emission of a response and
& Albin, 1996; Wacker et al., 1990). Typi- reinforcement to the time period between
cally, rates of communication following train- successive reinforcements (essentially chang-
ing are often as high as those observed for the ing a delay procedure to an FI schedule), a
problem behavior prior to training. Given that strong contingency is maintained. Although
most FCT studies also involve FR 1 reinforce- the FI schedule may engender different
ment for the alternative response, it seems un- problems (excessive response rates) than
likely that caregivers could maintain consisten- those observed with the reinforcement-delay
cy under such an arrangement. procedure, this arrangement may be prefer-
SCHEDULE THINNING 35

able if high rates of the alternative response of limiting access to the alternative response
are tolerable. For instance, if social reinforce- (i.e., the microswitch or other augmentative
ment were to be delivered for academic or communication systems such as picture
vocational behavior whose occurrence at cards) to times when reinforcement is avail-
high rates would not be disruptive (as op- able. The augmentative device itself should
posed to potentially distracting vocal mands come to occasion responding; no responding
for reinforcement), an FI schedule might be would occur when reinforcement is unavail-
attractive as a maintenance procedure. Alter- able (simply because it cannot occur in the
natively, Long (1962) showed that the use absence of the device). However, this ar-
of an external ‘‘clock’’ (a light that changed rangement may be feasible only when it is
from dim to bright as the interval was to possible to restrict access to the alternative
expire) reduced responding during the inter- response, which would not be the case if the
reinforcement interval of an FI schedule. A alternative response were vocal or gestural.
similar signaling procedure may be helpful The procedures described in the present
in generating desirable patterns of behavior study involved arbitrary, but salient and
during FCT interventions. transportable, discriminative stimuli to oc-
As an alternative to both reinforcement casion (white cards) or inhibit (red cards)
delay and FI schedules, the multiple-sched- responding. Although a variety of stimuli
ule arrangement was most effective in thin- that typically precede the delivery of rein-
ning reinforcement for alternative behavior forcement in natural settings may, over time,
while maintaining adequate rates of the al- occasion responding, these stimuli may not
ternative response and suppression of prob- necessarily be predictive of the availability of
lem behavior. The multiple schedule facili- reinforcement (e.g., social reinforcement is
tated rapid schedule control and subsequent not always available each time a parent en-
stimulus control over alternative responding ters the room, especially if he or she is about
such that it occurred either when and only to answer the phone). Arbitrary stimuli may
when (Jake) or predominantly when (Karen be preferred to more naturally occurring
and Julie) reinforcement was available for stimuli because better stimulus control
such behavior. The successive FR 1 periods might develop and override preexisting, but
allow the newly acquired alternative response only somewhat predictive, naturally occur-
to be reinforced frequently and immediately, ring discriminative stimuli. However, addi-
thereby maintaining a strong contingency. tional research is needed to identify methods
Although periods of nonreinforcement are for selecting discriminative stimuli (artificial
introduced, the contingency between the al- or naturally occurring) that are salient, func-
ternative response and reinforcement should tional across a variety of settings, and pre-
remain strong due to the infrequent occur- ferred by those who will use them. Ques-
rence of unreinforced responses engendered tions regarding the speed with which extinc-
by the inclusion of a distinctive stimulus (SD) tion periods can be introduced, the relative
during periods of extinction. Thus, the mul- effectiveness of fixed versus variable compo-
tiple schedule may facilitate the program- nent durations, and alternative strategies for
ming of extended periods during which al- producing desirable performance under
ternative behavior is not reinforced without DRA contingencies also could be addressed
degrading the alternative response–reinforcer in future studies. For example, differential-
relation. reinforcement-of-low-rate-behavior sched-
An alternative to the multiple-schedule ules (see Vollmer & Iwata, 1992, for a dis-
procedure used in the present study consists cussion of the clinical utility of these sched-
36 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

ules) or increasing the effort involved in functional equivalence training to reduce severe
challenging behavior: A case study. Journal of De-
emitting an alternative response (cf. Horner velopmental and Physical Disabilities, 5, 203–216.
& Day, 1991) may prove useful in produc- Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing be-
ing outcomes similar to those observed in havior problems through functional communica-
this study. tion training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
18, 111–126.
Finally, although the signaling procedures Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication
produced generally desirable response pat- training using assistive devices: Recruiting natural
terns during schedule thinning for the 3 par- communities of reinforcement. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 32, 247–267.
ticipants in this study, the procedures inev- Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional
itably involve gradual reintroduction of the communication training to reduce challenging be-
establishing operation for problem behavior havior: Maintenance and application in new set-
over successively longer periods of time. Al- tings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
251–264.
though discriminative control over alterna- Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1992). An analysis
tive responding may be achieved, schedule of maintenance following functional communi-
thinning may eventually evoke the problem cation training. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-
ysis, 25, 777–794.
behavior, especially as one nears the terminal Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of
schedule. Jake’s data (Figure 2) provide evi- reinforcement. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
dence of this phenomenon. This problem Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L.
might be avoided by making other sources (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary re-
inforcers as treatment for self-injurious behavior.
of reinforcement available at times when the Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.
reinforcer that maintains problem behavior Fisher, W. W., Kuhn, D. E., & Thompson, R. H.
is unavailable. For example, Fisher et al. (1998). Establishing discriminative control of re-
sponding using functional and alternative rein-
(1998) arranged a second alternative re- forcers during functional communication training.
sponse to produce an arbitrary reinforcer Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 543–560.
(i.e., one that was not functionally related to Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Jef-
ferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional
the problem behavior) when the maintaining communication training with and without extinc-
reinforcer was unavailable. This procedure tion and punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior
resulted in suppression of problem behavior Analysis, 26, 23–36.
even as the maintaining reinforcer was avail- Fisher, W. W., Thompson, R. H., Hagopian, L. P.,
Bowman, L. G., & Krug, A. (2000). Facilitating
able intermittently. The noncontingent de- tolerance of delayed reinforcement during func-
livery of arbitrary reinforcers has also been tional communication training. Behavior Modifi-
shown to maintain low rates of problem be- cation, 24, 3–9.
havior even when its establishing operation Frea, W. D., & Hughes, C. (1997). Functional anal-
ysis and treatment of social-communicative be-
is present (Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski, havior of adolescents with developmental disabil-
1997; Hanley, Piazza, & Fisher, 1997) and ities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 701–
may be effective in eliminating problem be- 704.
Goh, H., & Iwata, B. A. (1994). Behavioral persis-
havior during periods of extinction for newly tence and variability during extinction of self-in-
acquired alternative responses (see Fisher et jury maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Be-
al., 2000, for an example of this strategy). havior Analysis, 27, 173–174.
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., & Legacy, S. M.
(1994). Schedule effects of noncontingent rein-
REFERENCES forcement on attention-maintained destructive be-
havior in identical quadruplets. Journal of Applied
Bijou, S. W., & Orlando, R. (1961). Rapid devel- Behavior Analysis, 27, 317–325.
opment of multiple-schedule performances with Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Ac-
mentally retarded children. Journal of the Experi- quisto, J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness
mental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 7–16. of functional communication training with and
Campbell, R. V., & Lutzker, J. R. (1993). Using without extinction and punishment: A summary
SCHEDULE THINNING 37

of 21 inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior gressive and self-injurious behavior of institution-
Analysis, 31, 211–235. alized children through reinforcement of other be-
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). haviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7,
Noncontingent presentation of attention and al- 313–325.
ternative stimuli in the treatment of attention- Richards, R. (1981). A comparison of signaled and
maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied unsignaled delay of reinforcement. Journal of the
Behavior Analysis, 30, 229–237. Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 145–152.
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Contrucci, Schaal, D. W., Schuh, K. J., & Branch, M. N. (1992).
S. A., & Maglieri, K. A. (1997). Evaluation of Key-pecking of pigeons under variable-interval
client preference for function-based treatment schedules of briefly signaled delayed reinforcement:
packages. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, Effects of variable-interval value. Journal of the Ex-
459–473. perimental Analysis of Behavior, 58, 277–286.
Herrick, R. M., Myers, J. L., & Korotkin, A. L. Schroeder, S. R. (1972). Parametric effects of rein-
(1959). Changes in SD and in SD rates during the forcement frequency, amount of reinforcement,
development of an operant discrimination. Journal and required response force on sheltered work-
of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, shop behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analy-
359–364. sis, 5, 431–441.
Horner, R. H., & Day, H. M. (1991). The effects of Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S., Mazaleski, J.
response efficiency on functionally equivalent L., & Lerman, D. C. (1997). Does functional
competing behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior communication training compete with ongoing
Analysis, 24, 719–732. contingencies of reinforcement? An analysis dur-
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. ing response acquisition and maintenance. Journal
E., & Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a func- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 93–104.
tional analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Be- Shukla, S., & Albin, R. W. (1996). Effects of extinc-
havior Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from tion alone and extinction plus functional com-
Analysis and Intervention in Developmental Dis- munication training on covariation of problem
abilities, 2, 3–20, 1982) behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29,
Iwata, B. A., Duncan, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman, 565–568.
D. C., & Shore, B. A. (1994). A sequential, test- Vollmer, T. R., Borrero, J. C., Lalli, J. S., & Daniel,
control methodology for conducting functional D. (1999). Evaluating self-control and impulsiv-
analyses of self-injurious behavior. Behavior Mod- ity in children with severe behavior disorders.
ification, 18, 289–306. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 451–466.
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Wallace, Vollmer, T. R., & Iwata, B. A. (1992). Differential
M. D. (2000). A comparison of procedures for reinforcement as treatment for behavior disorders:
programming noncontingent reinforcement Procedural and functional variations. Research in
schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, Developmental Disabilities, 13, 393–417.
223–231. Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R.
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Non- G., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of atten-
contingent reinforcement as treatment for severe tion in the treatment of attention-maintained self-
problem behavior: Some procedural variations. injurious behavior: Noncontingent reinforcement
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 127–137. and differential reinforcement of other behavior.
Lattal, K. A. (1984). Signal functions in delayed re- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21.
inforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G.,
of Behavior, 42, 239–253. Berg, W., Reimers, T., Cooper, L., Cigrand, K.,
Long, E. R. (1962). Additional techniques for pro- & Donn, L. (1990). A component analysis of
ducing multiple-schedule control in children. functional communication training across three
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, topographies of severe behavior problems. Journal
443–455. of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R. (1996). Combining Weiner, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-interval
noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) and differ- performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis
ential reinforcement schedules as treatment for ab- of Behavior, 12, 349–373.
errant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal- Williams, B. A. (1976). The effects of unsignaled de-
ysis, 29, 43–51. layed reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental
Orlando, R., & Bijou, S. W. (1960). Single and mul- Analysis of Behavior, 26, 441–449.
tiple schedules of reinforcement in developmen-
tally retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Received August 21, 2000
Analysis of Behavior, 3, 339–348. Final acceptance November 30, 2000
Repp, A. C., & Deitz, S. M. (1974). Reducing ag- Action Editor, Wayne Fisher
38 GREGORY P. HANLEY et al.

STUDY QUESTIONS

1. How are NCR and DRO schedules typically thinned, and why are these procedures not
applicable to DRA thinning?

2. How were the functional analyses conducted? What are the advantages and disadvantages of
this approach relative to a more typical full-scale assessment?

3. Describe the results of the three thinning procedures for Karen in terms of their effects on
target behaviors, alternative responses, percentage of reinforcers earned, and percentage of
alternative responses resulting in reinforcement.

4. What is the difference between mixed and multiple schedules?

5. What was the terminal-schedule probe to which Jake was exposed, and what was its purpose?

6. Summarize the results obtained for Julie.

7. Summarize the results depicted in Figure 4. What undesirable characteristics of mixed sched-
ules are reflected in these data?

8. What characteristics of multiple schedules make them attractive as a means of thinning


reinforcement schedules during DRA interventions?

Questions prepared by Juliet Conners and Eileen Roscoe, The University of Florida

You might also like