You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis 2021, 9999, 1–13 NUMBER 9999 ()

Using brief habit reversal and an interdependent group


contingency to reduce public-speaking speech disfluencies
Christopher J. Perrin , Sarah A. Hensel, Durie L. Lynch, Lisa R. Gallegos,
Kendall Bell and Kristen Carpenter
Department of Psychology and Counseling, Georgian Court University

This study replicated and extended previous research on the effectiveness of brief habit reversal,
consisting of awareness training and competing response training (silent pause) in reducing the
rate of speech disfluencies during public speaking. Nine university students, divided into
3 groups, delivered short speeches on a novel topic. Brief habit reversal combined with an
interdependent group contingency resulted in a meaningful reduction in speech disfluencies
with only 1 group requiring booster sessions. Relative to baseline, competing responses increased
for all participants but only remained high for 1 participant following training. Reductions in
speech disfluencies maintained during follow-up when participants presented in front of a small
audience.
Key words: awareness training, brief habit reversal, competing response training,
interdependent group contingencies, public speaking

Public speaking is a valuable skill used in recent studies have evaluated interventions to
school, careers, and job interviews (Montes reduce such speech disfluencies (Mancuso &
et al., 2019; Spieler & Miltenberger, 2017). Miltenberger, 2016; Montes et al., 2019, 2021;
However, presenting can elicit anxiety and Pawlik & Perrin, 2020; Spieler &
evoke speech disfluencies, reducing the effec- Miltenberger, 2017). As speech disfluencies dur-
tiveness of the presentation (Bell, 2011; ing public speaking have many commonalities
Clark & Tree, 2002; Henderson, 2007) and in with other habit behaviors, this research consists
some cases result in a diagnosis of public speak- of applications of brief habit reversal (BHR).
ing anxiety (PSA). The DSM-5 (Diagnostic and Common components of BHR include
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders; Ameri- response description and detection (i.e., awareness
can Psychiatric Association, 2013) classifies training) followed by competing response training.
PSA as a social anxiety disorder. According to As an intervention for public-speaking speech
Tejwani et al. (2016), PSA affects 15% to 30% disfluencies, participants are trained on the topog-
of the general population, with similar percent- raphy of their speech disfluencies (i.e., response
ages among university students. Furthermore, description), and then they practice identifying
PSA has been found to impact success in both their speech disfluencies (i.e., response detection)
educational and employment settings (e.g., Stein from both recordings of their baseline speeches
et al., 1996). Given both the importance of pub- and while delivering speeches. During competing
lic speaking and the prevalence of PSA, several response training, participants are taught to emit
an often-incompatible response as a replacement
We have no known conflicts of interest to disclose. behavior and then practice emitting the competing
This research was funded by a Georgian Court Univer-
sity Faculty Development Mini-Grant. response while delivering speeches. Previous studies
Address correspondence to: Christopher J. Perrin, have shown BHR to be effective at reducing
Georgian Court University, Department of Psychology public-speaking speech disfluencies when it con-
and Counseling, 900 Lakewood Ave, Lakewood, NJ
08701. E-mail: cperrin@georgian.edu sists of awareness training and competing response
doi: 10.1002/jaba.867 training (Mancuso & Miltenberger, 2016),
© 2021 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior (SEAB).
1
2 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

awareness training and an instructed, as opposed The group contingency required all participants
to trained, competing response (Pawlik & to both choose to work for the larger reinforcer
Perrin, 2020), and awareness training alone as opposed to the smaller one, and then remain
(e.g., Montes et al., 2019, 2021; Spieler & engaged for the entire delay interval to earn the
Miltenberger, 2017). larger reinforcer. If any group member selected
Although each of these variations of BHR the smaller reinforcer or stopped working early,
were effective, the individual format of the pro- the entire group earned the small reinforcer. The
cedures may reduce the practicality of BHR as group contingency resulted in both groups
a treatment given the length of time necessary increasing choice of the delayed reinforcer and
to implement BHR. Implementing BHR in a their work duration by seven times the natural
small group format may improve efficiency, baseline duration. The group format meant that
resulting in more widespread adoption in set- the same resources needed to improve the self-
tings such as universities. We know of two control of two individuals in this case resulted in
studies that have evaluated group-based habit improvements for six individuals. If similar
reversal training, both in the treatment of improvements are observed when implementing
Tourette’s Syndrome (Nissen et al., 2019; BHR in a small group format, it may improve
Yates et al., 2016). Although data supported the likelihood of more widespread adoption.
the efficacy of group-based habit reversal, both In addition to evaluating group-based BHR,
studies used group designs and relied on indi- the use of an instructed competing response
rect measures of habit behaviors. In addition, requires additional investigation. Pawlik and
Nissen et al. (2019) combined the procedure Perrin (2020) is the only study we found that
with exposure and response prevention. examined the effectiveness of instructing the
A possible limitation of group-based habit use of a competing response to reduce public-
reversal training is the complexity of monitoring speaking speech disfluencies. Also, all previous
concurrent individual contingencies. One solu- studies evaluating BHR to reduce public-
tion to this would be an interdependent contin- speaking speech disfluencies used a non-
gency in which treatment decisions are based on concurrent multiple-baseline design, which is
the performance of the group as opposed to indi- subject to historical confounds. As such, the
vidual participants. Interdependent group contin- purpose of this present study was twofold; to
gencies have been successfully applied to a evaluate the effectiveness of small-group BHR
variety of target responses including disruptive with an interdependent group contingency, and
classroom behavior (e.g., Barrish et al., 1969), to further evaluate the efficacy of instructing a
engagement in physical activity (Hirsch competing response using a concurrent multi-
et al., 2016), participation in a college course ple baseline design.
review game (Neef et al., 2011), staff attendance
at a residential treatment facility (Brown &
Method
Redmon, 1989), and smoking abstinence
(Meredith & Dallery, 2013). In one application, Participants and Setting
Dixon and Holcomb (2000) used an Thirteen students attending a small Catholic
interdependent group contingency to teach self- University were recruited to participate in the
control to dually diagnosed adults. Like BHR, study. Based on their availability, four partici-
progressive delay procedures can be time con- pants were excluded from the study, three par-
suming when implemented in a one-on-one for- ticipants were assigned to Group 1 (Pamela,
mat. To increase efficiency, the authors arranged Jonathan, Hailey), and the remaining six partic-
the participants into two groups of three. ipants were randomly assigned to Groups
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 3

2 (Madison, Cora, Veronica) and 3 (Opal, analyzing and editing software, Audacity® 2.3.0
Alice, Stanley). All participants stated an inter- (Audacity Team, 2018). Frequency measures
est in improving their public speaking skills. As were converted to a rate by dividing the fre-
compensation for their time, participants quency by the speech duration, calculated by
received extra credit for one of their psychology recording the time elapsed between the
courses and a $50 prepaid VISA gift card after researcher stating, “Start” and either the partici-
completion of the study. pant or the experimenter holding up a red card.
Sessions occurred in one of two empty class- During BHR, frequency data were also col-
rooms at the university. The rooms were lected for response identification, defined as the
equipped with a podium, desks with chairs, a speaker raising their hand upon emitting a
computer to record the session, white and red speech disfluency. These data were converted
laminated cards (21.6 cm by 27.9 cm) to be to a percentage by dividing the number of
used as signals during speeches, and a digital responses identified by the total number of
timer. Lined paper, pencils, and highlighters speech disfluencies.
were also available for the participants to use to For group level analysis, speech disfluencies
prepare for their speeches. for each group member were added together to
form a group total frequency which was then
converted to a rate by dividing the group’s total
Target Behaviors frequency by the sum of each group member’s
Frequency data were collected on three types speech duration. Also, responses identified by
of speech disfluencies: filled pauses, tongue each group member were added together to
clicks, and inappropriate word use. Operational form a group response identification, which
definitions were identical to those described by was then converted to a percent identified by
Spieler and Miltenberger (2017) and Pawlik dividing the group’s total frequency by the
and Perrin (2020) with one exception. In addi- group’s total frequency of speech disfluencies
tion to collecting data on the inappropriate use and multiplied by 100.
of the word “like,” data were also collected on
inappropriate use of the phrase “you know.” Interobserver Agreement
This was defined as emitting the phrase “you A second observer collected data from the
know” as a filler or added to a sentence for video recordings for approximately 33% of ses-
emphasis (e.g., “I have, you know, brown hair” sions for each condition. Exact agreement was
or “I have brown hair, you know”). The use of calculated for speech disfluencies by dividing
the phrase “you know” in a grammatically and the session into 15 s intervals and dividing the
systematically appropriate way was not consid- number of intervals with agreement by
ered inappropriate (e.g., “You know that I have the number of intervals with agreement plus
brown hair”). Data for the three response forms disagreement and multiplying by 100. Mean
were combined into a single measure: speech agreement for baseline, post-BHR, and general-
disfluencies. As in Pawlik and Perrin, the com- ization was 93% (range, 83% to 98%) for filled
peting response was a silent pause defined as at pauses, 96% (range, 90% to 100%) for tongue
least a 3 s period of silence from the offset of clicks, and 94% (range, 85% to 100%) for
one speech sound until the onset of the next inappropriate use of the phrases “like” and
speech sound. Using the same methodology as “you know.” During BHR, mean agreement
Pawlik and Perrin, competing response data was 95% (range, 88% to 100%) for speech
were collected from the video recordings using disfluencies and 95% (range, 93% to 100%)
the silence finder function in the audio for response identification.
4 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

To assess the reliability of the Audacity® 2.3.0 the session procedures. Although sessions were
silence finder function, an experimenter collected conducted in a small group format, each member
data from the video recordings for approximately delivered their own speech, and members in the
21% of sessions. The sample included sessions audience were instructed to refrain from
from all participants and conditions, and com- responding during or providing feedback after a
peting responses occurred in 64% of sessions speech. All speeches were conducted in the fol-
assessed. Mean exact agreement for competing lowing sequence. The experimenter randomly
responses was 97% (range, 91% to 100%). selected two speech topics from a container of
25 possible topics. If a speech topic had already
been used, it was replaced with a new topic.
Procedure Speech topics were (a) identical to those used
A concurrent multiple baseline design across during both Spieler and Miltenberger (2017) as
groups was used to evaluate the effectiveness of well as Perrin and Pawlik (2020) and (b) based
small-group BHR and an interdependent group on opinion or personal experience (e.g., my first
contingency to decrease speech disfluencies dur- job, my dream place to live). The group was then
ing public speaking. The procedures were similar asked to choose one of the topics. Next, partici-
to those described by Pawlik and Perrin (2020) pants were instructed to each prepare a speech on
with several exceptions. First, video recordings the selected topic and to use the provided writing
were used as opposed to audio recordings. Sec- utensils and paper for note taking, but not to
ond, rather than meeting with each participant write a script. After 3 min, writing utensils were
individually, BHR was conducted in a small collected and a participant was randomly called to
group format using an interdependent group the front of the room to deliver their speech. The
contingency. Third, to clearly indicate the con- experimenter counted down from three, then told
clusion of their speech, participants were asked the participant to begin while starting the timer.
to hold up a card when finished. Fourth, partici- When 4 min elapsed, the experimenter held up a
pants were provided 3 min to prepare for a white card indicating that 1 min remained. After
speech as opposed to 5 min. 5 min, the experimenter held up a red card indi-
cating the allotted time was finished. At any point
Public-Speaking Abilities Rating Scale during the speech, the participant could raise a
Prior to the beginning of baseline and after second red card to signal the end of their speech.
completion of the study, all participants com- If the participant stopped speaking for 15 s or
pleted a public-speaking abilities rating scale raised the red card prior to 3 min, the experi-
(Mancuso & Miltenberger, 2016; Pawlik & menter stated, “Please continue.” This procedure
Perrin, 2020). The scale was a five-point Likert- was then repeated for the remaining participants
type self-rating scale about their current comfort, in the group.
confidence, and self-efficacy regarding their pub-
lic speaking abilities. The posttreatment self- Brief Habit Reversal
rating assessment scale included questions about At the start of BHR, the experimenter opera-
the participants’ abilities prior to and after the tionally defined each defined form of speech
study, as well as questions regarding their per- disfluency and described instances emitted by
ceived use of each specific target behavior. group members. For example, during an initial
BHR session, the experimenter stated, “One
Baseline common speech disfluency is what we refer to
During baseline, the experimenters met with as a filled pause. We have defined filled pauses
each group of participants separately and reviewed as speech sounds including ‘uh,’ ‘er,’ and ‘um’
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 5

that are not grammatically or semantically it less likely to occur. While speaking, if you feel
meaningful to the speech.” The experimenter yourself about to emit a speech disfluency, stop
then pointed out how certain group members talking and count to three silently.
said either “uh,” “er,” or “um” while delivering During each speech, the experimenter raised
their first speech. Next, participants watched a her hand for the first five speech disfluencies,
video containing a 1-min sample of each of the after which she only raised her hand if the par-
participants’ baseline speeches, which contained ticipant failed to detect a speech disfluency.
representative examples of their speech While other group members were giving their
disfluencies. Participants were instructed to speech, participants were asked to discreetly
raise their hand each time they heard a speech practice identifying speech disfluencies by
disfluency or if the experimenter raised her recording tally marks on a sheet of paper. Prac-
hand indicating a participant had missed a tice speeches continued until one of the follow-
speech disfluency. After the video ended, if any ing occurred: (a) all group members identified
of the participants failed to identify a speech 100% of their own speech disfluencies during
disfluency, the experimenter replayed that part their speech, (b) all group members identified
of the video, stated that “not all participants at least 85% of their own speech disfluencies
had identified the speech disfluency,” identified during two consecutive speeches, or (c) a group
the type of speech disfluency, and described member identified 84% or fewer speech
why it was a speech disfluency. For instance, disfluencies for three consecutive speeches. Par-
while meeting with one group, the experi- ticipants were not informed of these criteria or
menter showed a clip of a speech in which the progress toward meeting them. Rather, partici-
speaker emitted a tongue click that the group pants were told they were going to “continue
did not originally identify. The experimenter practicing speeches” or, if the group met a cri-
then paused the video, stated “not everyone terion, they were told they were going to “do
identified this tongue click,” explained that the something different.”
sound heard was a type of speech disfluency
referred to as a tongue click, and replayed the Post-BHR
clip once more for the group. At the conclusion Post-BHR was conducted at least 24 hr, but
of the video review, the participants were asked no longer than 3 days, after completion of
if they felt comfortable detecting speech BHR. Post-BHR procedures were identical to
disfluencies or wanted further practice. If any baseline. Participants were informed that they
participant wanted to practice further, the no longer needed to raise their hand to identify
group reviewed the video one additional time. speech disfluencies, nor would the experimenter
Participants then practiced identifying speech raise her hand. Post-BHR was complete when
disfluencies while giving speeches. Speeches were groups maintained a decreasing or stable trend
conducted in the same manner as baseline; how- of speech disfluencies with at least an 80%
ever, the same speech topic was used for the reduction from baseline rates.
entirety of BHR. Before the groups began deliver-
ing their individual speeches, the participants were Booster Sessions
told to raise their hand upon emitting a speech Booster sessions were conducted if a group
disfluency or if the experimenter raised her hand. did not collectively meet an 80% reduction in
In addition, the experimenter instructed a compet- speech disfluencies from baseline within the
ing response by stating, first two post-BHR sessions. The first booster
A 3-s pause can serve as a competing response, session occurred at least 24 hr after the previous
which can replace the speech disfluency making post-BHR session. Booster sessions followed
6 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

the same procedures and criteria as BHR with condition, rate of speech disfluencies was low for
the exception that participants did not practice all participants. All groups met an 80% reduction
identifying speech disfluencies while watching from baseline in rates of speech disfluencies.
the video. Levels of response identification varied across
participants. In Group 1, Pamela and Jonathan
Follow-up immediately demonstrated high, but variable
Follow-up probes were conducted 28 days levels of response identification, whereas
after post-BHR for Group 1, 21 days after Hailey’s levels of response identification
post-BHR for Group 2, and 20 days after post- occurred at moderate levels. Collectively, the
BHR for Group 3. Procedures were identical to group’s response identification occurred at
those during baseline, except speeches were moderately high levels (M = 69.2%; range,
delivered in front of a 14-person audience con- 57.1% to 77.8%). In Group 2, Cora and
sisting of members from all three groups and Veronica demonstrated moderate levels of
the researchers. response identification, while Madison’s levels
of response identification remained low
Treatment Rating Scale throughout BHR. Collectively, response identi-
At the completion of the study, social validity fication occurred at moderate levels with high
was assessed using a rating scale similar to that variability for Group 2 (M = 43.3%; range,
described by Mancuso and Miltenberger (2016) 33.3% to 55.6%), In Group 3, Opal identified
and Pawlik and Perrin (2020). The scale used a all of her speech disfluencies in the second
five-point Likert-Type scale to measure the par- BHR session, after which levels of response
ticipants’ perception of the acceptability, advan- identification reduced to a moderate level. For
tages, disadvantages, difficulties, and effectiveness Alice, response identification increased from
of the BHR aspect of the study. moderate to high levels across the condition.
Stanley’s response identification occurred at
moderately low levels initially and decreased
Results across the condition. As a whole, Group 3 dem-
Figure 1 depicts the rate of speech disfluencies onstrated the lowest level of response identifica-
and the percentage of responses identified across tion (M = 23.6%; range, 14.7% to 42.9%).
treatment conditions for each participant. During All groups had at least one member who did
baseline, all participants demonstrated moderate not identify 100% of speech disfluencies in one
to high rates of speech disfluencies. When session or 80% of speech disfluencies in two
summed, this resulted in an increasing trend for consecutive sessions (Group 1, Hailey; Group
Groups 1 (top panels, M = 5.8 per minuteute; 2, Madison; Group 3, Alice and Stanley). As a
range, 4.7 to 7.7) and 2 (middle panels, M = 7.4 result, the other five participants were required
per minuteute; range, 6.7 to 7.8), and stable rates to complete on average 3.6 speeches (range,
for Group 3 (M = 9.6 per minuteute; range, 1.9 1 to 7) after meeting one of the criteria. Total
to 11.5). time to complete BHR, including booster ses-
During BHR, speech disfluencies immediately sions, ranged from 180 to 220 min per group.
decreased for all participants in Groups 1 (M = 1 During post-BHR, all participants in Group
per minuteute; range, 0.3 to 2.7) and 3 (M = 3.1 1 demonstrated low and stable rates of speech
per minuteute; range, 1.0 to 4.6). Rates of speech disfluencies, with the group’s combined rate
disfluencies for participants in Group 2 demon- remaining below criterion (M = 0.9 per
strated a slow decreasing trend (M = 2.1 per minuteute; range, 0.5 to 1.2). Similar response
minuteute; range, 0.8 to 3.1). By the end of the patterns were observed for all participants in
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 7

Figure 1
Speech Disfluencies and Response Identification Across Conditions
BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR
24 100 24 100 24 100
20 80 20 80 20 80
16 16 16
60 60 60
12 Pamela 12 Jonathan 12 Hailey
40 40 40
8 8 8
4 20 4 20 4 20

0 0 0 0 0 0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

Percentage of Responses Identified


BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR
Responses Per Minute

24 100 24 100 24 100


20 80 20 80 20 80
16 16 16
60 60 60
Madison Cora Veronica
12 12 12
40 40 40
8 8 8
4 20 4 20 4 20

0 0 0 0 0 0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR


24 100 24 100 24 100
20 80 20 80 20 80
16 16 16
60 60 60
Opal Alice Stanley
12 12 12
40 40 40
8 8 8
4 20 4 20 4 20

0 0 0 0 0 0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

Sessions
Note. Dashed lines represent an 80% reduction from the group’s baseline rate of speech disfluencies. Inverted closed tri-
angles represent speech disfluencies during the follow-up probes. BL = baseline, BHR = Brief Habit Reversal, Post-
BHR = Post Brief Habit Reversal.

Group 3 (M = 1.5 per minuteute; range, 1.0 speech disfluencies during the first session but
to 2.0). For Group 2, rates of speech was required to complete two additional sessions
disfluencies were low for Cora and Veronica; as the group did not meet a mastery criterion.
however, Madison immediately demonstrated Following booster sessions, speech disfluencies
increased rates. As such, the group’s combined maintained at low rates for all participants in
rate of speech disfluencies occurred above an Group 2 (M = 1.3 per minuteute; range,
80% reduction from baseline for two consecutive 0.6 – 1.7).
sessions (M = 2.6 per minuteute; range, 2.3 to During the follow-up probe, speech
2.9) resulting in the need for booster sessions. disfluencies remained low and within the range
During booster sessions, speech disfluencies of those during post-BHR for seven of nine
immediately decreased for all participants participants. Rates of speech disfluencies for
resulting in a combined rate below criterion Pamela and Madison increased to levels similar
(M = 0.7 per minuteute; range, 0.4 to 1.0). to those at the start of BHR; however,
Response identification occurred at levels compa- remained below baseline. Collectively, rate of
rable to those during BHR (M = 28.4%, range; speech disfluencies occurred below the criterion
16.7% to 40.0%). Cora identified 100% of level for Group 1 (1.4 per minuteute) and
8 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

Figure 2
Competing Responses Across Conditions
BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR
3.5 3.5 3.5
3.0 3.0 3.0
2.5 2.5 2.5
2.0 Pamela 2.0 Jonathan 2.0 Hailey
1.5 1.5 1.5
1.0 1.0 1.0
0.5 0.5 0.5
0.0 0.0 0.0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR


Responses Per Minute

3.5 3.5 3.5


3.0 3.0 3.0
2.5 2.5 2.5
2.0 Madison 2.0 Cora 2.0 Veronica
1.5 1.5 1.5
1.0 1.0 1.0
0.5 0.5 0.5
0.0 0.0 0.0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR BL BHR Post-BHR


3.5 3.5 3.5
3.0 3.0 3.0
2.5 2.5 2.5
2.0 Opal 2.0 Alice 2.0 Stanley
1.5 1.5 1.5
1.0 1.0 1.0
0.5 0.5 0.5
0.0 0.0 0.0
5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25 5 9 13 17 21 25

Sessions
Note. Inverted closed triangles represent competing responses during the follow-up probes. BL = baseline, BHR = Brief
Habit Reversal, Post-BHR = Post Brief Habit Reversal.

Group 3 (0.4 per minuteute). For Group 3.0). Pamela (M = 0.3 per minute; range, 0 –
2, speech disfluencies occurred above criterion 0.6), Hailey (M = 0.6 per minute; range,
but remained below baseline (2.5 per 0 – 1.1) and Alice (M = 0.4 per minute; range,
minuteute; 66% reduction). 0 – 0.9) demonstrated low and variable rates of
Figure 2 depicts each participant’s rates of competing responses. For Opal, rates of com-
competing responses during each condition. peting responses were initially high, but
Except for Stanley (M = 0.74 per minute; decreased across the condition (M = 0.7 per
range, 0.3 – 1.0), all participants demonstrated minute; range, 0.3 – 1.3). Veronica and Cora
at or near zero rates of competing responses initially emitted competing responses, but rates
during baseline. During BHR, rates of compet- reduced to zero by the end of the condition.
ing responses increased for all participants. Jonathan only emitted competing responses in
Stanley emitted the highest rates of competing the final session. Following BHR, Veronica,
responses (M = 1.9 per minute; range, 1.2 – Alice, and Cora demonstrated zero rates of
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 9

Table 1 Table 2

Summary of Responses on the Public Speaking Rating Scale Summary of Responses on the Treatment Acceptability Rating
Scale
Mean Rating (Range)
Question Baseline Post-BHR Mean Rating
Question (Range)
1. How comfortable (do/did) you feel 3.2 (3 – 5) 2.4 (1 – 5)
when engaging in public speaking 1. How acceptable was the training? 4.8 (4 – 5)
(BEFORE training)? 2. How willing were you to participate in the 4.8 (4 – 5)
2. How comfortable do you feel when N/A 4.4 (4 – 5) training?
engaging in public speaking NOW 3. To what extent do you think there might 1.8 (1 – 2)
after training? have been disadvantages in the training?
3. How would you rate your overall 3.2 (3 – 5) 2.6 (1 – 5) 4. To what extent do you think there might 3.9 (2 – 5)
ability as a public speaker have been advantages in the training?
(BEFORE training)?
5. How difficult was it to participate in the 1.9 (1 – 4)
4. How would you rate your overall N/A 4.0 (3 – 5)
ability as a public speaker NOW
training?
after training? 6. How much did you like the training? 3.7 (3 – 4)
5. How confident (do/did) you feel 3.1 (3 – 5) 2.4 (1 – 5) 7. How effective was the training in terms of 4.1 (3 – 5)
when engaging in public speaking reducing your use of filled pauses?
activities (BEFORE training)? 8. How effective was the training in terms of 4.0 (3 – 5)
6. How confident do you feel NOW N/A 4.0 (3 – 5) reducing your use of tongue clicks?
when engaging in public speaking 9. How effective was the training in terms of 4.2 (3 – 5)
activities? reducing your inappropriate use of the
7. While publicly speaking, how 3.6 (3 – 5) 4.0 (2 – 5) words “like” or “you know”?
anxious (are/were) you (BEFORE 10. How effective was the training in terms of 4.3 (3 – 5)
training)? improving your overall ability to publicly
8. While publicly speaking, how N/A 2.1 (1 – 3) speak in front of an audience?
anxious are you NOW after 11. Would you recommend the training to 4.9 (4 – 5)
training? others who are also struggling with their
9. How often (do/did) you use fillers, 3.9 (3 – 4) 4.4 (3 – 5) public speaking?
such as um, uh, or er, during
public speaking (BEFORE
training)?
Note. Scores range from 1 = not at all/ none to 5 = very/
10. How often do you use fillers, such N/A 3.0 (1 – 3) many. Items 3 and 5 were reverse coded.
as um, uh, or er, during public
speaking NOW after training?
11. How often (do/did) you 3.9 (2 – 5) 4.4 (3 - 5)
inappropriately use the words participants to maintain moderate to low levels
“like” or “you know” during public
speaking (BEFORE training)? of competing responses, though Stanley’s rates
12.How often do you inappropriately N/A 2.6 (2 – 4) were similar to baseline by the end of the con-
use the words “like” or “you
know” during public speaking dition. Further reductions in competing
NOW after training? responses occurred during follow-up probes
13.How often (do/did) you use 1.6 (1 – 3) 2.9 (1 – 5)
tongue clicks during public with all participants demonstrating zero rates
speaking (BEFORE training)? except Hailey (0.4 per minute), Madison (0.3
14. How often do you use tongue N/A 1.8 (1 – 4)
clicks during public speaking per minute), and Stanley (0.2 per minute).
NOW after training? Table 1 depicts the results of the public
Note. Scores range from 1 = not at all / poor to 5 = very/ speaking rating scale. On their baseline scales,
excellent participants reported moderate levels of com-
fort, public speaking abilities, and confidence
with public speaking and moderately high
competing responses. Rates of competing levels of anxiety and use of most topographies
responses for Pamela, Jonathan, Madison, and of speech disfluencies. Following BHR, all par-
Opal were initially low, but decreased to zero ticipants reported an increase in comfort, pub-
across the condition. Hailey (M = 0.7 per lic speaking abilities, and confidence. Also,
minute; range, 0 – 2.5) and Stanley (M = 1.0 participants reported a decrease in anxiety and
per minute; range, 0.5 – 1.2) were the only usage of speech disfluencies from before BHR
10 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

to after BHR. In addition, most participants people separately. A possible way to improve
rated their pretraining performance worse fol- efficacy and efficiency would be to increase the
lowing BHR than they had prior to training. saliency of the interdependent contingency.
Results of the treatment rating scale indicated Although various aspects of the procedures and
moderate to high social validity (see Table 2). instructions indicated the interdependent
All participants positively rated BHR in the nature of the contingency (e.g., speech
areas of acceptability, willingness to participate, disfluencies were reviewed if any member failed
advantages, likability, effectiveness, and willing- to identify the disfluency), participants were
ness to recommend to others. All participants not explicitly informed that their progress was
rated BHR as having little disadvantages and dependent upon their team’s performance. As
low levels of difficulty. such, some of the beneficial social contingencies
enacted by the group contingency may have been
absent. An alternative option would be to use an
Discussion independent group contingency. Comparisons of
The results of this study showed that small independent and interdependent group contin-
group BHR with an interdependent group con- gencies have found similar levels of efficacy
tingency was effective at reducing speech (e.g., Groves & Austin, 2017). Using an inde-
disfluencies. Following BHR, levels of group pendent group contingency, participants would
speech disfluencies reduced by at least 80% receive BHR in a group format, but individual
from baseline levels for all groups. At the indi- responding would determine when a participant
vidual level, eight of nine participants demon- moves to post-BHR. In this arrangement, experi-
strated an 80% reduction in baseline levels of menters maintain the benefits of the group for-
speech disfluencies while the ninth participant mat while participants regain time savings
demonstrated a 72% reduction. associated with individual mastery criteria.
Whether small-group BHR was more effi- The second purpose of this study was to fur-
cient than BHR implemented on an individual ther evaluate the efficacy of instructing a compet-
basis is less clear. In the present study, none of ing response. During BHR, all participants
the groups met either of the response identifica- demonstrated an increase in competing responses,
tion criteria to progress to post-BHR due to replicating the finding from Pawlik and Per-
the performance of one or two members. As a rin (2020) and providing further evidence that a
result, all groups contained at least one member rule is effective at evoking a competing response.
who remained in BHR longer than if an indi- Unlike Pawlik and Perrin, rates of competing
vidual contingency had been in effect. In addi- responses were much lower than those of speech
tion, members of Group 2 were required to disfluencies during baseline. This is contrasted
participate in booster sessions as a result of with response identification, in which instruction
Madison’s increase in speech disfluencies. For and feedback produced differentially higher rates
those participants, small-group BHR was less of hand raises than competing responses for six of
time efficient. However, the group nature of nine participants. Furthermore, similar to previous
the intervention meant that nine participants research (Pawlik & Perrin, 2020; Spieler &
experienced BHR using the same resources Miltenberger, 2017) anecdotal observations indi-
(e.g., therapists, materials, rooms) previously cated that some participants emitted unnecessary
used for three participants. In addition, based hand raises during post-BHR. Collectively, these
on times reported in previous studies, the time findings suggest that teaching a competing
to complete small-group BHR was similar to response is likely more effective than instruction
the time required to complete BHR with three alone. Further, it raises questions about teaching
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 11

different responses for identification and competi- speaking rating scale (see Table 2), where all
tion with speech disfluencies. Rather, it may be participants’ ratings reported increased attend-
beneficial to teach the competing response during ing to all forms of speech disfluencies. Further-
the video review portion of awareness training. more, participants rated their pretraining
One interesting aspect of the data was the performance worse following BHR than they
low level of response identification demon- had prior to training, suggesting improved
strated by four of nine participants during awareness of their speech disfluencies. An alter-
BHR. Several components of the procedures nate explanation is that BHR involves punish-
may have contributed to this result. First, the ment. Some researchers (e.g., Miltenberger
descriptions and examples of speech et al., 1998) have suggested that competing
disfluencies provided during BHR reflected response training functions as positive punish-
those emitted by the group as opposed to a sin- ment, as the competing response does not need
gle participant. Second, the video recording to be incompatible with the habit response. It
only included a 1-min sample of each partici- is possible that prompting participants to raise
pant’s baseline speeches. Although this sample their hand when they failed to detect a speech
was representative, the limited duration meant disfluency functioned as a mild punisher. Addi-
it contained limited examples of some forms of tionally, Mancuso and Miltenberger (2016)
speech disfluencies and possibly none of others. extended BHR to public-speaking speech
Third, groups were not required to meet a spe- disfluencies due to the similarities between
cific level of response identification while view- speech disfluencies and stuttering. However,
ing videos of speeches before progressing to the competing response often used for
practice speeches. Collectively, these procedures stuttering is regulated breathing to relax the
may have reduced discrimination and subse- vocal musculature (Azrin & Nunn, 1974). In
quent acquisition of stimulus control during the absence of regulated breathing, simply
BHR. Montes et al. (2021) demonstrated that pausing and counting is more topographically
review of recorded speeches, though beneficial, similar to that of time-out from speaking
is not necessary to reduce speech disfluencies, (James, 1981). For most participants in the
suggesting the importance of the description present study, BHR produced immediate and
and examples of speech disfluencies. In addi- large decreases in speech disfluencies after
tion to issues with stimulus control, reductions which rates of competing responses decreased
in speech disfluencies resulted in fewer oppor- across BHR and post-BHR conditions. These
tunities for the occurrence of response identifi- response patterns are consistent with what
cation. Under such circumstances, failing to would be predicted if BHR functioned as pun-
identify even a single speech disfluency could ishment and speech fluency is strengthened
result in moderate to low levels of response through negative reinforcement.
identification, such as those demonstrated by Despite the promising results of this study,
Opal, Cora, and Hailey toward the end of the there are several limitations. One limitation is a
condition. function of a small group size when
In the present study, all participants demon- implementing an interdependent group contin-
strated reductions in speech disfluencies despite gency. Since there were only three members in
low rates of competing responses and, in some a group, each member’s responses greatly
cases, low levels of response identification. It is impacted the group’s data. This exacerbated the
possible that BHR resulted in improved attend- issue mentioned above regarding the impact of
ing despite these responses remaining low. This limited opportunities to identity speech
is supported by responses on the public disfluencies. A group member failing to identify
12 Christopher J. Perrin et al.

even a single speech disfluency could result in a concurrent multiple baseline design. Also, this
the group failing to meet criterion to progress study successfully addressed some of the sugges-
to post-BHR, especially if other group mem- tions for future research by Pawlik and Per-
bers emitted zero speech disfluencies. Given the rin (2020). Participants in the current study
impact of limited opportunities it may be bene- were not informed of booster sessions prior to
ficial to limit mastery criterion to reductions in entering them and a reliable cue to signal the
speech disfluencies; especially if high levels of conclusion of a speech was used. Future
response identification are not necessary for research should continue to explore ways to
reductions in speech disfluencies. improve effectiveness and promote adoption of
Another limitation was the lack of a question BHR for public-speaking speech disfluencies.
specifically addressing the acceptability of the
interdependent group contingency on the treat-
ment acceptability rating scale. Although partic- REFERENCES
ipants indicated that they liked the treatment American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and
and would recommend it to others, these ques- statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). https://
doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.529303
tions addressed the treatment package as a Audacity Team (2018). Audacity ®: Free Audio Editor and
whole. As such, we are unable to draw conclu- Recorder (Version 2.3.0) [Computer software].
sions about either individual components of https://audacityteam.org/
BHR or the interdependent group contingency. Azrin, N. H., & Nunn, R. G. (1974). A rapid method of
eliminating stuttering by a regulated breathing
BHR is a multicomponent intervention and par- approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 12(4),
ticipants may find some aspects more acceptable 279-286. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(74)
than others. Furthermore, the interdependent 90002-3
Barrish, H. H., Saunders, M., & Wolf, M. M. (1969).
nature of the group contingency may result in Good behavior game: Effects of individual contingen-
lower acceptability ratings. Previous research in cies for group consequences on disruptive behavior in
which participants experienced both independent a classroom. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2
(2), 119-124. https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1969.
and interdependent contingencies have provided 2-119
mixed findings. For instance, ratings on the lik- Bell, R. L. (2011, October). Is your speech filled with
ability of an independent contingency for um? 3 tips to eliminate filled pauses from your pro-
smoking abstinence were significantly higher fessional presentation. Supervision, 72(10), 10-13.
Retrieved from www.supervisionmagazine.com.
than those of an interdependent contingency Brown, N., & Redmon, W. K. (1989). The effects of a
(Meredith & Dallery, 2013) whereas Groves and group reinforcement contingency on staff use of
Austin (2017) found students voted to partici- unscheduled sick leave. Journal of Organizational
Behavior Management, 10(2), 2-17. https://doi.org/
pate in an interdependent version of the Good 10.1300/J075v10n02_02
Behavior Game more often than an independent Clark, H. H., & Tree, J. E. F. (2002). Using uh and um
version. Additional research on the conditions in spontaneous speaking. Cognition, 84(1), 73-111.
under which interdependent contingencies are http://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00017-3
Dixon, M. R., & Holcomb, S. (2000). Teaching self-
preferred is needed, as well as information about control to small groups of dually diagnosed adults.
the acceptability of individual aspects of BHR. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33(4), 611-614.
This study provides data suggesting the https://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2000.33-611
Groves, E. A., & Austin, J. L. (2017). An evaluation of
effectiveness of conducing BHR for public- interdependent and independent group contingencies
speaking speech disfluencies in a small group during the Good Behavior Game. Journal of Applied
format and provides future directions for incor- Behavior Analysis, 50(3), 552-566. https://doi.org/10.
porating group contingencies into BHR. In 1002/jaba.393
Henderson, J. (2007). There’s no such thing as public speak-
addition, this study addressed experimental ing: Making any presentation or speech as persuasive as
design concerns from previous studies by using a one-to-one-conversation. Prentice Hall Press.
Brief Habit Reversal and a Group Contingency 13

Hirsch, S. E., Healy, S., Judge, J. P., & Lloyd, J. W. Nissen, J. B., Kaergaard, M., Laursen, L., Partner, E., &
(2016). Effects of an interdependent group contin- Thomsen, P. H. (2019). Combined habit reversal
gency on engagement in physical education. Journal training and exposure response prevention in a group
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 975-979. https://doi. setting compared to individual training: A random-
org/10.1002/jaba.328 ized controlled clinical trial. European Child & Adoles-
James, J. E. (1981). Behavioral self-control of stuttering cent Psychiatry, 28(1), 57-68. https://doi.org/10.
using time-out from speaking. Journal of Applied 1007/s00787-018-1187-z
Behavior Analysis, 14(1), 25-37. https://doi.org/10. Pawlik, B., & Perrin, C. J. (2020). Reducing speech
1901/jaba.1981.14-25. disfluencies during public speaking using brief habit
Mancuso, C., & Miltenberger, R. G. (2016). Using habit reversal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(2),
reversal to decrease filled pauses and nervous habits 1080-1088. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.627
in public speaking. Journal of Applied Behavior Spieler, C., & Miltenberger, R. (2017). Using awareness
Analysis, 49(1), 188-192. http://doi.org/10.1002/ training to decrease nervous habits during public
jaba.267 speaking. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 50(1),
Meredith, S. E., & Dallery, J. (2013). Investigating group 38-47. https://doi.org/10.1002/jaba.362
contingencies to promote brief abstinence from ciga- Stein, M. B., Walker, J. R., & Forde, D. R. (1996). Pub-
rette smoking. Experimental and Clinical lic-speaking fears in a community sample: Prevalence,
Psychopharmacology, 21(2), 144-154. https://doi.org/ impact on functioning, and diagnostic classification.
10.1037/a0031707 Archives of General Psychiatry, 53, 169-174. https://
Miltenberger, R. G., Fuqua, R. W., & Woods, D. W. doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830020087010
(1998). Applying behavior analysis to clinical prob-
Tejwani, V., Ha, D., & Isada, C. (2016). Observations:
lems: Review and analysis of habit reversal. Journal of
Public speaking anxiety in graduate medical
Applied Behavior Analysis, 31(3), 447-469. https://
education--A matter of interpersonal and communi-
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.1998.31-447
cation skills? Journal of Graduate Medical Education,
Montes, C. C., Heinicke, M. R., & Geireman, D. M.
8(1), 111. https://doi.org/10.4300/JGME-D-15-
(2019). Awareness training reduces college student’s
00500.1
speech disfluencies in public speaking. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 52(3), 746-755. https:// Yates, R., Edwards, K., King, J., Luzon, O., Evangeli, M.,
Stark, D., McFarlane, F., Heyman, I., Ince, _ B.,
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.569
Montes, C. C., Heinicke, M. R., Guendulain, M. A., & Kodric, J., & Murphy, T. (2016). Habit reversal
Morales, E. (2021). A component analysis of aware- training and educational group treatments for chil-
ness training for reducing speech disfluencies. Journal dren with Tourette syndrome: A preliminary
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 54(2), 770-782. https:// randomised controlled trial. Behaviour Research and
doi.org/10.1002/jaba.795 Therapy, 80, 43-50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.
Neef, N. A., Perrin, C. J., Haberlin, A. T., & 2016.03.003
Rodrigues, L. C. (2011). Studying as fun and games:
Effects on college students’ quiz performance. Journal Received February 1, 2021
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 44(4), 897-901. https:// Final acceptance June 24, 2021
doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2011.44-897 Action Editor, Jeanne Donaldson

You might also like