You are on page 1of 9

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry **:* (2014), pp **–** doi:10.1111/jcpp.

12354

A randomized controlled trial of Pivotal Response


Treatment Group for parents of children with autism
Antonio Y. Hardan,1 Grace W. Gengoux,1 Kari L. Berquist,1 Robin A. Libove,1 Christina
M. Ardel,1 Jennifer Phillips,1 Thomas W. Frazier,2 and Mendy B. Minjarez3
1
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University, Stanford, CA; 2Center for Autism,
Cleveland Clinic, Cleveland, OH; 3Seattle Children’s Hospital/Seattle Children’s Research Institute,
Seattle, WA, USA

Background: With rates of autism diagnosis continuing to rise, there is an urgent need for effective and efficient
service delivery models. Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) is considered an established treatment for autism
spectrum disorder (ASD); however, there have been few well-controlled studies with adequate sample size. The aim of
this study was to conduct a randomized controlled trial to evaluate PRT parent training group (PRTG) for targeting
language deficits in young children with ASD. Methods: Fifty-three children with autism and significant language
delay between 2 and 6 years old were randomized to PRTG (N = 27) or psychoeducation group (PEG; N = 26) for
12 weeks. The PRTG taught parents behavioral techniques to facilitate language development. The PEG taught
general information about ASD (clinical trial NCT01881750; http://www.clinicaltrials.gov). Results: Analysis of
child utterances during the structured laboratory observation (primary outcome) indicated that, compared with
children in the PEG, children in the PRTG demonstrated greater improvement in frequency of utterances (F(2,
43) = 3.53, p = .038, d = 0.42). Results indicated that parents were able to learn PRT in a group format, as the
majority of parents in the PRTG (84%) met fidelity of implementation criteria after 12 weeks. Children also
demonstrated greater improvement in adaptive communication skills (Vineland-II) following PRTG and baseline
Mullen visual reception scores predicted treatment response to PRTG. Conclusions: This is the first randomized
controlled trial of group-delivered PRT and one of the largest experimental investigations of the PRT model to date.
The findings suggest that specific instruction in PRT results in greater skill acquisition for both parents and children,
especially in functional and adaptive communication skills. Further research in PRT is warranted to replicate the
observed results and address other core ASD symptoms. Keywords: Language deficits, treatment fidelity, group
parent training, social communication, naturalistic developmental behavior intervention.

is an evolution of ABA that also incorporates moti-


Introduction
vational variables into trials and is conducted in the
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by
child’s natural environment. The emerging support
social communication deficits and repetitive behav-
for these models warrants additional large-scale,
iors (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). With
well-controlled studies.
greater than 1% of children carrying an ASD diag-
Parent involvement is critical for effective inter-
nosis (Center for Disease Control, 2014), the devel-
vention with children with ASD (National Research
opment of empirically supported treatments for ASD
Council, 2001). A few treatment models increase
is a critical priority. While early intervention
treatment intensity by training parents to deliver
improves cognitive, language, and adaptive skills
intervention (Steiner, Koegel, Koegel, & Whitney,
for many children (Dawson & Burner, 2011), a broad
2012). Research shows parents can implement behav-
range of interventions are being used with varying
ioral interventions (Koegel, Bimbela, & Schreibman,
empirical support (Vismara & Rogers, 2010). Early
1996) and autism symptoms subsequently improve
studies of Applied Behavior Analysis (ABA) using
(Symon, 2005). Parent education may also promote
Discrete Trial Training (DTT; Lovaas, 1987) and
generalization to natural environments (Steiner
more recent studies of intensive behavioral interven-
et al., 2012) and improve parent psychological
tion have documented significant progress in cogni-
well-being (Minjarez, Mercier, Williams, & Hardan,
tive and language development (Reichow, 2012).
2013). Therefore, research on interventions that
There is also increasing support for treatments,
involve parents in treatment delivery is needed.
recently termed Naturalistic Developmental Behav-
Parent education is a core component of PRT
ior Interventions (NDBIs; Rogers et al., 2014;
(Koegel, Koegel, Harrower, & Carter, 1999), an NDBI
Schreibman et al., 2014) which combine ABA-based
which uses ABA principles to target ‘pivotal’ areas
treatment with a child-led developmental approach,
(e.g. motivation). The focus on pivotal areas is
as in the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM; Dawson
hypothesized to result in improvements in broad
et al., 2010). Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT;
areas of functioning not directly targeted with inter-
Mohammadzaheri, Koegel, Rezaee, & Rafiee, 2014)
vention (e.g. joint attention; Koegel, Koegel, & Brook-
man, 2005). Available evidence has demonstrated
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared. the efficacy of PRT for teaching social communica-

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
2 Antonio Y. Hardan et al.

tion skills (Bryson et al., 2007). However, the major- ity, and (d) active medical problems. No changes in inclusion or
ity of PRT research has involved training small exclusion criteria were applied during the study. The rationale
for limiting concomitant speech therapy was due to our
numbers of families individually and research has
interest in studying the effects of PRTG on acquisition of
just begun to investigate dissemination through functional communication skills and the concern that the
group therapy models (Baker-Ericz en, Stahmer, & inclusion of children receiving concurrent intensive speech
Burns, 2007). In an uncontrolled trial of PRT parent therapy would present a confound. Given that the majority of
training group (N = 17), the majority of parents children in our area get 30–60 min of speech therapy per week
it was not practical to exclude children receiving any speech
learned PRT within 10 weeks and their children
therapy; therefore, the decision was made to limit participation
showed gains in functional communication (Minj- to children receiving routine but not intensive speech therapy.
arez, Williams, Mercier, & Hardan, 2011).
The goal of the present study was to conduct a Procedures
randomized controlled 12-week trial to examine the
effectiveness of PRT, taught to parents in a group After baseline measures, eligible families were randomized in
parallel (1:1) to the treatment or control group. Stratification
format, in targeting functional communication defi- was based on gender, age (2–4:6 vs. 4:7–6:11 years), and
cits in young children with ASD. A parent psycho- intensity of ABA treatment (<10 vs. ≥ 10 hr/week). The
education control group was selected to examine the rationale for these procedures was based on the hypothesis
unique contribution of parent training in PRT. The that younger children might respond better to treatment and
primary goals of the study were to evaluate: (a) the concern that there might be differences in intensity of
community interventions (i.e. ABA treatment) which could
whether parents learn PRT from a 12-week parent influence study outcomes within a small sample. Randomiza-
group, and (b) whether children of parents partici- tion was done using a coin flip (www.random.org) by a senior
pating in the PRT group exhibit greater improvement investigator not involved in the clinical assessment or treat-
in functional communication skills than those ment. Sequence generation was done by the project coordina-
receiving general psychoeducation. tor who was not involved in the clinical ratings. After
randomization, the senior investigator informed the coordina-
tor about the group assignment. All data were collected during
visits to the autism program offices within a large academic
medical center and outcome measures were administered at
Methods baseline, week 6, and week 12. Data were managed using
Study design REDCap electronic data tools (Harris et al., 2009) hosted at the
This investigation involved a randomized controlled 12-week Stanford Center for Clinical Informatics.
trial examining the effectiveness of Pivotal Response Treatment
Group (PRTG) in targeting functional communication deficits Pivotal response treatment group (PRTG)
in young children with autism. This study was approved by
Stanford University’s institutional review board, and registered Pivotal response treatment training lasted 12 consecutive
in the Clinical Trials database (NCT01881750; http://www. weeks, with one session per week. The group was led by
clinicaltrials.gov). The full trial protocol is available upon psychologists specializing in PRT, utilizing the manual, How to
request. Teach Pivotal Behaviors to Children with Autism (Koegel et al.,
1989) and a standard set of PRT teaching materials and video
examples (Minjarez et al., 2011). Eight 90-min visits were
Participants parent-only group sessions consisting of 4–6 parents and 1–2
leading clinicians. For four of the visits, parent–child dyads
Recruitment occurred over 2 years (July 2010–June 2012). met individually with a clinician (60 min).
Participants were recruited through distribution of fliers at
clinics and autism awareness events, referral by local profes-
sionals, and word of mouth. Following informed consent, a Psychoeducation group (PEG)
comprehensive evaluation, including review of medical/psy-
The PEG also lasted 12 weeks, with a meeting each week. The
chiatric history and completion of psychological assessment
curriculum was based on an existing autism parent psycho-
batteries, was conducted to determine study eligibility.
education program at the university and was taught by
clinical psychology graduate students supervised by a
licensed psychologist. Ten sessions were parent-only group
Inclusion and exclusion criteria meetings (90 min), and two were individual meetings (60 min)
Participants included children: (a) 2–6 years old, (b) diagnosed between parent–child dyads and a psychologist (see online
with autism based on DSM-IV-TR criteria, Autism Diagnostic supplemental material for detailed information on PRTG and
Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, PEG, Appendix S1 and S2).
1994), Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord
et al., 2000), and expert clinical judgment, (c) communica- Measures
tion-delayed on Preschool Language Scale, 4th Edition (PLS-4;
Zimmerman, Steiner, & Pond, 2002; 2- and 3-year olds were ≥ Diagnostic instruments and screening. The ADI-R
1 SD below age level, 4-year olds ≥ 2 SDs, and 5–6-year olds ≥ 3 (Lord et al., 1994) and ADOS (Lord et al., 2000) were adminis-
SDs), (d) able to vocalize with intent when prompted by a tered for all study participants. The PLS-4 (Zimmerman et al.,
clinician during the screening visit, (e) with stable concomitant 2002) was used to assess baseline language level and as a
interventions (e.g. ABA, special education, medications, bio- secondary outcome measure at week 12. The Mullen Scales of
medical treatments) for 1 month prior to participation with no Early Learning (Mullen, 1995) assessed cognitive functioning.
planned changes, and (f) with at least one parent available to
consistently participate. Exclusion criteria were: (a) participa- Primary outcome measures. Primary outcome mea-
tion in more than 60 min weekly of individual speech therapy, sures included child frequency of utterances and parent
(b) comorbid severe psychiatric disorder, (c) genetic abnormal- fidelity of treatment implementation. Both measures were

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


RCT of pivotal response treatment group 3

obtained from a structured laboratory observation (SLO) at pattern of change in utterances across study time points.
baseline, week 6, and week 12, and videotaped and scored by Significant interactions were followed by independent samples
independent raters. SLO included a 10-min period during t-tests at each postbaseline time point (Weeks 6 and 12) to
which parents were instructed to try getting the child to determine the time of onset of treatment effects. Additional
communicate as much as possible. mixed effect regression models were separately computed for
Videos of parent–child interaction across both groups were each utterance type. Exploratory analyses using the same
scored for parent fidelity of PRT implementation with measures modeling approach were computed for Vineland Communica-
widely used in PRT research (Bryson et al., 2007; Symon, tion standard scores, Vineland Receptive and Expressive
2005). At least 80% correct on each of six PRT techniques was subscale standard scores, SRS total raw scores, number of
required to meet fidelity criteria. For each 10-min probe, raters words on the CDI out of 396 and 680, CDI mean length of
also tallied the child’s functional verbal utterances. Frequency longest utterance, PLS-4, and CGI-Severity (overall social/
counts were calculated for total functional utterances and for communication symptoms only). The effects of sex, age, and IQ
each utterance type: (a) unintelligible, (b) imitative, (c) verbally on the primary outcome (total child utterances) were also
prompted, (d) nonverbally prompted, and (e) spontaneous. investigated by reestimating the above model and including
Nonverbally prompted utterances were those that occurred these variables as fixed-effects covariates.
following a clear nonverbal prompt from the parent (parent Mixed effects regression models are advantageous com-
held item up or deliberately paused the activity and waited pared to repeated measures ANOVA in that they accommodate
expectantly for a verbal request), whereas spontaneous utter- missing time points, utilize all available data, and therefore
ances occurred in the absence of any effort from the adult to can be considered truly intent-to-treat models. Model fit was
elicit a response. considered by iteratively examining random effects for inter-
Raters were bachelors- and masters-level research assis- cept and slope and alternative repeated measures covariance
tants trained in scoring procedures by three senior investiga- structures (Peugh & Enders, 2005). The significance of
tors. Initial training was conducted until raters consistently random intercept and slope parameters was examined to
reached target interobserver agreement (IOA; e.g. 80%). Two determine whether individuals showed significant variability
raters then independently scored at least 33% of the videos in outcome measures at baseline and individual differences in
randomly selected from the total set. All raters were blind to change-over-time. Initial models indicated that an unstruc-
group assignment and treatment phase. For parent fidelity, tured repeated measures covariance matrix fit consistently
IOA was calculated, as well as kappa (Cohen, 1960) to correct better than autoregressive and simpler alternative structures.
for chance agreement. The average IOA across sessions was Final models presented are based on the unstructured
88% (range: 70–100%; k = .78). For child utterances, intra- covariance matrix. Type 1 error rate of 0.05 was used for
class correlation coefficients (ICC) were calculated to assess analyses of the primary outcome, subscales, and all explor-
reliability between raters. Results indicated high levels of atory measures. Multiple comparison correction was not
agreement on total number of utterances [ICC (2,1) = 0.993] performed for exploratory measures, as this is a small initial
and across all utterance types (Unintelligible = 0.947; Imitative study and the purpose of the exploratory analyses was to
= 0.962; Verbally Prompted = 0.982; Nonverbally Prompted = better understand the specificity of the treatment effects for
0.938; Spontaneous = 0.910; Shrout & Fleiss, 1979). future study planning.

Secondary outcome measures. The following mea-


sures were obtained: MacArthur-Bates Communicative Devel-
opment Inventories (CDI; Fenson et al., 2007) Words and Results
Gestures and Words and Sentences versions; Vineland Adap-
Study population
tive Behavior Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II; Sparrow,
Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005) Communication subscale; Social One hundred and thirty-five potential subjects were
Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino & Gruber, 2005); and
screened after inquiring about the study (Figure 1);
Clinical Global Impression Scale (Guy, 1976) severity and
improvement subscales (CGI-S and CGI-I). The CGI ratings 104 signed a consent form. Forty one did not meet
were assessed by a psychologist blind to group assignment and eligibility criteria and 10 decided not to participate
treatment phase, and were specifically focused on social and before baseline measures. Fifty-three subjects (41
communication skills (Appendix S1 for additional detail males, 12 females; 4.1  1.2 years; range: 2.1–6.9)
regarding outcome measures).
were randomized (PRTG = 27; PEG = 26). Target
sample size for ending the trial (52) was determined
Treatment monitoring measures. At baseline and
week 12, parents in both groups completed a brief question- by power analysis based on a pilot study (Minjarez
naire to report their children’s existing autism interventions et al., 2011). There were no differences between the
and a weekly concomitant therapies log was collected to two groups on any child demographic or clinical
document any changes. In the PRTG, parents were asked to baseline measures (Table 1). The majority (94%) of
practice PRT daily and video record at least 10 min each week
participating children were in school, primarily in
for review during group. PEG parents were asked to video
record 10-min parent–child interactions at baseline, week 6, special day classes (73%) and receiving an average of
and week 12. 19.2 hr of school per week (range = 0–45). Approx-
imately half of the subjects (52%) were receiving ABA
treatment with 34% of children receiving more than
Statistical analyses 10 hr per week. Almost all children were receiving
To examine the primary hypothesis that PRT would increase speech therapy (91%) with an average of 54 min
total child utterances in the SLO, we computed mixed effects individual therapy per week. Groups did not differ in
regression models with Treatment Group (two levels: PRTG vs.
the number of total outside intervention hours
PEG), Time (three levels: baseline, week 6, and week 12), and
their interaction as fixed-effects covariates. The interaction of received (PEG: 26.1  11.1; PRTG: 23.9  10.5;
Treatment Group X Time directly tests the hypothesis by t = .74; p = .46). Participating parents were primar-
examining whether treatment groups showed a different ily female (92%) and all had at least partial college

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


4 Antonio Y. Hardan et al.

135 subjects screened

104 subjects enrolled in


the study

10 decided not to 41 did not meet inclusion/


participate exclusion criteria

53 randomized

27 subjects assigned 26 subjects assigned


to PRTG to PEG

2 subjects excluded due 3 subjects excluded due to


to changes in changes in concomitant
concomitant therapies 25 subjects (92%) 22 subjects (84%) therapies
completed the study completed the study
1 subject lost to follow-up*

Figure 1 Patient flow diagram for Pivotal Response Treatment Group (PRTG) versus Psychoeducation Group (PEG) in the treatment of
children with autism. *Available data from this subject were included in analyses

education, with 80% college graduates and 35% with


Structured laboratory observation (SLO)
graduate degrees. Median annual household income
was reported in the $125,000–150,000 range. There Table 2 presents results for total, imitative, and
were no significant differences on these variables nonverbally prompted utterances from the SLO.
between groups (p > .1). Five subjects were excluded Irrespective of group, children showed significant
(two PRTG and three PEG) due to changes in improvement in total number of utterances across
concomitant therapies during the trial and their study time points (F(2, 43) = 6.12, p = .005; Fig-
data were not included in the analyses. Forty-seven ure 2). However, individuals receiving PRTG showed
subjects (PRTG = 25; PEG = 22) completed the study greater improvement (F(2, 43) = 3.53, p = .038).
with one participant in the PEG group lost to The treatment effect was most apparent for imita-
follow-up. No adverse effects were noted in either tive (F(2, 43) = 7.67, p = .001) and nonverbally
group. prompted utterances (F(2, 43) = 7.06, p = .002;
Table 2). Spontaneous utterances showed a nonsig-
nificant trend toward greater improvement in PRTG
Table 1 Baseline comparison of participants with autism in
the Pivotal Response Treatment Group (PRTG) and Psychoed- (F(2, 44) = 2.99, p = .060). The treatment effect was
ucation Group (PEG) not significant for unintelligible (F(2, 43) = 0.27,
p = .762) or verbally prompted utterances (F(2, 43) =
PRTG (SD) PEG (SD) 0.32, p = .725).
N 25 23a No parent met fidelity of implementation at base-
Male/Female 19/6 17/6 line. At week 12, 21 of 25 parents in PRTG, and none
Mean age in years 4.1 (1.2) 4.1 (1.3) in the PEG, met fidelity of PRT implementation.
SLO total utterances 45.7 (23.1) 40.8 (22.4) Including treatment fidelity as a covariate in regres-
CGI-severity score 5.2 (0.9) 5.1 (0.8)
Development quotient 52.8 (16.4) 53.5 (14.8)
sion models further qualified the above-described
Social Responsiveness Scale 77.9 (11.4) 78.9 (12.8) treatment effects. Within the PRTG, caregivers who
demonstrated fidelity at week 12 had children with
SD, standard deviation; CGI, Clinical global impression; greater total (F(2, 42) = 6.79, p = .003; Figure S1)
Development quotient measured by Mullen Scales; No statis-
and imitative (F(2, 46) = 6.95, p = .002) utterances
tical differences between the two groups on any of the baseline
clinical characteristics. relative to those who did not meet fidelity. Fidelity
a
Includes one subject who was randomized but did not did not modify the treatment effect for verbally
complete the study. prompted (F(2, 41) = 0.92, p = .405), nonverbally

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


RCT of pivotal response treatment group 5

Table 2 Treatment responses of participants with autism whose parents were assigned to Pivotal Response Treatment Group
(PRTG) or Psychoeducation Group (PEG)

Group X Time
Mean (SD) Interaction

Baseline Week 12

PRTG PRTG
(n = 25) PEG (n = 23) (n = 25) PEG (n = 22) F p Cohen’s d (Week 12)

SLO
Total utterances 45.7 (23.1) 40.8 (22.4) 64.5 (28.9) 51.4 (33.8) 3.53 .038 0.42
Unintelligible 27.64 (17.8) 22.7 (14.2) 26.1 (14.9) 24.6 (23.3) 0.27 .762 0.08
Imitative 9.7 (7.6) 6.1 (5.4) 19.4 (14.2) 7.1 (7.8) 7.67 .001 1.06
Verbally prompted 7.7 (9.3) 10.7 (10.4) 14.5 (15.2) 16.0 (17.0) 0.32 .725 0.09
Nonverbally prompted 0.2 (0.4) 0.5 (1.1) 2.7 (3.8) 0.1 (0.3) 7.06 .002 0.94
Spontaneous 0.5 (1.4) 0.8 (1.5) 0.9 (1.4) 0.4 (0.6) 2.99 .06 0.46
Vineland PRTG PEG PRTG PEG
Communication standard 69.9 (16.3) 71.6 (15.4) 78.9 (18.9) 72.8 (16.5) 3.80 .041 0.34
score
Expressive raw score 26.7 (12.1) 31.3 (18) 41.7 (14.7) 34 (18.9) 6.95 .004 0.46
Receptive raw score 17.8 (7.1) 18.1 (6.6) 21.5 (14.7) 18.9 (6.5) 4.27 .028 0.23
CDI
Mean length of longest 2.1 (1.7) 2.4 (1.7) 3.7 (2.1) 2.8 (1.4) 3.09 .059 0.50
utterance
Words produced out of 396 137.1 (118.1) 169.5 (134.2) 172.2 (123.6) 215.0 (118.3) 0.213 .736 0.35
Words produced out of 680 229.3 (183.8) 179.1 (163.8) 289.1 (181.9) 239.9 (187.1) 1.30 .284 0.27
PLS
Expressive standard score 62.6 (11.2) 63.9 (11.3) 63.9 (11.6) 63.0 (13.4) 2.00 .165 0.07
SRS total 77.9 (11.4) 78.9 (12.8) 74.9 (12.4) 80.6 (10.7) 0.26 .776 0.49
CGI-Severity 5.2 (0.9) 5.1 (0.8) 4.6 (0.9) 5 (0.8) 6.84 .003 0.47
CGI-Improvement – – 2.4 (0.7) 3.2 (0.7) 15.97 .001 1.14

Vineland: Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition; CDI: MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories;
PLS: Preschool Language Scale, 4th Edition; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; CGI: Clinical global impression (focused on social
and communication symptoms).

90 trend was observed for CDI mean length of longest


80 PEG utterance (F(2, 32) = 3.09, p = .059) with children in
70 PRTG the PRTG saying an average of 2.1-word (SD1.7)
Total utterances

60 utterances at baseline and an average of 3.7-word


50 (2.1) utterances at week 12, and children in the PEG
40 increasing from an average mean length of utterance
30 of 2.4 (1.7) to 2.8 (1.4). There were no significant
20 treatment effects for SRS total raw scores (F(2,
10 42) = 0.26, p = .776), CDI total words out of 396 (F
0 (2, 42) = .213, p = .736), and out of 680 (F(2,
Baseline Week 6 Week 12 42) = 1.30, p = .284), or PLS-4 expressive language
standard scores (F(1,40) = 2.00, p = .165). The treat-
Figure 2 Effect of Pivotal Response Treatment Group (PRTG) ment effect was observed for CGI-Severity of social
versus Psychoeducation Group (PEG) on total number of utter-
and communication symptoms (F(2, 42) = 6.84,
ances measured during SLO (F(2, 43) = 6.12, p = .005)
p = .003), and CGI-Improvement ratings were signif-
prompted (F(2, 42) = 0.87, p = .425), or spontaneous icantly better in PRTG relative to PEG at weeks 6 and
utterances (F(2, 44) = 0.57, p = .567). 12 (F(1, 44) = 15.97, p < .001).
The pattern of treatment effects for exploratory
measures did not change with fidelity included as a
Exploratory measures
covariate, and fidelity did not significantly modify the
A significant treatment effect was also observed for treatment effect. Finally, excluding participants
the Vineland-II Communication scale (F(2, receiving PRTG who did not meet fidelity did not
19) = 3.80, p = .041), with individuals receiving alter the findings.
PRTG showing larger improvements in communica-
tion (see Table 2). Vineland-II subscales showed a
Predictors of response
significant treatment effect for expressive (F(2,
23) = 6.95, p = .004) and receptive language raw Older children with higher baseline IQ had more
scores (F(2, 21) = 4.27, p = .028). A nonsignificant total utterances, but there were no effects of sex and

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


6 Antonio Y. Hardan et al.

50.00 (21/25) receiving PRTG demonstrated at least 80%


Change in total utterances from

PEG
40.00 PRT fidelity of implementation by week 12 and
PRTG
baseline to week 12

retention rate was high in PRTG (92%) suggesting


30.00
good acceptability of the treatment.
20.00 Child improvements were observed in increased
10.00
utterances during SLO and in adaptive communica-
tion on the Vineland-II. In contrast, supplemental
0.00
measures of language offered mixed results. While
–10.00 the CDI mean length of longest utterance
–20.00
approached significance, no differences were
Low visual reception High visual reception observed between groups on CDI number of words
produced, or PLS expressive standard scores. These
Figure 3 Total utterances measured during SLO in Pivotal
findings are consistent with several other studies
Response Treatment Group (PRTG) and Psychoeducation Group
(PEG) with low and high Mullen visual reception scores (median which have shown that parent education programs
split; F(2, 40) = 3.96, p = .027) may not lead to reliable changes on standardized
assessments of language functioning (Smith, Groen,
& Wynn, 2000), though improvements may be
the treatment effect did not differ across age or sex observed on other language measures (Roberts
(largest F(5, 41) = 0.90, p = .492). Baseline Mullen et al., 2011). These observations also suggest that
visual reception (VR) scores were a significant as a parent-delivered intervention, PRTG may have a
predictor of treatment response for total and imita- greater effect on functional use of skills during
tive utterances (smallest Group X Time X Mullen VR everyday interactions. This finding is consistent with
interaction F(2, 40) = 3.96, p = .027), accounting for the CGI results showing greater improvement in
14% and 18% of the variance in treatment response PRTG and greater reduction in severity of social
for total and imitative utterances, respectively. Fig- communication impairment when compared with
ure 3 displays total utterances in children receiving PEG. Future research including multiple measures
PEG and PRTG with low and high VR scores of skill generalization will be needed to investigate
(median split). Treatment response was not modified the extent to which child improvement at least in the
by baseline PLS total language standard scores, short-term reflects the parent’s improved ability to
SRS total t-scores, CDI words out of 396 or 680, elicit functional language and whether this potential
or CDI sentence length (largest F(2, 41) = 1.50, mechanism of change has implications for subse-
p = .236). quent generalization in child skill across interactive
partners and contexts. Longer duration trials may
also be warranted to evaluate changes on standard-
Discussion ized language measures.
Findings from this first randomized controlled trial of Observed gains in total frequency of utterances
group-delivered PRT support using PRT to improve came primarily from increases in intelligible speech.
functional communication skills for young children The greatest increases in PRTG were in imitative
with ASD. PRTG resulted in greater improvement in utterances, which are consistent with findings from
frequency of functional utterances compared to PEG. other short-term NDBIs (Ingersoll, Meyer, Bonter, &
These findings are consistent with a recent random- Jelinek, 2012). This is likely a result of the emphasis
ized controlled trial in older children with ASD in PRT training on the use of maintenance tasks to
reporting increase in mean length of utterance in enhance motivation (Koegel et al., 1989). Nonverbally
the PRT group compared to controls (Mohammadza- prompted speech was also shown to increase as a
heri et al., 2014). In addition, PRTG participants result of PRTG, suggesting parents may have begun
demonstrated greater improvement in adaptive com- incorporating nonverbal prompts as acquisition
munication skills as reported by parents. Finally, tasks. Parents in the PEG did use verbal prompts
baseline Mullen VR scores predicted treatment during the SLO at a frequency similar to that observed
response to PRTG. While PRT is considered an in the PRTG, but did not naturally use frequent
established autism treatment (National Autism Cen- imitative or nonverbal prompting strategies to scaffold
ter, 2009), this trial evaluated one of the largest child language development. Spontaneous utter-
groups of children to be treated with PRT in a ances were generally low in frequency, though parents
research study to date and participants were were instructed to actively engage their children,
well-characterized, allowing for further analysis of potentially limiting the opportunity for unprompted
factors contributing to treatment response. The verbalizations. Future studies should consider home
parent education programs were both provided videos or automated measures (e.g. Language ENvi-
against a backdrop of community-based treatments ronment Analysis system, LENA; Oller et al., 2010) for
including school and speech programming, as well assessing spontaneous speech.
as some ABA programs which were maintained Documented improvements in adaptive communi-
stable during the study. The majority of parents cation for the PRTG were consistent with prior

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


RCT of pivotal response treatment group 7

research (Baker-Ericz en et al., 2007). Increases in time-limited clinician involvement. It is also impor-
standard scores suggest a rate of improvement over tant to determine whether group models are more
12 weeks that is faster than normative expectations effective than individual approaches in subgroups of
and comparable to other controlled trials of early children or families with specific characteristics.
intervention programs (Dawson et al., 2010). This study had several limitations. The moderate
Improvement on the Vineland-II is particularly sample size meant that power to detect complex
meaningful, as adaptive skills are related to positive multicomponent patterns was limited. There is also
prognosis (Kanne et al., 2011). Increases in receptive no information available about longer term effects of
raw scores suggest spread of effect to receptive skills the treatment. Furthermore, children may have got-
not explicitly taught (Walton & Ingersoll, 2013). This ten varying doses of PRT depending on the consis-
pattern appears to indicate that participating chil- tency of parent implementation and parents who did
dren increased their understanding of speech as a not meet fidelity may have learned PRT if given more
result of the intervention. It may also reflect the fact time. For ethical reasons, it was necessary to allow
that parents became more effective at motivating children to continue community treatments during
their children to respond. This finding is consistent study. There were also a few differences between the
with research in children with developmental dis- PRTG and PEG including number of individual ses-
abilities, where progress in expressive language was sions, qualifications of intervention providers, and
associated with improvement in receptive skills use of homework assignments in the PRTG which
(Walton & Ingersoll, 2013; Wynn & Smith, 2003). could have influenced results. There were no data
Baseline Mullen VR scores predicted language regarding fidelity of treatment implementation by the
gains in both groups, while gender, age, and other group parent educators which would be important for
baseline measures did not. Children with high VR future studies. This study also targeted language
scores receiving PRTG had very large improvements in development only, though there is support for using
total and imitative utterances, while those with low VR PRT to target other skills (Koegel, Koegel, Boettcher,
scores had smaller but still meaningful improve- Harrower, & Openden, 2006; Schreibman, Stahmer,
ments. Children with high VR scores receiving PEG & Pierce, 1996). Finally, given that parent-report
showed modest improvements at week 12, while those measures (e.g. Vineland-II) cannot be ‘blind’ to treat-
with low VR scores did not show improvement. These ment condition in a parent education study, more
observations are consistent with reports documenting objective behavioral or automated measures such as
positive outcomes associated with higher baseline LENA System (Oller et al., 2010) and measures of
cognitive ability and nonverbal IQ in particular (Mag- in-home implementation would add important infor-
iati, Tay, & Howlin, 2014; Turner & Stone, 2007) and mation about generalization of skills.
highlight the need to individualize interventions so
that effective treatments are implemented early, when
the brain is most plastic. Conclusions
Consistent with recommendations regarding the The present study represents a step forward in
critical role of parent education in early intervention applying rigorous experimental design to evaluate
(National Research Council, 2001), this study pro- differential treatment effects by comparing an estab-
vided support for the model of training parents to lished treatment with general parent psychoeduca-
implement PRT. This mechanism for increasing inter- tion. The study also provides support for
vention intensity is promising, particularly given the group-based parent training, offering a promising
need for cost-effective services (Wise, Little, Holliman, new approach for dissemination to large numbers of
Wise, & Wang, 2010). A recent systematic review of families in an efficient manner. However, additional
meta-analyses indicated that parent involvement was large randomized controlled trials will be critical for
an important predictor of better child outcomes, determining the optimal intensity of treatment (Rog-
especially adaptive behavior gains (Strauss, Mancini, ers & Vismara, 2008). Supplementing parent train-
& Fava, 2013). Given that parent education models ing with clinician-delivered intervention and
offer opportunity for enhanced generalization (Steiner increasing treatment duration may lead to larger
et al., 2012), increased service intensity (Kaiser, Han- gains. Comparing established treatments will also be
cock, & Nietfeld, 2010; Laski, Charlop, & Schreibman, critical for analysis of predictors of treatment
1988), and improved parent psychological well-being response. Finally, while there is evidence from this
(Minjarez et al., 2013), additional studies examining and other studies to suggest that PRT may have an
effective parent educational models are needed. impact on social skills (Baker-Ericzen et al., 2007),
Delivery of parent training in a group format there is a need for more systematic research into the
makes this study unique, as there have been no effects of PRT on social behaviors.
randomized controlled trials of group PRT to date.
With limited access to evidence-based treatments for
ASD (Wise et al., 2010), it is especially meaningful to Supporting information
identify efficacious group models, which allow for Additional Supporting Information may be found in the
dissemination to large numbers of children with online version of this article:

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


8 Antonio Y. Hardan et al.

Appendix S1. Pivotal Response Treatment Group Con- not involved in the trial. Data management was sup-
tent. ported by the National Center for Research Resources
Table S1. Overview of pivotal response treatment group and the National Center for Advancing Translational
content. Sciences, National Institutes of Health, through grant
Table S2. Overview of psychoeducation group content. UL1 RR025744. The authors gratefully acknowledge
Figure S1. Total child utterances (+/– 95% CI) across the families for their participation.
study weeks. The authors have declared that they have no com-
Figure S2. Consort 2010 checklist of information to peting or potential conflicts of interest.
include when reporting a randomized trial.

Correspondence
Acknowledgements Grace Gengoux, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral
This work was supported by an Autism Speaks Treat- Sciences, Stanford University, 401 Quarry Road, Stanford,
ment Grant (#5773; PI: Hardan); the foundation was CA 94305, USA; Email: ggengoux@stanford.edu

Key points
• Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) is a naturalistic developmental behavior intervention (NDBI) focused on
motivating children with autism.
• This is the first randomized controlled trial of group parent training in PRT (PRTG).
• PRTG resulted in greater improvement in functional speech and adaptive communication, when compared
with a psychoeducation group.
• Additional large-scale studies are needed to identify optimal intensity and explore effects of PRT on other core
symptoms.

Harris, P.A., Taylor, R., Thielke, R., Payne, J., Gonzalez, N., &
References Conde, J.G. (2009). Research electronic data capture
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and (REDCap) – A metadata-driven methodology and workflow
statistical manual for mental disorders (5th edn). process for providing translational research informatics
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. support. Journal of Biomedical Informatics, 42, 377–381.
Baker-Ericz en, M.J., Stahmer, A.C., & Burns, A. (2007). Child Ingersoll, B., Meyer, K., Bonter, N., & Jelinek, S. (2012).
demographics associated with outcomes in a community- A comparison of developmental social–pragmatic and
based pivotal response training program. Journal of Positive naturalistic behavioral interventions on language use
Behavior Interventions, 9, 52–60. and social engagement in children with autism. Journal
Bryson, S.E., Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., Openden, D., Smith, I.M., of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 1301–
& Nefdt, N. (2007). Large scale dissemination and community 1313.
implementation of pivotal response treatment: Program Kaiser, A.P., Hancock, T.B., & Nietfeld, J.P. (2010). The effects
description and preliminary data. Research and Practice for of parent-implemented enhanced milieu teaching on the
Persons With Severe Disabilities, 32, 142–153. social communication of children who have autism. Early
Center for Disease Control (2014). Prevalence of autism Education and Development, 11, 423–446.
spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years-autism Kanne, S.M., Gerber, A.J., Quirmbach, L.M., Sparrow, S.S.,
and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Cicchetti, D.V., & Saulnier, C.A. (2011). The role of adaptive
sites, United States, 2010. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, behavior in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for
63, 1–21. functional outcome. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Cohen, J. (1960). A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Disorders, 41, 1007–1018.
Educational and Psychological Measurement, 20, 37–46. Koegel, R.L., Bimbela, A., & Schreibman, L. (1996). Collateral
Constantino, J.N., & Gruber, C.P. (2005). Social respon- effects of parent training on family interactions. Journal of
siveness scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 347–359.
Dawson, G., & Burner, K. (2011). Behavioral interventions in Koegel, R.L., Koegel, L.K., Boettcher, M.A., Harrower, J., &
children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: A Openden, D. (2006). Combining functional assessment and
review of recent findings. Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 23, self-mangagement procedures to rapidly reduce disruptive
616–620. behaviors. In R.L. Koegel & L.K. Koegel (Eds.), Pivotal
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., response treatments for autism (pp. 245–258). Baltimore,
Greenson, J., . . . & Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, controlled MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Company.
trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: The Early Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., & Brookman, L.I. (2005).
Start Denver Model. Pediatrics, 125, 17–23. Child-initiated interactions that are pivotal in intervention
Fenson, L., Marchman, V.A., Thal, D.J., Dale, P.S., Reznick, for children with autism. In E.D. Hibbs & P.S. Jensen (Eds.),
J.S., & Bates, E. (2007). MacArthur-bates communicative Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders:
development inventories. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Empirically based strategies for clinical practice (2nd edn,
Publishing Co. pp. 633–657). Washington DC: American Psychological
Guy, W. (1976). ECDEU assessment manual for psycho- Association.
pharmacology. Rockville, MD: U.S. Department of Health, Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., Harrower, J.K., & Carter, C.M.
Education, and Welfare. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


RCT of pivotal response treatment group 9

approach. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe home-based. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
Disabilities, 24, 174–185. 1553–1566.
Koegel, R.L., Schreibman, L., Good, A., Cerniglia, L., Murphy, Rogers, S.J., & Vismara, L.A. (2008). Evidence-based
C., & Koegel, L.K. (1989). How to teach pivotal behaviors to comprehensive treatments for early autism. Journal of
children with autism: A training manual. Santa Barbara, CA: Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 8–38.
University of California. Rogers, S.J., Vismara, L., Wagner, A.L., McCornick, C., Young,
Laski, K.E., Charlop, M.H., & Schreibman, L. (1988). Training G., & Ozonoff, S. (2014). Autism treatment in the first year of
parents to use the natural language paradigm to increase life: A pilot study of Infant Start, a parent-implemented
their autistic children’s speech. Journal of Applied Behavior intervention for symptomatic infants. Journal of Autism and
Analysis, 2, 391–400. Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E.H., Jr, Leventhal, doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2202-y.
B.L., DiLavore, P.C., . . . & Rutter, M. (2000). The autism Schreibman, L., Dawson, G., Rogers, S.J., Ingersoll, B., Kaiser,
diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard A.P., & Kasari, C., . . . & Halladay, A. (2014). Common
measure of social and communication deficits associated elements of research-based practices for toddlers with ASD.
with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Paper presented at the Autism Speaks Toddler Treatment
Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223. Network Meeting, Atlanta, GA.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Schreibman, L., Stahmer, A., & Pierce, K. (1996). Alternative
diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a applications of pivotal response training: Teaching symbolic
diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with play and social interaction skills. In L.K. Koegel, R.L. Koegel
possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive behavioral support: Including
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. people with difficult behavior in the community (pp. 353–
Lovaas, O.I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational 371). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal Shrout, P.E., & Fleiss, J.L. (1979). Intraclass correlations:
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9. Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86,
Magiati, I., Tay, X.W., & Howlin, P. (2014). Cognitive, language, 420–428.
social and behavioural outcomes in adults with autism Smith, T., Groen, A.D., & Wynn, J.W. (2000). Randomized trial
spectrum disorders: A systematic review of longitudinal of intensive early intervention for children with pervasive
follow-up studies in adulthood. Clinical Psychology Reviews, developmental disorder. American Journal of Mental
34, 73–86. Retardation, 105, 269–285.
Minjarez, M.B., Mercier, E.M., Williams, S.E., & Hardan, A.Y. Sparrow, S.S., Cicchetti, D.V., & Balla, D.A. (2005). Vineland
(2013). Impact of pivotal response training group therapy on adaptive behavior scales (2nd edn). San Antonio, TX:
stress and empowerment in parents of children with autism. Pearson.
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 15, 71–78. Steiner, A.M., Koegel, L.K., Koegel, R.L., & Whitney, A.E.
Minjarez, M.B., Williams, S.E., Mercier, E.M., & Hardan, A.Y. (2012). Issues and theoretical constructs regarding parent
(2011). Pivotal response group treatment program for education for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
parents of children with autism. Journal of Autism and and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1218–1227.
Developmental Disorders, 41, 92–101. Strauss, K., Mancini, F., & Fava, L. (2013). Parent inclusion in
Mohammadzaheri, F., Koegel, L.K., Rezaee, M., & Rafiee, S.M. early intensive behavior interventions for young children
(2014). A randomized clinical trial comparison between with ASD: A synthesis of meta-analyses from 2009 to 2011.
Pivotal Response Treatment (PRT) and structured Applied Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 2967–2985.
Behavior Analysis (ABA) intervention for children with Symon, J.B. (2005). Expanding interventions for children with
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, autism. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 7, 159–173.
44, 2769–2777. Turner, L.M., & Stone, W.L. (2007). Variability in outcome for
Mullen, E.M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning: AGS edn. children with an ASD diagnosis at age two. Journal of Child
Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service. Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 793–802.
National Autism Center (2009). National standards report. Vismara, L.A., & Rogers, S.J. (2010). Behavioral treatments in
Randolph, MA: National Autism center. autism spectrum disorder: What do we know? Annual
National Research Council (2001). Educating children with Review of Clinical Psychology, 6, 447–468.
autism. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Walton, K.M., & Ingersoll, B.R. (2013). Improving social skills
Oller, D., Niyogi, P., Gray, S., Richards, J., Gilkerson, J., Xu, in adolescents and adults with autism and severe to
D., . . . & Warren, S. (2010). Automated vocal analysis of profound intellectual disability: A review of the literature.
naturalistic recordings from children with autism, language Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 594–
delay, and typical development. Proceedings of the National 615.
Academy of Sciences, 107, 13354–13359. Wise, M.D., Little, A.A., Holliman, J.B., Wise, P.H., & Wang, J.
Peugh, J.L., & Enders, C.K. (2005). Using the SPSS mixed (2010). Can state early intervention programs meet the
procedure to fit cross-sectional and longitudinal multilevel increased demand of children suspected of having autism
models. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 65, spectrum disorders? Journal of Developmental and
717–741. Behavioral Pediatrics, 31, 469–476.
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early Wynn, J.W. & Smith, T. (2003). Generalization between
intensive behavioral intervention for young children with receptive and expressive language in young children with
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and autism. Behavioral Interventions, 18, 245–266.
Developmental Disorders, 42, 512–520. Zimmerman, I.L., Steiner, V.G., & Pond, R.E. (2002). Preschool
Roberts, J., Williams, K., Carter, M., Evans, D., Parmenter, T., language scale (4th edn). San Antonio, TX: The
Silove, N., . . . & Warren, A. (2011). A randomized controlled Psychological Corporation.
trial of two early intervention programs for young children
with autism: Centre-based with parent program and Accepted for publication: 20 September 2014

© 2014 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

You might also like