You are on page 1of 17

J Autism Dev Disord

DOI 10.1007/s10803-014-2235-2

ORIGINAL PAPER

Feasibility and Effectiveness of Very Early Intervention


for Infants At-Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Systematic
Review
Jessica Bradshaw • Amanda Mossman Steiner •

Grace Gengoux • Lynn Kern Koegel

Ó Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract Early detection methods for autism spectrum Keywords Autism  Early intervention  Infancy  High-
disorder (ASD) in infancy are rapidly advancing, yet the risk infants  Treatment
development of interventions for infants under two years
with or at-risk for ASD remains limited. In order to guide
research and practice, this paper systematically reviewed Introduction
studies investigating interventions for infants under
24 months with or at-risk for ASD. Nine studies were Research dedicated to advancing methods for early identifi-
identified and evaluated for: (a) participants, (b) interven- cation of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) has resulted in
tion approach (c) experimental design, and (d) outcomes. evidence of behavioral markers in the first year of life (Cha-
Studies that collected parent measures reported positive warska et al. 2013; Elsabbagh et al. 2012; Jones and Klin
findings for parent acceptability, satisfaction, and 2013; Maestro et al. 2002; Shic et al. 2014), the development
improvement in parent implementation of treatment. Infant of clinical autism-specific screeners that begin as early as
gains in social-communicative and developmental skills 12 months (Barbaro and Dissanayake 2013; Kleinman et al.
were observed following intervention in most of the 2008; Robins 2008), and a statement by the American Acad-
reviewed studies, while comparisons with treatment-as- emy of Pediatrics recommending routine screenings for ASD
usual control groups elucidate the need for further research. beginning at 18 months (Johnson and Myers 2007). Early
These studies highlight the feasibility of very early inter- identification of ASD in clinical and research settings neces-
vention and provide preliminary evidence that intervention sitates parallel development and investigation of early inter-
for at-risk infants may be beneficial for infants and parents. ventions for infants and toddlers that mitigate, and possibly
prevent, impairments associated with ASD (Dawson 2008). In
light of empirical research documenting significantly
improved outcomes with early intervention for preschool and
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this school-aged children with ASD (Granpeesheh et al. 2009;
article (doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2235-2) contains supplementary Rogers et al. 2012; Zachor et al. 2007), there is a critical need
material, which is available to authorized users.
for the development of effective early intervention techniques
J. Bradshaw (&)  L. K. Koegel during the first years of life.
Counseling, Clinical, and School Psychology Department, Developmental and transactional models of ASD empha-
Koegel Autism Center, Graduate School of Education, size interactive biological and environmental processes that
University of California, Santa Barbara, CA, USA
shape early development (Dawson 2008; Elsabbagh and
e-mail: jbradshaw@education.ucsb.edu
Johnson 2010; Mundy and Neal 2000). Evidence of neuro-
A. M. Steiner plasticity and critical periods of development in infancy lend
SIERRA Kids, Sacramento, CA, USA support for the investigation of very early interventions.
Effective interventions at the earliest age possible may be able
G. Gengoux
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Stanford to modify early experiences-effectively altering cortical
University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA, USA organization, enhancing learning, and potentially improving

123
J Autism Dev Disord

developmental trajectories (Fox et al. 2010; Johnson 2001; social communication. This developmental pathway is char-
Webb et al. 2014). As such, the present article seeks to review acterized by mutual gaze and shared positive affect (Parlade
current findings in the development and implementation of et al. 2009), social engagement and parental scaffolding
treatment for infants with or at-risk for ASD in the first two (Bakeman and Adamson 1984), and joint attention and gesture
years of life. use (Tomasello and Carpenter 2007), which emerge within the
first 12 months of life. The second year of life is characterized
Current Approaches to Intervention for Autism in Early by an exponential growth of verbal communication (Paul
Childhood 2007). By the time children with ASD are identified and
enrolled in treatment programs between 3 and 5 years (Shat-
Interventions for children with ASD encompass a wide tuck et al. 2009), the gap between their abilities and those of
range of techniques that target an array of behaviors or typically developing peers has often grown quite wide.
symptoms. Some of the most common therapies accessed This early stage of development prior to age two also marks
by parents of children with ASD include speech/language a critical period with regard to accelerated brain growth and
therapy, applied behavior analysis (ABA), occupational connectivity (Courchesne et al. 2003; Wolff et al. 2012),
therapy and sensory integration, as well as medication interactive specialization (Johnson 2001), language learning
treatments and special diets (Green et al. 2006; Goin-Ko- (Kuhl et al. 2005), and potentially social perception and social
chel et al. 2009; Love et al. 2009). Many community-based development (Dawson 2008; Schultz 2005). After 24 months,
intervention services for toddlers first diagnosed with ASD neurosynapatic development, including cortical specializa-
fall under the blanket term ‘‘Early Intervention’’ (e.g., tion and white matter growth, significantly attenuates, making
IDEA, Part C, and Birth to Three programs). Early Inter- this age a critical period in development and possibly in the
vention may consist of a variety of services, some of which pathogenesis of ASD (Wolff et al. 2012). It is hypothesized
are empirically validated while others carry minimal that atypical development of social mechanisms, including
empirical support (Stahmer et al. 2005). Despite the pop- attention, communication, and reward processing, may persist
ular use of services with limited empirical support, several without intervention and could have consequential effects on
evidence-based interventions have been established, eleven the subsequent development of social-communicative abili-
of which are recognized as ‘Established Treatments’ by the ties (Chevallier et al. 2012; Eyler et al. 2012; Schumann and
National Standards Project (National Autism Center 2009). Amaral 2006). Capturing developmentally delayed behaviors
At the time of the National Standards Project, the majority and providing enriched experiences and differential rein-
of the established treatments were associated with favor- forcement in this sensitive period, when neural plasticity is
able outcomes for preschoolers, school-age children, and heightened, could have a long-term impact on development.
middle-school aged children, with very few interventions That is, if developmental trajectories for vulnerable infants
suggested for use with children under 2 years of age. can be altered when the gap between infants with and without
While some highly structured interventions based on delays is small, long-term outcomes could be significantly
Applied Behavior Analysis have historically had the stron- improved.
gest research support (Lovaas 1987; Reichow 2012), early Evidence for observable prodromal symptoms of ASD
behavioral interventions for toddlers with ASD have evolved in infancy is mounting, making implementation of inter-
to incorporate elements from developmental psychology in vention in this critical period feasible (Barbaro and Dis-
addition to more ‘‘naturalistic’’ methods, in which the sanayake 2009; Vismara and Rogers 2008). As mentioned
intervention is more child-directed and occurs in the child’s above, symptoms of ASD have been documented in the
natural environment (Schreibman 2014). Some examples of first year of life (e.g. Chawarska et al. 2013; Elsabbagh
interventions that combine behavioral, naturalistic, and et al. 2012; Flanagan et al. 2012; Jones and Klin 2013).
developmental approaches (also termed Naturalistic Devel- Additionally, early screening strategies are being evaluated
opmental Behavioral Interventions; NDBIs) are the Early to enable detection and referral in the first year of life
Start Denver Model (Rogers and Dawson 2010), Enhanced (Bryson et al. 2008) and before the second birthday (Dietz
Milieu Teaching (Kaiser and Hester 1994), and Pivotal et al. 2006; Reznick et al. 2007; Robins 2008; Wetherby
Response Treatment (PRT; Koegel and Koegel 2012). et al. 2008). Further, accumulating studies are documenting
that many children can be reliably diagnosed at 18 months
Why Intervene So Early? of age (Chawarska et al. 2007; Guthrie et al. 2012). This is
consistent with parent observations confirming concerning
The first 2 years of infant development are marked by rapid behaviors before age two (Young et al. 2003). Clinical
change and an explosion of cognitive, language, and social assessments and early screeners can help to identify infants
abilities. Beginning at birth, early attentional preferences for and toddlers who are ‘‘at-risk’’ for ASD based on prodro-
social stimuli (Johnson et al. 1991) foster the emergence of mal behavioral features and might benefit from

123
J Autism Dev Disord

intervention and prevention strategies rather than waiting efforts that may help to mitigate early signs of ASD in
for the emergence of full diagnostic features (Crais and infancy and to improve developmental trajectories for at-
Watson 2014). In addition to infants who are identified as risk infants.
‘‘at-risk’’ due to behavioral presentation and screening
measures, infant siblings of ASD are also identified as ‘‘at-
risk’’ due to genetic contributions to the disorder (Ozonoff Methods
et al. 2011). Consequentially, pediatric practices are urged
to scrupulously monitor infants, especially at-risk infant This review involved a systematic analysis of studies that
siblings, and immediately refer them for intervention upon focused on interventions for infants with or at-risk for ASD
the observation of developmental red flags (Ozonoff et al. under 24 months of age. In order to provide a compre-
2011). hensive review of the current state of intervention research
While early detection techniques are being refined and for this population, each identified study that met prede-
encouraged, empirically based interventions for infants in this termined inclusion criteria was analyzed and summarized
early period before 24 months remain limited. The majority of in terms of (a) participant characteristics, (b) intervention
the existing early intervention research focuses on children approach, (c) experimental design, and (e) infant and par-
with ASD in the preschool years, between 2 and 4 years of age ent outcomes. More specifically, the summary of partici-
(see Boyd et al. 2010 for review). This is problematic con- pant characteristics included the number of participants in
sidering that current recommendations suggest initiation of each study, the average age, and participants’ risk for ASD.
intervention within 60 days following a diagnosis (Maglione The intervention approach was evaluated with a synopsis
et al. 2012). In a comprehensive review of interventions for of the empirical and theoretical basis of intervention, par-
toddlers with ASD under 36 months, Schertz et al. (2012) ent involvement, length and intensity of treatment, and
identified a total of 20 research studies, with only five studies specific intervention goals and strategies. The summary of
targeting infants under 2 years of age and only one study of a outcome included reports on infant-specific developmental,
single infant focused on treatment implementation within the diagnostic, social, and communication outcomes as well as
first year of life. Given the unique developmental character- parent measures such as satisfaction, feasibility, and
istics of children in the first and second year of life, which may acceptability.
warrant additional specialized modifications to intervention
focus and method of delivery even beyond those made for 2- to Search Procedures
3-year-olds, we elected to specifically focus on treatments
applied to these very young children for the present review. A systematic search procedure was used to identify studies
We focus narrowly on the critical period of birth to 24 months for possible inclusion in this review. First, a literature search
and the specific challenges associated with intervening at this of the following four electronic databases was conducted for
very early age, including developmental modifications nec- articles published from the beginning dates of the database
essary for implementation of intervention in the prelinguistic (1949 was the earliest resulting publication) up to June 2014:
period, and evaluating parent outcomes following interven- PsychINFO, Education Resources Information Center
tion. For these very young children there is a clear need to (ERIC), Academic Search Complete, and PubMED. The
address parental concerns as they arise and provide guidelines search only included studies written in English that appeared
for delivery of empirically based interventions (Webb et al. in peer-reviewed journals. The following combination of
2014). Furthermore, the complexities of identifying infants search terms was entered into each database: autism or
under 24 months with or at-risk for ASD are highlighted in autistic or ASD or PDD* or pervasive developmental dis-
this review. order AND infant or toddler AND intervention or treatment.
The present article reviews available research investi- Additionally, articles that were published online, ahead of
gating the feasibility and effectiveness of early interven- print, were searched for the same key terms from the fol-
tions for infants under 24 months, when routine ASD lowing publishers: Elsevier, Springer, and Sage. This initial
screenings are recommended and when enriched experi- search yielded a total of 2,353 articles across all databases
ence may have a significant impact on development tra- after duplications were removed. The articles were then
jectories. The following critical elements are explored independently screened by the first (JB) and last (LK) authors
within the available infant intervention research: (a) par- for the inclusion criteria, listed below.
ticipant characteristics, (b) intervention approach,
(c) experimental design, and (d) infant and parent out- Inclusion Criteria
comes. A synthesis of existing evidence is intended to
guide and inform practitioners as they develop very early Each study resulting from the initial search was evaluated
intervention programs for ASD and to stimulate research for the following pre-determined inclusion criteria:

123
J Autism Dev Disord

1. Research Design. The study involved systematic, were reported across all participants for both single-case and
experimentally controlled investigation of a non-phar- group design studies. The age range and mean age were
macological intervention. Example research designs computed for single-case designs in which the age of each
meeting inclusion criteria were randomized controlled participant was reported individually. Additionally, the
trials, quasi-experimental designs, and single-case method in which each study classified its participants as either
designs. Uncontrolled case studies (e.g., N = 1) were ‘‘at-risk’’ for ASD or diagnosed with ASD was summarized.
excluded from this review. In order to ensure the study
was evaluating intervention effectiveness, at least one Intervention approach
dependent variable had to be a child outcome measure.
2. Autism Risk Status. All participants in the study were Details regarding the intervention approach used by each
diagnosed with ASD or labeled by the study’s authors as study included the empirical and theoretical basis, the
at-risk for developing ASD. There were no restrictions length and intensity, and goals and strategies. The name
related to the study’s methodology for determining and theoretical basis of each intervention was extracted
diagnosis of ASD or determination of ‘‘at-risk’’ for ASD. from the description of the intervention. For any studies
3. Average Age Less than 24 Months. The study focused that did not thoroughly explain a theoretical basis, relevant
on children under 24 months of age. In order to references cited in that article were reviewed to identify the
account for studies that included primarily, but not theoretical foundation. Additionally, it was determined
exclusively, children less than 24 months, we included whether the intervention was primarily parent-mediated,
studies where the mean age of participants was clinician-directed, or a combination of both. The length and
\24.0 months at the start of intervention. intensity of each intervention was reported in terms of
number of individual sessions over a set period of time.
In order to ensure the accuracy of the systematic search,
Where possible, the length of each session was also
the first and last authors both independently completed the
included. If the intervention included group sessions or
search and made an initial determination as to whether
playgroups, this was indicated as well. The intervention
each study met inclusion criteria. The articles identified by
goal(s) of each study referred to how the authors described
each author were then compared for reliability. Reliability
the overall goal of their treatment. Intervention strategies
was calculated using percent agreement on the studies each
referred to specific strategies listed by the author that were
author positively identified as meeting inclusion criteria.
used or taught in order to meet the intervention goals.
This resulted in eight agreements and one disagreement,
yielding an overall agreement of 89 %. The one disagree-
Experimental design
ment was based on the Research Design criteria for a study
that utilized a quasi-experimental design. Through discus-
The type of experimental design used to evaluate effec-
sion it was ultimately decided that a quasi-experimental
tiveness and/or feasibility of the intervention was reported.
design with a post-test only control group was sufficient to
be included in this review. A final total of nine studies
Outcomes
met all the inclusion criteria.
Intervention outcomes for each study were collected for
Coding
both infants and parents. This included observed changes in
infant measures following the intervention as well as any
After the list of included studies was agreed upon, the first
parent measures that were collected, such as fidelity of
and last authors independently extracted information and
implementation or satisfaction.
answered 10 questions related to the four broad categories
used to evaluate each study (see Appendix): (a) participant
Reliability
characteristics, (b) intervention approach, (c) experimental
design (d) outcomes.
Coding reliability was calculated for all nine studies in
order to ensure accuracy in the summary of studies and to
Participant characteristics provide a measure of inter-rater agreement on data
extraction and analysis. There were 90 items on which
The number of total participants in each study, the age range, there could be agreement or disagreement (i.e., 9 studies
and the mean age of participants were collected from each with 10 questions per study). Reliability was determined by
study. The total number of participants was reported for sin- calculating percent agreement across all nine studies.
gle-case designs and the number of participants in each group Agreement was obtained on 80 of the 90 possible items
was reported for group designs. The age range and mean age yielding 80 % agreement. In the instances in which there

123
J Autism Dev Disord

was disagreement on any of the items, the items were Schertz and Odom 2007), Pervasive Developmental Dis-
discussed until consensus was obtained. The final summary orders Screening Test-II (PDD-ST-II; Schertz and Odom
was then checked by the co-authors for accuracy. The 2007; Siegel 2001), Systematic Observation of Red Flags
results are presented in Tables 1 and 2 and the four broad of ASD (SORF; Wetherby and Woods 2002, 2006), and the
domains are further addressed below. Screening Tool for Autism in Two-Year Olds (STAT;
Stone et al. 2004; Carter et al. 2011), .

Results Intervention Approach

Participant Characteristics Empirical and Theoretical Basis

The nine studies included a total of 353 participants Most of the reviewed studies adapted intervention models
between 4 and 30 months of age. This included 150 infants that had been previously applied for preschool-aged chil-
in the experimental treatment condition and 203 control dren. Pivotal Response Treatment (Koegel et al. 2013;
infants who were either typically developing or received Steiner et al. 2013), Early Start Denver Model (ESDM;
‘‘treatment-as-usual’’ (TAU). Three studies investigated Dawson et al. 2010; Rogers et al. 2012), and Hanen’s More
intervention for participants in the first year of life, Than Words (HMTW; Carter et al. 2011) have all been
between 4 and 12 months (Green et al. 2013; Koegel et al. researched for use with older children. All authors reported
2013; Steiner et al. 2013), while six studies intervened with that treatment procedures and targets were modified to be
infants primarily in the second year of life, (Carter et al. developmentally appropriate for infants under the age of
2011; Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002; Rogers et al. two. The studies by Green et al. (2013), Drew et al. (2002),
2012; Schertz and Odom 2007; Wetherby and Woods Schertz and Odom (2007), and Wetherby and Woods (2006)
2006). The latter six studies that focused on infants with represent the first investigation of the experimental inter-
mean age \24 months contributed the majority of treat- vention not previously studied with older populations. PRT
ment participants (N = 137 in the treatment condition) and ESDM were both reported to be founded in develop-
while the former three studies were relatively small and in mental and behavioral theories, with social motivation being
total reported treatment effects for 13 infants. emphasized in PRT while ESDM stressed a relationship-
All studies required that participants be either at-risk for based approach. The Early Social Interaction Project (ESI;
ASD (‘‘at-risk’’ due to behavioral symptoms of ASD or Wetherby and Woods 2006) and HMTW both described a
being a sibling of a child with ASD) or diagnosed with family centered and routine-based approach as key elements
ASD prior to participating in the intervention. Three gen- to their intervention. The Social-pragmatic joint attention
eral approaches were used to determine eligibility based on parent-training program (Drew et al. 2002) utilizes both
ASD diagnosis or risk status: (1) inclusion of any infant developmental and behavioral approaches to teach joint
sibling of a child with autism, regardless of behavioral attention. Schertz and Odom (2007) described the Joint
profile (Green et al. 2013; Steiner et al. 2013), (2) expert Attention Mediated Learning (JAML) intervention as a
clinical concern for ASD based on autism screeners or parent-mediated and family centered approach focusing on
behavioral assessments (Carter et al. 2011; Koegel et al. the developmental foundations of joint attention. The
2013; Rogers et al. 2012; Schertz and Odom 2007), and (3) ‘‘Intervention in BASIS’’ (iBASIS) program (Green et al.
clinical or provisional clinical diagnosis of ASD (Dawson 2013) highlighted attachment theory and the importance of
et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002; Wetherby and Woods 2006). parent-infant synchrony. Additionally, five out of the nine
Assessment procedures were aimed at ascertaining early reviewed studies reported using a manualized intervention
symptoms of ASD, and often used multiple sources. These (Dawson et al. 2010; Green et al. 2013; Rogers et al. 2012;
assessments included systematic behavioral observation Schertz and Odom 2007; Steiner et al. 2013).
(Drew et al. 2002; Koegel et al. 2013), Autism Diagnostic
Observation Schedule—Toddler Module (ADOS-T, Lord Parent Involvement
et al. 2012; Rogers et al. 2012), Autism Diagnostic Inter-
view—Revised (ADI-R) and Toddler Autism Diagnostic Nearly all interventions used parent-mediated procedures.
Interview (Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002; Lord That is, parents were taught specific intervention proce-
et al. 1994), Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; dures during treatment sessions and were expected to
Schertz and Odom 2007; Schopler et al. 1988), Infant implement them with their child during and outside of the
Social Communication Questionnaire (ISCQ; Schertz, sessions. Methods in which parents were taught interven-
unpublished; Schertz and Odom 2007), Modified Checklist tion strategies involved didactic sessions about treatment
for Autism in Toddlers (MCHAT; Robins et al. 2001; techniques in addition to a ‘‘practice-with-feedback’’

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 1 Description of reviewed studies


Study Participants Intervention approach Experimental
design
N and age Risk Empirical and Length and Intervention goals and strategies
determinants theoretical basis intensity
or diagnosis
of ASD

Carter N = 32 (intervention – Symptoms Hanen’s More 8 group and 3 Goal: Increase child communication Randomized
et al. group) and N = 30 of ASD Than Words individual using everyday routines controlled trial
2011 (control group); – Expert Program sessions over Strategies: Video feedback; responding with treatment-
15-24 months clinical (HMTW) 3.5 months to child’s communicative attempts; as-usual
(M = 20) judgment of – Social- following child’s lead; joint action control group
ASD interactionist routines; use of books to elicit and
theory reward communication; scaffolding
– Parent- peer play dates; visual supports
mediated
– Family-
centered
– Routine-based
Dawson N = 24 (intervention – Clinical Early Start 20-h/week over Goal: Improve developmental Randomized
et al. group) and N = 24 diagnosis of Denver Model 2 years outcomes controlled trial
2010 (control group); ASD or (ESDM) Strategies: Interpersonal exchange and with a
18-30 months PDD-NOS – Developmental positive affect; shared engagement; treatment-as-
(M = 23.1 and behavioral adult responsivity and sensitivity to usual control
intervention group; theory child cues; focus on verbal and group
M = 23.9 control – Relationship- nonverbal communication; behavioral
group) based principles (operant conditioning,
shaping and chaining); plan
– Therapist and
individualization
parent-
delivered
Drew N = 12 (intervention – Clinical Social-pragmatic One 3-h session Goal: Enhance communication skills Randomized
et al. group) and N = 12 diagnosis of joint attention every 6 weeks Strategies: Behavior management; joint controlled trial
2002 (control group); ASD focused parent over 12 months action routines; teaching joint with a
Under 24 months training attention behaviors (mirror games, treatment-as-
(M = 22.6) program pointing, following points, gaze usual control
– Developmental switching game); increase mutual group
and behavioral enjoyment; exaggerated prosody;
theory repetitive paraphrasing
– Parent-
mediated
Green N = 7 (case series – Sibling with Intervention in 12 sessions over Goal: Improve the quality of parent- Case series with
et al. group) and N = 70 ASD BASIS (i- 5 months infant interactions and increase a high-risk and
2013 (comparison BASIS) parent-infant synchrony low-risk
groups); – Developmental Strategies: Use of video aides; maternal control group
8–10 months (M = 8) and attachment sensitive and contingent responding;
theory affect matching; reciprocal
– Parent-infant vocalization
synchrony
Koegel N = 3; – Symptoms Pivotal Response 1 h/week for Goal: Increase motivation to engage in Multiple baseline
et al. 4–9 months (M = 6) of ASD Treatment 4-11 weeks social interaction design across
2013 – Expert (PRT) Strategies: Use of infant-preferred participants
clinical – Behavioral and activities; task variation; interspersal
concern for developmental of preferred and neutral activities;
ASD theory reinforcement
– Social
motivation
hypothesis

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 1 continued
Study Participants Intervention approach Experimental
design
N and age Risk Empirical and Length and Intervention goals and strategies
determinants theoretical basis intensity
or diagnosis
of ASD

Rogers N = 49 (intervention – Symptoms Parent- 1 h/week for Goal: Improve social, communicative, Randomized
et al. group) and N = 49 of ASD Implemented 12 weeks and developmental outcomes controlled trial
2012 (control group); – Expert Early Start Strategies: Increase child attention and with treatment-
12–24 Months clinical Denver Model motivation; use of sensory social as-usual
(M = 21) judgment of (P-ESDM) routines; joint activity routines; use of control group
ASD – Developmental antecedent-behavior-consequence
and behavioral relationships; use of prompting,
theory shaping, fading, and functional
– Relationship- behavior assessment
based
– Parent-
mediated
Schertz N = 3; – Symptoms Joint Attention 1-2 sessions/ Goal: Promote joint attention Multiple baseline
and 20–28 months of ASD Mediated week over Strategies: Engage in face-to-face design across
Odom (M = 23.7) – Positive Learning 9-26 weeks interactive games; turn-taking participants
2007 screen for (JAML) activities; respond to join attention;
ASD – Developmental initiating joint attention activities
theory
– Mediated
learning
– Family-
centered
– Parent-
mediated
Steiner N = 3; – Sibling with Pivotal Response Ten 1-h sessions Goal: Increase functional Multiple baseline
et al. 12 months (M = 12) ASD Treatment over 3 months communication and social motivation design across
2013 (PRT) Strategies: Following the child’s lead; participants
– Behavioral and providing clear prompts; interspersal
developmental of maintenance and acquisition tasks;
theory use of immediate, contingent, and
– Social natural reinforcement; reinforcement
motivation of attempts
hypothesis
Wetherby N = 17 (intervention – Provisional Early Social 2 individual Goal: Improve social communication in Quasi-
and group); clinical Interaction sessions/week the context of family routines experimental
Woods 12–24 months diagnosis of Project (ESI) and 9 Strategies: Goal individualization; design with a
2006 (M = 18) ASD – Developmental playgroup environmental arrangement; waiting; post-treatment
theory sessions over natural reinforcers; balanced turn- contrast group
N = 18 (3rd year 12 months
contrast group); – Parent- taking; modeling; contingent
mediated imitation; requesting imitation; time
25–36 months delay
(M = 31) – Family-
centered
– Routine-based

model in which parents and their infant practiced the Odom 2007; Wetherby and Woods 2006). Intervention
intervention while a clinician provided feedback about occurred primarily in a university clinic setting for the two
implementation. Only one study was therapist-delivered, remaining studies (Rogers et al. 2012; Steiner et al. 2013).
but incorporated a parent-training component (Dawson
et al. 2010). Parent education took place primarily in the Length and Intensity
natural environment (i.e., the home) for seven of the nine
studies (Carter, et al. 2011; Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. Length of treatment ranged from 4 weeks to 2 years and all
2002; Green et al. 2013; Koegel et al. 2013; Schertz and but one intervention were low-intensity, totaling no more

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 2 Infant and parent outcomes


Study Infant outcome Parent outcome

Carter et al. No main effects of treatment were observed for any child outcome Medium-to-large, but statistically insignificant,
2011 variables. A medium effect size for child social communication improvements in parent responsivity were observed
was found for both the HMTW and treatment-as-usual (TAU) following intervention
groups. Children in the HMTW group who exhibited less Child
Object Interest prior to intervention achieved greater gains in
initiating joint attention, initiating behavioral requests, intentional
communication, and parent-reported nonverbal communication
Dawson Improvement in nonverbal cognitive skills significantly greater No parent outcomes were reported
et al. 2010 than baseline assessment and the TAU group after 1 year of
intervention. After 2 years of intervention, the ESDM group
showed significantly greater improvements in receptive and
expressive language as well as parent-reported communication,
daily living skills, and motor skills. No significant group
differences in autism severity were observed, but toddlers in the
ESDM group were more likely to have improved diagnostic
status
Drew et al. Significantly more children in the Parent training group moved There were no group differences in parent-stress
2002 from being minimally verbal to having single word or phrase
speech than in the TAU group. The Parent training group had
marginally higher language comprehension than the TAU group.
There were no group differences in nonverbal IQ, words,
gestures, or symptom severity
Green et al. Preliminary results suggest improvement in infant liveliness for High session attendance and reports on parent satisfaction
2013 both treatment and control groups, but greater improvement in questionnaires suggest overall feasibility and
treatment group acceptability. Preliminary results show that all but one
mother who exhibited low synchrony behaviors, i.e. non-
directiveness and sensitive responsiveness, improved on
these measures following intervention
Koegel et al. Rapid increases in social engagement for all participants following All parents learned to correctly implement the intervention
2013 initiation of the intervention. Social engagement was defined as procedures and met the Fidelity of Implementation
increased happiness, interest, and response to name, as well as criterion during all intervention sessions
decreased eye contact avoidance during parent-infant
interactions. These gains were maintained at a two and six month
follow-up
Rogers Both groups (P-ESDM and TAU) made significant gains in rates of Parents in both P-ESDM and TAU groups showed
et al., 2012 acquisition of developmental skills and reduction of core significant improvement in their use of ESDM interaction
symptoms of ASD following the 12-week intervention. No group skills in the 12-week period. Although these groups did
differences on any primary child outcome variables were found. not significantly differ in their acquisition of the ESDM
Developmental and diagnostic improvements were associated treatment procedures, the P-ESDM group exhibited a
with more intervention hours and younger age at the start of larger effect size than the TAU group following
intervention. Children in the TAU group received a greater intervention. Further, parents in the P-ESDM group
number of intervention hours. reported a stronger working alliance with their therapist
Contrary to the authors’ hypothesis, social orienting and imitation than parents in the TAU group
did not moderate the child effects of P-ESDM.
Higher parent P-ESDM fidelity scores at the start of the study were
significantly related to milder ASD symptoms and higher
developmental scores
Schertz and All participants improved above baseline in focusing on the Parent satisfaction with their child’s progress following
Odom parents’ face, turn-taking, responding to parents’ joint attention intervention was high for two out of three parents and all
2007 overtures, and initiating joint attention encounters. One parents improved confidence in their ability to support
participant only slightly improved on the two joint attention child interactions. Parent participation was high for all
measures parents and fidelity of implementation was high for two
out of three parents
Steiner et al. All participants demonstrated rapid increases in frequency of Following intervention, all parents demonstrated
2013 communication with their parent following the start of independent, correct implementation of intervention
intervention. These gains maintained at post-treatment. Follow- procedures both with and without a clinician present.
up developmental testing at 36 months showed decreases in Additionally, all parents reported high overall satisfaction
autism symptomology with the intervention

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Table 2 continued
Study Infant outcome Parent outcome

Wetherby After one year of treatment, children in the intervention group None reported
and Woods exhibited significant improvements in all reported measures of
2006 social-communication, except for shared positive affect and
number of gaze shifts. The intervention group also exhibited
superior skills in social signals, rate of communicating,
communicative functions, and understanding compared to the
third year contrast group

than 2 h of intervention per week. All treatment programs autism-specific intervention strategies previously estab-
included the expectation that parents would frequently lished in the Preschool Autism Communication Trial
implement the intervention strategies throughout the week (PACT; Green et al. 2010). The primary aim of the inter-
and integrate them into daily activities. Dawson et al. vention was to increase parent-infant synchrony while also
(2010) was the only study to collect data on the amount of addressing emerging symptoms of ASD in 8–10 month-old
time the intervention procedures were implemented by the infants. Improvements in parent-infant synchrony were
parents outside of the intervention sessions (an average of hypothesized to lead to improvements in child dyadic
16.3 h per week). communication and reductions in ASD symptoms. Koegel
et al. (2013) also provided an intervention designed to
Intervention Goals and Strategies modify parent-infant interactions for prelinguistic infants.
Using PRT (Koegel and Koegel 2012), this brief inter-
Most of the reviewed studies discussed behavioral princi- vention program aimed to increase infants’ motivation to
ples as a foundational element of intervention (Carter et al. engage in social interaction. Specifically, goals were to
2011; Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002; Koegel et al. increase eye contact, happiness, and interest during parent-
2013; Rogers et al. 2012; Steiner et al. 2013; Wetherby and infant interactions. Similarly, in an intervention program
Woods 2006). Behavioral components that provided a for 12-month-old infants (Steiner et al. 2013), procedures
framework for intervention included: providing a learning and target behaviors of PRT were modified for use with
opportunity (antecedent), waiting for the child to respond infants. Traditionally, PRT has been studied with preschool
(behavior), and delivering appropriate reinforcement and school-aged children where the focus of intervention is
(consequence). Further, all interventions used naturalistic initially verbal communication (Koegel et al. 1999). These
approaches such that the infant’s daily routines, natural treatment targets were adapted for 12-month-olds to
interests, or preferred activities were infused into treatment include verbal (vocalizations, word approximations), non-
procedures. verbal (gestures, pointing, giving, showing, and commu-
Goals for intervention fit generally under the broad nicative reaching), and coordinated social communicative
developmental area of social-communication, constituting behaviors (communication involving multiple verbal and/
one of the two core areas of ASD in the Diagnostic and or nonverbal behaviors).
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition
(DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association 2013). All Second Year: 13–30 Months Six of the reviewed research
interventions embraced a developmental framework by studies investigated the effectiveness of interventions for
adapting treatment targets and intervention strategies to be infants with a mean age between 13 and 24 months of age
developmentally appropriate for the age of the sample. For (range 13–30 months). The ESDM (Dawson et al. 2010) is a
example, one of the treatment goals for 6-9-month-olds comprehensive developmental and behavioral treatment
was to increase social engagement, namely positive affect model that focuses on the use of naturalistic methods for
and social interest (Koegel et al. 2013) while an interven- supporting young child development. In addition to tradi-
tion program for 20-month-old toddlers targeted receptive tional ESDM, which focused on improving developmental
and expressive verbal communication (Carter et al. 2011). outcomes through an intensive therapist-delivered program,
For this reason, intervention procedures are summarized parent-implemented ESDM (P-ESDM; Rogers et al. 2012)
below in two developmental stages: the first and second focused on teaching parents strategies for engaging in child-
years of life. centered responsive interactions. Both intervention models
targeted the following child behaviors: attention, motivation,
First Year: 4–12 Months Green et al. (2013) developed social engagement in joint activity routines, verbal and
the ‘Intervention in BASIS’ (iBASIS) program based on nonverbal communication, imitation, and joint attention.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

The Early Social Interaction (ESI) Project (Wetherby and toddlers when the parent is the primary interventionist.
Woods 2006) aimed to teach parents to increase social Most studies reported very high satisfaction with the
communication in their toddler using strategies based on intervention on satisfaction questionnaires (Carter et al.
behavioral principles that included natural reinforcement, 2011; Green et al. 2013; Schertz and Odom 2007; Steiner
time delay, modeling, turn-taking, and imitation. Specific et al. 2013), while Rogers et al. (2012) reported stronger
target behaviors differed for each toddler based on individ- working alliances between parents and therapists in the
ual goals and parental input, but broadly related to social P-ESDM group over the comparison TAU group.
communication skills. The HMTW program (Carter et al. Additionally, most studies evaluated training fidelity for
2011) emphasized the use of everyday routines to target parent performance in adhering to intervention procedures
improvements in social communication. The intervention and correctly implementing techniques. Results of fidelity
aimed to teach parents to increase their responsivity to child of implementation measures showed that parents improved
communicative attempts, provide rewards, follow the child’s in their ability to implement the specific intervention
lead, and build joint action routines. HMTW target behav- strategies taught during intervention (Koegel et al. 2013;
iors included improving two-way interactions and increasing Rogers et al. 2012; Schertz and Odom 2007; Steiner et al.
expressive and receptive communicative abilities. Drew 2013). Additionally, some studies monitored treatment
et al. (2002) used a social-pragmatic joint attention focused integrity by collecting fidelity of implementation measures
parent-training program to promote joint action routines and for clinicians (Carter et al. 2011; Dawson et al. 2010;
teach children with ASD joint attention behaviors. Lan- Rogers et al. 2012; Wetherby and Woods 2006). These
guage-learning was promoted through the use of exagger- methods included completing self-assessment checklists
ated prosody and repetitive phrasing and behavioral and behavioral coding of videotaped intervention sessions.
management strategies were also taught to parents. Schertz However, one study that compared fidelity of imple-
and Odom (2007) also employed a joint attention focused mentation of parents in the active treatment condition to
intervention, but rather than explicitly teaching joint atten- parents in the control group found that parents in the TAU
tion using a behavioral framework, the JAML program group were equally as skilled at implementing the inter-
structures parent–child interactions in a way that promotes vention procedures despite never being taught the inter-
joint attention. vention (Rogers et al. 2012).

Developmental Outcome
Experimental Design
Assessments of behavioral functioning and developmental
Overall, effects of intervention were measured using pre-
ability were conducted pre- and post-treatment for five of
and post-treatment behavioral assessments. Four studies
the nine studies (Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002;
(Carter et al. 2011; Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002;
Green et al. 2013; Rogers et al. 2012; Steiner et al. 2013).
Rogers et al. 2012) employed randomized controlled trials
These measures included the Griffiths Scale of Infant
with a TAU control group. Wetherby and Woods (2006)
Development, MacArthur Communicative Development
used a quasi-experimental design in which post-intervention
Inventories, Mullen Scales of Early Learning, and Vine-
outcomes were compared to a TAU control group, however
land Adaptive Behavior Scales. Three studies observed
the control group was not available for comparison at pre-
within-group gains in cognitive abilities (Dawson et al.
intervention. A case series design in which a small sample of
2010; Rogers, et al. 2012; Steiner et al. 2013). Addition-
treatment participants was compared to both an at-risk non-
ally, Dawson et al. (2010) reported gains in cognitive skills
intervention control group as well as a low-risk non-inter-
and adaptive behavior greater than that of the TAU control
vention control group was reported in Green et al. (2013).
group, while Rogers et al. (2012) did not observe signifi-
Koegel et al. (2013), Schertz and Odom (2007), and Steiner
cant between-group differences in developmental outcome
et al. (2013) used a multiple baseline design in which change
following the brief 12-week intervention.
was measured through repeated observations prior to, dur-
ing, and after the intervention.
Diagnostic outcome

Intervention Outcome The effect of intervention on autism symptomology was


assessed through two autism-specific evaluations for
Feasibility and Acceptability infants and toddlers, the ADOS-T and the Autism Obser-
vation Scale for Infants (AOSI; Bryson et al. 2008).
Parent-perceived acceptability and feasibility are especially Although intervention for infants and toddlers with or at-
important components for interventions with infants and risk for ASD older than 12 months appeared to lessen

123
J Autism Dev Disord

autism symptom severity, again it was reported that these Discussion


improvements were not significantly different than
improvements of toddlers in the TAU control group Treatments for infants with or at-risk for ASD in the first
(Dawson et al. 2010; Drew et al. 2002; Rogers et al. 2012). 2 years of life are still emerging and at the time of this
Furthermore, measurement of autism symptomology ear- review, only nine identified studies have investigated the
lier in life using the AOSI and a visual disengagement task feasibility and effectiveness of very early intervention.
between 8 and 10 months of age revealed no obvious Three studies examined intervention for infants in the first
effects of intervention (Green et al. 2013). year, and six studies focused on infants in the second year
of life. There was a mix of experimental designs including
four randomized controlled trials (RCTs), three multiple
Social-Communication Outcome
baseline designs, and two quasi-experimental designs. The
multiple baseline designs demonstrated improvement in
Intervention effectiveness for the youngest group of infants
infant social engagement and communication behaviors
was measured primarily through change in infant social
following intervention (Koegel et al. 2013; Schertz and
engagement during parent-infant interactions. Intervention
Odom 2007; Steiner et al. 2013), while the RCTs had
for infants in the first year of life demonstrated improve-
mixed findings regarding enhanced effectiveness over the
ments in infant positive affect, eye contact, and response to
TAU comparison group (Carter et al. 2011; Dawson et al.
name (Koegel et al. 2013), as well as preliminary findings
2010; Drew et al., 2002; Rogers et al., 2012). The quasi-
indicating increases in infant liveliness (Green et al. 2013).
experimental designs documented feasibility and provided
Additionally, gains in emerging communication were
preliminary support for treatment effects on infant behav-
captured with increase in the use of eye contact, vocal-
ior, including increased infant liveliness (Green et al. 2013)
izations, and/or gestures during parent-infant interactions
and improved social communication (Wetherby and
(Steiner et al. 2013),
Woods 2006). Overall, the studies reviewed provide a
Infant functional communication was among the pri-
foundation for the further development and investigation of
mary outcome measures for infants older than 12 months.
interventions for infants and toddlers under 2 years of age
Observed gains included improved performance on the
who are at-risk for, or diagnosed with, ASD. These studies
expressive and receptive language (Dawson et al. 2010;
also highlight several issues in need of continued explo-
Drew et al. 2002; Rogers et al. 2012; Wetherby and Woods
ration, including effectiveness of intervention for reducing
2006), and an increase in behaviors related to joint atten-
symptoms of ASD in infancy, impact of intervention on
tion (Carter et al. 2011; Schertz and Odom 2007; Wetherby
parent stress and fidelity of implementation, and reliable
and Woods 2006). However, gains in social-communica-
strategies for identifying at-risk infants.
tion were only greater than the TAU group for children
enrolled in 2 years of ESDM (Dawson et al. 2010).
Treatment Effectiveness

Moderators of Outcome Empirical evidence for the effects of intervention on infant


and toddler social and communicative development pre-
Two studies included moderator variables to uncover the sented in this review is encouraging, but demonstrates the
effects of specific child and intervention characteristics on need for further research. Most studies demonstrated some
child outcomes. Rogers et al. (2012) identified several improvement in verbal and nonverbal communication,
variables leading to improved outcome for all toddlers, social engagement, and autism symptomology from pre- to
regardless of treatment condition. In particular, nonsocial post-intervention, providing preliminary evidence sup-
orienting prior to intervention predicted developmental porting the effectiveness of very early intervention. How-
ability and lower ADOS Social Affect scores, while social ever, two out of the four studies employing an RCT with a
orienting was found to predict decreases in the ADOS TAU control group found that the target interventions
Restricted and Repetitive Behavior at outcome. This study (P-ESDM and HMTW) did not result in gains significantly
also revealed significant relationships between both age at greater than the TAU comparison group. Notably, the only
the start of treatment and the number of intervention hours. study to successfully demonstrate efficacy of a specific
That is, toddlers who were younger and received more intervention (Dawson et al. 2010) was the most intensive
hours of intervention demonstrated enhanced develop- intervention and the only one to utilize both clinician-
mental gains and greater decreases in autism symptomol- delivered treatment methods and parent training. This
ogy. Finally, Carter et al. (2011) identified decreased object stresses the need for more rigorous investigation of the key
interest as a moderator for facilitated growth in commu- ingredients in intervention for this very young population.
nication for the HMTW group. Interestingly, Rogers et al. (2012) note that the TAU group

123
J Autism Dev Disord

in fact received a greater number of intervention hours than Additional moderators for treatment identified in the
the P-ESDM group, and that the number of hours positively studies reviewed include the infant’s age and the intensity
predicted improvement on child outcome measures. The of intervention. Younger children and those who received
authors also highlight the likelihood that the TAU group more hours of intervention seemed to benefit more from
received a parent education program that resembled many early intervention (Rogers et al. 2012). Intriguingly, no
aspects of their own P-ESDM program, calling to question studies reported moderating effects of cognitive ability or
the true difference in service delivery between the two verbal communication, despite other studies reporting these
groups. As a final note, the authors question the suitability factors in early childhood are predictors of positive out-
of some measures (e.g. ADOS, Vineland) in evaluating come later in life (e.g., Billstedt et al. 2007). These and
short-term changes following a 12-week intervention. other potential moderators should continue to be explored
These questions regarding validity of the TAU and selec- in future research. Most intervention programs for children
tion of appropriate outcome measures are critical consid- with ASD focus on increasing social-communication and
erations for accurately evaluating the effectiveness of early decreasing problem behaviors, assuming that poor social-
interventions. communicative abilities may have cascading long-term
It would be prudent for future research to weigh the effects on a variety of areas including academic success,
benefits of conducting an RCT using a TAU control group mental health, and independent living. Indeed, higher
design against the challenges when limited information is cognitive ability and early-onset verbal communication in
available about the nature of these community treatments. childhood are among the best-known prognostic indicators
For example, it may be more advantageous to directly for improved outcome (Billstedt et al. 2007). For infants
compare established treatments that can be delivered in a with prodromal symptoms of ASD, however, pivotal
standardized way at a specified intensity in order to behaviors that will maximally improve outcomes are not
evaluate the unique contributions of each treatment well understood. Long-term prognostic factors for infants
approach. This experimental design may be more effective at-risk for ASD should be investigated with an emphasis on
in understanding the effects of treatment. Multiple baseline improved functioning for the individual as well as the
design studies begin to allow for analysis of treatment family system. Recent research on long-term ASD outcome
effectiveness while controlling for maturation, an espe- has begun to scrutinize the meaning of ‘‘positive outcome’’
cially critical threat to validity in research with young for the individuals and their families (e.g., Henninger and
infants and toddlers. However, this approach is compli- Taylor 2013). Similar rigor should be applied in defining
cated by the heterogeneity of typical development and the positive outcome for infants, which will then directly
documented trends of some high-risk siblings who resolve influence the identification of target behaviors. The current
developmental delays quickly and without the need for studies have begun to tackle this issue by capitalizing on
intervention (Landa et al. 2007). Inherent weaknesses of family therapist collaborative decision-making processes
multiple baseline designs also include questions of gen- (e.g., Wetherby and Woods 2006) and pivotal areas, such
eralizability and specific treatment efficacy without a as motivation (e.g., Koegel et al. 2013), in goal
comparison group. Recommendations for optimal inter- development.
vention research designs include multisite RCTs that
control for several variables including intensity and length Feasibility
of treatment, active ingredients of treatment, and both
short and long-term effects on child outcome (Kasari Establishing feasibility was a key objective in all reviewed
2002). studies. Feasibility is an important component of inter-
Exploration of moderator variables associated with vention models as it provides preliminary support for wide-
outcome data can provide critical information about factors scale dissemination and yields information about accept-
influencing the effectiveness of intervention. For example, ability for clinicians and parents (Smith et al. 2007). Some
HMTW was documented to be more effective with chil- of the reviewed studies used measures of parent satisfac-
dren with less interest in objects, whereas children who had tion, parent involvement, and fidelity of implementation to
a high interest in objects were less amenable to the treat- address issues of feasibility. Despite emphasis on parental
ment program (Carter et al. 2011). Identifying behavioral responsibility to implement the intervention throughout
profiles that match particular intervention methods for daily routines, families reported very high satisfaction,
children with ASD has long been a topic of interest (e.g. high involvement in treatment sessions, and enjoyment of
Sherer and Schreibman 2005; Stahmer et al. 2011), and the intervention. In fact, Estes et al. (2013) reported in a
infant intervention research could benefit from identifying later study that P-ESDM parents (Rogers et al. 2012)
subgroups that might differentially benefit from specific experienced significantly less stress than the TAU control
treatment strategies. group, after controlling for autism social severity and

123
J Autism Dev Disord

baseline levels of stress. This finding is consistent with this research suggests that intensity of early interventions is
group also reporting stronger working alliances with ther- positively associated with improved child outcome (Reed
apists. Furthermore, Schertz and Odom (2007) used qual- et al. 2007), a finding that was replicated by Rogers et al.
itative methods to examine parent perspectives on the (2012). Despite the limited number of studies in this
intervention, revealing a relation between increased autism review, the research presented here could suggest that a
severity, slower progress in treatment, and lower parent more intensive intervention evaluated over a longer period
fidelity of implementation. of time (Dawson et al. 2010) may result in greater gains
Research suggests that generalization of learned than 1 h per week of parent-mediated intervention. Future
behaviors and maintenance of gains for children with ASD research should consider the possibility that more hours of
largely depends on the persistent and consistent imple- intervention are needed in order to improve symptoms of
mentation of naturalistic behavioral procedures, making ASD in infancy.
fidelity of implementation an important measure of feasi-
bility, and possibly child outcome (Kashinath et al. 2006; Early Identification and Prevention
National Research Council 2001; Rocha et al. 2007). The
studies reviewed here demonstrated the effectiveness of One of the greatest difficulties associated with designing
teaching parents to implement intervention techniques, research studies to investigate the effectiveness of very
possibly suggesting that this type of dissemination model early intervention for infants at-risk for ASD is the issue of
may be appropriate and implementable by some parents of accurate identification. The reviewed studies used three
at-risk infants. Interestingly, Rogers et al. (2012) reported primary methods to address this challenge. A firm clinical
that the TAU control group met fidelity of implementation diagnosis or provisional clinical diagnosis of ASD was
to the same degree as the P-ESDM group. The authors required for only three studies while four of the remaining
speculate that this was due to the community TAU parents studies required participants to exhibit behavior that war-
learning similar skills to the P-ESDM parents, and the ranted clinical concern for ASD based on quantifiable
community TAU infants receiving significantly more hours behavioral assessment and/or an autism screener. The use
of intervention. Furthermore, fidelity of implementation of both clinical judgment and behavioral assessment could
(FOI) scores were quite high prior to onset of intervention, reflect the concern that currently available diagnostic tools
suggesting a sample bias that could contribute to the lack of may lack the sensitivity and specificity to serve as the sole
FOI difference between the groups. Participants were determinant of risk for ASD in infancy. Alternatively, two
highly educated, 80 % of parents had received at least studies assumed a model of prevention and provided
some college education, and half of the participants had an intervention to all infants at increased genetic risk for ASD
older sibling with ASD or another developmental disabil- (i.e., younger siblings of children with ASD). However, a
ity. It is then possible that parents had a priori knowledge large proportion of these children would be expected to
of common intervention procedures and were already have typical developmental trajectories even without any
integrating some techniques into routine interactions with intervention, making evaluation of intervention effects
their at-risk infant. The challenge of gathering representa- challenging (Ozonoff et al. 2011).
tive participant samples is not unique to this study and is These approaches to ascertaining risk for ASD in
acknowledged as a limitation by the authors. There remains infancy give rise to the question of whether or not a
an urgent need to recruit representative samples and iden- diagnosis of ASD is necessary to provide intervention.
tify the effects of such demographic variables on FOI and Public health models are increasingly adopting models of
child outcome. Future research investigating parent FOI preventative care, in which medical professionals note
and child outcome will help to guide clinical practice. prodromal features and, in an effort to avoid the manifes-
It is also interesting to note that although our search tation of full-scale symptomology, provide the indicated
criteria included all delivery modes for intervention, all of treatment prior to diagnosis (American Academy of Pedi-
the studies that met our inclusion criteria were at least atrics 2006). The preliminary, but generally positive,
partially parent-mediated. Eight of the nine intervention treatment results of the reviewed studies support the further
studies used a parent-mediation model that was very low- development of prevention models and warrant additional
intensity, averaging 1–2 h per week. While this is in line experimentally controlled research designs to establish the
with several intervention studies for toddlers with ASD that effectiveness of behavioral intervention for infants in the
have demonstrated effectiveness of low-intensity inter- first 2 years of life. The data presented here may suggest
ventions (e.g., Vismara et al. 2009), it is significantly less that intervention beginning as early as 6 months of age
than the Early Intensive Behavior Intervention (EIBI) could lead to observable improvements in core areas of
model in which 40 h per week of clinician-delivered ASD, at least in the short-term, and if continued research
intervention is recommended (Lovaas 1987). Additionally, supports these findings, the argument for identifying and

123
J Autism Dev Disord

referring at-risk infants in the first 2 years of life, before levels of parent satisfaction following intervention are
ASD is typically diagnosed, is even stronger. encouraging and long-term outcome studies will enrich our
understanding of the effects of receiving intervention during
Limitations the first 2 years of life.

This review paper has several limitations. Due to the lim- Acknowledgments This study was not directly funded, however
conceptualization for this article was aided by a predoctoral fellow-
ited number of studies on this topic to date, conclusions ship awarded to the first author by the Autism Science Foundation
presented here are based upon a relatively small sample (11-1014). We would like to express our appreciation to all families
size, especially regarding interventions for infants in the who continue to dedicate their time to autism research.
first year of life. It was also necessary to analyze treatments
for infants with confirmed diagnosis of ASD along with
treatments for infants at-risk for ASD, which makes con- References
clusions from this heterogeneous group more challenging
to interpret. It is possible that other relevant findings were American Academy of Pediatrics. (2006). Identifying infants nad
young children with developmental disorders in the medical
excluded based on criteria related to experimental design or home: An algorithm for developmental surveillance and screen-
publication in English-language peer reviewed journals. ing. Pediatrics, 118, 405–420.
Though an effort was made to include studies where the American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical
majority of children were under 24 months (mean age did manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: Amer-
ican Psychiatric Association.
not exceed 24 months), the review did not include studies Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to
where only a few participants fell in this age range. Finally, people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction.
the diverse methodological approaches and varied out- Child Development, 55(4), 1278–1289.
comes measured across studies necessitate additional rep- Barbaro, J., & Dissanayake, C. (2009). Autism spectrum disorders in
infancy and toddlerhood: A review of the evidence on early
lication of results to confirm the initial findings for each of signs, early identification tools, and early diagnosis. Journal of
these novel treatment applications. While it is premature to Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30(5), 447–459.
make conclusions about differential effects of the treat- Barbaro, J., & Dissanayake, C. (2013). Early markers of autism
ments reviewed, this review serves as a guide summarizing spectrum disorders in infants and toddlers prospectively identi-
fied in the Social Attention and Communication Study. Autism,
research efforts to date. 17(1), 64–86.
Billstedt, E., Carina Gillberg, I., & Gillberg, C. (2007). Autism in
Future Directions adults: Symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. Use of
the DISCO in a community sample followed from childhood.
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(11), 1102–1110.
Prevention and intervention efforts for infants at-risk for Boyd, B., Odom, S., Humphreys, B., & Sam, A. (2010). Infants and
ASD in the first 2 years of life are part of a burgeoning trend toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Early identification and
in the field, with several studies documenting preliminary early intervention. Journal of Early Intervention, 32(75), 75–98.
positive findings. The evidence suggests that parent-medi- Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., McDermott, C., Rombough, V., &
Brian, J. (2008). The autism observation scale for infants: Scale
ated treatment programs may improve short-term outcomes development and reliability data. Journal of Autism and
for at-risk infants and toddlers, including social engagement Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 731–738.
and communication, yet additional research is needed in this Carter, A. S., Messinger, D. S., Stone, W. L., Celimli, S., Nahmias, A.
area (National Autism Center 2009). The heterogeneity of S., & Yoder, P. (2011). A randomized controlled trial of Hanen’s
‘More Than Words’ in toddlers with early autism symptoms.
both typically developing infants and those with ASD calls Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(7), 741–752.
for both acute scientific rigor and careful evaluation and Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., & Volkmar, F. (2007). Autism
description of presenting symptoms. Experimental design spectrum disorder in the second year: Stability and change in
issues, including the use of a TAU control group, potential syndrome expression. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychi-
atry, 48(2), 128–138.
sample biases, and threats to internal and external validity, Chawarska, K., Macari, S., & Shic, F. (2013). Decreased spontaneous
must be thoughtfully considered in future research for this attention to social scenes in 6-month-old infants later diagnosed
population. Research exploring novel methodologies for with autism spectrum disorders. Biological Psychiatry, 74(3),
identifying infants and toddlers who are ‘‘at-risk’’ and for 195–203.
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T.
determining appropriate targets for intervention is needed. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in
Family context and cultural variables should also be of Cognitive Sciences, 16(4), 231–239.
primary importance in designing individual treatment plans Council, National Research, & Committee on Educational Interven-
and delivering parent education. Furthermore, intervention tions for Children with Autism. (2001). Educating children with
autism. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
studies should carefully consider moderator variables that Courchesne, E., Carper, R., & Akshoomoff, N. (2003). Evidence of
may influence differential treatment effectiveness for such a brain overgrowth in the first year of life in autism. The Journal of
heterogeneous population. Observed child gains and high the American Medical Association, 290(3), 337–344.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Crais, E. R., & Watson, L. R. (2014). Challenges and opportunities in change in clinical diagnosis and symptom presentation. Journal
early identification and intervention for children at-risk for of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(5), 582–590.
autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Speech- Henninger, N. A., & Taylor, J. L. (2013). Outcomes in adults with
Language Pathology, 16(1), 23–29. autism spectrum disorders: A historical perspective. Autism,
Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain plasticity, 17(1), 103–116.
and the prevention of autism spectrum disorder. Development Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans.
and Psychopathology, 20(3), 775–803. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(7), 475–483.
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., Greenson, Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991).
J., et al. (2010). Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its
for toddlers with autism: The Early Start Denver Model. subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1), 1–19.
Pediatrics, 125(1), e17–e23. Johnson, C. P., & Myers, S. M. (2007). Identification and evaluation
Dietz, C., Swinkels, S., van Daalen, E., van Engeland, H., & of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5),
Buitelaar, J. K. (2006). Screening for autistic spectrum disorder 1183–1215.
in children aged 14–15 months. II: Population screening with the Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in
Early Screening of Autistic Traits Questionnaire (ESAT). Design decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism.
and general findings. Journal of Autism and Developmental Nature, 504, 427–431. doi:10.1038/nature12715.
Disorders, 36(6), 713–722. Kaiser, A. P., & Hester, P. P. (1994). Generalized effects of enhanced
Drew, A., Baird, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Slonims, V., milieu teaching. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37(6),
Wheelwright, S., et al. (2002). A pilot randomised control trial 1320–1340.
of a parent training intervention for pre-school children with Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early intervention programs
autism. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 11(6), for children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
266–272. Disorders, 32(5), 447–461.
Elsabbagh, M., & Johnson, M. H. (2010). Getting answers from Kashinath, S., Woods, J., & Goldstein, H. (2006). Enhancing
babies about autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(2), 81–87. generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by parents
Elsabbagh, M., Mercure, E., Hudry, K., Chandler, S., Pasco, G., of children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language and
Charman, T., et al. (2012). Infant neural sensitivity to dynamic Hearing Research, 49(3), 466.
eye gaze is associated with later emerging autism. Current Kleinman, J. M., Robins, D. L., Ventola, P. E., Pandey, J., Boorstein,
Biology, 22(4), 338–342. H. C., Esser, E. L., et al. (2008). The modified checklist for
Estes, A., Vismara, L., Mercado, C., Fitzpatrick, A., Elder, L., autism in toddlers: A follow-up study investigating the early
Greenson, J., et al. (2013). The impact of parent-delivered detection of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
intervention on parents of very young children with autism. Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 827–839.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(2), Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (2012). The PRT pocket guide: Pivotal
353–365. response treatment for autism spectrum disorders. Baltimore,
Eyler, L. T., Pierce, K., & Courchesne, E. (2012). A failure of left MD: Brookes Publishing Company.
temporal cortex to specialize for language is an early emerging Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, C. M. (1999).
and fundamental property of autism. Brain, 135(3), 949–960. Pivotal response intervention I: Overview of approach. Research
Flanagan, J. E., Landa, R., Bhat, A., & Bauman, M. (2012). Head lag and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 24(3),
in infants at risk for autism: A preliminary study. The American 174–185.
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5), 577–585. Koegel, L. K., Singh, A. K., Koegel, R. L., Hollingsworth, J. R., &
Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). How the timing and Bradshaw, J. (2013). Assessing and improving early social
quality of early experiences influence the development of brain engagement in infants. Journal of Positive Behavior Interven-
architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 28–40. tions, 16(2), 69–80.
Goin-Kochel, R. P., Mackintosh, V. H., & Myers, B. J. (2009). Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Padden, D., Nelson, T., & Pruitt, J.
Parental reports on the efficacy of treatments and therapies for (2005). Early speech perception and later language development:
their children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Implications for the’’ Critical Period’’. Language Learning and
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 528–537. Development, 1(3–4), 237–264.
Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., & Wilke, Landa, R. J., Holman, K. C., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2007). Social and
A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treatment intensity on communication development in toddlers with early and later
behavioral intervention outcomes for children with autism diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General
spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, Psychiatry, 64(7), 853–864.
3(4), 1014–1022. Lord, C., Luyster, R., Gotham, K., & Guthrie, W. J. (2012). Autism
Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Slonims, V., diagnostic observation schedule: Toddler module. Los Angeles,
Howlin, P., et al. (2010). Parent-mediated communication- CA: Western Psychological Services.
focused treatment in children with autism (PACT): A random- Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic
ised controlled trial. The Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for
Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, M., & caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental
Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments used by parents disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
of children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24(5), 659–685.
27(1), 70–84. Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal educational
Green, J., Wan, M. W., Guiraud, J., Holsgrove, S., McNally, J., and intellectual functioning in young autistic children. Journal of
Slonims, V., et al. (2013). Intervention for infants at risk of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 3.
developing autism: A case series. Journal of Autism and Love, J. R., Carr, J. E., Almason, S. M., & Petursdottir, A. I. (2009).
Developmental Disorders, 43(11), 2502–2514. Early and intensive behavioral intervention for autism: A survey
Guthrie, W., Swineford, L. B., Nottke, C., & Wetherby, A. M. (2012). of clinical practices. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
Early diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: Stability and 3(2), 421–428.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Maestro, S., Muratori, F., Cavallaro, M. C., Pei, F., Stern, D., Golse, Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1988). The Childhood
B., et al. (2002). Attentional skills during the first 6 months of Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Los Angeles, CA: Western
age in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Psychological.
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(10), Schreibman, L., et al. (2014). Common Elements of Research-Based
1239–1245. Practices for Toddlers with ASD: Naturalistic Developmental
Maglione, M. A., Gans, D., Das, L., Timbie, J., & Kasari, C. (2012). Behavioral Interventions. Autism Speaks Toddler Treatment
Nonmedical interventions for children with ASD: Recommended Network Meeting, International Meeting for Autism Research,
guidelines and further research needs. Pediatrics, 130(Supple- May 14, 2014.
ment 2), S169–S178. Schultz, R. T. (2005). Developmental deficits in social perception in
Mundy, P., & Neal, R. (2000). Neural plasticity, joint attention, and a autism: The role of the amygdala and fusiform face area.
transactional social-orienting model of autism. International International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2),
Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 139–168. 125–141.
National Autism Center. (2009). National standards report. Ran- Schumann, C. M., & Amaral, D. G. (2006). Stereological analysis of
dolph, MA: National Autism Center. amygdala neuron number in autism. The Journal of Neurosci-
Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D., Yirmiya, N., ence, 26(29), 7674–7679.
Zwaigenbaum, L., et al. (2011). Recurrence risk for autism Shattuck, P. T., Durkin, M., Maenner, M., Newschaffer, C., Mandell,
spectrum disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium D. S., Wiggins, L., et al. (2009). Timing of identification among
study. Pediatrics, 128(3), e488–e495. children with an autism spectrum disorder: Findings from a
Parlade, M. V., Messinger, D. S., Delgado, C. E., Kaiser, M. Y., Van population-based surveillance study. Journal of the American
Hecke, A. V., & Mundy, P. C. (2009). Anticipatory smiling: Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 474–483.
Linking early affective communication and social outcome. Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (2005). Individual behavioral
Infant Behavior and Development, 32(1), 33–43. profiles and predictors of treatment effectiveness for children
Paul, R. (2007). Language disorders from infancy through adoles- with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
cence: Assessment and intervention (3rd ed.). St. Louis: Mosby/ 73(3), 525.
Elsevier. Shic, F., Macari, S., & Chawarska, K. (2014). Speech disturbs face
Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Corness, M. (2007). Brief report: Relative scanning in 6-month-old infants who develop autism spectrum
effectiveness of different home-based behavioral approaches to disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 75(3), 231–237.
early teaching intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmen- Siegel, B. (2001). Pervasive developmental disorder screening test-II.
tal Disorders, 37(9), 1815–1821. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early intensive Smith, T., Scahill, L., Dawson, G., Guthrie, D., Lord, C., Odom, S.,
behavioral intervention for young children with autism spectrum et al. (2007). Designing research studies on psychosocial
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, interventions in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
42(4), 512–520. Disorders, 37(2), 354–366.
Reznick, J. S., Baranek, G. T., Reavis, S., Watson, L. R., & Crais, E. Stahmer, A. C., Collings, N. M., & Palinkas, L. A. (2005). Early
R. (2007). A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year- intervention practices for children with autism: Descriptions
olds at risk for an eventual diagnosis of autism: The first year from community providers. Focus on Autism and Other Devel-
inventory. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, opmental Disabilities, 20(2), 66–79.
37(9), 1691–1710. Stahmer, A. C., Schreibman, L., & Cunningham, A. B. (2011).
Robins, D. L. (2008). Screening for autism spectrum disorders in Toward a technology of treatment individualization for young
primary care settings. Autism, 12(5), 537–556. children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research, 1380,
Robins, D. L., Fein, D., Barton, M. L., & Green, J. A. (2001). The 229–239.
modified checklist for autism in toddlers: An initial study Steiner, A. M., Gengoux, G. W., Klin, A., & Chawarska, K. (2013).
investigating the early detection of autism and pervasive Pivotal response treatment for infants at-risk for autism spectrum
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental disorders: A pilot study. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Disorders, 31(2), 131–144. Disorders, 43(1), 91–102.
Rocha, M. L., Schreibman, L., & Stahmer, A. C. (2007). Effective- Stone, W. L., Coonrod, E. E., Turner, L. M., & Pozdol, S. L. (2004).
ness of training parents to teach joint attention in children with Psychometric properties of the STAT for early autism screening.
autism. Journal of Early Intervention, 29(2), 154–172. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6),
Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). Early start Denver model for 691–701.
young children with autism: Promoting language, learning, and Tomasello, M., & Carpenter, M. (2007). Shared intentionality.
engagement. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Developmental Science, 10(1), 121–125.
Rogers, S. J., Estes, A., Lord, C., Vismara, L., Winter, J., Fitzpatrick, Vismara, L. A., Colombi, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2009). Can one hour per
A., et al. (2012). Effects of a brief Early Start Denver Model week of therapy lead to lasting changes in young children with
(ESDM)-based parent intervention on toddlers at risk for autism autism? Autism, 13(1), 93–115.
spectrum disorders: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the Vismara, L. A., & Rogers, S. J. (2008). The early start denver model a
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10), case study of an innovative practice. Journal of Early Interven-
1052–1065. tion, 31(1), 91–108.
Schertz, H. H., & Odom, S. L. (2007). Promoting joint attention in Webb, S. J., Jones, E. J., Kelly, J., & Dawson, G. (2014). The
toddlers with autism: A parent-mediated developmental model. motivation for very early intervention for infants at high risk for
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(8), autism spectrum disorders. International Journal of Speech-
1562–1575. Language Pathology, 16(1), 36–42.
Schertz, H. H., Reichow, B., Tan, P., Vaiouli, P., & Yildirim, E. Wetherby, A. M., Brosnan-Maddox, S., Peace, V., & Newton, L.
(2012). Interventions for toddlers with autism spectrum disorders (2008). Validation of the infant—Toddler Checklist as a
an evaluation of research evidence. Journal of Early Interven- broadband screener for autism spectrum disorders from 9 to
tion, 34(3), 166–189. 24 months of age. Autism, 12(5), 487–511.

123
J Autism Dev Disord

Wetherby, A., & Woods, J. (2002). Systematic observation of red present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. The
flags for autism spectrum disorders in young children, unpub- American journal of psychiatry, 169(6), 589–600.
lished manual. Tallahassee, FL: Florida State University. Young, R. L., Brewer, N., & Pattison, C. (2003). Parental identifi-
Wetherby, A. M., & Woods, J. J. (2006). Early social interaction cation of early behavioural abnormalities in children with
project for children with autism spectrum disorders beginning in autistic disorder. Autism, 7(2), 125–143.
the second year of life a preliminary study. Topics in Early Zachor, D. A., Ben-Itzchak, E., Rabinovich, A. L., & Lahat, E.
Childhood Special Education, 26(2), 67–82. (2007). Change in autism core symptoms with intervention.
Wolff, J. J., Gu, H., Gerig, G., Elison, J. T., Styner, M., Gouttard, S., Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(4), 304–317.
et al. (2012). Differences in white matter fiber tract development

123

You might also like