You are on page 1of 5

Int. J.

Devl Neuroscience 39 (2014) 44–48

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ijdevneu

Diagnosing young children with autism


Johnny L. Matson, Rachel L. Goldin ∗
Louisiana State University, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The starting point for any research on Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) involves the identification of
Received 23 December 2013 people who evince the condition. From this point follows research on symptom presentation, genetics,
Received in revised form 22 February 2014 epidemiology, animal models, treatment efficacy, and many other important topics. Major advances have
Accepted 23 February 2014
been made in differential diagnosis, particularly with young children. This fact is particularly important
since ASD is a life long condition. This review documents recent advances and the current state of research
Keywords:
on this topic.
Autism
© 2014 ISDN. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Diagnosis
Young children

The field of Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) has expanded Lewis et al., 2007). Adding further to the heterogeneity of the con-
rapidly in the last three decades (Lai et al., 2013; Matson and dition is the co-occurrence of a host of problematic behaviors and
LoVullo, 2009). Once believed to be rare, and to be due to poor disorders. Intellectual disability is the most common disorder noted
parenting, researchers have now established that the disorder is in conjunction with ASD. The overlap with ASD may be as high
common and a biobehavioral model of etiology has been estab- as 70% (Lai et al., 2013; Matson et al., 2009). Challenging behav-
lished (Matson and Kozlowski, 2011). While the exact cause of iors such as aggression, tantrums, eating disorders, stereotypic and
ASD has yet to be determined, genetics are definitely implicated self-injurious behavior are also common (Matson and Rivet, 2008;
(Poultney et al., 2013). In utero insult, the presents of toxins dur- Moore, 2009). Other frequent comorbidities include ADHD, anxi-
ing and after gestation, have also been suggested as a possible cause ety disorders, depression, and obsessive compulsive behavior, as
(Chauhan and Chauhan, 2006; Garrecht and Austin, 2011; Kim et al., well as deficits in a host of adaptive skills (LoVullo and Matson,
2010). 2009; Smith and Matson, 2010a,b,c). If that is not enough, seizures,
Long term prognosis of ASD is poor without early and prolonged developmental coordination disorder, gastrointestinal problems,
treatment. At present, operant conditioning (e.g., applied behavior and cerebral palsy are also common co-occurring disorders (Matson
analysis) is the core intervention, structured educational environ- and Goldin, 2013; Surén et al., 2012).
ments, speech/communication therapy, occupational and physical These issues have led researchers to begin to question the
therapy also have strong empirical support as important add on possibility of a common genetic profile and neurodevelopmental
therapies (Causin et al., 2013). There are no pharmacological meth- pathways that may exist between ASD and a host of other disorders.
ods which have proven to be effective for the treatment of the core This evolution in diagnostic thinking also points out and under-
symptoms of ASD at this time. However, medications are often used scores the link between basic research and applied methods and
to treat comorbid conditions such as seizures, anxiety disorders, procedures. The study of genetics and physiological structures and
depression, and ADHD (Horovitz et al., 2012a; Lake et al., 2012; mechanisms relies in large part on the link to behavioral expression
Mannion et al., 2013). of symptoms. These symptoms are currently captured largely via
Core symptoms of ASD consist of deficits in communication, observation and the use of paper and pencil tests given by trained
social skills, and stereotypic and ritualistic behaviors (Matson and clinicians. Thus, systematic, comprehensive, reliable, and valid test-
Wilkins, 2009). These deficits are consistent across individuals ing methods are a starting point for human research on ASD. The
diagnosed with ASD, but the severity of individual symptoms may researcher must have an accurate appraisal as to who has ASD and
vary considerably from case to case (Gürkan and Hagerman, 2012; who does not.

1. First concern
∗ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, LSU, Baton Rouge, LA
70803, USA. Tel.: +1 225 578 1494. The advent of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention has put
E-mail address: rgoldi3@lsu.edu (R.L. Goldin). considerable pressure on clinicians to diagnose at younger and

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.02.003
0736-5748/© 2014 ISDN. Published by Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1873474x, 2014, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.02.003 by Univeresidad Do Extremo Sul Catarinense, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
J.L. Matson, R.L. Goldin / Int. J. Devl Neuroscience 39 (2014) 44–48 45

younger ages. Until recently, diagnosis most frequently occurred ASD symptoms are present by the time the child reaches one
when the child reached school age (5–7 years of age; Howlin, 1997; year of age in many cases (a subset of children develop fairly nor-
Mandell et al., 2002, 2005; Yeargin-Allsopp et al., 2003). However, mally until about two years of age and then “regress” into full
efforts are being made to push the age down to younger children. blown autism; Chakrabarti, 2009; Kishore and Basu, 2011). Sim-
There is no consensus on when the best time would be to start treat- ilarly, intellectual disabilities and some commonly co-occurring
ment. Most researchers simply say the earlier the better (Ortega medical problems such as seizures and cerebral palsy are evident
et al., 2013; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2013). One line of assessment very early on in the child’s life. A number of mental health prob-
research aimed at early diagnosis is referred to as “age of first con- lems are also present but emerge later. Common difficulties the
cern” (Twyman et al., 2009). Among the problems first reported child may display include ADHD, anxiety disorder, obsessive com-
by parents are language delays, poor social skills, and unusual, pulsive disorder, depression, and challenging behaviors. Some of
unwanted behaviors (Gaspar de Alba and Bodfish, 2011). Addition- these problems occur very frequently in conjunction with ASD.
ally, first concerns are evident in ASD before they are evident in ADHD, for example, has been reported in half the ASD cases (Leyfer
many other developmental disabilities. While results have been et al., 2006). Some symptoms of ADHD can be detected as early as
mixed, researchers have also reported that parents concern for ASD two to three years of age, but ADHD diagnosis tends to be given
may occur earlier in females than males (Horovitz et al., 2012b; once the child reaches school age. Demand to sit quietly and attend
Volkmar et al., 1993). to designated tasks increases dramatically, and as a result, makes
Parents, particularly mothers, report that they believe some- symptoms of ADHD much easier to detect.
thing is wrong very early, prior to two year of age (Chakrabarti, Anxiety among persons with ASD is also most frequently seen
2009; De Giacomo and Fombonne, 1998). However, early on par- at school age and in adolescents (Vasa et al., 2013). Up to 40% of the
ents often have difficulty pinpointing exactly what is wrong. One 1316 people they assessed, evinced anxiety at a clinically significant
method researcher have used to help pinpoint specific symptoms level, while 26% evinced subclinical levels of anxiety. Also, individ-
are home movies (Saint-Georges et al., 2010). Researchers viewed uals with ASD and an anxiety disorder were more likely to have
movies of children 24 months of age or younger who were later other comorbid conditions including ADHD, oppositional defiant
diagnosed with ASD. Most of these studies looked at older infants disorder, and somantic symptoms.
and toddlers. A number of researchers have reported deficits in all For the clinician this means initial diagnosis of core symptoms at
core symptoms of ASD. Among these problems were flat affect, lack an early age; two to three years old. Stability of clinical diagnoses
of interest in others, abnormal gaze, deficits in a host of communica- made at age two or three has been found to be high (Chawarska
tion skills and motor delays. The autistic children also evinced more et al., 2007; Eaves and Ho, 2004; Gillberg et al., 1990). Eaves and
stereotyped vocalizations, poor attention, inappropriate object use, Ho (2004) reexamined four and a half year old children who had
and odd play habits (Baranek et al., 2005; Colgan et al., 2006; been diagnosed with ASD at two years of age. They found that 79%
Eriksson and DeChãteau, 1992; Maestro et al., 1999). Thus, diag- of the children retained their diagnosis of ASD. At the time of ini-
nosis at an early age is possible, since a range of symptoms can be tial diagnosis, the assessment of several comorbid disorders (see
identified early on in the child’s life. above) would also begin. Staged assessments at various periods of
the child’s development would then be needed as other problems
2. Heterogeneity emerge over time.
What do these data mean for basic research? First, it is impor-
The range of core symptom severity varies considerably from tant to be familiar with protocols that result in reliable diagnoses
one individual to another diagnosed with ASD. The more severe the and comorbid disorders. Being licensed or certified as a clinical
core symptoms, the earlier parents are likely to detect and report psychologist, psychiatrist, pediatrician, or child neurologist does
concerns (Howlin and Asgharian, 1999; Sivberg, 2003). Often par- not guarantee an accurate diagnosis. Second, these data point out
ents do not realize that these symptoms characterize ASD. Rather, many possible interlocking disorders that may be explained with
they are simply aware that something is amiss. Nonetheless, early advances in genomic science. Third, more needs to be done to link
detection of possible problems and referral to professionals, can basic and applied research on ASD, but that likely will be part of the
increase the chance of early assessment and diagnosis. A paradox natural evolution of the field. Finally, basic and applied researchers
however, is that while the most severely afflicted children are most should become more familiar with the work of researchers across
readily identified, they also require the most intervention. Addi- the breath of disciplines working in the ASD area. This approach will
tionally, these more severely impaired children have the worst strengthen cross disciplinary research and decrease the likelihood
prognosis (Baird et al., 2006; Klin et al., 2007; Rojahn et al., 2009). of unnecessary replication of findings.
The factor most closely associated with poor prognosis is not
symptoms of ASD. Rather, intellectual disability, and in turn the
severity of the cognitive deficits, is the most critical factor. Upwards 3. Diagnostic problems in clinical practice
of 70% of persons with ASD also have intellectual disability. Short
and Schopler (1988) noted that parents were more likely to report As noted, many professionals who are licensed and/or certified
concerns at an early age when low IQ was present. Others have in their discipline and who conducted a diagnostic workup of ASD
found the opposite; IQ was not a factor in early detection (Rogers may not perform the task correctly and may produce an inaccu-
and DiLalla, 1990; Volkmar et al., 1985). These authors are all likely rate result. This fact may explain why waits of up to two to three
to be right. Mild deficits in IQ may have little effect on early detec- years while seeing an average of four and a half professionals has
tion, while moderate deficits in IQ are likely to result in earlier been reported before an ASD diagnosis is made (Chakrabarti, 2009;
concerns due to delays in developmental milestones at the same Goin-Kochel et al., 2006). Not surprisingly, the more profession-
time detectable core deficits in ASD are present (Kozlowski et al., als consulted, the more dissatisfaction the parents report with the
2011; Lord, 1995). Conversely, where severe IQ deficits are present, process. What may be more surprising is that parents had the for-
speech and other developmental milestones may be so great that titude to continue to seek out professionals until the ASD diagnosis
many core ASD symptoms may not be detected. These factors all was made. Also, given this high number of consults, many parents
lead to the conclusion that an experienced diagnostician is needed may have given up before obtaining a diagnosis. Another issue is
as are standardized test to help detect specific symptoms and their that parents who truly wanted an ASD diagnosis, may have “doctor
severity in a systematic way. shopped” until they received the desired outcome.
1873474x, 2014, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.02.003 by Univeresidad Do Extremo Sul Catarinense, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
46 J.L. Matson, R.L. Goldin / Int. J. Devl Neuroscience 39 (2014) 44–48

Delays in diagnosis have also reportedly caused parental stress Taking these issues into account, the state of Louisiana initiated
in many instances. Typical concerns parents report include anxiety, a state wide program to identify every “at risk” child from 16 to 37
helplessness and uncertainty (Midence and O’Neill, 1999; Schall, months of age. Children are then assessed by one of approximately
2000). Lack of professional collaboration with parents and failure 200 professionals trained to assess for developmental milestones
to build adequate rapport has also been associated with parental and ASD. The senior author of this paper developed a measure
dissatisfaction (Moh and Magiati, 2012). Furthermore, simply hav- specifically to diagnoses ASD and the comorbid problems noted
ing a small child with ASD can bring on considerably more anxiety above. This scale is called the Baby and Infant Screen for Children
and stress than raising a typically developing child (Boyd, 2002; with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT; Matson and Tureck, 2012). The term
Dunn et al., 2001; Ornstein Davis and Carter, 2008). And, the time was selected since a BISCUIT is defined as a “small, sweet” cake,
and financial costs are also issues that often surface during the an appropriate description of little children. The test has excellent
diagnostic process. reliability and validity, it has a parent report and observational pro-
To enhance diagnosis, and make it more systematic, various cedures. The test has been factor analyzed and has age based norms
strategies may be useful. First, the entry point for most children who broken down in small increments in the 16–37 month cohort. At
eventually are diagnosed with ASD will be parent–pediatrician con- this point over 10,000 children have been evaluated with the BIS-
tact. Various media and information campaigns have been launched CUIT, and over 70 papers have been published on the scale.
to aid parents in early signs and symptoms they should look for Other measures commonly used to screen for ASD include
with respect to possible ASD. This factor, combined with recom- the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Generic (ADOS-G; Lord
mendations such as those proposed by the American Academy of et al., 2000), Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS; Schopler et al.,
Pediatrics, should prove to be of considerable value. The American 1988), and the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT;
Academy of Pediatrics provides pediatricians with a clear step-by- Robins et al., 2001). The ADOS-G is a semi-structured observa-
step algorithm to follow depending on the answers provided by tion/interactive measure composed of 4 modules graded according
parents. The American Academy of Pediatrics stress that the pedi- to language and developmental level. This design allows the mea-
atrician should ask parents about any developmental concerns at sure to be administered to a wide range of ages and abilities. The
every well-child visit. If parental concerns are raised, it is recom- ADOS-G is one of the most widely used measures and has good
mended that a standardized screening for ASD be administered. inter-rater reliability, ranging from .65 to .78 (Lord et al., 2000). The
Further, if concerns are raised at 18 month of age, even if nothing measure however takes about two hours to complete and requires
comes of the screening at that point, it is recommended that pedia- the examiner to complete extensive training. The CARS, unlike the
tricians repeat the screening at 24 months to identify any child that ADOS-G, is designed as a rating scale which includes items that
may have regressed after 18 months of age. Results of that screening require observation information. The CARS consists of 15 scales and
then determine whether a comprehensive evaluation is needed or can be used on children 2 years or older and adults. The scale has
not (Johnson et al., 2007). been shown to have high reliability with an inter-rater reliability of
A diagnostic measure does not need to differentiate normal .71, an internal consistency of .94, and a test-retest reliability of .88
development from atypical development. There is ample research (Schopler et al., 1980). Additionally, the scale takes about 30 min to
that parents are very capable of accurately making such distinc- administer and has been translated into several languages. Briefer
tions (Baghdadli et al., 2003; Chawarska, 2007; De Giacomo and than the CARS, the M-CHAT is designed to screen for ASD using 23
Fombonne, 1998). Thus, the purpose of an early ASD diagnosis scale items taking about 5–10 min to complete. Internal consistency was
should be to first differentiate children with ASD from children at found to be adequate at ˛ = 0.85, and it was reported the M-CHAT
risk for other developmental disabilities. Distinguishing ASD from to have slightly higher predictive validity compared its predeces-
disorders such as language disorder, ID, and general developmental sor the CHAT (Robins et al., 2001). These measures along with the
delays can be difficult in young children due to overlapping symp- BISCUIT and many other not mentioned are a crucial component of
toms (Baird et al., 2003; Charman and Baird, 2002; Lord, 1995; the ASD diagnostic process.
Van Daalen et al., 2009). For example, certain features of ASD,
such as stereotypical behaviors, are also common in those with
those with ID. Researchers have shown however that the quality 4. Categorizing ASD
of the stereotyped behaviors can be differentiated (Bodfish et al.,
2000; Carcani-Rathwell et al., 2006; Matson and Dempsey, 2008; Accurate diagnosis is a linchpin for basic and applied research.
McClintock et al., 2003). Children with ASD have been identified as Obviously, accurate classification is essential. Assessment instru-
exhibiting more motor stereotypies, and engaging in more complex ments have improved markedly in the last decade, both for early
hand/finger (e.g., clapping, tapping) stereotypies and stereotypical diagnosis and for life long assessment. Also, the general aware-
gait movements (e.g., spinning, skipping; Goldman et al., 2009), ness for ASD and the symptoms that characterize it are much
than children with ID. Knowledge of distinguishing features such better known by the general public than only a few years ago.
as these, must be noted for accurate diagnosis to occur. Further, However, recent developments may hamper consistent diagno-
clinicians must consult diagnostic criteria and determine that the sis internationally a great deal. DSM-5 was published in 2013
symptoms they observe cannot be better accounted for by another amid great controversy, no more so than for the ASD diagno-
disorder. For instance, a child presenting with deficits in language sis. Asperger’s Syndrome, Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not
acquisition and production, but exhibits no repetitive or restricted Otherwise Specified, Rett Syndrome, and Childhood Disintegrative
behaviors, or deficits in socialization that cannot be explained by Disorder were all dropped. Some researchers have found that the
the language impairments, receive a diagnosis of language disorder new diagnostic criteria have increased specificity (.95 versus .97)
rather than ASD. which may reduce false positive diagnoses (Frazier et al., 2012;
Second, the scale should measure other common comorbid McPartland et al., 2012). However, despite claims by the commit-
disorder among young children, such as some types of psy- tee that these changes would not affect who is diagnosed, other
chopathology including ADHD and anxiety disorders. Challenging researchers worldwide have now demonstrated that as many as
behaviors such as aggression and tantrums are also important to 40% of people previously diagnosed with ASD will no longer meet
assess. This latter approach of evaluating symptoms that commonly criteria, mostly affecting those with PDD-NOS and Asperger’s Syn-
occur with ASD symptoms is important for identifying future treat- drome (Mayes et al., 2014; McPartland et al., 2012). This issue is
ment goals for Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions. compounded by the fact that the European version of DSM-5, the
1873474x, 2014, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.02.003 by Univeresidad Do Extremo Sul Catarinense, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
J.L. Matson, R.L. Goldin / Int. J. Devl Neuroscience 39 (2014) 44–48 47

World Health Organization - ICD-10 plans to stick with the crite- Gaspar de Alba, M.J., Bodfish, J.W., 2011. Addressing parental concerns at the ini-
ria established in DSM-IV. Thus, international studies will be more tial diagnosis of an autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum
Disorders 5, 633–639.
difficult since the definition of ASD will be so different. Further com- Gillberg, C., Ehlers, S., Schaumann, H., Jakobsson, G., Dahlgren, S., Lindblom, R.,
pounding this problem is the fact that insurance companies in the Bagenholm, A., Tjuus, T., Blidner, E., 1990. Autism under age 3 years: a clini-
US use the ICD criteria. Thus, many clinicians and researchers are cal study of 28 cases referred for autistic symptoms in infancy. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry 31, 921–934.
likely to bypass the DSM-5 criteria altogether. It will be interesting Goin-Kochel, R.P., Mackintosh, V.H., Myers, B.J., 2006. How many doctors does it take
to see how this problem is resolved. to make an autism spectrum diagnosis? Autism 10, 439–451.
Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M.W., Greene, P.E., Kim, M., Rapin, I., 2009. Motor
stereotypies in children with autism and other developmental disorders. Devel-
5. Final points opmental Medicine and Child Neurology 51, 30–38.
Gürkan, C.K., Hagerman, R.J., 2012. Targeted treatments in autism and fragile X
syndrome. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 1311–1320.
The issues involved in the diagnosis of ASD are evolving rapidly. Horovitz, M., Matson, J.L., Barker, A., 2012a. The relationship between symptoms
Only recently have very young children been reliably diagnosed. of autism spectrum disorder and psychotropic medication use in infants and
Various tests and screening methods are being developed. This area toddlers. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 1406–1411.
Horovitz, M., Matson, J.L., Turygin, N., Beighley, J.S., 2012b. The relationship between
of research will be very active for the near future. Other major issues gender and age of first concern in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.
such as diagnostic criteria are in flux and could have a domino effect Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 466–471.
on many aspects of basic and applied research. Other issues likely to Howlin, P., 1997. Prognosis in autism: do specialist treatments affect long-term
outcome? European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 6 (2), 55–72.
add to better diagnosis in the future are likely to involve neuroimag- Howlin, P., Asgharian, A., 1999. The diagnosis of autism and Asperger syndrome:
ing methods. They are currently receiving a great deal of research findings from a survey of 770 families. Developmental Medicine and Child Neu-
attention in the ASD area. Similarly, genomic research holds great rology 41, 834–839.
Johnson, C.P., Myers, S.M., The Council on Children with Disabilities, 2007. Identi-
potential for diagnosis. Given, the amount of interest in the topic, fication and evaluation of children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics
developments are likely to increase exponentially. 720, 1183–1215.
Kim, S.M., Han, D.H., Lyoo, H.S., Min, K.J., Kim, K.H., Renshaw, P., 2010. Exposure to
environmental toxins in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder.
References Psychiatry Investigation 7, 122–127.
Kishore, M.T., Basu, A., 2011. Early concerns of mothers of children later diagnosed
Baghdadli, A., Picot, M.C., Pascal, C., Pry, R., Aussilloux, C., 2003. Relationship between with autism: implications for early identification. Research in Autism Spectrum
age of recognition of first disturbances and severity in young children with Disorders 5, 157–163.
autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 12 (3), 122–127. Klin, A., Saulnier, C.A., Sparrow, S.S., Cicchetti, D.V., Volkmar, F.R., Lord, C., 2007. Social
Baird, G., Slonims, V., Cass, H., 2003. Diagnosis of autism. British Medical Journal and communication abilities and disabilities in higher functioning individuals
(International Edition) 327 (7413), 488–493. with autism spectrum disorders: the Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism
Baird, G., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., Charman, T., and Developmental Disorders 37 (4), 748–759.
2006. Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of Kozlowski, A.M., Matson, J.L., Horovitz, M., Worley, J.A., Neal, D., 2011. Parents’
children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP). Lancet first concerns of their child’s development in toddlers with autism spectrum
368, 210–215. disorders. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 14, 72–78.
Baranek, G.T., Barnett, C., Adams, E., Wolcott, N., Watson, L., Crais, E., 2005. Object Lai, D.C., Tseng, Y.-C., Guo, H.-R., 2013. Trends in the prevalence of childhood disabil-
play in infants with autism: methodological issues in retrospective video anal- ity: analysis of data from the national disability registry of Taiwan, 2000–2011.
ysis. The American Journal of Occupational Therapy 59, 20–30. Research in Developmental Disabilities 34, 4203–4209.
Bodfish, J.W., Symons, F.J., Parker, D.E., Lewis, M.H., 2000. Varieties of repetitive Lake, J.K., Balogh, R., Lunsky, Y., 2012. Polypharmacy profiles and predictors among
behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
Developmental Disorders 30, 237–243. 6, 1142–1149.
Boyd, B., 2002. Examining the relationship between stress and lack of social support Lewis, F.M., Murdoch, B.E., Woodyatt, G.C., 2007. Linguistic abilities in children
in mothers of children with autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 1,
Disabilities 17, 208–215. 85–100.
Carcani-Rathwell, I., Rabe-Hasketh, S., Santosh, P.J., 2006. Repetitive and stereotyped Leyfer, O.T., Folstein, S.E., Bacalman, S., Davis, N.O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., Lainhart, J.E.,
behaviours in pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology 2006. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: interview devel-
and Psychiatry 47, 573–581. opment and rates of disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Causin, K.G., Albert, K.M., Carbone, V.J., Sweeney-Kerwin, E.J., 2013. The role of joint 36, 849–861.
control in teaching listener responding to children with autism and other devel- Lord, C., 1995. Follow-up of two-year-olds referred for possible autism. Journal of
opmental disabilities. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 997–1011. Child Psychology and Psychiatry 36, 1365–1382.
Chakrabarti, S., 2009. Early identification of autism. Indian Pediatrics 46, 412–414. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E.H., Levanthal, b., DiLavore, L.P.C., Rutter, M.,
Charman, T., Baird, G., 2002. Practitioner review: diagnosis of autism spectrum dis- 2000. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure
order in 2- and 3-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.
43 (3), 289–305. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 30, 205–223.
Chawarska, K., 2007. Parental recognition of developmental problems in toddlers LoVullo, S.V., Matson, J.L., 2009. Comorbid psychopathology in adults with autism
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders spectrum disorders and intellectual disabilities. Research in Developmental Dis-
37 (1), 62–72. abilities 30, 1288–1296.
Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., Volkmar, F., 2007. Autism spectrum disorder in Mandell, D.S., Listerud, J., Levy, S.E., Pinto-Martin, J.A., 2002. Race differences in the
the second year: stability and change in syndrome expression. Journal of Child age at diagnosis among medicaid-eligible children with autism. Journal of the
Psychology and Psychiatry 48, 128–138. American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 41 (12), 1447–1453.
Chauhan, A., Chauhan, V., 2006. Oxidative stress in autism. Pathophysiology 13, Mandell, D.S., Novak, M.M., Zubritsky, C.D., 2005. Factors associated with age of
171–181. diagnosis among children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 116 (6),
Colgan, S.E., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L.R., Crais, E.R., 2006. Analysis of social 1480–1486.
interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology 12, 307–319. Maestro, S., Casella, C., Milone, A., Muratori, F., Palacio-Espasa, F., 1999. Study of the
De Giacomo, A., Fombonne, E., 1998. Parental recognition of developmental abnor- onset of autism through home movies. Psychopathology 32, 292–300.
malities in autism. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 7 (3), 131–136. Mannion, A., Leader, G., Healy, O., 2013. An investigation of comorbid psycholog-
Dunn, M., Burbine, T., Bowers, C., Tantleff-Dunn, S., 2001. Moderators of stress in ical and epilepsy in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder.
parents of children with autism. Community Mental Health Journal 37, 39–52. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 35–42.
Eaves, L.C., Ho, H.H., 2004. The very early identification of autism: outcome to age Matson, J.L., Dempsey, T., 2008. Stereotypy in adults with autism spectrum disor-
41/2–5. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34, 367–378. ders: relationship and diagnostic fidelity. Journal of Developmental and Physical
Eriksson, A.S., DeChãteau, P., 1992. A girl two years and seven months with autistic Disabilities 20, 155–165.
disorder videotaped from birth. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Matson, J.L., Goldin, R.L., 2013. Comorbidity and autism: trends, topics and future
22, 127–129. directions. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 7, 1228–1233.
Frazier, T.W., Youngstrom, E.A., Speer, L., Embacher, R., Law, P., Constantino, J., Fin- Matson, J.L., Kozlowski, A.M., 2011. The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum
dling, R.L., Hardan, A.Y., Eng, C., 2012. Validation of proposed DSM-5 criteria disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 5, 418–425.
for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Matson, J.L., LoVullo, S.V., 2009. Trends and topic in autism spectrum disorders
Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 28–40. research. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3, 252–257.
Garrecht, M., Austin, D.W., 2011. The plausibility of a role of mercury in the etiology Matson, J.L., Rivet, T.T., 2008. Characteristics of challenging behaviours in adults with
of autism: a cellular prospective. Toxicological and Environmental Chemistry autistic disorder, PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual and
93, 621–634. Developmental Disability 33, 313–329.
1873474x, 2014, 1, Downloaded from https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2014.02.003 by Univeresidad Do Extremo Sul Catarinense, Wiley Online Library on [26/04/2024]. See the Terms and Conditions (https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/terms-and-conditions) on Wiley Online Library for rules of use; OA articles are governed by the applicable Creative Commons License
48 J.L. Matson, R.L. Goldin / Int. J. Devl Neuroscience 39 (2014) 44–48

Matson, J.L., Rivet, T.T., Fodstad, J.C., Dempsey, Boisjoli, T.J.A., 2009. Examination Saint-Georges, C., Cassel, R.S., Cohen, D., Chetowani, M., Laznik, M-C., Maestro, S.,
of adaptive behavior differences in adults with autism spectrum disorders and Muratori, F., 2010. What studies of family home movies can teach us about
intellectual disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities 30, 1317–1325. autistic infants: a literature review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 4,
Matson, J.L., Tureck, K., 2012. Early diagnosis of autism: current status of the Baby 355–366.
and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT – Parts 1, 2, and 3). Schall, C., 2000. Family perspectives on raising a child with autism. Journal of Child
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 1135–1141. and Family Studies 9, 409–423.
Matson, J.L., Wilkins, J., 2009. Psychometric testing methods for children’s social Schopler, E., Reichler, R., Renner, B., 1988. The Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS).
skills. Research in Developmental Disabilities 30, 249–274. Western Psychological Services, Los Angeles.
Mayes, S.D., Calhoun, S.L., Murray, M.J., Pearl, A., Black, A., Tierney, C.D., 2014. Final Schopler, E., Reichler, R., DeVellis, R., Daly, K., 1980. Toward objective classification
DSM-5 under-identifies mild Autism Spectrum Disorder: agreement between of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism
the DSM-5, CARS, CASD, and clinical diagnosis. Research in Autism Disorders 8, and Developmental Disorders 10, 91–103.
68–73. Short, A., Schopler, E., 1988. Factors relating to age of onset in autism. Journal of
McClintock, K., Hall, S., Oliver, C., 2003. Risk markers associated with challenging Autism and Developmental Disorders 18, 207–216.
behaviours in people with intellectual disabilities: a meta-analytic study. Journal Sivberg, B., 2003. Parents’ detection of early signs of their children having an autism
of Intellectual Disability Research 47, 405–416. spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18, 207–216.
McPartland, J.C., Reichow, B., Volkmar, F.R., 2012. Sensitivity and specificity of pro- Smith, K.R.M., Matson, J.L., 2010a. Social skills: differences among intellectually dis-
posed DSM-5 diagnostic criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the abled adults with co-morbid autism spectrum disorders and epilepsy. Research
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 51, 368–383. in Developmental Disabilities 31, 1366–1372.
Midence, K., O’Neill, M., 1999. The experience of parents in the diagnosis of autism: Smith, K.R.M., Matson, J.L., 2010b. Behavior problems: among intellectually disabled
a pilot study. Autism 3, 273–285. adults with autism spectrum disorders and epilepsy. Research in Developmental
Moh, T.A., Magiati, I., 2012. Factors associated with parental stress and satisfaction Disabilities 31, 1062–1069.
during the process of diagnosis of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Smith, K.R.M., Matson, J.L., 2010c. Psychopathology: differences among adults with
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 6, 293–303. intellectual disability, and comorbid autism spectrum disorders, and epilepsy.
Moore, T.R., 2009. A brief report on the effects of a self-management treatment Research in Developmental Disabilities 31, 743–749.
package on stereotypic behavior. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3, Surén, P., Bakken, I.J., Aase, H., Chin, R., Gunnes, N., Lie, K.K., Magnus, P., Stoltenberg,
695–701. C., 2012. Autism spectrum disorder, ADHD, epilepsy, and cerebral palsy in Nor-
Ornstein Davis, N., Carter, A.S., 2008. Parenting stress in mothers and fathers of tod- wegian children. Pediatrics 130, e152–e158.
dlers with autism spectrum disorders: associations with child characteristics. Twyman, K.A., Maxim, R.A., Leet, T.L., Ultmann, M.H., 2009. Parents’ developmen-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 38, 1278–1291. tal concerns and age variance at diagnosis of children with autism spectrum
Ortega, J.V., García, V.R., Yu, C.T., 2013. Prediction of treatment outcomes and lon- disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3, 489–495.
gitudinal analysis in children with autism undergoing intensive behavioral Van Daalen, E., Kemner, C., Dietz, C., Swinkels, S.N., Buitelaar, J.K., Van Engeland,
intervention. International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology 13 (2), H., 2009. Inter-rater reliability and stability of diagnoses of autism spectrum
91–100. disorder in children identified through screening at a very young age. European
Poultney, C.S., Goldberg, A.P., Drapeau, E., Kou, Y., Harony-Nicolas, H., Kajiwara, Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 18 (11), 663–674.
Y., Buxbaum, J.D., 2013. Identification of small exonic CNV from whole-exome Vasa, R.A., Kalb, L., Mazurek, M., Kannes, S., Freedman, B., Keefer, A., Clemons, T.,
sequence data and application to autism spectrum disorder. The American Jour- Murray, D., 2013. Age-related differences in the prevalence and correlates of
nal of Human Genetics 93, 607–619. anxiety in youth with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum
Robins, D.L., Fein, D., Barton, M.L., Green, J.A., 2001. The Modified Checklist for Disorders 7, 1358–1369.
Autism in Toddlers: an initial study investigating the early detection of autism Volkmar, F.R., Stier, D.M., Cohen, D.J., 1985. Age of recognition of pervasive devel-
and pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental opmental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry 142, 1450–1452.
Disorders 31, 131–144. Volkmar, F.R., Szatmari, P., Sparrow, S.S., 1993. Sex differences in pervasive develop-
Rogers, S.J., DiLalla, D.L., 1990. Age of symptom onset in young children with per- mental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 23, 579–591.
vasive developmental disorders. Journal of American Academy of Child and Yeargin-Allsopp, M., Rice, C., Karapurkar, T., Doernberg, N., Boyle, C., Murphy, C.,
Adolescent Psychiatry 29, 863–872. 2003. Prevalence of autism in a US metropolitan area. Journal of the American
Rojahn, J., Matson, J.L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J.C., Knight, C., Sevin, J.A., Sharp, B., 2009. Medical Association 289 (1), 49–55.
Cutoffs, norms, and patterns of problem behaviors in children with an ASD on Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Garon, N., 2013. Early identification of autism spectrum
the Baby and Infant Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT-Part 3). disorders. Behavioural Brain Research 251, 133–146.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders 3 (4), 989–998.

You might also like