You are on page 1of 7

Opinion

Reworking the language network


Evelina Fedorenko1 and Sharon L. Thompson-Schill2
1
Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA
2
University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA

Prior investigations of functional specialization have (e.g., does Broca’s area selectively support speech produc-
focused on the response profiles of particular brain tion?). Techniques like fMRI that were lauded for their
regions. Given the growing emphasis on regional covar- ability to track regionally specific changes in metabolic
iation, we propose to reframe these questions in terms activity seemed perfectly suited to tackle such questions.
of brain ‘networks’ (collections of regions jointly Today, however, fMRI data are routinely used to describe
engaged by some mental process). Despite the chal- the statistical interdependencies among brain regions. In
lenges that investigations of the language network face, particular, hundreds of studies have now reported regional
a network approach may prove useful in understanding covariation that manifests in a wide range of dependent
the cognitive architecture of language. We propose that measures, including signal amplitude – either static, aver-
a language network plausibly includes a functionally aged across long time windows of task performance or rest
specialized ‘core’ (brain regions that coactivate with (e.g., functional connectivity [4]) or dynamic, varying on a
each other during language processing) and a domain- shorter timescale [5] – or, more recently, pattern separ-
general ‘periphery’ (a set of brain regions that may ability (informational connectivity [6]). Because regions
coactivate with the language core regions at some times that share functional properties can be distant, spanning
but with other specialized systems at other times, lobes and hemispheres, their collections are referred to as
depending on task demands). Framing the debate large-scale distributed neural networks, where the regions
around network properties such as this may prove to are the nodes and the inter-regional (implied) connections
be a more fruitful way to advance our understanding of are the edges (Box 1). Furthermore, given that complex
the neurobiology of language. cognitive processes – be it face recognition or sentence
comprehension – recruit a host of different brain regions
In search of the language organ [7], it may be time (some might argue, long past time) to
Many deeply important questions in cognition hinge on start thinking about functional specialization at the level
whether two mental processes rely on the same pool of of brain networks (e.g., is the collection of regions recruited
cognitive and neural resources. Is processing faces distinct by sentence comprehension specialized for solving this
from processing other classes of visual objects? Do we use the particular problem?).
same mechanisms to extract meaning from words versus Tests of functional specificity of a brain region routi-
pictures? Does resolving linguistic ambiguity draw on the nely begin by defining, on anatomical and/or functional
same resources as other demanding tasks? This is one class grounds, a region of interest (ROI) and then measuring
of question where functional MRI (fMRI) can inform and the relative response of that region across varying cog-
constrain cognitive theories (cf. [1]; see [2] for discussion). nitive demands (e.g., task conditions). In principle, the
One such question that has motivated research for decades logic of assessing the specificity of a brain network is
concerns the existence of a specialized ‘language organ’ (and similar: how does a network of interest (NOI) respond
its possible computations [3]). In particular, are some com- across varying cognitive demands? However, in practice,
putations unique to human language or can language be the functional specialization of a network can be eval-
‘solved’ by more general-purpose mental operations? uated in numerous ways. Here we briefly discuss how the
Although fMRI cannot directly answer questions of this notion of functional specialization can be scaled up from
sort, knowing under which conditions a region or a collection brain regions to brain networks and then consider the
of regions is engaged will constrain hypotheses about the question of functional specialization of the language
likely computations those regions perform. network. To foreshadow the take-home message, we
Traditionally, questions about functional specialization argue that the language network includes both rela-
have been asked at the level of individual brain regions tively specialized and highly domain-general compo-
nents and that investigating the dynamic interactions
Corresponding authors: Fedorenko, E. (evelina9@mit.edu); Thompson-Schill, S.L. between the two can inform the computations conducted
(sschill@psych.upenn.edu).
Keywords: domain specificity; domain generality; language network; cognitive
by each.
control; fMRI.

1364-6613/$ – see front matter Scaling up the notion of functional specialization: from
ß 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.006
nodes to networks
What does it mean for a collection of brain regions to be
functionally specialized for some mental process x? There
are at least three ways to approach this question.
120 Trends in Cognitive Sciences, March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

Box 1. Challenges for network neuroscience One strategy is to focus on the functional profiles of the
Requisite care in using the term ‘network’
individual nodes. For example, a network may be function-
Although the terms ‘network’ and ‘connectivity’ are widely used ally specialized for mental process x if all of its nodes are
when talking about regional covariation in the human brain, it is functionally specialized for x (e.g., Figure 1A). Alterna-
important to keep in mind that no human data at present allow us to tively, perhaps the presence of at least one functionally
make inferences about brain regions forming networks in the true specialized node is sufficient to qualify the whole network
sense of the word. In particular, under a technical definition, two
brain regions form a network if they are anatomically connected,
as functionally specialized. (Note that the presence of at
typically via monosynaptic projections. In living humans, we rarely, least one domain-general node cannot be sufficient to
if ever, can say anything conclusive about anatomical connections qualify the network as domain general if we are to preserve
among brain regions. In particular, functional correlation data (task any notion of functional specialization, because domain-
based or resting state) cannot be used to infer anatomical general processes like attention or cognitive control are
connectivity because the relationship between the two is complex
[56,57] and diffusion tractography remains severely limited [58–60].
likely to play a role in all mental processes.)
Consequently, although we follow the literature in adopting the Another strategy is to focus on the edges (i.e., the
term ‘network’ to refer to collections of regions that share functional patterns of ‘connections’ among brain regions; cf. Box 1).
properties, these collections of regions are more appropriately In this approach, the properties of the nodes are less
characterized as functional systems.
important; they may be functionally specialized, domain
Uncertainty about the number and nature of brain networks
Network neuroscience is still in its youth and there is at present no general, or a mixture of the two. What matters is whether a
agreement on many important questions. For example, what is the unique combination of nodes and edges is recruited for the
right way to parcellate the brain into regions (nodes)? How many relevant mental process x. If so, such a network would be
networks does the human brain encompass and how should we considered functionally specialized for x even if all of the
functionally interpret these networks (see [61,62] for some recent
individual nodes are domain general (Figure 1B), and even
proposals)? What is the structure of each network and does this
structure differ across networks (see [63] for a review)? The answer the same exact combination of nodes can contribute dif-
to these questions is likely to arise from a combination of data- ferently to different mental processes when the nodes are
driven approaches that attempt to carve up the entire brain based on characterized by different patterns of connections.
patterns of inter-regional covariation and approaches that more Yet another possibility is to take time into account and
narrowly focus on a particular subset of the brain (e.g., the language
network), because a deep understanding of the structure and
focus on the so-called ‘network dynamics’; that is, the
function of one network – including the contributions of each node changes – or lack thereof – in the patterns of coupling
and edge and their dynamics – may shed light on the broader (coactivation) between each node and the other nodes of the
functional architecture of the human brain. network as well as the rest of the brain. In particular,

(A) (B)

t=1 t=2

(C) (D)

t=1 t=2

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences

Figure 1. Hypothetical network configurations. A schematic illustration of several hypothetical network configurations [for (B) and (D), we show network configurations at
two different time points]. Single-color nodes represent functionally specific brain regions (i.e., regions that are selectively engaged by a particular mental process);
multiple-color nodes represent domain-general brain regions (i.e., regions with functionally diverse responses). (A) Two networks comprising functionally specific brain
regions and one network comprising domain-general brain regions, with no interaction between the networks. (B) No functionally specific brain regions; all brain regions
are domain general but combine in different ways to solve different tasks. (C). Two networks comprising functionally specific brain regions and one network comprising
domain-general brain regions, with one node of the latter serving as a ‘hub’ via which the specialized networks can interact with the domain-general network. (D) Two
networks comprising functionally specific brain regions and one network comprising domain-general brain regions. The latter can combine with either of the two
specialized networks at different times, thus becoming the ‘periphery’ of the pink network at t = 1 and of the green network at t = 2.

121
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

Box 2. Is ‘language’ a natural kind? language network, which presupposes that language is a
One might object that questions about the language network are ill
natural kind (Box 2).
posed, because language is not a single thing. Indeed, when talking We have two observations about this potential quag-
about whether ‘language’ relies on domain-specific versus domain- mire. First, the daunting task of specifying exactly what
general machinery (or some combination of the two), researchers language is (and what language is not) has not completely
are often referring to different mental processes that language halted progress: the term ‘the language network’ is being
encompasses and there is no agreement on the right ontology of
these processes. Such ontologies in human cognitive neuroscience
used increasingly frequently (e.g., in the PubMed database
are typically inspired by theoretical and experimental behavioral an average of five articles per year used this term between
work in psychology and cognitive science, although they often lag 2001 and 2005, an average of 17 articles per year between
behind. At present, based on differences in functional profiles and 2006 and 2010, and an average of 35 articles per year since
some neuropsychological patient evidence, we can at least distin- 2011). And second, those who have attempted to charac-
guish between: (i) the sensory language regions (in the auditory and
visual cortices); (ii) the speech articulation regions; and (iii) the
terize the language network have not arrived at the same
‘higher-level’ language-processing regions (Figure 2). For example, answer to the question of how to define it. Below we
in contrast to the high-level language regions, the sensory regions consider three common approaches (see Figure 2 for sche-
appear to respond to stimuli that are devoid of meaning; the visual matic illustrations of each).
word-form area responds as much to consonant strings as to real
(1) Response [e.g., a change in the magnitude of the blood
words [64,65]. Similarly, the speech articulation regions [66] can be
driven by low-level production tasks like repeating non-words. Even oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) signal] to language
within the higher-level language interpretation, we may want to above a low-level baseline
distinguish between phonological (sound-level) processing, lexical At first glance, this criterion seems the least con-
(word-level) processing, and combinatorial (syntactic and composi- troversial: regions that support linguistic processing
tional semantic) processing, although more recent, usage-based
should respond to language stimuli (e.g., words or
approaches argue against sharp boundaries between them [67–69]
(see [24] for some fMRI support). In summary, although language sentences). However, although this criterion does
processing plausibly relies on several distinct kinds of computation include the ‘classic’ regions on the lateral surfaces of
implemented in distinct brain regions, there appears to be a subset the left frontal and temporal cortices, it also includes
of our brain that is consistently engaged when we produce and/or the regions of the bilateral domain-general cognitive
understand language that is at least partially distinct from brain
regions that support other mental processes [28,30]. Talking about
control network (also known as the ‘task-positive
language as a natural kind is thus not unreasonable, although of network’, the ‘frontoparietal attention network’, or the
course if/when dissociations among different language components ‘multiple-demand network’). The latter network spans
are discovered – be it by studying the functional response profiles of extended portions of the frontal and parietal cortices
the relevant regions or their dynamic network properties – those and its regions are engaged in a wide range of goal-
dissociations should be taken into account when thinking about the
computations that those components require.
directed behaviors [12,13]. Furthermore, for a lan-
guage comprehension task, we would expect to see
activity in the primary and higher-level sensory
networks are not static: they change under different task regions: in the auditory cortices for listening [14]
conditions as well as during different stages of a single and in the visual cortices for reading [15]. And for a
task. For example, Cole and colleagues [8] demonstrated language production task, we would expect to see
that a frontoparietal brain network shifted its correlation activity in the regions that support articulation [16]. In
patterns more than did other functional networks as task addition, we may observe responses in brain regions
demands changed. In other words, this frontoparietal net- implicated in, for example, social cognition, emotional
work was promiscuous, partnering with a visual network processing, and high-level visual processing. In the
at times and an auditory network at other times (e.g., extreme, then, the whole brain is probably engaged –
Figure 1D; see also [9,10]). Bassett and colleagues [11] in some way – during language processing, but the
measured how often nodes changed teams across a learn- notion of a language network is useful only if we agree
ing task and found that the flexibility of a node’s team that some subset of the brain is more strongly,
allegiance across time (across individuals) predicted beha- consistently, and/or causally engaged in language
vior. So the notion of functional specialization may be processing than the rest of the brain.
linked to the stability of the node/network; a network (2) Response to a conjunction of either listening and
may be functionally specialized for some mental process reading or comprehension and production above a low-
if its nodes are stable community members and it may be level baseline
domain general if its nodes frequently change allegiances. These criteria will lead to the elimination of the
sensory regions [17,18] and/or the articulation regions
The language network of interest [19]. However, at least some parts of the cognitive
We turn now to our primary topic; namely, the language control network are still likely to be included.
network. To ask questions about the nodes, edges, or (3) A greater response to intact language stimuli than to
dynamics of the language network, as described above, various degraded versions of those stimuli, matched
we need to define the language network. Immediately, we for low-level features
have a problem: what is language? That is, whatever task This criterion is frequently used and translates into
(or task comparison) one might choose to define the lan- contrasts like speech versus backward speech [20],
guage network will require assumptions about what puta- sentences versus false fonts [21], sentences versus lists
tive operations comprise language. One could rightly of unconnected words [17,22], including parametric
question whether it even makes sense to ask about a versions of this contrast [23], and sentences versus
122
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

(A) (B)

Language comprehension > low-level baseline:


(1) + / + + ...

Language producon > low-level baseline:


+ + + ...

Language comprehension (intersecon of


visual and auditory) > low-level baseline:
(2) +

Key: Intersecon of language comprehension and


language producon > low-level baseline:
“High-level” language regions
Speech percepon regions +
vWFA Language comprehension: intact>
(3)
Arculaon regions linguiscally degraded smuli:
Cognive control regions + (maybe: / )

TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences

Figure 2. The language network under different definitions. (A) A schematic depiction of five sets of brain regions that are sometimes included in the language network: red,
the classic high-level language-processing regions; yellow, speech perception regions; green, visual word-form area; purple, speech articulation regions; and blue,
cognitive control regions. (B) A schematic illustration of possible definitions of the language network, ranging from very liberal (1) to more conservative (2 and 3).

lists of non-words [17]. Depending on how low level the included – has almost certainly contributed to past dis-
baseline condition is, these contrasts may or may not agreements, some of which may have been superficial
include parts of the higher-level sensory regions (e.g., (definitional) in nature. However, this problem is not
the visual word-form area for visual stimuli). Impor- unique to the domain of language or to the network-level
tantly, however, these contrasts will generally not approach, similarly characterizing investigations that
include the cognitive control regions because those have focused on single brain regions. Furthermore,
regions tend to respond more strongly to linguistically although any conclusions one draws about a brain network
degraded conditions [13,24], presumably because of are bound to be affected by how that network is defined, we
the greater cognitive effort required to process cannot abandon the enterprise simply because we cannot
degraded stimuli (cf. similar effects in visual proces- agree on the inclusion criteria. Instead, as we argue below,
sing where non-degraded images produce stronger network approaches may be able to help us sharpen the
responses in the visual cortices, but degraded images definition of the language network, which will, in turn,
more strongly activate the cognitive control network have implications for understanding the specificity of the
[25]). cognitive and computational mechanisms required during
language production and comprehension.
Of course, a particular functional signature (as in 1–3
above; Figure 2) is not the only way to define the language Is the language network functionally specialized?
network. For example, one might want to emphasize the The extent to which language – including its many com-
causal role in linguistic processing, such that a region is ponents (like speech perception, letter/word recognition,
included only if a transient or more permanent disruption articulation, and syntactic processing; Box 2) – relies on
of its activity leads to linguistic deficits. Another possible functionally specialized versus domain-general cognitive
criterion has been adopted by Hasson and colleagues, who and neural machinery has been long debated. One impor-
have focused on the across-subjects similarity in the time- tant take-away message from the preceding section is that,
courses of neural activity. For example, in studies of nar- under many definitions, the language network includes
rative comprehension, they reported inter-subject correla- both relatively functionally specialized brain regions
tions across large extents of cortex, including what appear [24,28–30] and brain regions better thought of as part of
to be cognitive control regions in the dorsolateral prefron- a domain-general cognitive control network [31–35] (for a
tal cortex and around the inferior parietal sulcus [26]. They review, see [36]). However, our opening question, and a
further showed that even some regions that do not show an central concern for the field, is whether language – not any
above-baseline response to language (cf. criterion 1), like particular brain region – requires specific computations.
the precuneus, show correlated activity across subjects That is, is the language network – which comprises both
during sentence comprehension [27]. specialized and domain-general nodes – functionally spe-
So where does this leave us? It should be clear that the cialized for language processing (or some aspects thereof)?
‘right’ criterion (or set of criteria) for the language network Let us revisit the possible definitions of a functionally
is subject to debate. Also, the multitude of possible defini- specialized brain network sketched above.
tions of the language network – especially differences in If we focus on the properties of the individual nodes, one
whether the domain-general cognitive control regions are could argue that either: (i) the language network is not
123
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

functionally specialized for language because not all of its regions are those that maintain their allegiance through
nodes are functionally specialized for language; or (ii) the time whereas peripheral regions do not). These two dimen-
language network is functionally specialized, if the pre- sions are plausibly not independent. In particular, the
sence of some specialized nodes is sufficient. ‘promiscuity’ of a brain region (i.e., its tendency to couple –
If we focus on the edges, the language network would that is, to coactivate – with other brain regions) may be
qualify as functionally specialized because the specialized inversely related to the degree of its functional specializa-
regions would be engaged only during language-processing tion. If a brain region supports computations that are well
tasks and thus, by definition, the combination of brain suited to solving a particular task (including, for example,
regions (and presumably the connections among them) relying on domain-specific knowledge structures), it is
engaged during language processing would be unique, likely to partner only with regions that implement similar,
because no other mental process would recruit the specia- specific computations. By contrast, if a brain region sup-
lized regions of the language network. ports highly generic computations (e.g., exclusive OR
Finally, let us consider the dynamic aspects of the [52]), it can partner with a wide array of functionally
language network. It is important to note that, although diverse regions because such generic computations are
the functional profiles of the different brain regions com- plausibly useful across domains. Thus, a language net-
prising this network vary considerably – with some being work plausibly includes a functionally specialized core and
relatively functionally specialized for language processing a domain-general periphery. The existence of core regions
and others highly domain general – these sets of brain supporting a particular mental process is perfectly com-
regions must nevertheless have a way to communicate patible with the importance of domain-general circuits for
with one another (cf. Figure 1A). Such interactions are that mental process (cf. [53]; see [54,55] for discussions of
essential given that domain-general brain regions – like the relationship between functional specificity and encap-
those that support, for example, cognitive control and sulation).
working memory – are likely to participate in all mental As we foreshadowed above, this dynamic network
processes, including language comprehension and produc- approach may enable one to discover the language net-
tion. The role of these domain-general mechanisms in work: (i) without needing to define it a priori; or (ii)
language processing is hard to dispute given the abundant starting with a very liberal, all-inclusive definition. Any
behavioral evidence from dual-task paradigms [37–39] and given language task (e.g., sentence comprehension) is
individual-differences investigations [40–45], as well as likely to require both domain-general processes and (if
neuroimaging evidence from many linguistic-complexity they exist) language-specific processes; however, assum-
manipulations [46–50]. Thus, any putatively language- ing those processes are supported by different nodes in a
specific components of the language network cannot be network, the dynamic relations among these nodes should
encapsulated in the Fodorian sense (cf. Figure 1A). vary across the task in a way that is detectable with
How exactly the functionally specialized and domain- network approaches and that potentially informs the
general components of the language network interact function of the nodes. This, in turn, can motivate future
remains to be discovered. For example, such interactions studies aimed at understanding the response properties of
could occur via stable hubs [8] that are always ‘partnering’ different sets of nodes (whether they be core or periphery
with one or more specialized networks (Figure 1C). Alter- in one task) under different cognitive conditions. Thus, we
natively, they could occur via dynamic changes in the proceed.
patterns of connections between the specialized and
domain-general sets of regions (Figure 1D). For example, Concluding remarks
during language processing the language network config- For many years, we – both as the individual scientists
uration may look like Figure 1D at t = 1, with the specia- coauthoring this opinion article and as two representative
lized (pink) and domain-general (multicolored) regions researchers of the neurobiology of language – have been
working together. At other times, the same domain-gen- trying to understand the cognitive and neural architecture
eral regions may partner with a different network (e.g., of language using regionally specific fMRI responses. Much
Figure 1D at t = 2). of this work has been framed as a debate about the func-
This notion of node ‘stability’ has motivated a distinc- tional specificity of regions recruited during language pro-
tion between a network core (i.e., a set of brain regions that cessing, and we have contributed to each side of this
– consistently across time – interact with one another) and debate. One of us has argued that there is ‘a high degree
a network periphery (i.e., a set of brain regions that inter- of functional specificity in the brain regions that support
act with a different specialized set of brain regions at language’ [28]. The other has advanced hypotheses about
different times, depending on current goals [51]). We pro- putative language regions that are, instead, ‘grounded in
pose that this distinction between the core and periphery of domain-general terms’ [41]. Here we have joined forces in
a network could be a useful tool for exploring the functional an attempt to redirect the empirical enterprise, by calling a
specialization of any network, including the language net- ceasefire on arguments about whether an individual brain
work. In our review of this question above and in our region is domain specific or domain general. Instead, we
previous work [28,36], we focused on cross-task structure propose that our understanding of the computations that
(i.e., functionally specific regions are those that are active enable language and the neural systems that support them
in specific tasks but not across tasks whereas domain- will advance more rapidly if we follow the example of many
general regions are active across tasks). In dynamic net- other subfields of neuroscience and turn our efforts
work terms, the focus is on cross-time structure (core towards characterizing properties of the language network
124
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

Box 3. Outstanding questions Acknowledgments


The authors thank Danielle Bassett, Ted Gibson, Nancy Kanwisher, and
 Are any of the regions engaged in language processing truly
Sarah Solomon for comments on drafts of this manuscript. They were
selective for language?
supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD R00 award HD-057522
Fedorenko and colleagues [24,28] have shown that several
to E.F. and NIH R01 DC009209 to S.L.T-S.
language regions show functionally specific responses. However,
other non-linguistic processes remain to be tested and either
outcome – functional specificity versus overlap with some non- References
linguistic processes in some or all of the language regions – will 1 Coltheart, M. (2013) How can functional neuroimaging inform
further constrain the space of possibilities for what these regions cognitive theories? Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 98–103
could be doing and may reveal important differences among 2 Mather, M. et al. (2013) How fMRI can inform cognitive theories.
regions. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 8, 108–113
 What role do domain-general cognitive control brain regions play 3 Hauser, M.D. et al. (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it,
in language comprehension and production? and how did it evolve? Science 298, 1569–1579
As discussed in the main text, the engagement of domain-general 4 Friston, K.J. (2011) Functional and effective connectivity: a review.
cognitive control/working memory mechanisms in language is not Brain Connect. 1, 13–36
under debate, but many important questions remain about the 5 Hutchison, R.M. et al. (2013) Dynamic functional connectivity:
nature and significance of this engagement, in particular the promises, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage 80, 360–378
following. 6 Coutanche, M.N. and Thompson-Schill, S.L. (2013) Informational
(i) Given the extent (and the bilateral nature) of the cognitive connectivity: identifying synchronized discriminability of multi-voxel
control network, are some of its components more important patterns across the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7, 15
for language processing than others? If so, which ones? 7 Mesulam, M. (1990) Large-scale neurocognitive networks and
(ii) What is the significance, if any, of the proximity of the language distributed processing for attention, language, and memory. Ann.
and cognitive control regions in the left frontal lobe [24]? For Neurol. 28, 597–613
example, perhaps some properties of the cells in the left frontal 8 Cole, M.W. et al. (2013) Multi-task connectivity reveals flexible hubs for
cortex or its position within the large-scale network structure adaptive task control. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1348–1355
make it well suited for performing both some aspects of 9 Chadick, J.Z. and Gazzaley, A. (2011) Differential coupling of visual
language processing and the kinds of generic computation that cortex with default or frontal–parietal network based on goals. Nat.
are likely to take place in the cognitive control regions [36]. Neurosci. 14, 830–832
(iii) What are the precise circumstances under which cognitive 10 Al-Aidroos, N. et al. (2012) Top-down attention switches coupling
control regions are engaged during language processing? Can between low-level and high-level areas of human visual cortex. Proc.
we predict their engagement from current models of linguistic Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 14675–14680
complexity (e.g., memory-based models [70–73], experience-/ 11 Bassett, D.S. et al. (2011) Dynamic reconfiguration of human brain
surprisal-based models [74,75])? networks during learning. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 7641–7646
(iv) Do cognitive control regions provide alternative routes for 12 Duncan, J. (2010) The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate
‘solving’ language or do they merely provide extra computa- brain: mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14,
tional power (a workspace of sorts [76]) when the core language 172–179
regions run into difficulty? 13 Fedorenko, E. et al. (2013) Broad domain generality in focal regions of
(v) What is the time – course of the interaction between the language frontal and parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 16616–
regions and the cognitive control regions? For example, when an 16621
ambiguous word or structure is encountered, is it the language or 14 DeWitt, I. and Rauschecker, J.P. (2013) Wernicke’s area revisited:
cognitive control regions that respond first? How is the interac- parallel streams and word processing. Brain Lang. 127, 181–191
tion between these regions manifested? That is, do they show 15 Yeatman, J.D. et al. (2012) Development of white matter and reading
increases in synchronization of neural activity (which would skills. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, E3045–E3053
suggest parallel – although possibly distinct – computations) or 16 Grabski, K. et al. (2012) Functional MRI assessment of orofacial
anti-correlations (which would suggest some kind of trade-off)? articulators: neural correlates of lip, jaw, larynx, and tongue
 How might network dynamics vary across individuals and what movements. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2306–2321
might that variation predict about individual differences in 17 Fedorenko, E. et al. (2010) New method for fMRI investigations of
behavior? language: defining ROIs functionally in individual subjects. J.
Neurophysiol. 104, 1177–1194
18 Braze, D. et al. (2011) Unification of sentence processing via ear and
eye: an fMRI study. Cortex 47, 416–431
19 Menenti, L. et al. (2011) Shared language: overlap and segregation of
the neuronal infrastructure for speaking and listening revealed by
functional MRI. Psychol. Sci. 22, 1173–1182
(or networks). We have outlined here a strategy for char- 20 Bedny, M. et al. (2011) Language processing in the occipital cortex of
acterizing some properties of these networks, including congenitally blind adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 4429–
properties that may speak directly to the specificity of 4434
21 Noppeney, U. and Price, C.J. (2004) Retrieval of abstract semantics.
language functions. We have argued that this approach Neuroimage 22, 164–170
does not require agreement on how to define language to 22 Snijders, T.M. et al. (2009) Retrieval and unification of syntactic
begin to characterize properties of these networks. We structure in sentence comprehension: an FMRI study using word-
have borrowed the concepts of a network core and periph- category ambiguity. Cereb. Cortex 19, 1493–1503
23 Pallier, C. et al. (2011) Cortical representation of the constituent
ery from other areas of network neuroscience, to propose a
structure of sentences. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 2522–2527
means for identifying functionally specific and functionally 24 Fedorenko, E. et al. (2012) Language-selective and domain-general
diverse nodes in a language network by tracking the regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Curr. Biol. 22, 2059–2062
structure of their responses across time (instead of across 25 Gorlin, S. et al. (2012) Imaging prior information in the brain. Proc.
tasks). We are confident that this approach will identify Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 7935–7940
26 Lerner, Y. et al. (2011) Topographic mapping of a hierarchy of
both domain-specific and domain-general machinery, and
temporal receptive windows using a narrated story. J. Neurosci.
we can then begin to understand the relative contributions 31, 2906–2915
of each as well as the dynamics of their interaction and how 27 Ben-Yakov, A. et al. (2012) Loss of reliable temporal structure in event-
those dynamics affect behavioral outcomes (Box 3). related averaging of naturalistic stimuli. Neuroimage 63, 501–506

125
Opinion Trends in Cognitive Sciences March 2014, Vol. 18, No. 3

28 Fedorenko, E. et al. (2011) Functional specificity for high-level 52 Rigotti, M. et al. (2010) Internal representation of task rules by
linguistic processing in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. recurrent dynamics: the importance of the diversity of neural
S. A. 108, 16428–16433 responses. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 4, 24
29 Monti, M.M. et al. (2009) The boundaries of language and thought in 53 Blumstein, S.E. and Amso, D. (2013) Dynamic functional organization
deductive inference. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 12554–12559 of language: insights from functional neuroimaging. Perspect. Psychol.
30 Monti, M.M. et al. (2012) Thought beyond language: neural dissociation Sci. 8, 44–48
of algebra and natural language. Psychol. Sci. 23, 914–922 54 Coltheart, M. (1999) Modularity and cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3,
31 Thompson-Schill, S.L. et al. (1997) Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex 115–120
in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proc. Natl. Acad. 55 Barrett, H.C. and Kurzban, R. (2006) Modularity in cognition: framing
Sci. U.S.A. 94, 14792–14797 the debate. Psychol. Rev. 113, 628–647
32 Thompson-Schill, S.L. et al. (1999) Effects of repetition and competition 56 Honey, C.J. et al. (2009) Predicting human resting-state functional
on activity in left prefrontal cortex during word generation. Neuron 23, connectivity from structural connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.
513–522 106, 2035–2040
33 Bedny, M. et al. (2008) Semantic adaptation and competition during 57 O’Reilly, J.X. et al. (2013) Causal effect of disconnection lesions on
word comprehension. Cereb. Cortex 18, 2574–2585 interhemispheric functional connectivity in rhesus monkeys. Proc.
34 Hindy, N.C. et al. (2012) The effect of object state-changes on event Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 13982–13987
processing: do objects compete with themselves? J. Neurosci. 32, 5795– 58 Le Bihan, D. et al. (2006) Artifacts and pitfalls in diffusion MRI. J.
5803 Magn. Reson. Imaging 24, 478–488
35 Hindy, N.C. et al. (2013) A cortical network for the encoding of object 59 Jones, D.K. and Cercignani, M. (2010) Twenty-five pitfalls in the
change. Cereb. Cortex http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht275 analysis of diffusion MRI data. NMR Biomed. 23, 803–820
36 Thompson-Schill, S.L. et al. (2005) The frontal lobes and the regulation 60 Jones, D.K. et al. (2013) White matter integrity, fiber count, and other
of mental activity. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15, 219–224 fallacies: the do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 73, 239–254
37 Gordon, P.C. et al. (2002) Memory-load interference in syntactic 61 Yeo, B.T.T. et al. (2011) The organization of the human cerebral cortex
processing. Psychol. Sci. 13, 425–430 estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J. Neurophysiol. 106,
38 Fedorenko, E. et al. (2007) The nature of working memory in linguistic, 1125–1165
arithmetic and spatial integration processes. J. Mem. Lang. 56, 246– 62 Power, J.D. et al. (2011) Functional network organization of the human
269 brain. Neuron 72, 665–678
39 Rodd, J.M. et al. (2010) The role of domain-general frontal systems in 63 Bassett, D.S. and Lynall, M.E. (2013) Network methods to characterize
language comprehension: evidence from dual-task interference and brain structure and function. In Cognitive Neurosciences: The Biology
semantic ambiguity. Brain Lang. 115, 182–188 of the Mind (Fifth Edition) (Gazzaniga, M., Ivry, R.B. and Mangun,
40 Carretti, B. et al. (2009) Role of working memory in explaining the G.R., eds), W.W. Norton & Company
performance of individuals with specific reading comprehension 64 Baker, C.I. et al. (2007) Visual word processing and experiential origins
difficulties: a meta-analysis. Learn. Individ. Differ. 19, 246–251 of functional selectivity in human extrastriate cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad.
41 Novick, J.M. et al. (2009) A case for conflict across multiple domains: Sci. U.S.A. 104, 9087–9092
memory and language impairments following damage to ventrolateral 65 Hamamé, C.M. et al. (2013) Dejerine’s reading area revisited with
prefrontal cortex. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 26, 527–567 intracranial EEG: selective responses to letter strings. Neurology 80,
42 Cragg, L. and Nation, K. (2010) Language and the development of 602–603
cognitive control. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2, 631–642 66 Bohland, J.W. and Guenther, F.H. (2006) An fMRI investigation of
43 Khanna, M.M. and Boland, J.E. (2010) Children’s use of language syllable sequence production. Neuroimage 32, 821–841
context in lexical ambiguity resolution. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 63, 160–193 67 Goldberg, A.E. (2003) Constructions: a new theoretical approach to
44 McVay, J.C. and Kane, M.J. (2012) Why does working memory capacity language. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 219–224
predict variation in reading comprehension? On the influence of mind 68 Jackendoff, R. (2002) Foundations of Language: Brain, Meaning,
wandering and executive attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 302–320 Grammar, Evolution, Oxford University Press
45 Novick, J.M. et al. (2013) Clearing the garden-path: improving 69 Bybee, J.L. (2010) Language, Usage and Cognition, Cambridge
sentence processing through cognitive control training. Lang. Cogn. University Press
Process. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2012.758297 70 Gibson, E. (1998) Linguistic complexity: locality of syntactic
46 Novais-Santos, S. et al. (2007) Resolving sentence ambiguity with dependencies. Cognition 68, 1–76
planning and working memory resources: evidence from fMRI. 71 McElree, B. et al. (2003) Memory structures that subserve sentence
Neuroimage 37, 361–378 comprehension. J. Mem. Lang. 48, 67–91
47 January, D. et al. (2009) Co-localization of stroop and syntactic 72 Gordon, P.C. et al. (2001) Memory interference during language
ambiguity resolution in Broca’s area: implications for the neural processing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 27, 1411–1423
basis of sentence processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 2434–2444 73 Lewis, R.L. et al. (2006) Computational principles of working memory
48 McMillan, C.T. et al. (2012) fMRI evidence for strategic decision- in sentence comprehension. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 447–454
making during resolution of pronoun reference. Neuropsychologia 74 Hale, J. (2001) A probabilistic Earley parser as a psycholinguistic
50, 674–687 model. In Proceedings of the Second Meeting of the North American
49 Nieuwland, M.S. et al. (2012) Brain regions that process case: evidence Chapter of the Association for Computational Linguistics on Language
from Basque. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2509–2520 Technologies. pp. 1–8, Association for Computational Linguistics
50 Wild, C.J. et al. (2012) Effortful listening: the processing of degraded 75 Levy, R. (2008) Expectation-based syntactic comprehension. Cognition
speech depends critically on attention. J. Neurosci. 32, 14010–14021 106, 1126–1177
51 Bassett, D.S. et al. (2013) Task-based core–periphery organization of 76 Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J.P. (2011) Experimental and theoretical
human brain dynamics. PLoS Comput. Biol. 9, e1003171 approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 70, 200–227

126

You might also like