You are on page 1of 8

Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2006.00863.

x
162
    pp  –   

Blackwell Publishing LtdOxford, UKJIRJournal of Intellectual Disability Research0964-26332006 Blackwell Publishing Ltd2006512162169Original ArticleWorking memory in children with MIDM. J. Van der Molen et al

Verbal working memory in children with mild


intellectual disabilities
M. J. Van der Molen,1,2 J. E. H. Van Luit,1 M. J. Jongmans1 & M. W. Van der Molen3
1 Department of General and Special Education, Utrecht University, Utrecht, the Netherlands
2 ’s Heeren Loo Kwadrant, Ermelo, the Netherlands
3 Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Abstract The finding of a phonological-loop capacity deficit


has important implications for the remedial training
Background Previous research into working mem-
of children with mild ID.
ory of individuals with intellectual disabilities (ID)
has established clear deficits. The current study Keywords automatic rehearsal, central executive,
examined working memory in children with mild ID mental retardation, mild intellectual disabilities,
(IQ –) within the framework of the Baddeley phonological loop, working memory
model, fractionating working memory into a central
executive and two slave systems, the phonological
loop and visuo-spatial sketchpad. Introduction
Method Working memory was investigated in three
Several studies have shown that children with intel-
groups:  children with mild ID (mean age  years
lectual disabilities (ID) suffer from working-memory
 months),  chronological age-matched control
problems (Hulme & Mackenzie ; Russell et al.
children (mean age  years  months) and  mental
; Jarrold & Baddeley ; Jarrold et al. ,
age-matched control children (mean age  years
). Working memory is referred to as a system in
 months). The groups were given multiple assess-
which information can be temporarily stored and
ments of the phonological-loop and central-executive
manipulated so as to support ongoing complex
components.
cognitive activities, such as reading and listening
Results The results showed that the children with
(Daneman & Carpenter ; Baddeley ). The
mild ID had an intact automatic rehearsal, but per-
complexity of this construct is illustrated by the work-
formed poorly on phonological-loop capacity and
ing-memory model proposed by Baddeley ().
central-executive tests when compared with children
This model consists of three components coined the
matched for chronological age, while there were only
‘phonological loop’, the ‘visuo-spatial sketchpad’,
minimal differences relative to the performance of the
and the ‘central executive’. More recently, a fourth
children matched for mental age.
component, the ‘episodic buffer’, has been added
Conclusions This overall pattern of results is consis-
(Baddeley ). The phonological loop is conceived
tent with a developmental delay account of mild ID.
of as a mechanism storing verbal information on a
temporarily basis. An automatic rehearsal process can
Correspondence: Mariët Van der Molen, Heidelberglaan ,  CS activate the incoming information for the loop. The
Utrecht, the Netherlands (e-mail: m.vandermolen@fss.uu.nl). visuo-spatial sketchpad is a mechanism dedicated to
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
163
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

the storage of dynamic and static visuo-spatial infor- This pattern of findings is difficult to interpret in
mation for a short period. The phonological loop and terms of either a ‘developmental-delay’ hypothesis
the visuo-spatial sketchpad are considered slave sys- (i.e. cognitive processes in children with MID are the
tems that are coordinated by the central executive same as in typically developing children, but develop
(CE). In addition to coordinating the slave systems, slower and reach asymptotic levels at an earlier age)
the CE is involved in retrieving and manipulating or ‘developmental-difference’ account (i.e. children
information from long-term memory and in the inhi- with MID are assumed to have a kind of structural
bition of prepotent responses. Finally, the episodic deficit) of MID (for a review, see Bennett-Gates &
buffer is assumed to temporarily hold information Zigler ).
from working memory and long-term memory in a The primary goal of the present study was to
multi-modal code (for a recent review, see Baddeley extend these findings by examining working-mem-
). ory function in children with MID by presenting
Studies applying the Baddeley model for examin- them multiple assessments of both the phonologi-
ing working memory in individuals with ID have cal-loop and CE components of the Baddeley
focused primarily on the phonological-loop compo- model. Their performance will be compared with
nent. These studies revealed that the capacity of the children matched for CA and MA, as in the Henry
phonological loop is constrained in those individuals, & MacLean () study. Based on their study, it
both in adults (Numminen et al. ; Numminen is assumed that children with MID will perform
et al. ) and in children (Russell et al. ; worse than the children matched for CA on all
Jarrold & Baddeley ; Henry ; Henry & tasks. The most interesting comparison is between
Maclean ). Furthermore, automatic rehearsal the children with MID and those matched for MA.
does not seem to take place in children with a mental This comparison should reveal whether the uneven
age (MA) below  years (Jarrold et al. ). These pattern of phonological-loop findings observed by
findings have been interpreted to suggest that a defi- Henry & MacLean () is robust. In addition,
cient phonological loop contributes to the cognitive this comparison should decide between develop-
deficits seen in individuals with Down syndrome mental-delay and developmental-difference
(Kanno & Ikeda ), or at least contributes to accounts of MID at the level of the CE component
their learning problems (e.g. Hulme & Mackenzie of working memory.
).
Unfortunately, little is known about the function-
ing of working memory in children with mild ID Method
(MID; IQ ranging between  and ). To the best of
Participants
our knowledge, there are only three studies available
applying the Baddeley model to working-memory A total of  children participated in the study.
function in children with MID. Henry () Among them were  children with MID ( boys,
observed a reduced phonological-loop capacity and  girls), ranging in age from  to  years. These
for the children with an IQ in the lower range also children attended a special school for mild intellec-
poor performances on tasks used to assess the CE. tually disabled pupils. Half of these children lived at
Rosenquist et al. () found that automatic home, while the other half lived in special residential-
rehearsal was deficient in children with MID. Finally, care settings. Children, diagnosed by psychiatrists,
Henry & MacLean () compared memory perfor- who also have attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,
mance of children with MID with control children Pervasive Developmental Disorder Not Otherwise
matched either on chronological age (CA) or on MA. Specified (PDD-NOS), Down syndrome or other
The results show that children with MID performed specific aetiologies were excluded. Two control
consistently poorer on phonological-loop tests and on groups were recruited: one group matched for CA
CE tests than children matched for CA. Compared and the other matched for MA. The CA-matched
with those matched for MA, the children with MID control group, recruited from a secondary school,
did worse on some of the phonological-loop and CE consisted of  typically developing children ( boys,
tests, but they performed equally well on other tests.  girls), who ranged in age from  to  years. The
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
164
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

Table 1 Participants’ details

MID group MA-matched group CA-matched group


(n = 50) (n = 25) (n = 25)

M SD M SD M SD

Chronological age (months) 182.96 12.32 131.52 13.27 183.24 9.31


Mental age (Raven SPM) 128.26 23.48 131.96 17.59 179.99 19.78

CA, chronological age; MA, mental age; MID, mild intellectual disabilities; SPM, Standard Progressive Matrices.

MA-matched control group, recruited from two pri- Central executive


mary schools, consisted of  typically developing
Four tests were administered to assess different
children ( boys,  girls), who ranged in age from 
aspects of executive functioning; dual-task manage-
to  years. Scores on the Raven Standard Progres-
ment, information retrieval and manipulation,
sive Matrices (Raven et al. ) were used for
planning and inhibition. The test for dual-task
matching (see also Numminen et al. ). Informed
management was taken from Baddeley et al. ().
consent was obtained for all participants. Descriptive
Participants first performed a verbal span test and a
information of the participants is provided in Table .
visuo-spatial test, both in isolation, and then are
Group comparisons indicated that the MID group
required to perform both tests at the same time. The
had a similar mean age as the group matched for
dual-task score is expressed relative to those obtained
CA (P = .) and did not differ statistically on
during single-task performance.
mean mental age from the group matched on MA
A word-fluency test (Luteijn & Van der Ploeg )
(P = .).
was used to assess the ability to retrieve and manip-
ulate information from long-term memory. Two ver-
Materials and procedure sions were administered: an ‘animal’ and a ‘letter k’
version. The child has to name as many words (ani-
Phonological loop
mals and words starting with the letter ‘k’, respec-
Two tests were used to assess phonological-loop tively) as possible in  min. The score is the total
capacity: a digit span and a non-word test. In both amount of correct words in both versions.
tests, the participant has to immediately repeat digits Planning ability was assessed by administering the
or non-words in exactly the same order as presented. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised
Both tests yield span scores, that is, the maximum (WISC-R) subtest mazes (De Bruyn et al. ).
amount of items that could be correctly repeated. Finally, digit generation (DG) was used to assess
Two tests both with two conditions were presented the ability to inhibit prepotent responses (Towse &
to allow for an examination of automatic rehearsal; a Mclachlan ). This test requires participants to
digit span test (with vs. without articulatory suppres- produce a number between  and ,  times. This
sion) that provides an estimate of the automatic test generates multiple scores of randomness that can
rehearsal ability, articulatory suppression requires the be calculated using the RGCalc scoring program
participants to repeat out loud the Dutch word ‘de’ (Towse & Neil ). In addition to the commonly
(in English ‘the’) during the presentation of the dig- used scores, Evans’ Random Number Generator
its; and a non-word test (with monosyllabic vs. two Score (RNG), Redundancy (R), Turning Point Index
syllabic words) to provide an estimate of the word (TPI), Phi Index gram (Phi), Phi Index gram
length ability (Baddeley ). All phonological-loop (Phi) and Adjacent Value Score (A-score), the num-
tests started with two items and, following correct ber of omissions was registered (i.e. the number of
repeats, one item is added until a maximum of times that the child was prompted to generate a num-
 items. ber but failed to do so).
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
165
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

Participants were tested individually in a separate Phonological loop


room at the school premises. All tests were completed
Significant group effects were found for both the digit
in one session of  h. There were short intermissions
span scores and the non-word scores (see Table ).
between tests, and longer rests were given on
Between-group analyses indicated that the perfor-
demand.
mance of the children with MID on the digit span
test was significantly worse compared with both the
group matched for MA (P < .) and the group
Results
matched for CA (P < .). In addition, children with
The results will be presented in three sections. The MID performed worse on the non-word test than the
first section presents the outcomes of the between- group matched for CA (P < .), but their scores
group comparisons for the phonological-loop tests, did not differ significantly from those of the group
and the second section presents the results that matched for MA.
emerged from the CE tests. All scores were submitted Two additional s, including an additional
to , Bonferroni tests were used for multiple within-subjects Task factor, were carried out to assess
comparisons, and Tamhane’s test was used when vari- group differences in automatic rehearsal. In one
ance differed between groups (Tabachnick & Fidell , the Task factor referred to word length (one
). The third section presents the outcomes of a vs. two syllables), and in the other referred to sup-
principal component analysis (PCA) of all test scores. pression (digit span vs. suppressed digit span). These
The factor scores were then submitted to a  analyses revealed significant effects of word length
to assess group differences in the functioning of the (F2,96 = ., P < .) and articulatory suppression
phonological loop and CE. Preliminary analyses (F2,97 = ., P < .). Both analyses failed to
revealed that gender did not alter the results reported reveal significant group by task effects, indicating that
below (F1,97 = ., P = .). all groups show similar articulatory suppression and

Table 2 Mean performance and standard deviations of the groups on the phonological-loop and the central-executive tests

MID group MA-matched CA-matched


(n = 50) group (n = 25) group (n = 25)

M SD M SD M SD F (P)

Digit 4.16 0.65 4.65 0.71 5.36 1.04 19.88 (<0.01)


Non-word 2.78 0.58 3.09 0.73 3.28 0.68 5.44 (<0.01)
Dual-task 94.69 11.46 97.25 11.20 97.34 10.09 0.69 (0.51)
Word fluency animal 17.86 4.92 20.64 5.65 22.12 6.33 5.56 (<0.01)
Word fluency letter 9.08 4.01 11.36 4.59 11.80 4.03 4.52 (<0.05)
Mazes 27.37 5.98 28.88 6.04 31.68 3.46 5.13 (<0.01)
DG RNG 0.31 0.08 0.28 0.05 0.25 0.06 5.36 (<0.01)
DG R 2.24 1.73 2.61 1.50 2.81 4.49 0.41 (0.64)
DG TPI 83.10 16.43 78.17 10.46 91.01 10.18 5.62 (<0.01)
DG Phi2 −3.73 0.41 −3.42 0.87 −3.92 0.59 4.36 (<0.05)
DG Phi7 −2.04 1.65 −1.14 1.21 −2.59 1.24 6.36 (<0.01)
DG A 33.44 14.04 29.87 6.96 24.29 7.98 5.48 (<0.01)
DG O 3.33 4.55 4.64 5.23 0.36 0.99 7.05 (<0.01)

CA, chronological age; DG, digit generation; MA, mental age; MID, mild intellectual disabilities.

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
166
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

word length effects, implying that the use of rehearsal The first factor explained .% of the total vari-
is comparable. ance and related to all four phonological-loop scores.
Thus, this factor was coined ‘phonological loop’. The
second factor explained .% of the total variance
Central executive
and related to the DG scores DG-RNG, DG-TPI
Virtually, all CE test scores discriminated signifi- and DG-A. A similar factor has been obtained previ-
cantly between groups, with the exception of the ously by Towse and co-workers, who dubbed this
dual-task test score and the DG test R score (see factor ‘pre-potent associates’ (Towse & Neil ;
Table ). Subsequent between-group comparisons Towse & Mclachlan ). DG-R and DG-Phi
indicated that the children with MID performed loaded on the third factor explained .% of the total
more poorly than the group matched for CA on word variance. This factor was entitled ‘production’, as
fluency animal (P < .), word fluency letter DG-R reflects the ability to produce each digit with
(P < .), mazes, DG RNG (Ps < .), DG TPI the same frequency and DG-Phi reflects the ability
(P < .), DG A and DG O (Ps < .), but not on to avoid specific response sets. The fourth factor was
DG Phi and DG Phi  (P > .). CE test scores labeled ‘memory retrieval and information manipu-
did not differ significantly between the children with lation’, as both word fluency tasks loaded on this
MID and the group matched for MA. factor explained .% of the total variance. The
maze score and the last two scores of DG, DG-Phi
and DG-O, loaded on the fifth factor, explained
Principal component analysis
.%. This factor was named ‘planning’. Finally, the
All test scores were submitted to a PCA with varimax dual-task test loaded on the sixth factor called ‘coor-
rotation (Tabachnick & Fidell ). The analysis dination’, explained .% of the total variance.
yielded six factors explaining .% of the total vari- The outcomes of the  are presented in
ance. The correlation coefficients of the test scores Table . It can be seen that the score on ‘memory
with the factor components are presented in Table . retrieval and information manipulation’ (factor )

Table 3 Loadings for the principal component analysis of the phonological-loop and the central-executive tests

Component

Test 1 2 3 4 5 6

Digit 0.788 0.215 0.105 0.165 −0.021 −0.089


Non-word 0.736 0.295 −0.097 0.074 0.127 −0.196
Long non-word 0.820 0.083 0.072 −0.028 −0.188 0.100
Articulatory suppressed digit 0.762 −0.019 −0.059 0.262 0.024 0.139
DG RNG −0.137 -0.778 0.226 −0.153 0.042 0.048
DG TPI 0.274 0.696 0.342 −0.083 −0.240 −0.101
DG A −0.156 -0.865 −0.171 −0.158 −0.139 −0.071
DG R −0.030 −0.072 0.839 0.183 0.022 0.129
DG Phi2 −0.042 −0.156 -0.832 0.137 0.039 0.100
Word fluency animal 0.079 0.117 0.038 0.823 −0.168 −0.037
Word fluency letter 0.273 0.097 −0.024 0.688 0.081 −0.071
Mazes −0.026 0.312 −0.207 0.112 -0.655 0.186
DG Phi7 −0.095 0.221 −0.112 0.202 0.724 0.175
DG O −0.047 −0.032 −0.094 −0.409 0.614 0.043
Dual-task 0.016 −0.026 0.023 −0.103 0.042 0.939

DG, digit generation.


Bold text indicates factor loadings >..

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
167
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

Table 4 Mean principle component analysis scores for each group

MID group MA-matched CA-matched


(n = 50) group (n = 25) group (n = 25)

Component M SD M SD M SD F (P)

1 Phonological loop −0.35 0.76 −0.04 0.82 0.75 1.20 12.67 (<0.01)
2 Pre-potent associates −0.18 1.22 −0.01 0.58 0.37 0.74 2.58 (0.08)
3 Production 0.05 0.59 −0.32 0.94 0.22 1.53 1.96 (0.15)
4 Memory RIM −0.30 0.87 0.33 1.13 0.28 0.95 4.65 (<0.01)
5 Planning 0.04 1.08 0.53 0.86 −0.62 0.56 8.28 (<0.01)
6 Coordination −0.20 0.99 0.21 1.02 0.19 0.95 2.06 (0.13)

CA, chronological age; MA, mental age; MID, mild intellectual disabilities; RIM, retrieval and information manipulation.

was lower for the children with MID compared with but on the tests tapping phonological-loop capacity,
the group matched for MA (P < .). The scores of they performed even more poorly than the group
the children with MID were lower than the CA group matched for MA. This differentiated pattern of
on the factors ‘phonological loop’ (factor , P < .), results suggests that the children with MID are defi-
‘memory retrieval and information manipulation’ cient in phonological storage, but their automatic
(factor , P < .) and ‘planning’ (factor , rehearsal seems to be intact. Although the results of
P < .). the PCA must be interpreted with caution, given the
modest sample size, the scores of the ‘phonological
loop’ factor, that emerged from this analysis, discrim-
inated significantly between the children with MID
Discussion
and their CA-matched controls. This finding is con-
This study set out to assess working memory in chil- sistent with the ID literature reporting recurrent
dren with MID within the context of the Baddeley phonological-loop deficits in individuals with mild
() model. The current focus was twofold. First, (Henry ; Henry & MacLean ) or severe
participants were presented with a battery of tests to (Jarrold & Baddeley ; Jarrold et al. ) ID.
assess the potential contributions of the phonological The results obtained from the executive-function
loop and CE in providing an account of the alleged tests indicated that, relative to age-matched controls,
deficiencies in working memory of individuals with the children with MID performed more poorly on all
MID. Second, the performance of the children with tests with the exception of the dual-task test. The
MID was compared with two control groups, one dual-task test, assessing the ability to direct and allo-
matched for MA and the other for CA, to assess cate attention, has been used widely to assess the
whether the alleged deficiencies of working memory functioning of the CE (e.g. Baddeley & Logie ).
refer to a structural defect vs. a developmental delay. Possibly, the current version of the dual-task test was
Most of the test scores revealed that children with not sufficiently effortful to qualify as a valid test of
MID performed as well as the children matched for the CE (see also Bull & Scerif ; Holtzer et al.
MA, ruling out a developmental-difference account ). The results that emerged from the PCA devi-
of the working-memory deficits. The children ated from the above pattern, in that the factor ‘pre-
matched for CA outperformed the children with potent associates’, associated with the RNG, TPI and
MID on most phonological-loop and CE tests. Over- A scores, failed to distinguish between groups. By
all, the current pattern of results is compatible with contrast, the factors ‘memory retrieval and informa-
the developmental-delay hypothesis of ID. tion manipulation’, associated with the word fluency
More specifically, the children with MID did show tests, and ‘planning’, associated with the maze test
the word length and articulatory-suppression effects, and two DG scores (DG-Phi and DG-O), discrim-
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
168
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

inated significantly between the children with MID ory of children with MID. Although the PCA finding
and their CA-matched controls. This differential pat- must be interpreted with caution given the modest
tern of findings is important for at least three reasons. sample size, the apparent limitation of the phonolog-
First, the finding that word fluency test and the maze ical store may have important implications for train-
test loaded on different factors contributes to the ing and education. In case of a reduced capacity of
literature supporting the fractionated nature of exec- the phonological store in children with MID, then
utive function (e.g. Miyake et al. ). Second, the there may be little, if anything, to improve their mem-
finding that the factor ‘pre-potent associates’ failed to ory function. These children may be best served by
discriminate between groups suggests that the ability removing the need to rely on verbal short-term mem-
to inhibit is intact in children with MID. This prelim- ory and, instead, presenting them with visual rather
inary conclusion should be assessed in future than verbal information (cf. Jarrold et al. ).
research employing a range of inhibition tasks, as the
pertinent literature suggests that the ability to inhibit
may comprise various components (e.g. Nigg ; Acknowledgements
Huizinga et al. ). Third, the finding that the fac-
This research was funded by grants of Stichting Ste-
tor ‘memory retrieval and information manipulation’
unfonds ‘s Heeren Loo, Stichting tot Steun VCVGZ
did discriminate between children with MID and the
and ‘s Heeren Loo Kwadrant, the Netherlands.
group matched for CA indicates that the ability to
The authors are grateful to Paul Eling and Gerty
retrieve information from long-term memory and
Lensveld-Mulders for their guidance.
holding this information in memory is compromised
in children with MID. Although the modest group
size precludes strong conclusions, the observation
that the scores of children with MID on this factor References
were even worse than those of the children matched Baddeley A D. () Working Memory, Vol. 11. Clarendon
for MA may point in a structural memory deficit. Press, Oxford.
This possibility should be pursued in future work. Baddeley A. D. () Exploring the central executive.
In conclusion, the current pattern of findings Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A,
demonstrates that children with MID have multiple –.
working-memory deficits. Within the context of the Baddeley A. D. () The episodic buffer: a new compo-
nent of working memory? Trends in Cognitive Sciences ,
Baddeley () model of working memory, the data
–.
present evidence to suggest that the storage of the
Baddeley A. () Working memory: looking back and
phonological loop is constrained while the ability to looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , –.
rehearse seems to be intact. In addition, the tests Baddeley A. D. & Logie R. H. () Working memory: the
used to assess the integrity of the CE control of multiple component model. In: Models of Working Memory
working memory indicate that children with MID (ed. A. M. P. Shah), pp. –. University Press, New
perform more poorly than the group of CA-matched York.
children. The majority of the current findings are Baddeley A., Della Sala S., Gray C., Papagno C. & Spinnler
consistent with a developmental-delay account of H. () Testing central executive functioning with a
pencil-and-paper test. In: Methodology of Frontal and Exec-
mild ID. Following the proponents of the develop-
utive Functions (ed. P. Rabbit), pp. –. Psychology
mental-delay hypothesis, this pattern of results indi- Press, Hove.
cates that working memory of children with MID has Bennett-Gates D. & Zigler E. () Resolving the devel-
a similar structure compared to typically developing opmental-difference debate: an evaluation of the triarchic
children; there are no particular strengths or weak- and systems theory models. In: Handbook of Mental Retar-
nesses (e.g. Bennett-Gates & Zigler ). This con- dation and Development (eds R. M. H. J. A. Burack &
E. Zigler), pp. –. Cambridge University Press,
clusion must be qualified by two findings. That is, the
Cambridge.
scores on the memory factor and the performance on
Bull R. & Scerif G. () Executive functioning as a pre-
the phonological storage tests discriminated between dictor of children’s mathematics ability. Shifting, inhibi-
children with MID and MA-matched controls. Both tion, and working memory. Developmental
findings point to structural deficits in working mem- Neuropsychology , –.

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
169
M. J. Van der Molen et al. • Working memory in children with MID

Daneman M. & Carpenter P. A. () Individual differ- Miyake A., Friedman N. P., Emerson M. J., Witzki A. H.,
ences in integrating information between and within sen- Howerter A. & Wager T. D. () The unity and diver-
tences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, sity of executive functions and their contributions to com-
Memory, and Cognition , –. plex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: a latent variable analysis.
De Bruyn E. E. J., Vander Steene G., Van Haassen P. P., Cognitive Psychology , –.
Coetsier P., Pijl Y. L., Spoelders-Claes R., Poortinga Y. Nigg J. T. () On inhibition/disinhibition in develop-
H., Stinissen J. & Lutje Spelberg H. C. () Wechsler mental psychopathology: views from cognitive and per-
Intelligence Scale for Children – Revised Nederlandse Uitgave sonality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy.
[WISC Dutch Version]. Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse. Psychological Bulletin , –.
Henry L. A. () How does the severity of a learning Numminen H., Service E., Ahonen T. & Ruoppila I. ()
disability affect working memory performance? Memory Working memory and everyday cognition in adults with
, –. Down’s syndrome. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Henry L. A. & Maclean M. () Working memory Research , –.
performance in children with and without intellectual Numminen H., Service E. & Ruoppila I. () Working
disabilities. American Journal on Mental Retardation , memory, intelligence and knowledge base in adult per-
–. sons with intellectual disability. Research in Developmental
Holtzer R., Stern Y. & Raktin B. C. () Age-related Disabilities , –.
differences in executive control of working memory. Raven J. C., Court J. H. & Raven J. () Standard Pro-
Memory and Cognition , –. gressive Matrices. Oxford Psychologists Press, Oxford.
Huizinga M., Dolan C. & van der Molen M. W. () Rosenquist C., Conners F. A. & Roskos-Ewoldsen B.
Age-related change in executive function: developmental () Phonological and visuo-spatial working memory
trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychology (in in individuals with intellectual disability. American
press). Journal on Mental Retardation , –.
Hulme C. & Mackenzie S. () Working Memory and Russell J., Jarrold C. & Henry L. () Working memory
Severe Learning Difficulties. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. in children with autism and with moderate learning dif-
Jarrold C. & Baddeley A. D. () Short-term memory for ficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry ,
verbal and visuospatial information in Down’s syndrome. –.
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry , –. Tabachnick B. G. & Fidell L. S. () Using Multivariate
Jarrold C., Baddeley A. D. & Hewes A. K. () Geneti- Statistics, th edn. Allyn and Bacon, Needham Heights,
cally dissociated components of working memory: evi- MA.
dence from Down’s and Williams syndrome. Towse J. N. & Mclachlan A. () An exploration of
Neuropsychology , –. random generation among children. British Journal of
Jarrold C., Baddeley A. D. & Hewes A. K. () Verbal Developmental Psychology , –.
short-term memory deficits in Down syndrome: a conse- Towse J. N. & Neil D. () Analyzing human random
quence of problems in rehearsal? Journal of Child Psychol- generation behavior: a review of methods used and a
ogy and Psychiatry , –. computer program for describing performance. Behaviour
Kanno K. & Ikeda Y. () Word-length effect in verbal Research and Methods, Instruments, Computers , –.
short-term memory in individuals with Down’s syn-
drome. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , – Accepted  March 
.
Luteijn F. & Van der Ploeg F. A. E. () Git: Groninger
Intelligentie Test. Handleiding [Groninger Intelligence Test.
Manual]. Swets & Zeitlinger, Lisse.

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd

You might also like